You are on page 1of 8

Contribution of Tactile and Kinesthetic

Perceptions to Handwriting in Taiwanese


Children in First and Second Grade
Tzu-Ying Yu, PhD, OT; Jim Hinojosa, PhD, OT, FAOTA; Tsu-Hsin Howe, PhD, OTR;
Gerald T. Voelbel, PhD
key words: proprioception, child development, psychomotor performance

ABSTRACT

This study examined the contribution of tactile and kinesthetic perceptions to handwriting
legibility and speed of 177 Taiwanese children in first and second grade. Five standardized
instruments assessed tactile and kinesthetic perceptions using handwriting legibility and speed
as outcome measures. Fine motor coordination, mental processing speed, age, and gender were
measured and served as covariates. Pearson correlations and regression analyses examined the
relationship between handwriting and tactile and kinesthetic perceptions. Handwriting speed
and legibility both significantly correlated with tactile perception, kinesthetic perception, and
covariates. Results from the regression analysis supported tactile and kinesthetic perceptions
as being significant predictors of both handwriting speed (F (6,170) = 25.87, p < .001, R2 =
.477) and legibility (F (6,170)= 11.043, p < .001, R2 = .280). Tactile perception contributed
more to handwriting speed and legibility than kinesthetic perception. Tactile and kinesthetic
perception should be assessed when evaluating handwriting. When children have difficulty
writing quickly or legibly, professionals should assess children’s tactile and kinesthetic abilities.

T actile and kinesthetic perceptions influence


a child’s movement and motor execution
through neurophysiologic mechanisms
(Gandevia, Butler, Hodges, & Taylor, 2002; Noback,
Strominger, Demarest, & Ruggiero, 2005). While
receives sensory information from the child’s hands
and fingers and uses this information to adjust and
correct the hand’s trajectory. The synthesis of tac-
tile and kinesthetic perception information during
writing provides essential feedback to a child about
writing, a child’s central nervous system carries both touch, pressure, orientation, and position of hands
tactile and kinesthetic perceptions to the somato- (Gardner, 1988; Goodwin & Wheat, 2004).
sensory cortex, which processes tactile and kines- Furthermore, tactile and kinesthetic perceptions
thetic perceptions and transfers this information to provide a child with the information needed to
the motor cortex. The motor cortex is then able to grasp and manipulate a pencil while writing (Cer-
regulate motor control based on these perceptions so mak, 2005; Ziviani & Wallen, 2005). During the task
that the child can perform fine-tuned motor control of writing, a child perceives tactile information from
of his or her hands and fingers for efficient writ- the hands and fingers about shapes, sizes, textures,
ing (Goodwin & Wheat, 2004; Noback et al., 2005). and pressure that allows the child to manipulate the
As a child writes, the nervous system constantly pencil and paper effectively. Kinesthetic perception

Tzu-Ying Yu, PhD, OT, is Assistant Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, I-Shou University, Kaohsiung City, Taiwan.
Jim Hinojosa, PhD, OT, FAOTA, is Professor, Tsu-Hsin Howe, PhD, OTR, is Assistant Professor, and Gerald T. Voelbel, PhD, is
Professor, Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, New York University, New York, New York.
Originally submitted June 30, 2011. Accepted for publication November 15, 2011. Posted online December 19, 2011.
The authors have no financial or proprietary interest in the materials presented herein.
Address correspondence to Tzu-Ying Yu at tyy207@nyu.edu.
This manuscript was accepted under the editorship of Jane Case-Smith, EdD, OTR/L, FAOTA.
doi: 10.3928/15394492-20111209-02

OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health • Vol. 32, No. 3, 2012 87


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015
provides a child with information about positions cil grip and the motor control of writing tools. Laszlo
and movements of the hand and fingers so they can and Bairstow (1983) demonstrated that kinesthetic
be adjusted to produce legible writing (Goodwin & training improved drawing in children with kines-
Wheat, 2004; Noback et al., 2005). thetic perceptual deficits and teachers reported that
Prior studies have determined that handwrit- their handwriting improved in the classroom. Zivi-
ing quality and speed is influenced by develop- ani et al. (1990) verified that kinesthetic perception
mental factors, which were used in this study as contributed to handwriting legibility in children
covariates. Specifically, age (Graham, Weintraub, with myelomeningocele, concluding that kinesthetic
Berninger, & Schafer, 1998; Hamstra-Bletz & Blote, perception influences motor control of handwriting.
1990; Rueckriegel et al., 2008), gender (Graham et al., Occupational therapists assume that tactile and
1998; Weintraub, Drory-Asayag, Dekel, Jokobovits, kinesthetic perceptual abilities exert a significant
& Parush, 2007), fine motor coordination (Volman, influence on children’s development of skilled
Van Schendel, & Jongmans, 2006; Feder et al., 2005; movements such as handwriting (Amundson, 1992;
Tseng & Chow, 2000; van Hoorn, Maathuis, Peters, & Benbow, 2005; Feder & Majnemer, 2007). However,
Hadders-Algra, 2010), and mental processing speed there is a lack of empirical evidence to support this
(Floyd, McGrew, & Evans, 2008; McCutchen, 2000) theoretical postulate. Thus, this study examined the
have all been verified as influences on handwriting contribution of tactile and kinesthetic perceptions to
quality and speed. handwriting legibility and speed in typically devel-
In contrast, limited research has examined the re- oping first and second grade children with devel-
lationship between tactile and kinesthetic perceptions opmental factors including age, gender, fine motor
and handwriting. These studies have examined the coordination, and mental processing speed as co-
relationship between handwriting and tactile percep- variates. We hypothesized that while controlling for
tions by assessing in-hand manipulation (Cornhill developmental factors associated with handwriting,
& Case-Smith, 1996; Feder et. al., 2005), finger iden- tactile and kinesthetic perceptions influence hand-
tification (Feder et al., 2005; Parush, Lifshitz, Yoch- writing speed and handwriting legibility.
man, & Weintraub, 2010), and handwriting pressure
(Khalid, Yunus, & Adnan, 2010; Wann, 1991). Feder Method
et al. and Cornhill and Case-Smith observed that tac- Sample
tile perception measured by finger identification and A power analysis for multiple regression (Faul,
in-hand manipulation significantly correlated with Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) determined a mini-
handwriting legibility in both children born prema- mum sample size of 130 was needed to achieve a
turely and typically developing children. Feder et al. power of 0.9 and to detect a small to medium effect
(2005) also reported a significant correlation between size of 0.1 with a level of .05. The first author recruited
finger identification and in-hand manipulation with from two public elementary schools in Taiwan where
slow handwriting speed. However, no associations there are no children with special needs. Both schools
between tactile perception and handwriting legibil- use a curriculum established by the Ministry of Edu-
ity or speed were found in other studies (Parush et cation. In Taiwan, children with special needs attend
al., 2010; Ziviani, Hayes, & Chant, 1990). Khalid et al. special education schools. A description of the study
(2010) and Wann (1991) examined handwriting pres- and invitations for the children’s participation were
sure as a component of tactile perception. Khalid et sent to 390 students’ parents. Parents of 239 children
al. concluded that both too much pressure and not returned signed consent forms. Sixty-two children
enough pressure negatively influenced children’s did not meet the inclusion criteria (3 had hand inju-
handwriting. These findings confirmed Wann’s kine- ries and 59 did not meet the English requirements).
matic study of 11 adults that found that modulation No child whose parents had agreed to allow him or
of handwriting pressure affected handwriting speed. her to take part refused to participate.
Weintraub and Graham (2000) and Preminger, The inclusion criteria were: (1) attended first or
Weiss, and Weintraub (2004) investigated the rela- second grade; (2) had been taking English classes for
tions between kinesthetic perception and hand- 1 year and could write the 26 letters of the English al-
writing. Findings from both studies supported that phabet; (3) reported by the teacher to be typically de-
handwriting legibility is related to kinesthetic per- veloping; and (4) received written permission from
ceptions. Occupational therapists (Benbow, 2005; parents or legal guardians and received children’s
Rosenblum, Goldstand, & Parush, 2006; Sudsawad, assents for participating in the study. The study was
Trombly, Henderson, & Tickle-Degnen, 2002) have approved by the University Committee on Activities
asserted that kinesthetic perception influences pen- Involving Human Subjects.

88 Copyright © American Occupational Therapy Foundation


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015
Instrumentation completes the task twice using each hand and the
The first author tested all children and asked each test is scored using the total time in seconds. Shorter
child directly for assent before testing. Predeter- completion time indicates better fine motor coordi-
mined random sequence testing took 25 to 35 min- nation.
utes for each child in a private and quiet room. Prior The Coding Subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence
to the data collection, the first author established Scale for Children-IV (WISC-Coding) (Wechsler,
acceptable inter-rater reliability and scoring pro- 2003) measures mental processing speed and has
cedures with experienced researchers who use the good reliability (r = .85) (Wechsler, 2003) and va-
tests frequently. lidity (Donders & Janke, 2008; Launey, Carroll, &
The Minnesota Handwriting Assessment–manu- Van Horn, 2007; Wechsler, 2003). In this study, the
script version (Reisman, 1999) is a norm-referenced WISC-Coding A was administered to children be-
instrument that assesses both handwriting legibil- tween 6 and 7 years of age and the WISC-Coding B
ity and speed of children in first and second grade was administered to 8-year-old children. The WISC-
based on copying 34 letters in 2.5 minutes. It has Coding A and B are time-limited tasks that require a
excellent inter-rater (r = .90 to .99) and intra-rater child to recognize which symbol is associated with
(r = .47 to .94) reliability (Feder & Majnemer, 2003), a matched geometric shape or number and to draw
and good content validity (Reisman, 1999). In this the symbol in its corresponding box or shape. The
study, raw scores were used to measure legibility standard scores of the WISC-Coding A and B were
and speed was measured by counting the number of used in the analyses. A low score suggests slow men-
letters completed in 2.5 minutes. tal processing speed, which means slow information
The Tactual Performance Test (Reitan & Wolf- processing and interpretation of stimuli (Marshall,
son, 1985) assesses tactile perception by evaluating Forstot, Callies, Peterson, & Schenck, 1997).
ability of a child (5 to 8 years of age) to recognize
the shape of an object through active manipulation Data Analysis
while blindfolded. The Tactual Performance Test re- First, univariate analyses were conducted on
quires a child to fit six blocks into the proper spaces background information and developmental factors
in 15 minutes under three test conditions: (1) with associated with handwriting. Second, Pearson cor-
the dominant hand; (2) with the non-dominant relations were used to determine the associations
hand; and (3) using both hands. The child’s score between all variables. Third, hierarchical multiple
is the average time (in seconds) taken to complete regression models were used to test the contribution
the three trials. Shorter completion time indicates of tactile and kinesthetic perceptions to handwriting
better tactile perception. Internal reliability is ac- legibility and speed. In the regression, handwriting
ceptable (range: .61 to .90) (Charter, 2001a, 2001b, legibility and speed were the dependent variables
2001c) and the intra-rater reliability is moderate (r and tactile and kinesthetic perceptions were the
= .76) (Schludermann, Schludermann, Merryman, & predictor variables. The covariates were age, gen-
Brown, 1983). der, fine motor coordination, and mental processing
The Imitating Hand Positions Subtest of the NEPSY- speed. Normality, linearity, and multicollinearity
II (Korkman, 1998) assesses neuropsychological devel- were examined prior to data analysis. All data were
opment in children aged 3 to 12 years and has a high analyzed using SPSS-PC 16.0 software (Norusis,
reliability (r > .80) and validity (Brooks, Sherman, & 2008).
Strauss, 2010; Korkman, 1998). It assesses kinesthetic
perception by having a child imitate 12 different hand Results
and finger positions with each hand as modeled by the
examiner. Scoring is 1 point for each correct response One hundred seventy-seven children, consisting
for a maximum of 24, with a low score indicative of of 93 boys (52.5%) and 84 girls (47.5%), participated
poor kinesthetic perception. in this cross-sectional study. Eighty-eight (52.5%)
The Grooved Pegboard Test (Klove, 1963; Mat- were in the first grade and 89 in the second grade
thews & Klove, 1964) measures fine motor coordina- (50.3%). Student ages ranged from 72 to 107 months
tion (Trites, 1989) and has good reliability (r = .80 to (mean = 93 months, standard deviation [SD] = 10.39
.81) (Knights & Moule, 1968) and concurrent validity months) with 47 (26.6%) aged 6 years, 47 (26.6%)
(Knights et al., 1991). For the child version of the test aged 7 years, and 83 (46.8%) aged 8 years. Almost all
(5 to 8 years of age), the child is instructed to pick children were right handed (n = 171, 96.6%). Pear-
up a peg from a container and place it into one of son correlations were used to examine the associa-
the holes of a pegboard (10 pegs in total). The child tions between handwriting and main outcome vari-

OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health • Vol. 32, No. 3, 2012 89


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015
Table 1
Multiple Regression Analyses of the Contribution of the Tactile and Kinesthetic Perceptions in Handwriting Speed (N = 177)a
Model 1 Model 2
2
R R2
Predictors ␤ t p R2 Change Predictors R2 Change
Block 1 (covariates)
Age .44 6.78 < .001 Age
Gender .02 0.27 .79 Gender
.355**b .355**b .355**,b .355**,b
GPT .10 1.51 .13 GPT
WISC-Coding Test .13 2.32 .02 WISC-Coding Test
Block 2
TPT -.37 -5.63 < .001 .464** .11** IHP .38** .025**
Block 3
IHP .12 2.04 .04 .477** .013* TPT .477** .097**
2
Final model R = .477, F = 25.87, p < .001
GPT = Grooved Pegboard Test; WISC = Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children; TPT = Tactual Performance Test; IHP = Imitation Hand Positions Test.
a
Dependent variable = Minnesota Handwriting Assessment–Handwriting Speed.
b
Included covariates.
*p < .05. **p < .01, indicated significant predictor.

ables (tactile and kinesthetic perceptions), as well handwriting speed relatively more than kinesthetic
as developmental factors. Results indicate that both perception.
handwriting speed and legibility significantly corre- The same procedure of regression models was
lated with tactile perception (r = .52 and .41, p < .01), conducted to obtain the respective contributions of
kinesthetic perception (r = .30 and .36, p < .01), age tactile and kinesthetic perceptions to handwriting
(r = .56 and .34, p < .01), fine motor coordination (r = legibility. The dependent variable was handwriting
.33 and .22, p < .01), and mental processing speed (r legibility as measured by the Minnesota Handwrit-
= .27 and .16, p < .01). ing Assessment–manuscript version. The indepen-
dent variables were the predictor variables and the
Contributions of Tactile and Kinesthetic covariates (age, gender, fine motor coordination,
Perceptions to Handwriting Performance and mental processing speed). Table 2 shows that
Two hierarchical regression models on handwrit- the regression model was found to be significant in
ing speed determined the respective contributions predicting handwriting legibility (F (6,170) = 11.043,
of tactile and kinesthetic perceptions. In the regres- p < .001, R2 = .280) with developmental factors to-
sion model, the covariates were entered into the first gether explaining 28.0% of the variance. Table 2 also
block. Tactile perception and kinesthetic perception presents the predictors that significantly contributed
were entered into the second and third blocks, re- to the prediction of handwriting legibility. It shows
spectively, to determine kinesthetic perception’s that tactile perception (t = -4.09, p < .001), kinesthetic
prediction of handwriting speed when controlling perception (t = 3.65, p < .001), and age (t = 2.85, p
for covariates and tactile perception. Then, model 2 = .005) significantly contributed to the prediction of
was conducted with the covariates in the first block handwriting legibility. Similar to handwriting speed,
but with the order of entering the predictor variables the results indicated that tactile perception contrib-
into the second and third blocks reversed; that is, uted relatively more than kinesthetic perception to
kinesthetic perception and tactile perception were handwriting legibility.
entered into the second and third blocks, respec-
tively, to determine tactile perception’s prediction Discussion
of handwriting speed when controlling for devel-
opmental factors and kinesthetic perception. Table This study confirms that both tactile and kines-
1 summarizes the contribution of tactile and kines- thetic perceptions correlate significantly with hand-
thetic perceptions to handwriting speed. The results writing speed and legibility. It also confirms that tac-
also indicated that tactile perception contributes to tile and kinesthetic perceptions significantly predict

90 Copyright © American Occupational Therapy Foundation


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015
Table 2
Multiple Regression Analyses of the Contribution of the Tactile and Kinesthetic Perceptions in Handwriting Legibility (N = 177)
Model 1 Model 2
2
R R2
Predictors ␤ t p R2 Change Predictors R2 Change
Block 1 (covariates)
Age .22 2.85 .005 Age
Gender .05 .70 .48 Gender
.133**,b .133**,b .133**,b .133**,b
GPT .09 1.13 .26 GPT
WISC-Coding Test .05 .78 .44 WISC-Coding Test
Block 2
TPT -.32 -4.09 < .001 .224** .091** IHP .210** .077**
Block 3
IHP .25 3.65 < .001 .280** .056** TPT .280** .071**
2
Final model R = .280, F = 11.043, p < .001
GPT = Grooved Pegboard Test; WISC = Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children; TPT = Tactual Performance Test; IHP = Imitation Hand Positions Test.
a
Dependent variable = Minnesota Handwriting Assessment–Handwriting Speed.
b
Included covariates.
*p < .05. **p < .01, indicated significant predictor.

handwriting speed and legibility and account for perception and handwriting legibility or speed in
variance in handwriting performance. Although the older children with handwriting difficulties. In an-
findings support a correlation between tactile and other study of older children, Chang (1997) found
kinesthetic perception and handwriting, they cannot no correlation between handwriting legibility and
be interpreted to suggest causality. The findings only speed and tactile perception in 30 typically develop-
support an association between tactile and kinesthet- ing children in third grade. Based on her findings,
ic perceptions and handwriting speed and legibility. she suggested that older children depend less on
In addition, the study reveals that tactile perception tactile perception for their handwriting legibility
contributes more to the outcomes of handwriting than younger children do. The discrepancy between
speed and legibility than kinesthetic perception for previous studies and this current study can be ex-
typically developing children between the ages of 6 plained by the different populations, the different
and 8 years. measure used, or the possibility that children at
Theoretically, sensory-perceptual stimuli con- various ages might rely on tactile and kinesthetic
tribute to motor performance and control (Cermak, perception to different degrees while handwriting.
2005; Noback et al., 2005). These findings provide ad- In this study, the participants were first and second
ditional empirical evidence to support the theoreti- grade students who were learning handwriting and
cal postulate that tactile and kinesthetic perceptions may have needed to rely more on tactile and kin-
from hands and fingers during writing are associat- esthetic perception to write faster and more legibly.
ed with the motor control and motor programming When children learn how to write, they have to pay
required for legible and fast handwriting. The find- close attention to the feeling from their hand and fin-
ings indicate that children in first and second grade ger positions and touch sense to grasp the writing
rely on both tactile and kinesthetic perceptions while tools appropriately. Younger writers who engage in
writing. Thus, the findings provide evidence sup- writing tasks develop motor control in their hands
porting the theoretical postulate described as the and fingers over time.
feedback loop of tactile and kinesthetic perceptions An important finding in this study was that the
in children’s handwriting. 6- to 8-year-old children relied on tactile perception
This study’s findings were consistent with those more than kinesthetic perception for both writing
of previous studies (Cornhill & Case-Smith, 1996; speed and legibility. This finding suggests that chil-
Feder et al., 2005), but were different than those dren’s legible handwriting involves tactile discrimi-
reported by Ziviani et al. (1990) and Parush et al. nation of the objects through manipulation (i.e.,
(2010), who found no association between tactile tactile perception) more than the ability to perceive

OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health • Vol. 32, No. 3, 2012 91


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015
the hand position and movement (i.e., kinesthetic ing quickly. In addition, this study again supported
perception). These findings corroborate previous that the covariates age, fine motor coordination, and
research (Cornhill & Case-Smith, 1996; Feder et al., mental processing speed were factors significantly
2005; Khalid et al., 2010; Wann, 1991) supporting that associated with handwriting speed and legibility. In
factors of tactile perception, such as in-hand manip- summary, results support that professionals should
ulation, finger identification, or handwriting pres- assess tactile perception and kinesthetic perception
sure, influence handwriting legibility and speed. as part of evaluation of children’s handwriting.
One interesting observation about the research
sample is that they had “very good” fine motor Acknowledgments
coordination with mean scores on the Grooved This article is based on research conducted by the first
Pegboard Test two standard deviations above the author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the
mean of the norms for American children. They also Doctorate of Philosophy in Occupational Therapy in the
Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human De-
scored 1 standard deviation above the mean of the
velopment at New York University. The authors thank all
norms for American children on mental processing of the children who participated in the study. Presented at
speed (WISC-Coding Test). Better performance on the World Federation of Occupational Therapists (WFOT)
the WISC-Coding Test suggests that these Taiwanese XV Congress, May 4, 2010, Santiago, Chile, and the 11th
children efficiently recognized morphological forms Occupational Therapy Annual Conference, April 9, 2011,
and could proficiently execute the hand movements. Taipei City, Taiwan. Supported by Dean’s Grants for Stu-
Prior studies (Chow & Henderson, 2003; Hender- dent Research, New York University, 2009-2010.
son & Sugden, 1992; Hsu, Cherng, Yu, & Cheng-Sea,
2004) support that Chinese children perform signifi- References
cantly better on manual dexterity tasks than Ameri- Amundson, S. J. (1992). Handwriting: Evaluation and interven-
tion in school settings. In J. Case-Smith & C. Pehoski (Eds.),
can children. Other researchers (Li, 2002; Tseng, Development of hand skills in the child (pp. 63-78). Rockville,
1998) hypothesized that Chinese children develop MD: American Occupational Therapy Association.
better manual dexterity as they are encouraged to Benbow, M. (2005). Principles and practices of teaching handwrit-
engage in academic activities, like handwriting and ing. In A. Henderson & C. Pehoski (Eds.), Hand function in the
drawing, at a younger age. Future studies should child (2nd ed., pp. 321-344). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
explore whether the contributions of tactile and kin- Brooks, B. L., Sherman, E. M., & Strauss, E. (2010). Test
esthetic perceptions to handwriting in Chinese chil- Review: NEPSY-II: A Developmental Neuropsychological
dren in Taiwan can be generalized to other regions Assessment, Second Edition. Child Neuropsychology, 16, 80-101.
doi:10.1080/09297040903146966
of the world or other cultures.
Cermak, S. (2005). Perceptual functions of the hand. In A.
Henderson & C. Pehoski (Eds.), Hand function in the child (2nd
Limitations and Directions for Further Research ed., pp. 63-88). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
The results of the study must be interpreted with Chang, S. H. (1997). Relationship between handwriting and perceptual
caution due to the restricted age range and the char- performance in third-grade Chinese children (Master’s thesis).
acteristics of the sample population of Taiwanese Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database.
children. Future studies should examine kinesthetic (UMI No. 1384887)
and tactile as a function of age. These studies would Charter, R. A. (2001a). Coefficients alpha for the tactual perfor-
lead to an understanding of the contributions of tac- mance test trials. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 893.
tile and kinesthetic perceptions to handwriting at Charter, R. A. (2001b). Difference score reliability for tactual per-
different times in children’s development. formance test trials. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 941-942.
doi:10.2466/PMS.92.3.911-942
Charter, R. A. (2001c). Tactual performance test trials: Internal
Clinical Implications
consistency reliability using the Gilmer-Feldt coeffi-
Professionals often assume that the tactile and cient. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 363-366. doi:10.2466/
kinesthetic perceptual abilities of children exert a PMS.93.5.363-366
significant influence on their development of skilled Chow, S. M. K., & Henderson, S. E. (2003). Interrater and
movements needed for handwriting. These study test-retest reliability of the Movement Assessment Battery
findings demonstrate that tactile and kinesthetic for Children for Chinese preschoolers. American Journal of
perceptions contribute to handwriting speed and Occupational Therapy, 57, 574-577.
legibility. The findings suggest that professionals Cornhill, H., & Case-Smith, J. (1996). Factors that relate to
good and poor handwriting. American Journal of Occupational
should pay close attention to children’s tactile abil-
Therapy, 50, 732-739.
ity to perceive the shape, size, texture, and pressure
Donders, J., & Janke, K. (2008). Criterion validity of the Wechsler
exerted on objects through manual and in-hand ma- Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition after pedi-
nipulations when children are having difficulty writ- atric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International

92 Copyright © American Occupational Therapy Foundation


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015
Neuropsychological Society, 14, 651-655. doi:10.1017/ Knights, R. M., & Moule, A. D. (1968). Normative data on the
S1355617708080752 motor steadiness battery for children. Perceptual and Motor
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power Skills, 26, 643-650.
3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the Korkman, M. (1998). NAPSY - A Developmental Neuropsychological
social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Methods, 39, 175-191. Laszlo, J. I., & Bairstow, P. J. (1983). Kinaesthesis: Its mea-
Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting development, surement, training and relationship to motor control.
competency, and intervention. Developmental Medicine & Child Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 411-421.
Neurology, 49, 312-317. doi:10.1080/14640748308402142
Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2003). Children’s handwriting Launey, K. B., Carroll, J., & Van Horn, K. R. (2007). Concurrent
evaluation tools and their psychometric properties. Physical validity of the WISC-IV in eligibility decisions for students
& Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 23(3), 65-84. doi:10.1080/ with educable mental disabilities. Psychological Reports, 100,
J006v23n03_05 1165-1170. doi:10.2466/PR0.100.3.1165-1170
Feder, K. P., Majnemer, A., Bourbonnais, D., Platt, R., Blayney, Li, Y. R. (2002). Student homework practices in Taipei city elemen-
M., & Synnes, A. (2005). Handwriting performance in pre- tary schools (Unpublished master’s thesis). Taipei Municipal
term children compared with term peers at age 6 to 7 University of Education, Taipei City, Taiwan.
years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 47, 163-170. Marshall, P. S., Forstot, M., Callies, A., Peterson, P. K., & Schenck,
doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01110.x C. H. (1997). Cognitive slowing and working memory dif-
Floyd, R. G., McGrew, K. S., & Evans, J. J. (2008). The relative ficulties in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychosomatic Medicine,
contributions of the Cattell-Horn-Carroll cognitive abilities in 59, 58-66.
explaining writing achievement during childhood and ado- Matthews, C. G., & Klove, H. (1964). Instruction manual for the
lescence. Psychology in the Schools, 45, 132-144. doi:10.1002/ Adult Neuropsychology Test Battery. Madison, WI: University of
pits.20284 Wisconsin Medical School.
Gandevia, S. C., Butler, J. E., Hodges, P. W., & Taylor, J. L. (2002). McCutchen, D. (2000). Knowledge, processing, and working
Balancing acts: Respiratory sensations, motor control and memory: Implications for a theory of writing. Educational
human posture. Clinical and Experimental Pharmacology and Psychologist, 35, 13-23. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3501_3
Physiology, 29, 118-121. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1681.2002.03611.x
Noback, C. R., Strominger, N. L., Demarest, R. J., & Ruggiero, D.
Gardner, E. P. (1988). Somatosensory cortical mechanisms of A. (2005). The human nervous system: Structure and function (6th
feature detection in tactile and kinesthetic discrimination. ed., pp. 155-328). Totowa, NJ: Human Press.
Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 66, 439-454.
doi:10.1139/y88-074 Norusis, M. (2008). SPSS 16.0 Guide to data analysis. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Goodwin, A. W., & Wheat, H. E. (2004). Sensory signals in neural
populations underlying tactile perception and manipulation. Parush, S., Lifshitz, N., Yochman, A., & Weintraub, N. (2010).
Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 53-77. doi:10.1146/annurev. Relationships between handwriting components and under-
neuro.26.041002.131032 lying perceptual-motor functions among students during
copying and dictation tasks. OTJR: Occupation, Participation
Graham, S., Weintraub, N., Berninger, V., & Schafer, W. (1998). and Health, 30, 39-48. doi:10.3928/15394492-20091214-06
Development of handwriting speed and legibility in
Grades 1-9. The Journal of Educational Research, 92, 42-52. Preminger, F., Weiss, P. L., & Weintraub, N. (2004). Predicting
doi:10.1080/00220679809597574 occupational performance: Handwriting versus keyboarding.
American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 58, 193-201.
Hamstra-Bletz, L., & Blote, A. W. (1990). Development of hand-
writing in primary school: A longitudinal study. Perceptual and Reisman, J. (1999). Minnesota Handwriting Assessment. San
Motor Skills, 70, 759-770. doi:10.2466/PMS.70.3.759-770 Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1992). Movement Assessment Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan
Battery for Children. London, England: Psychological Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and clinical interpretation.
Corporation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.

Hsu, Y. W., Cherng, R. J., Yu, T. Y., & Cheng-Sea, M. J. (2004). Rosenblum, S., Goldstand, S., & Parush, S. (2006). Relationships
Comparison of the performance of preschoolers from Taiwan among biomechanical ergonomic factors, handwriting prod-
and USA in the Movement Assessment Battery for Children. uct quality, handwriting efficiency, and computerized hand-
Formosan Journal of Physical Therapy, 29, 307-316. writing process measures in children with and without hand-
writing difficulties. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
Khalid, P. I., Yunus, J., & Adnan, R. (2010). Extraction of dynamic 60, 28-39. doi:10.5014/ajot.60.1.28
features from hand drawn data for the identification of chil-
dren with handwriting difficulty. Research in Developmental Rueckriegel, S. M., Blankenburg, F., Burghardt, R., Ehrlich,
Disabilities, 31, 256-262. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2009.09.009 S., Henze, G., Mergl, R., et al. (2008). Influence of age and
movement complexity on kinematic hand movement param-
Klove, H. (1963). Clinical neuropsychology. In F. M. Forster (Ed.), eters in childhood and adolescence. International Journal
The medical clinics of North America (pp. 1647-1658). New York: of Developmental Neuroscience, 26, 655-663. doi:10.1016/j.
Saunders. ijdevneu.2008.07.015
Knights, R. M., Ivan, L. P., Ventureyra, E. C., Bentivoglio, Schludermann, E. H., Schludermann, S. M., Merryman, P. W., &
C., Stoddart, C., Winogron, W., & Bawden, H. N. (1991). Brown, B. W. (1983). Halstead’s studies in the neuropsychol-
The effects of head injury in children on neuropsycho- ogy of aging. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 2, 49-172.
logical and behavioural functioning. Brain Injury, 5, 339-351. doi:10.1016/0167-4943(83)90016-X
doi:10.3109/02699059109008107

OTJR: Occupation, Participation and Health • Vol. 32, No. 3, 2012 93


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015
Sudsawad, P., Trombly, C. A., Henderson, A. & Tickle-Degnen, L. Wann, J. (1991). The control of pen pressure in handwrit-
(2002). Testing the effects of kinesthetic training on handwrit- ing: A subtle point. Human Movement Science, 10, 223-246.
ing performance in first-grade students. American Journal of doi:10.1016/0167-9457(91)90005-I.
Occupational Therapy, 56, 26-33. doi:10.5014/ajot.56.1.26 Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th
Trites, R. (1989). Lafayette Grooved Pegboard Task. Instruction/Owner’s ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Manual. Lafayette, IN: Lafayette Instrument Company. Weintraub, N., Drory-Asayag, A., Dekel, R., Jokobovits, H., &
Tseng, M. H. (1998). Development of pencil grip position in pre- Parush, S. (2007). Developmental trends in handwriting per-
school children. The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, formance among middle school children. OTJR: Occupation,
18, 207-224. Participation and Health, 27, 104-112.
Tseng, M. H., & Chow, S. M. (2000). Perceptual-motor function of Weintraub, N., & Graham, S. (2000). The contribution of gender,
school-age children with slow handwriting speed. American orthographic, finger function, and visual-motor processes to
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 54, 83-88. the prediction of handwriting status. The Occupational Therapy
van Hoorn, J. F., Maathuis, C. G., Peters, L. H., & Hadders-Algra, Journal of Research, 20, 121-140.
M. (2010). Handwriting, visuomotor integration, and neuro- Ziviani, J., Hayes, A., & Chant, D. (1990). Handwriting: A percep-
logical condition at school age. Developmental Medicine & Child tual motor disturbance in children with myelomeningocele.
Neurology, 52, 941-947. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2010.03715.x The Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 10, 12-26.
Volman, M. J., Van Schendel, B. M., & Jongmans, M. J. (2006). Ziviani, J., & Wallen, M. (2005). The development of graphomotor
Handwriting difficulties in primary school children: A search skills. In A. Henderson & C. Pehoski (Eds.), Hand function in
for underlying mechanisms. American Journal of Occupational the child (2nd ed., pp. 217-236). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Therapy, 60, 451-460. doi:10.5014/ajot.60.4.451

94 Copyright © American Occupational Therapy Foundation


Downloaded from otj.sagepub.com at PURDUE UNIV LIBRARY TSS on May 18, 2015

You might also like