You are on page 1of 22

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/282820265

Concept maps and conceptual change in physics

Chapter · January 2009


DOI: 10.4018/978-1-59904-992-2.ch017

CITATIONS READS
3 1,532

5 authors, including:

Manuel Montanero Pedro J. Pardo


Universidad de Extremadura Universidad de Extremadura
97 PUBLICATIONS   432 CITATIONS    136 PUBLICATIONS   467 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Aprendizaje cooperativo View project

Asesoramiento educativo View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Manuel Montanero on 22 January 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


325

Chapter 17
Concept Maps and Conceptual
Change in Physics
Angel Luis Pérez Rodríguez
University of Extremadura, Spain

Maria Isabel Suero López


University of Extremadura, Spain

Manuel Montanero-Fernández
University of Extremadura, Spain

Pedro J. Pardo Fernández


University of Extremadura, Spain

Manuel Montanero-Morán
University of Extremadura, Spain

Abstract
The authors describe and discuss some recent applications of concept maps to physics teaching. They
begin by reviewing the literature on applications of concept maps to science teaching, and argue for
the usefulness of this resource in facilitating processes of conceptual change. They then describe two
experiments on the collaborative use of concept maps to this end. The first was a study of how a team
of teachers designed learning sequences using three-dimensional maps. In the second, concept maps
were constructed and then collaboratively re-constructed by various groups of students. Finally, they
discuss the preliminary results of these experiments on the processes of conceptual change, and suggest
lines for further research.

Introduction veloped systems for the graphical representation


of declarative knowledge. The resulting diagrams
From the mid 20th century onwards, researchers consist of polygons and lines linking them to rep-
in various disciplines – philosophy, philology, resent the underlying structure of the knowledge
artificial intelligence, and psychology – have de- concerned. The polygons symbolize various kinds
of nodes (objects, concepts, events, actions, etc.).
DOI: 10.4018/978-1-59904-992-2.ch017
The relationships represented by the links may be

Copyright © 2010, IGI Global. Copying or distributing in print or electronic forms without written permission of IGI Global is prohibited.
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

semantic (as in the classic associative networks of learn how to learn (Novak & Gowin, 1984). In
Quilliam), sequential (as in flowcharts), or func- a series of studies, Okebukola and Jegede have
tional (as in organigrams). A concept map is a type documented some of the main advantages of the
of diagram that represents semantic relationships use of concept maps as a strategy for learning
between concepts in the form of a tree. The links science content, in particular, their positive influ-
connecting the concepts are labelled with words ence on students’ attitudes, strategic behaviour,
or phrases that indicate some property of one of and academic performance (Okebukola, 1990;
the concepts. The overall diagram represents a Okebukola & Jegede, 1988, 1989). In this vein,
hierarchical propositional structure of the body McCrudden, Schraw, Lehman and Poliquin (2007)
of knowledge. found that subjects who studied a scientific text
Since their introduction by Novak in the 1960s, accompanied by a diagram representing the se-
concept maps have grown into a powerful teaching mantic organization of the content understood
tool with multiple applications because of their it better than another group that spent the same
capacity to specify, design, and share knowledge. amount of time in studying the text alone.
Their use has spread into many domains of learn- Concept maps have also been used as a teach-
ing. In science education in particular, several ing resource in contexts of joint teacher-student
studies have demonstrated their usefulness as a activities and to support students’ collaborative
strategy for evaluating, learning, and designing learning at different stages of the teaching-learning
the teaching of scientific content. process. In science education in particular, their
Their primary use has been as a strategy for utility has been studied as pre-organizers, i.e., as
evaluation. Indeed, there is clear evidence for a means of presenting an initial overview of the
their effectiveness in assessing students’ prior content and connecting it with the students’ prior
knowledge of scientific content and how it is knowledge (Montanero & Montanero, 1995). By
organized (Anderson-Inman, Ditson, & Ditson, comparing various methods of teaching a topic of
1998; Caswell & Wendel, 1992), and the degree science at the pre-university secondary education
of understanding that students attain (Markham, level, Hernández and Serio (2004) showed that the
Mintzes, & Jones, 1994; Novak, Gowin, & Johan- students’ preparation of a concept map is especially
sen, 1983). The nature of the information that can useful as pre-organizer, whether it is presented by
be gathered in a map on students’ learning depends, the teacher or the students themselves construct
however, on the type of task asked of them. The it, as long as the teacher explicitly helps them
more open procedures, such as creating a map of a connect it with what has just been learnt. Other
single concept, provide very different information studies have also demonstrated their usefulness as
from more structured procedures, such as filling a form of synthesizing the content studied during
out an incomplete map or constructing one from or at the end of a learning sequence (Horton, Mc-
a set list of concepts (see Ruiz-Primo, 2004). conney, Gallo, Woods, Senn, & Hamelin, 1993;
Concept maps also constitute an interesting Pankratius, 1990).
resource to support the learning processes of Finally, concept maps provide a useful frame-
conceptual content, whether within a framework work for representing knowledge in a form that
of autonomous learning activities or with the help can be taught or shared in a variety of contexts
of the teacher. Some studies, for example, have – scientific, educational, or professional (see
reported their usefulness for the students them- Cañas, Hill, Carff, Suri, Lott, Eskridge, et al.,
selves to reconstruct strategically the knowledge 2004). In the educational context, the maps help
they have acquired from various sources, and to “unbundle” a teacher’s expert knowledge in
then to apply it to different learning tasks, i.e., to the design of content and learning sequences. The

326
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

process of analysis that concept mapping demands Many studies concur in that misconceptions
can help teachers reflect on what is the best way condition the acquisition of knowledge in specific
to organize the content to be learnt, what of their domains. There is no consensus, however, on
previous knowledge the students need to use, the whether they constitute genuine implicit theories
most complex conceptual relationships, etc. (Lautrey & Mazens, 2004). For Di Sessa (1993),
This last application has not as yet given rise it is a case of “knowledge in pieces” rather than
to sufficient empirical research. The main purpose a logically organized body of knowledge. Vosnia-
of the present chapter in this context is therefore dou (1992) argues, however, that spontaneous
to analyze the potential of concept maps as a tool knowledge develops into spontaneous theories,
for collaboration in learning physics. On the one at two levels of depth: “framework theories”
hand, we shall analyze how concept maps can and “specific theories”. Framework theories are
enhance teamwork when teachers are designing based on certain ontological and epistemological
learning sequences for the sciences. And on the presuppositions that define a domain (such as
other, we shall describe some collaborative learn- the presupposition of the solidity or continuity
ing experiments involving students preparing of matter). Specific theories are constrained by
and revising concept maps. In both applications, some framework theory. They are based on beliefs
we will study the potential of concept maps to that facilitate the construction of mental models of
facilitate the processes of conceptual change in specific physical phenomena (such as a collision
learning physics. between two bodies).
It is more difficult to change framework
theories than specific theories. According to the
Fundamentals Incompatibility Hypothesis (Chi, Slotta & de
Misconceptions and Conceptual Leeuw, 1994), this difficulty is largely explained
Change in Physics by “the existence of a mismatch or incompat-
ibility between the categorical representation that
The spontaneous conceptions that students build students bring to an instructional context, and the
from their everyday experience have an extraor- ontological category to which the science concept
dinary influence on their learning of scientific truly belongs”. Many students misunderstand such
content. The naive ideas that students show about concepts as light or electric current because they
physical phenomena are often “primitive” and categorize them ontologically as matter, when they
“phenomenological” (Di Sessa, 1993). They are are rather processes or states (which belong to the
primitive because they are based on simple and category “constraint-based interaction”).
intuitive explanations, and phenomenological We should now briefly describe the hierarchi-
because they derive from the experiences that cal construction of its theories so as to understand
the subjects obtain from their interaction with how the teacher can intervene in the process of
the physical world. These consist of causal, linear students’ conceptual change.
and highly simplified explanations that underpin The first rung of the hierarchy consists of
knowledge that is useful in everyday life (Claxton, the clearly identified physical objects which the
1987). Phenomena are explained in terms of a learner faces. This identification must be given
single cause which generates the observed change explicitly even when changes may occur through
in the physical reality. In the official theories of their possible evolution. For example, a physical
science, however, there prevails an analysis of object (or system) might be a pendulum consist-
reality as a “state”, defined by the interaction of ing of a thread suspended at one end and a sphere
systems (Montanero, Pérez, & Suero, 1995). hanging from the other which can move from

327
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

one side to the other. Another simple example the pendulum, the states will refer to the various
would be a tin rod of a certain length and thick- positions of the sphere, also including knowledge
ness which can be heated and change in length of its velocities and accelerations. In other words,
and appearance. the definition of the states would imply the use
The second step in the construction of physical from the very beginning of learning of a number
explanations is a mental schematic representation, of concepts (such as mass, velocity, acceleration,
formed by certain abstractions of that physical energy,…) which are contained in those concepts
object. This is therefore a deliberate simplifica- supported by the initial explanations, as will be
tion made to facilitate the idea of it in the mind. seen below. They will therefore constitute simple
Hence, some of the features and properties of the hierarchies of learning (from the bottom up) to be
objects will be removed from consideration, and taken into account as prerequisites of knowledge
attention will be focused on those that are really in support of the physical explanations. Similarly,
of interest. This is what we would call the physi- in the case of the tin rod, the states would be de-
cal model. Thus, in the aforementioned physical termined by values of the length, temperature…
system of the pendulum, the thread is assumed This form of in-depth analysis of the genesis
to be massless and inextensible, and one is only of physical theories, originating in the philosophy
interested in the mass of the sphere hanging from and epistemology of science (López Rupérez,
it, not its size or shape. Similarly, in the example 1990), is unfortunately taken very little into ac-
of the tin rod, only its length and mass are con- count by teachers in dealing pædagogically with
sidered, and one is not interested in its hardness, the inevitable analysis of the physical models
flexibility, etc. (Meléndez, Pacual, & Rosado, 1989) that underlie
It is a proven fact that the first deficiencies any explanation of the phenomena. These aspects
observed in both the formal learning of physics are of particular interest when one is dealing with
and in the formation of implicit theories concern implicit theories, in which these phases of the
the lack of clarity and explicitness in this neces- construction of scientific thought are never made
sary set of factual concepts (Bunge, 1978). As explicit. In sum, as was mentioned above, one
noted above, there must be room in the concept needs to deal with this “modeling” of the physical
maps incorporating the instruction sequences for explanation for factual change to be converted
the proposal of a factual conflict corresponding into true conceptual change. Concept maps again
to these initial levels of elaboration. prove to be a very useful tool for unpacking the
The third stage of the formation of physical analysis of this content, as we will have the op-
theories consists of assigning certain states to portunity to show below.
the physical model adopted previously. These The last step that we shall consider is the
states would be determined by clearly defined establishment of a theoretical model. This will
magnitudes. This is what is known as a theoretical consist of the theoretical system seen previously,
model (or theoretical system) which, moreover, together with a set of statements (laws) obtained by
is specific to each type of physical phenomenon, induction from the general facts. In the pendulum
unlike the physical model which is applicable example, this statement is the equation of motion,
to different physical systems (for example, the which, given the initial data of a particular case,
physical model consisting of isolated particles determines all the states of motion. Likewise, the
can belong to both the theoretical system of the law of linear thermal expansion would provide the
kinetic of the perfect gases as any other piece of way to obtain the different lengths of the tin rod as
the statistical physics). a function of temperature, and statements of the
In the example that we have been using of laws of melting would enable one to know what

328
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

the physical state of this physical object is. explanations and justifications they recorded in
Modifying the students’ implicit theories in the discussion, the greater was the likelihood of
learning physics is therefore a complex task. conceptual change .
It requires their strongly active participation in But not only how extensive the students’
activities that lead them to an awareness of, and exchanges are is important. Above all, it is im-
a subsequent change in, their misconceptions. An portant that they reach agreement in the group.
educational strategy that has proved effective in Mercer (1996) examined different types of verbal
this regard is to design hands-on activities that exchanges typical of collaborative learning situ-
lead to factual conflict. In other words, the student ations. The students were not really negotiating
experiences that his or her implicit theory about a meanings in conversations that were merely
certain physical phenomenon leads to predictions “cumulative talk” or “disputational talk”. In
that are not fulfilled. This psychological state of “exploratory talk”, however, they were critically
uncertainty and dissatisfaction facilitates their engaged in refuting their colleagues’ ideas and
becoming aware of and revising their particular in reaching consensus, processes that are more
mental model (Posner, Strike, Hewson, & Gert- conducive to learning.
zog, 1982). Other studies have found a relationship between
This empirical conflict would, however, be the quality of these communicative exchanges and
sterile unless it were accompanied by a genu- the nature of the learning task. Tasks in which the
ine conceptual conflict, i.e., unless the students students share goals and working tools encourage
become aware of their ideas, verbalize them, “positive student interdependence” (Cohen, 1994).
and contrast them against alternative versions And tasks that require externalization of specific
(Hewson & Thorley, 1989; Pozo, 1999). This visible products (as would be the case of a concept
requires a process in which physics knowledge is map) provide a space for joint work that is more
abstracted and made explicit which would be most useful for explicit negotiation of the students’
difficult to foster outside systematic instruction. representations, thereby facilitating the processes
Indeed, such conceptual change is based on the of conceptual change (Roth & Roychoudhury,
teacher’s intense support, for the students to be 1992). Another important factor is that the tasks
able to transform their mental models with which should elicit the participants’ prior knowledge
they explain physical phenomena (Rodrigo & on the topic, and should be oriented towards the
Correa, 1999). construction of consensus, either through conflict
or through integration of their contributions (Fis-
Collaborative Learning and cher, Bruhn, Gräsel & Mandl, 2002).
Conceptual Change
Concept Maps and
Collaborative learning is based on the idea that Conceptual Change
students influence each other in their learning
when they exchange knowledge and negotiate its In this context, we can argue for several reasons
meaning (Baker, Hansen, Joiner & Traum, 1999; why concept maps can help facilitate conceptual
Barron, 2003). There is evidence of the effect of change in physics teaching. Every conceptual
this type of classroom task in changing mistaken change is based on a gradual re-development
preconceptions in physics. Analyzing how several of the theoretical model that explains certain
pairs of students discussed the relationship be- physical phenomena. At the beginning of learn-
tween physical concepts, Roth and Roychoudhury ing, such knowledge only allows relatively simple
(1992) found that the more extensive were the and unidirectional explanations of the content of

329
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

physics that is to be constructed. Subsequently, the learning (Okebukola, 1990; Okebukola & Jegede,
students will be able to understand and use more 1988). Van Boxtel, van der Linden & Kanselaar
complex and interdependent theoretical models. (2000) observed that the task of constructing a
As an instructional design strategy, the use of map of concepts about electricity generated more
concept maps can be an invaluable tool to guide discussion in which conceptual arguments and
the teacher’s reflection on the different levels of conflicts were recorded than the preparation of a
development of these theoretical models – from poster, even though the authors note that, in this
the simplest causal representations to others that case, the results of learning were not significantly
are more complex and abstract. better.
Concept maps can also lend support to activities
aimed at provoking conceptual conflict. Analyz-
ing tutoring activities involving science content, Applications of Concept
Chi (1996) found that help from the teacher Maps to the Design of
aimed at eliciting self-explanations or revision of Elaboration-Theory-Based
mental models is the most effective at promoting Learning Sequences
learning in depth and changes in erroneous pre-
conceptions. When the students ask themselves A key problem that all teachers have to resolve
about the why of conceptual relationships, they is how to organize the content so as to facilitate
are setting out on a process of revising the new its learning as far as is possible. Over the past
information which often comes into conflict with few decades, two general procedures have been
a prior causal model that had until then only been developed with which to establish teaching and
implicit. In another experiment, it was found that learning sequences.
eliciting self-explanations had a positive impact One is Task Analysis. This decomposes and
on causal understanding, even when some of those orders the skills that students are expected to
explanations were wrong and never corrected by master by the end of the learning process (Gagnè,
an expert (Chi, De Leeuw, Chiu, & Lavancher, 1985). The result is a hierarchy ascending from
1994). Graphical representations of scientific ex- the most basic learning to more complex strategies
planations may represent an interesting resource that require good prior mastery of the preceding
in this regard. The task of constructing maps can skills.
lead the students to an awareness of their own The other is Content Analysis. This, in contrast,
implicit causal models of the physical phenomenon focuses on identifying and organizing knowledge,
being dealt with, and to an active involvement in especially conceptual knowledge. In this case,
revising their own explanations. it is postulated that the learning sequence must
In accordance with the studies discussed in proceed overall from the more general to the more
the previous section, collaborative construction specific, alternating processes of analysis and of
of concept maps would seem to be potentially synthesis (Ausubel, Novak, & Hanesian, 1978).
ideal tasks with which to generate processes of Concept maps fit very well into this approach,
conceptual change. In these tasks, the students since they allow this conceptual hierarchy to be
share working tools and generate explicit repre- represented graphically.
sentations of their thinking which they find easy The Elaboration Theory of Reigeluth and Stein
to manipulate and negotiate. (1983, 1987) takes an alternative approach to
In the particular case of scientific content, Oke- structuring learning, conceiving of that structure in
bukola showed that the collaborative construction the form of a spiral. It has been found particularly
of maps promotes critical thought and meaningful useful in physics teaching (Pérez, Suero, Montan-

330
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

ero, & Montanero, 2001). In this approach, every determined degree of complexity. Each of these
learning sequence has to be based on two prior levels should in their turn begin with a “panoramic
analyses: reflection on the fundamental content vision” (or epitome) of the more general content
best suited to forming the scaffold on which the that will subsequently be dealt with in detail. The
students’ learning can be constructed, and an epitome synthesizes those more general ideas
analysis of the different levels of elaboration. at the same level, in a simplified and practical
Curricular design begins with reflection on the form, i.e., taking what is the most inclusive and
type of content (conceptual, procedural, etc.) to then presenting it in the most concrete and factual
form the scaffold on which the learning sequence fashion. The student identifies them as part of a
will be organized. The entire content will then be structured whole, since the teacher’s explanation
organized at different levels of elaboration. A level describes successive approximations to these ideas
of elaboration reflects a point in the process of one at a time, but without exhausting them in the
constructing the learning content that has some first presentation. This progressive “zooming” into

Figure 1. Educational components of elaboration theory (© 2008 Pérez, Suero, Montanero-Fernández,


Pardo, & Montanero-Morán)

331
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

the content therefore entails going back over the Suero, Montanero, & Montanero, 2000). Such
same content but in steadily greater depth. maps define, therefore, the factual context of
Each epitome is in itself an item of teaching the content to be taught, so that the hierarchical
content, presented in a practical and hands-on structure of the physical phenomena provide us
way. Herein lies the greatest difficulty in the de- the leitmotif of the learning sequence, i.e., what
sign process, because the student initially needs before we called the organizer content.
a working knowledge of the learning content This simple modification entails activities that
to serve as the basis for further learning. At the are important for the students to gain awareness
completion of each phase of increasing depth of the erroneous theories that they implicitly use
(“elaboration”), one must insist on presenting its to explain physical phenomena, and to be able to
relationship with the overall general plan, which incorporate more complex theoretical models into
will thus be enriched and extended. This is the their growing knowledge base. The observation
“expanded epitome”. At the end of the successive of reality – in particular, of a variety of physical
phases, one will have a “final epitome” in which phenomena – is one of the foundations on which
the basic content of the first level will now appear the process of generating scientific theories will
already reformulated, with the more complex be based, as we have described before. Often
and abstract conceptual relationships that were indeed, the implicit theories themselves were the
generated in the learning process. Figure 1 is a result of this need being insufficiently satisfied in
concept map summarizing the major educational formal education. The observation, prediction, and
components of elaboration theory. discussion of the underlying processes in physi-
cal phenomena form the best scenario in which
Adaptation of Elaboration to foster conceptual change.
Theory to Physics Teaching Let us consider at this point an example of the
application of these principles to a thermodynamics
As we have shown elsewhere, elaboration theory learning sequence. The initial epitome might focus
offers interesting theoretical support for science on the phenomena of expansion and change of state
teaching (Pérez et al., 2001). The importance and that bodies undergo with increasing temperature.
difficulty of the process of conceptual change Meaningful learning of the content of thermody-
in physics teaching, however, require some namics requires that the students’ analysis of these
modifications to be made to the initial theory of physical phenomena will be approached from the
Reigeluth and Stein. The most important innova- base of a theoretical model appropriate to their
tion is to take physical phenomena (rather than pre-existing knowledge. The students are likely to
concepts or principles) as being the organizer begin learning with misconceptions, such as that
content in the learning sequences. To this end, the temperature of the change of state depends on
a map of phenomena is elaborated on the basis the amount of substance or on the intensity of the
of the ordered set of concepts and principles, as heat source. The physical model that the student
well as their relationships, about the content to be can understand at this stage is based on a mental
taught (often referred to as the logical structure of representation of bodies as consisting of moving
the content in its final stage of elaboration). This point particles, linked by some sort of bond of
map will have to contain all the phenomena to varying intensity. The support conceptual content
be studied in a related and hierarchical form, as (temperature, thermometer, states of matter, etc.)
a theoretical-descriptive superordinated structure should be introduced gradually, not in a “strictly
(Reigeluth, Merril, & Bunderson, 1978). We shall scientific” way, but in terms of everyday knowl-
call it a three-dimensional expert map (Pérez, edge that will subsequently be re-elaborated as

332
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

Figure 2. Three-dimensional map of the initial epitome. (© 2008 Pérez, Suero, Montanero-Fernández,
Pardo, & Montanero-Morán)

hierarchies of learning (from the bottom up) as keep them together are weakened. A practical
we have proposed before. (see Figure 2) experiment that might help the students reach this
As new levels of elaboration are introduced, conclusion could be to heat a container contain-
learning is enriched with progressively more ing a piece of ice, another container with a piece
complex explanations and theoretical models. of tin, and another with lead (which have differ-
For example, in the second epitome of a thermo- ent melting points) over the flame of a cigarette
dynamics learning sequences, new phenomena lighter. The question that the students should try
related to heat transfer would be studied (conduc- to answer would be: “Why do these bodies melt?”,
tion, convection, and radiation). In the third level or “What will have happened inside them?”. The
epitome, one would now address the “ideal gases” basic causal explanation that the students should
(isochoric, isobaric, isothermal, and adiabatic try to discover from the perceptual features of the
processes). phenomenon is that on “providing the body with
The preparation of this type of learning heat energy”, and thus increasing its temperature,
sequence requires three types of learning activ- its particles vibrate with more energy and move
ity that have a marked hands-on or experiential away from each other, until the interaction hold-
character. ing the particles together gets so reduced that the
The first consists of activities in which physical body ends up “melting”. (see Figure 3)
phenomena are observed in all their phases and The second type of discovery learning activity
then discussed as to their possible explanations. is for the students to perform small experiments,
This basic causal explanation (BCE) should be in particular, to formulate hypotheses and make
based on the scientific model closest to the stu- predictions about the variables involved in a given
dent’s prior knowledge at each level of elabora- physical phenomenon, manipulate those variables
tion. Consider for example the observation of a systematically, record the results, and confirm or
phenomenon of change of state. At the first level reject those hypotheses. Whenever possible, the
of elaboration, the BCE that the student should activities should allow the students to recognize
deduce with the teacher’s help could be summa- proportional or interdependence relationships,
rized in the idea that, when a body’s temperature leading to their suggesting an initial idea (not nec-
increases, there is an increase in the movement essarily mathematical) of some simple laws. Using
of its particles to the point where the links that the same example as above, since the temperature

333
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

Figure 3. Map of the learning microsequence developing one of the topics of the content. (© 2008 Pérez,
Suero, Montanero-Fernández, Pardo, & Montanero-Morán)

should rise with the energy supplied in heating the awareness of the conflict between the students’
container, the students could be asked to discuss pre-existing causal theories (as yet “implicit”)
the following questions: Is the temperature rise and the new causal explanations that have been
always the same? Could it reach a plateau? How constructed on the basis of the study of the physi-
would that be possible? Why must we raise the cal phenomena. The activities in this case will be
temperature more for lead than for tin, and more of a “pencil and paper” type, based on questions
for tin than for ice? The first three questions guide about predictions or explanations of natural phe-
the student to an initial formulation of the law of nomena related to the course content, or on the
constancy of temperature during melting. The graphical representation of that content in the
last question would help them to conclude with form of concept maps. In both cases, the teacher
another law relative to pure substances each hav- will discuss the responses and maps constructed
ing its own melting point. (see Figure 4) by the students with the class as a whole, with the
The third type of activity should facilitate aim of pointing out the erroneous preconceptions

Figure 4. Three-dimensional map of the second level of elaboration epitome. (© 2008 Pérez, Suero,
Montanero-Fernández, Pardo, & Montanero-Morán)

334
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

that they contain and the changes undergone by simulate the spiral sequence of different levels of
the students’ implicit theories during the learning elaboration of the content. Third, they provide a
process1. vehicle for teamwork among teachers designing
There are two major advantages for science their learning sequences.
learning and conceptual change in this adaptation The innovation that makes these goals at-
of Reigeluth’s theory to physics teaching. First, the tainable is based on adapting concept maps to
design of levels of elaboration helps the students a three-dimensional format. Traditionally, maps
with their construction of new knowledge using have been used to display knowledge in just two
a dual approach: from the simple to the complex, dimensions – horizontal and vertical – with the
and from the particular to the abstract. Learning relationships of semantic subordination between
science does not consist in merely incorporating concepts of different levels of inclusiveness be-
new knowledge, or replacing a number of mis- ing represented vertically, and the relationships
conceptions with others that conform the truth. between concepts that are at the same hierarchi-
Rather it is a constant re-working of the conceptual cal level being represented horizontally. Three-
relationships that underpin a theory. At the begin- dimensional maps allow one to represent a third
ning of learning, the students’ knowledge only type of content relationship: “depth”, i.e., the dif-
allows them to construct relatively simple and ferent levels of elaboration that can be established
unidirectional explanations of the physics content. in the instructional sequence.
Subsequently, they will be able to understand and The computer application CmapTools (de-
use more complex and interdependent theoretical signed by the Florida Institute for Human and
models. Second, organizing the content around Machine Cognition) makes the construction of
physical phenomena facilitates both hands-on such maps straightforward, thanks to an option
learning and teaching the scientific method. The that allows one to link new maps with any of the
processes of conceptual change of the students’ content already represented. The resulting maps
misconceptions require learning that goes from contain text-labelled lines linking different items
the implicit to the explicit, and from realism to of the content (which are usually contained in a
perspectivism. The students must first of all be- rectangular or elliptical frame). Also, for some of
come aware of their own implicit theories about the content there appears a symbol indicating the
the physical phenomena they are studying. They connection in depth with another map.
will have to put these into words, and contrast We have used this simple technology to
them with alternative explanations and models construct three-dimensional maps of the entire
(Rodrigo & Correa, 1999). In this complex process, “Bachillerato” (pre-university secondary educa-
concept maps can have an important role to play, tion) physics curriculum (see Pérez et al., 1999,
as will be shown below. 2000)2. By way of example, the following figures
show some maps depicting the thermodynamics
Applications of CmapTools learning sequence described above.
to Elaboration Theory: Three- We have found evidence that the application of
Dimensional Maps this type of instructional design is more effective
for science learning and conceptual change than
There are various ways in which concept maps the traditional alternatives at the Bachillerato level.
can be useful in the type of instructional design The participants in our study were 5 teachers and
described above. First, they provide a visual repre- 341 students from various secondary schools in
sentation of the logical structure of the knowledge Spain. Each teacher worked with three groups of
that is to be taught. Second, they allow one to students for approximately 15 hours. With two of

335
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

these groups, the instruction sequence followed & Chang (2000) investigated group interaction
was based on an elaboration-theory design with processes in network-supported collaborative
three-dimensional maps. With the third group, it concept mapping. It was found that group concept
was based on the teacher’s usual method. The pre- mapping performance was significantly correlated
and post-trial evaluations of the students’ learning to the quantity of group interaction, particularly
consisted of three tests: a test of the comprehen- complex cooperation interaction.
sion of the scientific concepts studied; another Using the application “Soups” of CmapTools3,
with problems to identify any possible miscon- Cañas, Ford, Hayes, Reichherzer, Suri, Coffey,
ceptions; and a third with tasks that required the et al. (1998) carried out various experiments in
interpretation of phenomena or their application which they explored the educational possibilities
to everyday situations. Comparison of the pre- and of collaborative learning with maps. As a student
post-trial results showed improvement in all the is draughting a concept map, CmapTools lets him
participating groups. Nonetheless, the improve- or her publish any idea (as a minimum, two linked
ments in all the tests were significantly greater in concepts) onto a network so that it is potentially vis-
the groups using the elaboration theory sequences. ible to other students on that network. The images
The greatest differences were observed in the tests are stored in a database (the “knowledge soup”)
that evaluated conceptual changes (Montanero, that all the students share and use in construct-
Pérez, Suero, & Montanero, 2001). ing their own maps4. Any student can question a
claim made by another if he or she disagrees with
it or finds it ambiguous. The other students can
Applications of Concept Maps respond, initiating a parallel discussion on some
to Activities of Collaborative specific content.
Reconstruction of The collaborative elaboration of concept maps
Knowledge in Physics with such tools is an extremely powerful resource
with which to encourage the students’ active in-
As was pointed out above, students can also use volvement in processes of meaningful learning
concept maps in certain activities of the elabo- and conceptual change. As was observed above,
ration sequence. Indeed, these maps represent changing implicit theories requires activities
an interesting resource for fostering conceptual that will lead the students to an awareness of the
change through collaborative learning activi- theory that they use to explain physical phenom-
ties. The graphical representation of the theories ena, and to contrast it with alternative versions.
explaining the physical phenomena at each level The construction of collaborative maps can be of
of elaboration greatly facilitates discussion of particular help in attaining this important objective
possible misconceptions, as well as the students’ in physics teaching.
own perception of the changes they undergo dur- In a series of educational experiments (see
ing the course of learning. Collaborative group Pérez, Suero, & Pardo, 2008), we tested the imple-
design of concept maps also provides a context mentation of concept maps in support of processes
for questioning and negotiating their conceptual of the collaborative reconstruction of knowledge
representations, which may be especially useful and conceptual change in undergraduates. The
for them to revise their implicit theories. experiments were carried out in four phases:
There has been some previous work on the col-
laborative elaboration of concept maps in which • Firstly, the students were asked to pre-
computer resources were used to facilitate teacher- pare individual concept maps about cer-
student and student-student activities. Chiu, Huang tain physical phenomena of optics and

336
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

electricity. them. They also included notes clarifying


• Secondly, they reviewed and proposed why the amendments proposed by class-
changes to the maps made by their class- mates had been accepted or rejected.
mates. One of the students took the re-
sponsibility of synthesizing the proposals The negotiation and exchange of maps took
of amendments to each of the individual place directly in the classrooms themselves, or over
maps. The result was what we denominat- Internet by accessing either the Cmap site or the
ed “the map in revision”. Web page to which they were being published5.
• Thirdly, the students discussed the changes In the following paragraphs, we shall illustrate
proposed for each map. Each author had to this working strategy with an example of a map
justify his or her acceptance or rejection of one of the participating students concerning the
of incorporating the changes. The result content of optics relative to refraction.
of this process was each student’s “revised As can be seen in Figure 5, the original map
map”. contains three links – to the same map in revision,
• Finally, another student draughted the to the map as subsequently revised by the author,
“consensus map”, summarizing the essen- and to the final map reached as a consensus of
tial parts of all the revised maps. The maps the entire class. Some of her classmates analyzed
in revision and those already revised con- her original map, and proposed changes to some
tained links to the originals, allowing easy of its parts. These proposed changes are outlined
comparison of the differences between with dashed lines in the figure.

Figure 5. The original map of an undergraduate student on the refraction of light. (© 2008 Pérez, Suero,
Montanero-Fernández, Pardo, & Montanero-Morán)

337
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

Figure 6 shows the map in revision resulting refractive indices”). Note 6 suggests indicating
from incorporating the classmates’ proposals that the second law (which the “Second” concept
(indicated by dashed lines and in red). The figure refers to) is Snell’s Law.
also shows the 22 notes in which those classmates The focus of the help in the form of syntheses
clarify or argue for their proposed changes. is on integrating information that appears disag-
Analysis of the dashed-line links and the con- gregated in the original map. This is the case of
tent of the notes showed that there were essentially the proposal (in red on the right-hand side of the
five types of help contributed by colleagues to map) connecting ni>nr and ni<nr with the idea of
facilitate the re-draughting of the original rep- approaching or moving away from the normal,
resentation: clarifications, syntheses, examples, respectively. Similarly, Note 11 suggests removing
evaluations, and conflicts. the link to which it is attached, and integrating the
Most of the dashed lines correspond to concepts of angles of incidence and of refraction
clarifications (proposing new concepts or links) into a single box.
clarifying, complementing, or justifying an idea Help in the form of examples proposes cases
already expressed in the map. Other added de- that are concrete instances of some idea in the
tails are given in the notes. Note 2, for example, original map. Thus, in the central part of the map,
proposes adding further information about the a student suggests considering corrective lenses as
concept of “variation” and “refractive index” (“It an example of an optical instrument that is based
is important to say that the light rays in refraction on the phenomenon of refraction.
cross the surface separating media with different In their evaluations, the other students may

Figure 6. Map in revision incorporating modifications to the original map of Figure 5. (© 2008 Pérez,
Suero, Montanero-Fernández, Pardo, & Montanero-Morán)

338
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

pose questions, or point to some incorrect or in one medium and the refracted ray in another.
confusing idea in the original map. For example, Often, help of this type from different students is
in Note 7 a student indicates that he does not un- redundant. Thus, others of her colleagues bring
derstand the relationship expressed between the up this error in Notes 4 (“When does refraction
indices of refraction, and that ni is greater than take place?”) and 12 (“Plane or medium?”). The
or less than nr. A similar problem is detected with instructor too can occasionally post notes that
respect to other links in Notes 13 and 22. generate a conflict or help to revise relevant
Finally, in the type of help that we have called items of the map. In this case, for example, the
“conflicts”, other students may point to contra- instructor suggested including an explanation of
dictions, biases, or errors. Such is the case of the cases in which the refraction has characteristics
new link (“are in the same” on the left-hand side that make the medium transparent and others that
of the map) proposed between concepts relating make it merely translucent.
to the first law of refraction and the plane. This In the revised map shown in Figure 7, the
brings out the grave conceptual mistake the au- author has analyzed her classmates’ proposals for
thor of the map had made in confusing “plane” amendment of her original map (Figure 5). As can
and “medium”. Instead of “different plane”, the be seen, she accepts four amendments that greatly
author should have written “different media”. improve her original map. More importantly, her
She would thus have managed to explain that, review process introduces new concepts and rela-
for there to be refraction, the incident ray must be tionships (such as the idea that refraction is a wave

Figure 7. Map revised by the author. (© 2008 Pérez, Suero, Montanero-Fernández, Pardo, & Montanero-
Morán)

339
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

phenomenon). All the participating students made which provoke the students’ own explanations and
similar modifications to their original maps, by revisions of their mental models (Chi, 1996). In
including the suggestions of their classmates. the maps resulting from the experiment described
Finally, the students negotiated a map on the above, each student contributed an average of 4
refraction of light that integrated as well as pos- inputs to each of the original maps of their col-
sible the set of representations that they had all leagues (mostly clarifications, evaluations, and
constructed and re-constructed. The most viable conflicts, in that order). Also, on average, each
strategy that was followed for this step began student integrated into their revised map more than
with taking the revised map that they considered 6 inputs from their peers. Misconceptions about
to be the closest to complete. They then discussed the physics content were detected in more than
which elements of the other maps could be added, 80% of the original maps, which, by the end of the
ending with the consensus map (Figure 8). process, the authors’ of the maps themselves had
This learning activity is a clear illustration of identified and revised. In another experiment with
how certain strategies of intersubject negotiation the content of electricity, we analyzed the influ-
(Wertsch, 1998) and “scaffolding” of students’ ence of this type of activity using an objective test
cognitive representations can lead to conceptual for the evaluation of misconceptions. Compared
change. We have shown that the most effective with an equivalent control group, the students who
activities to promote conceptual change are those participated in the experiment responded 17%

Figure 8. Final consensus map. (© 2008 Pérez, Suero, Montanero-Fernández, Pardo, & Montanero-
Morán)

340
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

better to the test items (Pérez et al., 2008). tool to help teams of teachers discuss and design
In sum, resources like CmapTools provide learning sequences at different levels of elabora-
an ideal scenario for the collaboration of teacher tion. Compared with other types of instructional
and students in the construction of a conceptual designs, the sequences in which three-dimensional
system. The graphical representation that they maps were used led to better learning outcomes,
provide plays a very important role in making particularly in tests that measured the modifica-
the students’ respective internal representations tion of misconceptions about physics content. A
explicit, as well as facilitating the inclusion of help similar conclusion can be drawn from the experi-
from colleagues in the group and the consequent ments in which undergraduate students carried
redefinition of those representations. The task of out a collaborative activity of construction and
reconstructing their maps facilitates in particular re-construction of concept maps. Our analysis of
the students’ active participation in recalling the the original and revised maps showed clear evi-
necessary prior knowledge, in inferring causal dence for the usefulness of this task in facilitating
relationships, and in modifying their implicit processes of conceptual change.
causal model of the physical phenomenon being Another line of work with concept maps that
studied. has as yet been relatively unexplored is their use
to analyze teachers’ explanations in the classroom.
In this sense, CmapTools can be used to represent
Conclusions and the configuration of the verbal discourse that is
Future Prospects made public in the classroom. In a recent study, we
used various codes (types of polygons and links) to
Over the past 15 years, the Grupo Orión at the represent the classes of concept that teachers were
University of Extremadura has been undertaking explaining, the kinds of relationship, the order in
an extensive program of educational research and which they were being presented, the supporting
innovation, focused on the application of concept resources the teachers provided, and the degree
maps to physics teaching. In the preceding pages, of student participation in the construction of the
we have analyzed two specific recent applications ideas that were made public (Montanero, Lucero,
carried out in a framework of collaborative work & Méndez, 2008. Such applications will permit
of teachers and students. We first described how an analysis of the process of shared construction
the construction of three-dimensional maps can of knowledge as a function of various parameters
facilitate the collaborative design of learning – the degree of articulation of the explanation,
sequences based on the elaboration theory of how it is ordered, the degree of redundancy or
Reigeluth and Stein. We then reported on some density, the aid provided to the students to help
experiments and preliminary results suggestive of them understand the content, etc.
the potential of this tool in support of students’
collaborative learning.
We have also analyzed the reasons why the two
applications can have a result on learning that is
especially important in science education – con-
ceptual change. The results of these experiments
demonstrated the potential of concept maps to
foster first awareness, and then the modification,
of implicit theories about physical phenomena.
Three-dimensional maps constitute a very useful

341
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

References Caswell, R. M., & Wendel, C. T. (1992, April).


Conceptual changes occurring in elementary
Anderson-Inman, L., Ditson, L. A., & Ditson, M. teachers’ knowledge of science concepts through-
T. (1998). Computer-based concept mapping: out long-term instruction. Paper presented at the
Promoting meaningful learning in science for American Educational Research Association
students with disabilities. information Technol- Annual Meeting, San Francisco, CA.
ogy and Disabilities. Retrieved September 22,
2008 from http://people.rit.edu/easi/easisem/ Chi, M. T. H. (1996). Constructing self-explanations
itdscience.htm. and scaffolded explanations in tutoring. Applied
Cognitive Psychology , 10, 33–49. doi:10.1002/
Ausubel, D. P., Novak, K. D., & Hanesian, H. (SICI)1099-0720(199611)10:7<33::AID-
(1978). Educational Psychology. New York: Holt, ACP436>3.0.CO;2-E
Rinehart & Winston.
Chi, M. T. H., N. De Leeuw, M. H. Chiu, and C.
Baker, M., Hansen, T., Joiner, R., & Traum, D. Lavancher (1994). Eliciting self-explanations
(1999). The role of grounding in collaborative improves understanding. Cognitive Science , 18,
learning tasks. In P. Dillenbourg (Eds.), Collab- 439–477.
orative learning: cognitive and computational
approaches (pp. 31-63). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Chi, M. T. H., J. D. Slotta, and N. de Leeuw (1994).
Science Ltd. From things to processes: a theory of conceptual
change for learning science concepts. Learning
Barron, B. (2003). When smart groups fail. and Instruction , 4, 27–43. doi:10.1016/0959-
Journal of the Learning Sciences , 12, 307–359. 4752(94)90017-5
doi:10.1207/S15327809JLS1203_1
Chiu, C. H., C. C. Huang, and W. T. Chang (2000).
Bunge, M. (1978). Filosofía de la Física. Buenos The evaluation and influence of interaction in
Aires, Venezuela: EUDEBA. network supported collaborative concept mapping.
Cañas, A. J., Ford, K. M., Hayes, P. J., Reich- Computers & Education , 34, 17–25. doi:10.1016/
herzer, T., Suri, N., Coffey, J., et al. (1998). Co- S0360-1315(99)00025-1
laboración en la Construcción de Conocimiento Claxton, G. (1987). Vivir y aprender. Madrid:
Mediante Mapas Conceptuales. Proceedings of Alianza editorial.
the VIII Congreso Internacional sobre Tecnología
y Educación a Distancia, San José, Costa Rica, Cohen, E. G. (1994). Restructuring the classroom:
(pp. 25- 42). conditions for productive small groups. Review
of Educational Research , 64, 1–35.
Cañas, A. J., Hill, G., Carff, R., Suri, N., Lott, J.,
Eskridge, T., et al. (2004). CmapTools: A Knowl- Di Sessa, A. A. (1993). Towards an epistemol-
edge Modeling and Sharing Environment. In J.A. ogy of physics. Cognition and Instruction , 10,
Cañas, J.D. Novak, & F.M. González (Eds.), 105–225. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1002&3_2
Concept Maps: Theory, Methodology, Technology: Fischer, F., J. Bruhn, C. Gräsel, and H. Mandl
Proceedings of the First International Conference (2002). Fostering collaborative knowledge con-
on Concept Mapping (pp. 125-133). Pamplona, struction with visualization tools. Learning and
Spain: Universidad Pública de Navarra. Retrieved Instruction , 12, 213–232. doi:10.1016/S0959-
September 2, 2008 from http://cmc.ihmc.us/pa- 4752(01)00005-6
pers/cmc2004-283.pdf

342
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

Gagnè, R. (1985). The Condition of Learning of In- Mercer, N. (1996). The quality of talk in children’s
struction. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. collaborative activity in the classroom. Learning
and Instruction , 6, 359–377. doi:10.1016/S0959-
Hernández, P., and A. Serio (2004). ¿Cómo hacer
4752(96)00021-7
eficaces los mapas conceptuales en la instruc-
ción? Infancia y Aprendizaje , 27, 247–265. Montanero, M., M. Lucero, and J. M. Méndez
doi:10.1174/021037004323038851 (2008). Enseñanza de contenidos históricos me-
diante diagramas multicausales. Revista Española
Hewson, P. W., and R. N. Thorley (1989). The
de Pedagogía , 239(1), 27–48.
conditions of conceptual change. International
Journal of Science Education , 11, 541–553. Montanero, M., & Montanero, M. (1995). Didác-
doi:10.1080/0950069890110506 tica del momento angular de una partícula.
Badajoz, Spain: ICE.
Horton, P. B., A. A. Mcconney, M. Gallo, A. L.
Woods, G. J. Senn, and D. Hamelin (1993). An Montanero, M., A. L. Pérez, and M. I. Suero
investigation of the effectiveness of concept map- (1995). Survey of students and teacher conceptions
ping as an instructional tool. Science Education , of action-reaction dynamics: Implicit alternative
77, 95–111. doi:10.1002/sce.3730770107 theories are manifest in the consistency of incor-
rect responses. Physics Education , 30, 277–283.
Lautrey, J., and K. Mazens (2004). Is children’s
doi:10.1088/0031-9120/30/5/004
naive knowledge consistent? A comparison of
the concepts of sound and heat. Learning and Montanero, M., A. L. Pérez, M. I. Suero, and M.
Instruction , 14, 399–423. doi:10.1016/j.learnin- Montanero (2001). Cambio conceptual y ense-
struc.2004.06.011 ñanza de la Física: Aplicaciones en el marco de
la Teoría de la Elaboración. Revista de Educación
López Rupérez, F. (1990). Epistemología y didác-
, 326, 311–332.
tica de las ciencias: Un análisis de segundo orden.
Ensenañza de las Ciencias , 8, 65–74. Novak, J. D., & Gowin, D. B. (1984). Concept
Mapping for Meaningful Learning. Learning How
Markham, K. M., J. J. Mintzes, and M. Jones
to Learn. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
(1994). The concept map as a research and evalu-
Press.
ation tool: Further evidence of validity. Journal
of Research in Science Teaching , 31, 91–101. Novak, J. D., D. B. Gowin, and G. Johansen (1983).
doi:10.1002/tea.3660310109 The use of concept mapping and knowledge vee
mapping with junior high school science students.
McCrudden, M. T., G. Schraw, S. Lehman, and
Science Education , 67, 625–645. doi:10.1002/
A. Poliquin (2007). The effect of causal diagrams
sce.3730670511
on text learning. Contemporary Educational
Psychology , 32(3), 367–388. doi:10.1016/j. Okebukola, P. A., and O. J. Jegede (1988). Cogni-
cedpsych.2005.11.002 tive preference and learning mode as determinants
of meaningful learning through concept mapping.
Meléndez, A., I. Pacual, and L. Rosado (1989). La
Science Education , 72, 489–500. doi:10.1002/
enseñanza de los modelos sobre la naturaleza de
sce.3730720408
la luz. Ensenañza de las Ciencias , 7, 271–276.

343
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

Okebukola, P. A., and O. J. Jegede (1989). Posner, G. J., K. A. Strike, P. W. Hewson, and W.
Students’ anxiety towards and perception of A. Gertzog (1982). Accomodation of scientific
difficulty of some biological concepts under conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change.
the concept mapping heuristic. Research in Science Education , 66, 211–227. doi:10.1002/
Science & Technological Education , 7, 85–92. sce.3730660207
doi:10.1080/0263514890070109
Pozo, J. I. (1999). Más allá del cambio concep-
Okebukola, P. A. O. (1990). Attaining meaningful tual: el aprendizaje de las ciencias como cambio
learning of concepts in genetic and ecology: An representacional. Ensenañza de las Ciencias , 17,
examination of the potency of the concept mapping 513–520.
technique. Journal of Research in Science Teach-
Reigeluth, C. M., M. D. Merril, and C. V. Bunder-
ing , 27, 493–504. doi:10.1002/tea.3660270508
son (1978). The structure of subject matteer
Pankratius, W. J. (1990). Building an organized content and its instructional desing implications.
knowledge base: Concept mapping and achieve- Instructional Science , 7, 107–126. doi:10.1007/
ment in secondary school physics. Journal of BF00121929
Research in Science Teaching , 27, 315–333.
Reigeluth, C. M., & Stein, F. S. (1983). The Elabo-
doi:10.1002/tea.3660270404
ration Theory of Instruction. In C.M. Reigeluth
Pérez, A. L., Suero, M. I., Montanero, M., & (Ed.), Instructional design theories and models:
Montanero, M. (1999). Mapas de experto tridi- An overview of their current status (pp. 335-381).
mensionales: Aplicaciones al diseño de secuencias Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
instruccionales de Física. In M.E.C., Premios
Reigeluth, C. M., & Stein, F. S. (1987). Lesson
Nacionales de Investigación Educativa 1998 (pp.
blueprints based on the Elaboration Theory of
93-116). Salamanca, Spain: CIDE.
Instruction, In C.M. Reigeluth (Ed.), Instructional
Pérez, A. L., Suero, M. I., Montanero, M., & theories in action. Lesson illustrating selected
Montanero, M. (2000). Mapas de expertos tridi- theories and models (pp. 245-288). Hillsdale,
mensionales. Aplicaciones al diseño de secuencias NJ: Erlbaum.
instruccionales de Física, basadas en la teoría
Rodrigo, M. J., & Correa, N. (1999). Teorías im-
de la Elaboración. Badajoz, Spain: Junta de
plícitas, modelos mentales y cambio educativo.
Extremadura.
In I. Pozo & C. Monereo (Eds.), El aprendizaje
Pérez, A. L., M. I. Suero, M. Montanero, and estratégico (pp. 75-86). Madrid: Santillana.
M. Montanero (2001). Propuestas de innovación
Roth, W., and A. Roychoudhury (1992). The
entorno al análisis y secuenciación de contenidos
social construction of scientific concepts or the
en los diseños curriculares de Física. Bordón ,
concept map as conscription device and tool
53, 279–286.
for social thinking in high school science. Sci-
Pérez A.L., Suero, M.I., & Pardo, P.J. (2008) Uti- ence Eductation , 76(5), 531–557. doi:10.1002/
lización de Cmaps para mejorar los conocimientos sce.3730760507
relativos a la refracción de la Luz mediante su
Ruiz-Primo, A. (2004, September). Examining
“Reconstrucción Colaborativa”. Óptica pura y
concept maps as an assessment tool. Paper pre-
aplicada, 41, 17-23.
sented at the I International Conference on Concept
Mapping, Pamplona, Spain.

344
Concept Maps and Conceptual Change in Physics

Suero, M. I., Pérez, A. L., & Montanero, M. (Eds.). various editions of one of the most widely
(2003). Física 2. Madrid: Santillana. used textbooks at this stage of education in
Spain (Suero, Pérez, & Montanero, 2003).
Van Boxtel, C., J. van der Linden, and G. Kanse-
Much of the resulting material can be found
laar (2000). Collaborative learning tasks and the
on the Web at http://grupoorion.unex.es.
elaboration of conceptual knowledge. Learning 3
In particular, CmapTools contains three
and Instruction , 10, 311–330. doi:10.1016/S0959-
applications that empower the collabora-
4752(00)00002-5
tive construction of maps via the Internet:
Vosniadou, S. (1992). Knowledge acquisition Synchronous Maps, Discussion Lists, and
and conceptual change. Applied Psychology . Soups.
International Review (Steubenville, Ohio) , 41(4), 4
To select the most relevant ideas, the system
347–357. uses filtering heuristics based on the degree
of semantic interrelation with which each
Wertsch, J. (1998). Mind as Action. Oxford, UK:
student has published the ideas in the “soup”.
Oxford University Press.
(This, in turn, reinforces the students’ moti-
vation to make contributions.)
5
The results of these experiments are available
Endnotes deposited in our Cmap Site: “Universidad
de Extremadura (España)”, in the directory
1
The manner in which the maps can constitute “Asignatura de Didáctica de la Física” and in
a vehicle for the collaborative construction that corresponding to the 2004/05 academic
of learning in this type of activity requires year. They can be used interactively by means
a more detailed analysis, which will be of the CmapTools computer application.
specifically addressed in the next section These maps can also be viewed on the Web
2
This innovation was honoured with the 2nd at: http://grupoorion.unex.es:8001.Figure
National Educational Research Award 1998, Captions
and has also served as the foundation for the

345

View publication stats

You might also like