You are on page 1of 18

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/8888491

The Measurement of Effort-Reward Imbalance at Work

Article  in  Social Science & Medicine · April 2004


DOI: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00351-4 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
1,309 5,237

7 authors, including:

Dagmar Starke Tarani Chandola


Akademie für Öffentliches Gesundheitswesen, Germany The University of Manchester
46 PUBLICATIONS   1,997 CITATIONS    150 PUBLICATIONS   8,974 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Isabelle Godin Michael G Marmot


Université Libre de Bruxelles University College London
95 PUBLICATIONS   2,381 CITATIONS    1,324 PUBLICATIONS   115,276 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

PrivMort: The Impact of Privatization on the Mortality Crisis in Eastern Europe View project

ActiFE View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Isabelle Godin on 17 October 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


ARTICLE IN PRESS

Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

The measurement of effort–reward imbalance at work:


European comparisons
Johannes Siegrista,*, Dagmar Starkea, Tarani Chandolab, Isabelle Godinc,
Michael Marmotb, Isabelle Niedhammerd, Richard Petere
a
Department of Medical Sociology, University of Duesseldorf, PO Box 10 10 07, D-40001 Duesseldorf, Germany
b
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College of London, London, UK
c
D!epartment d’ Epid!emiologie et Promotion de la Sant!e, Universit!e Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium
d
#
INSERM U88, Hopital National de St. Maurice, France
e
Department of Epidemiology, University of Ulm, Germany

Abstract

Using comparative data from five countries, this study investigates the psychometric properties of the effort–reward
imbalance (ERI) at work model. In this model, chronic work-related stress is identified as non-reciprocity or imbalance
between high efforts spent and low rewards received. Health-adverse effects of this imbalance were documented in
several prospective and cross-sectional investigations. The internal consistency, discriminant validity and factorial
structure of ‘effort’, ‘reward’, and ‘overcommitment’ scales are evaluated, using confirmatory factor analysis.
Moreover, content (or external) validity is explored with respect to a measure of self-reported health.
Data for the analysis is derived from epidemiologic studies conducted in five European countries: the Somstress
Study (Belgium; n ¼ 3796), the GAZEL-Cohort Study (France; n ¼ 10; 174), the WOLF-Norrland Study (Sweden;
n ¼ 960), the Whitehall II Study (UK; n ¼ 3697) and the Public Transport Employees Study (Germany; n ¼ 316).
Internal consistency of the scales was satisfactory in all samples, and the factorial structure of the scales was consistently
confirmed (all goodness of fit measures were >0.92). Moreover, in 12 of 14 analyses, significantly elevated odds ratios
of poor health were observed in employees scoring high on the ERI scales.
In conclusion, a psychometrically well-justified measure of work-related stress (ERI) grounded in sociological theory
is available for comparative socioepidemiologic investigations. In the light of the importance of work for adult health
such investigations are crucial in advanced societies within and beyond Europe.
r 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Effort–reward imbalance; Self-rated health; Work stress questionnaire; Europe

Introduction adult socioeconomic status. Beyond economic livelihood


a person’s occupation is important for socialization. It
In advanced societies, work continues to play an provides opportunities for personal growth and devel-
important role for health and well-being in adult life. opment, including the favorable experience of self in a
Having a job is often a prerequisite for a continuous core social role, and allows participation in social
income, and, more thoroughly than any other social networks beyond primary groups.
circumstances, employment characteristics determine The nature of work has undergone profound changes
during recent decades. Today, fewer jobs are defined by
*Corresponding author. Department of Medical Sociology, physical demands and more by mental and emotional
Heinrich Heine Universitat, Universitastrasse 1, Duesseldorf demands. While jobs in mass production are decreasing,
40225, Germany. Fax: +49-81-12390. employment in the service sector continues to rise.
E-mail address: siegrist@uni-duesseldorf.de (J. Siegrist). Computer-based information processing is becoming a

0277-9536/$ - see front matter r 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00351-4
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1484 J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

part of a growing number of job profiles. In a large epidemiologic investigations, thus enabling comparisons
survey of European workers (Paoli & Merlli!e, 2001) 60 across different working populations in Europe.
percent work at very high speeds, 40 percent are
confined to monotonous work, 49 percent indicate The theoretical model
permanent and direct contact with clients, and 25
percent are working on shift rotation. Health adverse and health promoting psychosocial
These changes in the nature of work have coincided work environments are defined by the interaction
with changes in the labor market. Women and older between a person’s cognitions, emotions, and behaviors
people comprise a growing proportion of the work and his or her social environment. Traditional biome-
force. Part-time working and flexible work arrangements dical approaches cannot directly measure this interac-
have also increased. Perhaps most importantly for tion. While crude measures may focus on particular
health, job instability, forced mobility and unemploy- characteristics of the work place, the stressful character
ment are becoming more prevalent, resulting in an of a psychosocial factor is more appropriately assessed
increase in job career discontinuity, forced early retire- by using self-report data obtained from questionnaires
ment and job loss. Surveys report that 30 percent of and interviews. But, given the complexity and variability
workers are currently in insecure jobs (Ferrie, Marmot, of work-related psychosocial factors, these measure-
Griffiths, & Ziglio, 1999), and a significant proportion of ments need to be based on a theoretical model.
middle-aged men and women are no longer participating Theoretical models of work stress selectively reduce
in the labor market due to early, often involuntary the complexity and variation of interactions between the
retirement (Brugiavini, 2001). work environment and the working person by focusing
Social inequalities in the quality of work and in access on those general components that may explain its direct
to the labor market have been reported in numerous or indirect effects on health. Several such models have
studies, such that it can be concluded that the benefits been developed and tested (for review, e.g., see Cooper,
and costs of working life are unequally distributed 1998; Dunham, 2001), with two models receiving
across society. This conclusion also holds true for the particular attention in recent years: the demand-control
health burden of working life. A social gradient of model (Karasek, 1979; Karasek & Theorell, 1990) and
health according to occupational standing has been the model of effort–reward imbalance (Siegrist,
documented for all-cause mortality (Fox & Adelstein, 1996a, b). The former model has been tested in a large
1978; Kitagawa & Hauser, 1973; Marmot, Shipley, & number of investigations (for review see Marmot et al.,
Rose, 1984), coronary heart disease morbidity and 2002; Schnall, Belkic, Landsbergis, & Baker, 2000).
mortality (Marmot, Adelstein, Robinson, & Rose, Although a few studies show negative findings, an
1978; Marmot, Bosma, Hemingway, & Stansfeld, impressive body of evidence documents the explanatory
1997), psychiatric disorders, especially depression (Mar- power of the model or its single components.
mot, Shipley, Brunner, & Hemingway, 2001; Stansfeld, The latter model has been developed and tested more
Fuhrer, Shipley, & Marmot, 1999) and chronic bron- recently, with a rapid increase of evidence documenting
chitis (Marmot et al., 2001). These social inequalities in its role in explaining adverse health (for review see
health deserve particular attention considering that after Schnall et al., 2000; Marmot et al., 2002; Siegrist, 2002).
the first years of childhood, midlife is the time when While these two models complement each other (see
social differences in mortality are most pronounced Discussion) the focus of this contribution is on the
(Goldblatt, 1990; Marmot et al., 1998). Therefore, the effort–reward imbalance model, and more specifically
role of work in explaining health inequalities in midlife on the psychometric properties of its measurement. This
provides a core research task. More specifically, an model assumes that effort at work is spent as part of a
adverse psychosocial work environment is likely to contract based on the norm of social reciprocity where
contribute to the unequal health burden in a significant rewards are provided in terms of money, esteem, and
way (Marmot, Theorell, & Siegrist, 2002; Schrijvers, van career opportunities including job security. Further-
de Mheen, Stronks, & Mackenbach, 1998). Despite more, the model assumes that work contracts often fail
considerable progress in this field there is still a lack of to be fully specified and to provide a symmetric
agreement among scientists on how to define and exchange where complete equivalence exists between
measure an adverse psychosocial work environment. requested efforts and given rewards. In particular, this is
The aim of this article is to advance this debate by the case when there is little choice on the part of
focusing on one measurement approach termed effort– employees, such as a lack of alternatives in the labour
reward imbalance at work. The theoretical model market (e.g. low level of skill) or restricted mobility.
underlying this measure is briefly outlined, and the Moreover, employees themselves may accept such
scales measuring the components of this model are contracts for strategic reasons (e.g. to improve their
described. In the main part, psychometric properties of chances for career promotion at a later stage). Non-
these measures are analyzed by drawing upon several symmetric contracts are expected to be frequent in a
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499 1485

global economy that is characterized by job insecurity, of information according to predefined procedures (see
forced occupational mobility, short-term contracts and below) provides a more accurate estimate of the adverse
increased wage competition. health effects resulting from non-reciprocity compared
According to the model, the experience of a lack of to a restriction of information to one of these sources.
reciprocity in terms of high ‘costs’ and low ‘gains’ elicits
negative emotions in exposed people. Feelings of not
being appreciated in an adequate way or of being treated Methods
unfairly and disappointments resulting from inappropri-
ate rewards are paralleled by sustained strain reactions Study samples
in the autonomic nervous system. This is due to the fact
that the recurrent experience of reward deficiency in a This report is based on data obtained from working
core social role impairs successful self-regulation (Sieg- men and women in five different European countries:
rist, 2000). Thus, in the long run, the imbalance between Belgium, France, Germany, Sweden and UK. Coordi-
high effort and low reward at work increases illness nated data collection and analysis was the result of the
susceptibility as a result of continued strain reactions. authors’ collaboration in the frame of a scientific
So far, structural conditions of ‘high cost/low gain’ programme on social variations in health expectancy
conditions at work have been specified by the model. A in Europe, supported by the European Science Founda-
final assumption concerns individual differences in the tion. In most instances the measures analyzed in this
experience of effort–reward imbalance. People charac- contribution were administered as part of data collection
terized by a motivational pattern of excessive work- in already established longitudinal observational inves-
related commitment and a high need for approval tigations.
(‘overcommitment’) are at increased risk of strain from
non-symmetric exchange. For instance, they may expose The Somstress Study (Belgium)
themselves more often to high demands at work, or they The Belgian data came from the baseline data
exaggerate their efforts beyond what is formally needed. collection of the Somstress Study and was conducted
As a result, their susceptibility to the frustration of in four companies across Belgium in the years 1999 and
reward expectancies is increased. There is some evidence 2000 (Godin & Kittel, 2003). The sample is made of
of intrapersonal stability over time of this motivational 3796 employees (2056 men, 1740 women) aged 18–66
pattern (Siegrist, 1996b). Therefore, it can be considered years (mean age 4078,7). Companies were selected
a psychological risk factor in its own, even in the according to their structural environment and job
absence of structural conditions of imbalance at work. stability. Among the four worksites, one can be
However, the proposed model posits that strongest considered stable, two are in an in-between situation—
effects on health and well-being are expected to occur if with one company in a more unstable environment than
structural and personal conditions act in concert. Hence, the other one—and one is categorized as unstable. The
on this theoretical ground, overcommitment is hypothe- aim of the Somstress Study is to analyze the health
sized to modify (i.e. increase) the effect on health effects of these contextual economic conditions and, in
produced by effort–reward imbalance at work. the process, to explore the role of an adverse psycho-
In summary, the proposed theoretical model is based social work environment. The overall participation rate
on the sociological hypothesis that formalized social of employees from the four companies was approxi-
exchange, as mediated through core social roles (the mately 40 percent, with participants being representative
work role), is rooted in contracts of reciprocity of cost of the total registered work force of the enterprises
and gain. It defines structural conditions of non- in terms of sociodemographic and socioeconomic
symmetric contracts that result in strain from an characteristics.
imbalance between high effort and low reward. In
addition, it addresses individual differences in the The GAZEL-Cohort Study (France)
experience of this imbalance (overcommitment) by The GAZEL-Cohort was established in 1989 and
assuming an effect modification of overcommitment on included 20,624 subjects aged 35–50 years working at the
the association of effort–reward imbalance with health. French National Electric and Gas Company Electricit!e
It is important to stress that this model does not de France-Gaz de France (for detailed baseline sample
represent a black-box approach towards studying work description see Goldberg et al., 2001). Since 1989 this
stress that is restricted to the structural level of analysis cohort has been followed by yearly self-administered
(such as e.g. the demand-control model (Karasek & questionnaires and by data collection from personnel
Theorell, 1990)), but integrates structural and personal and medical departments of the company. In the 1998
components. However, these two components are survey the French version of the effort–reward imbal-
distinguished both at the conceptual and methodological ance questionnaire was applied (Niedhammer & Siegrist,
level. It is assumed that a combination of both sources 1998; Niedhammer, Siegrist, Landre, Goldberg, &
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1486 J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

Leclerc, 2000). The response rate to the 1998 ques- different occupational groups in the sample were
tionnaire was 73 percent. Retired people (n ¼ 4259) and comparable to the company’s work force as a whole.
other non-working subjects (n ¼ 206) were excluded, Women were underrepresented in the sample. Mean age
thus leaving a sample of 10,174 subjects (7251 men and of participants was 44.8 years (77.6) among men and
2923 women). 43.7 (76.4) among women.

The WOLF-Norrland Study (Sweden) Measures


Men and women beyond age 33 from several
companies representing different sectors in the Northern As the aim of the present study was to assess the
region of Sweden (Norrland) were invited to participate reliability and validity of the effort–reward imbalance
in an observational study on associations between (ERI) questionnaire in different study samples from
working conditions and cardiovascular risk factors, different countries, internal consistency of the scales
with a focus on hypertension, high levels of blood lipids (Cronbach’s a) and factorial validity (confirmatory
and hemostatic factors. The study protocol was almost factor analysis) were analyzed. Moreover, content (or
identical to that of a previous, much larger investigation external) validity of the questionnaire was assessed in
conducted in the greater Stockholm area (Peter et al., four of the five data sets (excluding WOLF-Norrland) as
1998). The WOLF-Norrland Study included the original an identical measure of health, and that of self-rated
Swedish version of the effort–reward imbalance ques- health was available. This measure was based on
tionnaire at baseline screening in a sample of 960 answers to a single Likert-scaled item: ‘How do you
participants (738 men and 222 women). Compared to rate your general health status?’ ranging from ‘bad’ to
the total group this sample was somewhat older and ‘very good’ (5-point scale in all studies except GAZEL
consisted of a larger proportion of men. where an 8-point scale was used). This question was
shown to predict subsequent deterioration in health
The Whitehall II Study (UK) status in a number of prospective investigations (Idler &
The Whitehall II Study of 10,308 male and female Benyamini, 1997).
civil servants was set up in 1985 to investigate and
understand the causes of social inequalities in health The effort–reward imbalance (ERI) questionnaire
(Marmot et al., 1991). A major aim of the study has
been to investigate occupational and other social The measurement of the model of effort–reward
influences on a range of diseases, most notably coronary imbalance at work is restricted to self-report data for
heart disease. At baseline all participants attended three reasons. First, because it combines descriptive and
medical examinations and completed questionnaires evaluative information on perceived demands (effort)
covering a wide range of information including: health and rewards. Secondly, it requires information on
status, work stress, social support, health behaviors, personal coping characteristics (overcommitment).
stressful life events and mental health. Several follow-up Thirdly, information on distant working conditions
assessments were conducted. At phase five, 7270 cannot be collected by other techniques (e.g. job
participants were included, 5000 of whom were still in security, promotion prospects, adequacy of salary in
employment and eligible to fill in the effort–reward view of training). Thus, assessment of effort–reward
imbalance questionnaire. The response rate was 74 imbalance at work relies on indicators that are measured
percent (n ¼ 3697). This group was representative of by psychometric scales containing Likert-scaled items.
the total Whitehall II phase five sample with the Essentially, three psychometric scales are measured
exception of a slight underrepresentation of women (effort, ‘reward’, overcommitment), and the scale
and lower-educated civil servants. ‘reward ‘ is assumed to act as a latent factor underlying
three theoretically postulated subscales of occupational
The Public Transport Employees Study (Germany) rewards. The combination of information from these
A group of female and male employees of a large scales according to a pre-defined algorithm provides an
public transport company in a major city in Germany opportunity of measuring the theoretical construct.
was invited to participate in a cross-sectional study. The
occupations consisted of bus and subway drivers, repair Effort
personnel and administrative staff (Joksimovic, Starke, Effort is measured either by five or six items that refer
Knesebeck, & Siegrist, 2002). Data collection was to demanding aspects of the work environment (three
conducted during two working days only and selection items measuring quantitative load, one item measuring
criteria for participation were age (35–60 years), dura- qualitative load, one item measuring increase in total
tion of membership in the company (>2 years) and load over time). One item measuring physical load is
fluency in German language. A total of 316 employees sometimes included as described below. In previous
participated (268 men, 48 women). Proportions of the studies, the five-item version excluding physical load has
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499 1487

been found to be psychometrically appropriate in analysis), was clearly improved (0.951) in a second-order
samples characterized predominantly by white collar factor analysis testing the three reward subscales if
jobs whereas the six-item version was appropriate in compared to a uni-dimensional factor analysis (0.915).
blue collar samples and occupational groups with Similar results were obtained from a respective analysis
manual workers. While this observation may support of data from the Public Transport Employees Study
the validity of the measure, it is recommended to include (GFI=0.962 vs. 0.871).
the item on physical load into the scale in those The answering and scoring procedures are those for
occupational groups only where prevalence of physical the effort items. A score of 11 indicates the perception of
workload is part of the typical task profile. Accordingly, the lowest rewards whereas a score of 55 reflects a very
as the majority of jobs included in this analysis are high level of reward (reverse coding).
white-collar rather than blue-collar jobs, the following
analyses are based on the five-item version (see Effort–reward ratio
Appendix). Items are answered in two steps. First, Given that a rather complex set of subjectively
subjects agree or disagree whether or not the item experienced work-related circumstances is being mea-
content describes a typical experience of their work sured by a limited number of items, we attempt to
situation. Subsequently, subjects who agree are asked to analyze this information as exhaustively as possible.
evaluate to what extent they usually feel distressed by Thus, in a first approach, the two sum scores can be used
this typical experience. The rating procedure is defined for estimation of their separate effects on outcome
as follows: (1) does not apply; (2) does apply, but subject criteria, such as health indicators. For instance, in
does not consider herself or himself distressed; (3) does occupational groups characterized by underload rather
apply and subject considers herself or himself somewhat than overload (e.g. doormen), low reward per se may be
distressed; (4) does apply and subject considers her or associated with an elevated risk.
himself distressed; (5) does apply and subject considers However, in keeping with the theoretical assumption
herself and himself very distressed. A sum score of these stated above, it is postulated that the imbalance between
ratings is constructed as the uni-dimensionality of the effort and reward is what matters most. Imbalance may
scale has been documented in several studies (Hanson, be constructed and analyzed by using a pre-defined
Schaufeli, Vrijkotte, Plomp, & Godaert, 2000; Nied- algorithm based on information obtained from the
hammer et al., 2000; Tsutsumi, Ishitake, Peter, Siegrist, effort and reward scales (Peter et al., 1998; for discussion
& Matoba, 2001; see also Results section). Thus, a total see Siegrist, 2002). A ratio is computed for every
sum score based on the five items measuring effort varies respondent according to the formula: e=ðrxc) where ‘e’
between 5 and 25. The higher the score, the more is the sum score of the effort scale, ‘r’ is the sum score of
perceived demands are experienced as stressful. the reward scale and ‘c’ defines a correction factor for
different numbers of items in the nominator and
Reward denominator. The correction factor is 0.454545 if the
Reward is measured by 11 items. Theoretically, we nominator contains five items (5/11). As a result, a value
expect three factors to result from factor analysis: (a) close to zero indicates a favorable condition (relatively
financial reward, (b) esteem reward, (c) reward related to low effort, relatively high reward), whereas values
promotion prospects (career) and job security. In beyond 1.0 indicate a high amount of effort spent that
empirical terms, it is almost impossible to disentangle is not met by the rewards received or expected in turn.
financial from career-related aspects. Therefore, we The construction of this ratio serves as an approx-
postulate a three-factorial structure of the construct of imate estimate of the costs and gains experienced in
occupational reward with a first factor defined by everyday working life that are not repeatedly subject to
financial and career-related aspects, a second factor explicit reasoning on trade-off by working people
defined by esteem rewards, and a third factor defined by themselves. Thus, the ratio may capture part of the
the gratification of job security (see Appendix). The strain reactions that would be missed if the measurement
three dimensions are assumed to load on one latent of imbalance were based exclusively on subjective
factor (reward). This assumption has been tested and evaluation (that is, asking the respondent if reciprocity
confirmed in several second-order factor analyses, exists). In stress physiological terms, according to the
including data sets from this study. Although this test theory of affective information processing (Le Doux,
is not subject of the current confirmatory factor analyses 1996), this assumption is crucial as only part of the flow
where the model fit of a three-factorial construct is of information resulting from the psychosocial environ-
analyzed with regard to its consistency across different ment is subject to conscious computational processing.
occupational groups and across the different language Strain reactions and negative emotions elicited by the
versions of the measures, the following information may experience of non-reciprocal exchange at work may
strengthen our approach. For instance, in the Somstress bypass conscious awareness, as is the case for a
Study, a measure of goodness of fit (GFI) (see Statistical substantial part of affective processing in general.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1488 J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

As an independent predictor of health outcomes this found that six items documented consistently high
ratio is either transformed into a binary variable (values loadings on one latent factor. Five of these items
p1.0 vs. >1.0) or transformed logarithmically into a belonged to the subscale ‘inability to withdraw from
continuous measure. Logarithmic transformation of the work obligations’, and one item to the subscale
continuous scale is performed in order to place 1.0 as the ‘impatience and disproportionate irritability’. These
center point for the scale. The use of a continuous two subscales were also highly correlated with coeffi-
measure has the advantage of providing more informa- cients ranging from r ¼ 0:74 to 0:88:
tion and generating more robust statistical effects These six items were subjected to confirmatory factor
compared to the binary measure (Niedhammer, Teck, analysis to evaluate goodness of fit, and results yielded
Starke, & Siegrist, 2003; Pikhart et al., 2001). Based on acceptable reliabilities (Siegrist et al., 2001). Gender
continuous information, quartiles or tertiles of the ratio differences and differences in health outcomes were then
can be defined, and dose–response relationships between analyzed comparing the original and the newly devel-
effort–reward ratio and health can be explored (see oped short scale with excellent properties observed for
Discussion). However, if several data sets are analyzed the short scale (Joksimovic et al., 2002; Siegrist et al.,
in a comparative way, as is the case in this study, 2001). Based on this evidence, the short version of the
transformation of the ratio into a binary variable (>1.0 scale overcommitment (six items, see Appendix) is
vs. p1.0) may be useful. recommended for measuring the personal component
of the model. Given the uni-dimensionality of this scale
Overcommitment the six four-point Likert scaled items are computed to a
Originally, the cognitive-motivational pattern of total score varying from 6 to 24. The higher the score,
coping with demands characterized by an excessive the more likely a subject is to experience overcommit-
work-related overcommitment and a high need for ment at work.
approval has been measured by a psychometric scale The 23 items of the effort–reward imbalance ques-
containing 29 items (‘need for control’; Siegrist, 1996a). tionnaire were translated from German to French,
The 29 items measure four relevant aspects of this English and Swedish, and the adequacy of translation
coping pattern: need for approval (6 items), competi- was checked by respective experts. In addition, an
tiveness and latent hostility (6 items), impatience and independent back-translation was performed, according
disproportionate irritability (8 items), and inability to to established standards of cross-cultural research, and
withdraw from work obligations (9 items). The four the version was adapted as necessary. In case of the
respective subscales were repeatedly found to load on Whitehall II Study, a few items were not included (see
one latent factor in second-order factor analysis. A high footnotes in Tables 2 and 3).
score, as defined by the upper tertile of the total factor
score of these 29 items, was shown to predict adverse Statistical analysis
health in several prospective and cross-sectional studies
(Siegrist, 2002). Psychometric information on the scales includes
Despite the predictive validity of the total score the mean, standard deviation, and scale reliability (Cronba-
factorial structure of the four subscales was not ch’s a). Information on discriminant validity is given for
successfully replicated in several studies (Hanson et al., gender, age and education. Confirmatory factor analysis
2000; Niedhammer et al., 2000), and the full psycho- was performed to test the dimensional structure of the
metric scale was considered too long for epidemiologic theoretical model (using the statistical package AMOS
research. A shorter version of the overcommitment scale 4). Goodness of fit was assessed by the GFI and the
was developed focusing on the core notion of spending AGFI index as well as Chi-square and root-mean square
excessive effort at work as evidenced by the respondent’s residual (RMR). GFI and AGFI indicate the amount of
inability to withdraw from work obligations and to variance and covariance explained by the model (Back-
develop a more distant attitude towards job require- haus, Erichson, Plinke, & Weiber, 2000; Byrne, 2001).
ments. This subscale (inability to withdraw from work Values X0.90 are considered satisfactory. RMR is the
obligations) resulted in predicted health outcomes of square root of the mean of the squared discrepancies
similar strength as those by the longer scale. Moreover, between the implied and the observed covariance
it showed the highest internal consistency in previous matrices (Kelloway, 1998). A value p0.05 is considered
analyses and had an acceptable scalability (Hanson et al., satisfactory.
2000; Siegrist et al., 2001). Exploratory and confirma- The maximum likelihood method was applied to
tory factor analyses were conducted using data from estimate parameters (Hayduk, 1987).
different study samples to develop a statistically more In addition to these test-statistical models, we applied
appropriate short scale. logistic regression analysis to test associations of the
Based on varimax-rotated exploratory main compo- components of the model with a binary outcome, self-
nent analysis of data from four study samples it was rated health (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989). Prevalence
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499 1489

odds ratios and 95 percent confidence intervals derived this end, means and standard deviations of the
from the logistic regression models are provided. effort, reward and overcommitment scales according to
gender, age and educational degree are displayed (see
Table 2).
Results The effort scale is composed of five items with a score
ranging from 5 to 25. Mean scores are highest in the
The five study populations vary considerably with Belgian and the Swedish groups. In three countries, the
regard to their occupational and sociodemographic score was higher in men than in women whereas a
composition. While the French GAZEL cohort, the reverse tendency was found in the Whitehall group.
Swedish WOLF cohort and the Belgian Somstress Scores decreased with age in the two elderly cohorts
cohort are composed of different occupational groups, (GAZEL, Whitehall), but not consistently among the
including blue-collars and white-collars, the British remaining groups. Mean effort is significantly higher
Whitehall cohort of civil servants and the German among better-educated groups in four samples, and a
public transport employees represent professionally similar non-significant tendency is observed in the
more homogeneous groups. In terms of age and gender smaller German sample. Overall, the internal consis-
these samples also differ quite remarkably, as can be tency of the scale is satisfactory.
seen from Table 1. In each study, the proportion of men The reward scale consists of 11 items (score ranging
is higher than the proportion of women, most notably in from 11 to 55). Reward scores are lower in the
the case of public transport workers and least so in the Belgian sample compared to the other groups;
case of the Belgian sample. Mean age of employees in however, approximately half of the Belgian study
the GAZEL and the Whitehall Study is higher compared population was threatened by job instability, whereas
to mean age in the remaining three studies. Variations in the four remaining populations job stability was
according to level of education are also present. When relatively high. Reward scores do not differ according
interpreting respective figures one should bear in mind to gender in a consistent way, but there is a tendency
the limited comparability of educational levels across of higher scores among older employees and especially
different countries and age cohorts. As can be seen, the in men (significant associations in four of five sub-
proportion of employees with less than college education groups). A positive association of reward with educa-
is lowest in the Swedish and the British samples, and tional degree is observed in the GAZEL and Somstress
highest in the French and the German samples. samples. In the Whitehall sample this association
Educational differences according to gender are most is not present. However, when replacing education by
pronounced in the Whitehall population and least employment grade (a more accurate indicator of
pronounced in the Somstress sample. socioeconomic position that was not available in a
These variations in types of occupation, education, age comparative way for the remaining studies) a
and gender provide opportunities to evaluate the work clear-cut gradient is observed with higher reward
stress measure in different populations. Moreover, some scores among higher employment grades. The internal
aspects of discriminant validity of the scales with respect consistency of the reward scale is satisfactory in all
to major sociodemographic variables can be tested. To samples.

Table 1
Sociodemographic characteristics of the study samples
GAZEL Whitehall II WOLF-Norrland Public transport workers Somstress

Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women

N % N % N % N % N % N % N % N % N % N %

Age (years) 7251 2923 2783 914 738 222 267 48 2055 1739
o40 — — — — — — — — 62 8.4 17 7.7 80 29.9 15 31.3 630 30.8 977 56.3
40–44 — — 273 9.3 — — — — 102 13.8 26 11.7 60 22.4 10 20.8 453 22.1 360 20.7
45–49 1304 17.9 1351 46.2 869 31.2 299 32.7 124 16.8 46 20.7 51 19.0 13 27.1 580 28.3 248 14.3
50–54 5147 70.9 1056 36.1 1097 39.4 334 36.5 115 15.6 39 17.6 36 13.4 10 20.8 316 15.4 114 6.5
>54 800 11.0 243 8.3 817 29.4 281 30.7 335 45.4 94 42.3 40 14.9 — — 66 3.2 35 2.0

Education 7251 2923 2687 847 738 222 267 48 2055 1739
oCollege 4788 66.0 2117 72.4 714 26.6 366 43.2 272 36.9 66 29.7 238 89.1 37 78.7 1262 62.9 745 43.2
College 485 6.7 365 12.5 818 30.4 185 21.8 291 39.4 90 40.5 8 3.0 5 10.6 490 24.4 747 43.3
University 1976 27.2 441 15.1 1155 43.0 296 34.9 175 23.7 66 29.7 21 7.9 5 10.6 253 12.6 231 13.4
1490
Table 2
Internal consistency and discriminant validity of the three scales of the model (differences calculated with t-tests for two independent samples, Kolmogorov–Smirnov–Test (Z),
ANOVA or Kruskal Wallis respectively)

Effort
GAZEL Whitehall IIa WOLF-Norrland Public transport workers Somstressb

J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499


Men Women tðpÞ Men Women ZðpÞ Men Women tðpÞ Men Women ZðpÞ Men Women tðpÞ
ðN ¼ 6447) ðN ¼ 2454Þ ðN ¼ 2783Þ ðN ¼ 914Þ ðN ¼ 738Þ ðN ¼ 222Þ ðN ¼ 256Þ ðN ¼ 46Þ ðN ¼ 2045Þ ðN ¼ 1724Þ

Sum score7SD 11.5774.28 11.3474.27 0.025 8.2472.80 8.5473.12 0.043 13.3174.87 12.3174.97 0.009 12.6573.93 11.7073.89 0.720 14.4272.52 14.1972.78 0.007

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Cronbach’s a 0.75 0.75 0.73 0.76 0.71 0.78 0.68 0.61 0.64 0.72

Age
o40 — — — — 13.58 10.59 13.17 12.29 14.51 14.12
40–44 — 11.23 — — 13.75 11.77 13.12 11.44 14.32 14.49
45–49 11.70 11.57 8.84 8.79 14.01 12.61 12.68 10.92 14.49 14.05
50–54 11.65 11.28 8.34 9.05 13.63 12.67 12.55 12.10 14.25 14.29
>54 10.82 10.32 7.63 7.75 12.75 12.49 10.85 — 14.69 13.81
0.000 0.002 0.000 0.000 0.075 0.590 0.008 0.846 0.396 0.182

Education
oCollege 11.46 11.10 7.86 7.96 12.22 10.86 12.66 11.22 14.11 13.58
College 11.59 11.68 8.17 8.56 13.19 12.44 11.13 12.50 14.72 14.61
University 11.82 12.13 8.53 9.30 15.19 13.59 13.10 15.00 15.39 14.84
0.014 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.006 0.394 0.120 0.000 0.000

Reward
GAZEL Whitehall IIc WOLF-Norrland Public transport workers Somstressb

Men Women tðpÞ Men Women ZðpÞ Men Women tðpÞ Men Women ZðpÞ Men Women tðpÞ
ðN ¼ 6447Þ ðN ¼ 2454Þ ðN ¼ 2783Þ ðN ¼ 914Þ ðN ¼ 738Þ ðN ¼ 222Þ ðN ¼ 248Þ ðN ¼ 42Þ ðN ¼ 2012Þ ðN ¼ 1671Þ

Sum score7SD 46.7177.86 46.6578.24 0.025 34.4875.60 33.9176.09 0.048 46.4077.7 45.1977.1 0.029 41.5278.93 42.7979.01 0.894 28.8674.82 29.1774.88 0.057
Cronbach’s a 0.86 0.88 0.83 0.84 0.79 0.70 0.86 0.87 0.78 0.77

Age
o40 — — — — 46.16 46.41 38.85 40.00 29.58 29.24
40–44 — 46.97 — — 47.23 45.65 40.50 39.20 28.87 29.28
45–49 46.22 46.66 33.77 33.71 46.29 45.09 40.67 46.92 28.41 28.93
50–54 46.59 46.53 34.13 33.23 45.31 45.08 44.03 45.25 28.11 28.48
>54 48.22 46.72 35.72 34.95 46.61 44.93 47.56 — 29.83 29.74
0.000 0.905 0.000 0.002 0.44 0.94 0.000 0.115 0.000 0.474

Education
o College 46.25 46.76 34.46 34.59 46.64 47.15 41.95 44.15 28.44 28.78
College 46.49 45.57 34.36 34.08 46.09 44.56 34.13 36.75 29.14 29.23
University 47.85 47.01 34.55 33.36 46.55 44.08 40.00 38.75 30.52 30.15
0.000 0.038 0.841 0.032 0.67 0.024 0.087 0.309 0.000 0.010

Overcommitment

J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499


GAZEL Whitehall IId WOLF-Norrland Public transport workers Somstress

Men Women tðpÞ Men Women ZðpÞ Men Women tðpÞ Men Women ZðpÞ Men Women tðpÞ
ðN ¼ 6289Þ ðN ¼ 2404Þ ðN ¼ 2783Þ ðN ¼ 914Þ ðN ¼ 738Þ ðN ¼ 222Þ ðN ¼ 265Þ ðN ¼ 48Þ ðN ¼ 2056Þ ðN ¼ 1740Þ

Sum score7SD 2.5170.61 2.5770.63 0.000 2.0270.76 2.0670.78 0.810 1.7970.61 1.8570.61 0.24 2.047.63 2.107.61 0.819 2.5770.60 2.5370.59 0.031

ARTICLE IN PRESS
Cronbach’s a 0.79 0.79 0.81 0.82 0.80 0.80 0.74 0.64 0.81 0.80

Age
o40 — — — — 1.83 1.88 1.90 2.20 2.47 2.47
40–44 — 2.52 — — 1.83 1.89 2.07 2.02 2.54 2.56
45–49 2.48 2.58 2.09 2.16 1.95 1.93 2.22 1.95 2.64 2.63
50–54 2.51 2.58 2.04 2.08 1.80 1.81 2.22 2.22 2.69 2.67
>54 2.54 2.48 1.93 1.89 1.90 1.80 1.91 — 2.68 2.76
0.086 0.16 0.000 0.000 0.004 0.83 0.008 0.675 0.000 0.000

Education
oCollege 2.48 2.56 1.93 1.88 1.67 1.85 2.03 2.05 2.59 2.54
College 2.51 2.56 2.02 2.08 1.72 1.74 2.19 2.17 2.55 2.51
University 2.59 2.60 2.11 2.27 2.07 1.99 2.12 2.27 2.51 2.55
0.000 0.482 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.045 0.683 0.828 0.079 0.531

p-values underlined.
a
Whitehall II: Effort excludes question ERI 6.
b
Somstress: Items were measured by a 4-point Likert scale.
c
Whitehall II: Reward excludes questions ERI 14, ERI 17.
d
Whitehall II: Overcommitment excludes question OC 1

1491
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1492 J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

Overcommitment, measured by six items, has scores reward scale seems to reflect contextual conditions, such
ranging from 6 to 24. Overall, a U-shaped distribution is as increased job instability, as evidenced by low scores in
observed with increasing age with men and women aged the Belgian sample where job instability is higher than in
45–54 generally having the highest scores. Employees the remaining groups.
with higher educational levels tend to exhibit higher Table 3 summarizes the results of confirmatory factor
scores of overcommitment, but again, this association is analyses for the effort, reward and overcommitment
not significant for all subgroups. The internal consis- scales. Respective analyses are performed separately for
tency of the scale is appropriate in all samples, with the the three scales within each sample, based on three
exception of the small sample of female transport statistical models specified according to the theoretical
workers in Germany. assumptions (see Introduction and Methods; see Ap-
Despite the heterogeneity of the samples in terms of pendix). This information is crucial as the statistical
occupation, education, age and gender composition, justification of using the single scales and the composite
there are some rather consistent tendencies. The mean measure of the effort–reward ratio for explaining health
levels of effort and reward are higher in better-educated relies on the goodness of fit of the measurement model
groups. Lower effort is associated with increasing age in with the data. With regard to the effort scale, the model
the two study populations with a high proportion of fit based on the assumption of one latent factor with five
elderly subjects, and higher reward is associated with variables loading on this factor was appropriate in all
increasing age, in particular among men. Finally, the five samples. Mean residual variances and covariances

Table 3
Criteria of goodness of fit of the three components of the model (confirmatory factor analysis)

Effort
GAZEL Whitehall II WOLF-Norrland Public transport company Somstress
ðN ¼ 8901Þ ðN ¼ 3697Þ ðN ¼ 960Þ ðN ¼ 283Þ ðN ¼ 3685Þ

Chi2 ðpÞ 373.44 (0.000) 21.39 (0.000) 22.94 (0.000) 6.72 (0.242) 93.19 (0.000)
Chi2/df 74.69 10.57 4.59 1.35 18.64
GFI 0.984 0.997 0.991 0.991 0.990
AGFI 0.953 0.986 0.972 0.972 0.971
RMR 0.039 0.012 0.035 0.036 0.018
Loading (e.) 0.54–0.79 0.30–0.67 0.51–0.84 0.51–0.69 0.42–0.57
a 0.79 0.74 0.71 0.72 0.68

Reward
GAZEL Whitehall II WOLF-Norrland Public transport companya Somstress
ðN ¼ 8901Þ ðN ¼ 3697Þ ðN ¼ 960Þ ðN ¼ 283Þ N ¼ 3534

Chi2 ðpÞ 1966.02 (0.000) 280.24 (0.000) 222.25 (0.000) 47.60 (0.191) 996.96 (0.000)
Chi2/df 47.95 16.49 5.56 1.19 24.32
GFI 0.957 0.981 0.959 0.971 0.951
AGFI 0.931 0.959 0.933 0.952 0.921
RMR 0.052 0.032 0.063 0.049 0.030
Loading (e.) 0.24–0.87 0.35–0.74 0.37–0.83 36–0.87 0.33–0.83
a 0.85 0.83 0.80 0.86 0.79

Overcommitment
GAZEL Whitehall II WOLF-Norrland Public transport company Somstress
ðN ¼ 8695Þ ðN ¼ 3697Þ ðN ¼ 894Þ ðN ¼ 283Þ ðN ¼ 3699Þ

Chi2 ðpÞ 618.51 (0.000) 33.33 (0.000) 56.63 (0.000) 19.37 (0.02) 28.43 (0.000)
Chi2/df 68.723 6.670 6.292 2.15 5.69
GFI 0.976 0.996 0.979 0.980 0.997
AGFI 0.944 0.989 0.951 0.953 0.991
RMR 0.0288 0.015 0.025 0.032 0.009
Loading (e.) 0.44–0.84 0.27–0.78 0.48–0.84 0.31–0.84 0.55–0.86
a 0.79 0.81 0.79 0.73 0.82
a
In the Public Transport Study a correlation between two measurement errors on items belonging to the same factor (ERI7 and
ERI8) was accepted in order to improve the model fit.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499 1493

Table 4
Self-rated health and the core components of the ERI model (results form logistic regression analyses)

GAZEL Whitehall II Public transport workers Somstress

Men Women Men Women Total Men Women


ðN ¼ 5343Þ ðN ¼ 1815Þ ðN ¼ 2668Þ ðN ¼ 843Þ ðN ¼ 283Þ ðN ¼ 2056Þ ðN ¼ 1740Þ

OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI

Effort–reward ratio
p1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
>1 2.6 2.0–3.3 2.8 2.0–4.1 3.5 2.3–5.3 5.2 3.0–9.3 3.1 1.4–6.5 3.2 1.8–5.7 1.6 0.8–3.0

Overcommitment
Low 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
High 1.5 1.3–1.8 1.8 1.5–2.3 2.2 1.7–2.8 1.7 1.1–2.5 1.5 0.9–2.5 3.5 2.2–5.6 2.6 1.4–4.9

(RMR) were well below the threshold of 0.05, and the Last, we present separate information on the content
model explains more than 95 percent of observed validity of the model’s core components (i.e. the effort–
variance and covariance (GFI, AGFI). Additional reward ratio (>1.0 vs. p1.0)) and overcommitment (i.e.
information reveals that the latent construct explains a upper tertile of scores vs. remaining group), using self-
substantial part of the observed variance of the single reported health as an indicator. As a measure of self-
variables, as evidenced by respective factor loadings. rated health was not available from the WOLF-Norr-
The model of the reward scale is more complex as land Study, the following analyses are restricted to the
three latent factors are specified: esteem, salary and remaining four samples. Table 4 displays the results of
career, and job insecurity (see Methods section and logistic regression analyses using a dichotomous out-
Appendix). Table 3 indicates that the residual variances come criterion (poor vs. remaining categories of self-
and covariances are again satisfactory though slightly rated health). Analyses are performed separately for
elevated for the five study samples, and the proportion men and women in the three investigations with large
of explained variances and covariances is therefore samples, and combined in the case of German transport
slightly lower, ranging from 92.1 to 95.9 percent (after employees due to small sample size. Results indicate
adjustment for degrees of freedom). Factor loadings (l) significantly elevated odds ratios of poor self-rated
and Cronbach’s a are well comparable to those of the health in the groups characterized by high effort and
remaining scales. In all analyses, moderate or high low reward in all four samples (varying between 2.4 and
correlations between the three factors esteem, salary and 5.2) with the one exception of women in the Somstress
career, and job insecurity were observed, thus support- Study. Similarly, scoring high on overcommitment is
ing the notion of an underlying ‘reward’ factor. associated with significantly elevated risk of poor self-
For the overcommitment scale findings are similar to rated health in all analyses except the public transport
the effort scale in which one latent factor is assumed employees. Elevated odds ratios vary between 1.5 and
with all variables loading on this factor. In all samples 3.5, indicating that effects are somewhat less pro-
mean residual variances and covariances were very low, nounced than with the effort–reward ratio. Controlling
ranging from 0.009 to 0.032, and, after adjusting for the for age did not significantly modify the observed effects.
number of degrees of freedom (AGFI), the model
explains between 94.4 and 99.1 percent of observed
variances and covariances. Moreover, factor loadings (l) Discussion
and Cronbach’s a are again high. Thus, this information
provides a solid test statistical basis of the short version The results of this comparative epidemiologic inves-
of the overcommitment scale (see Appendix). tigation in five European countries reveal good psycho-
In summarizing the results of the factorial structure, metric properties of the scales measuring effort–reward
the constructs underlying the theoretical effort–reward imbalance at work.
imbalance model have been replicated in five indepen- For the five samples, internal consistency and
dent samples of working men and women in different discriminant validity were assessed and their factorial
European countries. Neither the fit of the models nor the structure was analyzed by using parallel confirmatory
regression coefficients changed when analyses were factor analyses. In addition, one aspect of content
performed separately for men and women. Therefore, validity was examined with self-rated health as the
the model holds equally true for men and women. outcome. In almost all subgroups, significantly elevated
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1494 J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

odds ratios of poor health are observed among employ- policy: whereas the control paradigm points to the
ees who have an imbalance between effort and reward structure of power, division of labor and democracy at
and those who are characterized by a high level of work, the reward paradigm addresses the issue of
overcommitment. distributive justice and fairness. The two models offer
This is the first report assessing the reliability and opportunities for combining information on work strain
validity of the short form (23 items) of the effort–reward and health as conditions of low control and low reward
imbalance questionnaire in a comparative analysis based at work often cumulate within specific occupational
on samples of employed men and women from five groups. Preliminary evidence from a large-scale study
different European countries. In addition to testing indicates that combined effects of the two models on
psychometric properties of the scales, this comparative cardiovascular health are considerably stronger than
analysis is of theoretical relevance as it adds two aspects their separate effects (Peter, Siegrist, Hallqvist, Reuter-
to the current understanding of psychosocial stress at wall, Theorell & the SHEEP Study Group, 2002).
work. First, it underlines the importance of reciprocated Moreover, in the Whitehall II Study (Bosma, Peter,
exchange in a core social role (work) for maintaining Siegrist, & Marmot, 1998) it was found that the
good health as high cost and low gain states increase the demand-control model (the control dimension only)
risk of illness. Secondly, it illustrates the advantage of and the effort–reward imbalance model predict the
disentangling the extrinsic from the intrinsic compo- incident coronary heart disease with equal strength
nents of the complex person–work environment inter- when using appropriate statistical controls. There is now
action at the conceptual and operational level. More a growing number of studies available that contain
specifically, the model’s extrinsic component (effort– information on both models and, thus, explore their
reward ratio) has been complemented by introducing a separate and combined effects on health (see Siegrist,
personal style of coping with work demands (over- 2002).
commitment). Combining these components improves Several other general concepts of work stress have
the model’s predictive power. been proposed and tested, most notably the person
Before discussing methodological aspects of this environment fit model (Edwards, Caplan, & van
study, the relation of the current model to alternative Harrison, 1998) and models based on equity theory in
or complementary formulations of work stress needs to social psychology (Walster, Walster, & Berscheid, 1978).
be addressed. While the person environment fit model has brought
The demand-control model of work stress is the most conceptual and methodological innovations into the
influential and most widely tested approach towards field of occupational stress, it remains difficult to assess
assessing psychosocial stress at work (Karasek & its ability to predicting adverse health effects. Because
Theorell, 1990). There are several similarities between the model is complex, it is difficult to deduce and test
the demand-control and the effort–reward imbalance predictions in an unequivocal way and, thus, compare it
model. They both represent general models of work with the other two models.
stress with a selective analytical focus. Both are At first glance, core notions of equity theory and of
measured by a standardized self-administered question- the effort–reward imbalance model seem quite similar.
naire and data are analyzed according to predefined Yet, a closer look reveals clear differences. First, equity
procedures. However, the demand-control model is theory predicts a curvilinear relationship between equity
open to alternative or supplementary measures (e.g. and strain: perceiving oneself as deprived as well as
imputation of job characteristics scores (Amick et al., perceiving oneself as advantaged evokes negative emo-
2002; Schnall et al., 2000)). There is also a strong tions. Accordingly, receiving too much is considered as
conceptual and methodological overlap between the two detrimental as receiving too little. This proposition
scales of demand and effort. clearly differs from the effort–reward imbalance model
Yet, clear conceptual and methodological differences where inappropriately low gain only matters for adverse
exist. First, the demand-control model has been outcomes. This conclusion is in accordance with the
introduced and measured as a concept that is restricted predictions of prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky,
to the structural aspects of the psychosocial work 1979) and of the conservation of resources theory
environment, whereas the effort–reward imbalance (Hobfall, 1998). In addition, it is confirmed by evidence
model includes both structural and personal character- derived from neuroscience research (Gehring & Wil-
istics. Secondly, components of the effort–reward loughby, 2002). Secondly, while equity theory proposes
imbalance model (salaries, career opportunities/job two levels of comparison (equity in interpersonal and
security) are linked to more distant macro-economic intrapersonal terms) without specifying their separate or
labor market conditions, while the demand-control combined effects on strain the current model analyses
model’s major focus is on work place characteristics. equity of reward exclusively as a function of invested
Finally, the two different stress theoretical orientations effort. This restriction is in accordance with propositions
(control vs. reward) have different implications for derived from sociological exchange theory (Gouldner,
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499 1495

1960; for additional discussion of equity theory in this common method variance. While these problems cannot
framework see Siegrist, 2002). be resolved in the study, the focus of this contribution is
Several methodological aspects of this study need to on psychometric properties of the scales in a compara-
be discussed. First, the validity of self-assessed measures tive perspective. Because other prospective and cross-
of work stress is a controversial topic. Although in sectional studies have documented associations of the
conceptual terms there is a need to include the working effort–reward imbalance model with self-rated health
person’s experience into any comprehensive assessment (Niedhammer et al., 2003; Pikhart et al., 2001), reverse
of work stress, such measures are often subject to causation is unlikely to invalidate the findings.
reporting bias and may be influenced by specific It should also be mentioned that the samples included
personality characteristics, moods and contexts (Kahne- in this comparative study are heterogeneous in terms of
man, Diener, & Schwarz, 1999; Schnall et al., 2000; professions. While this heterogeneity has been intended
Semmer, Zapf, & Greif, 1996; Spector, Zapf, Chen, & in order to test the application of the measures in a
Frese, 2000). To minimize these sources of error, several broad spectrum of employed populations, it remains
strategies were followed in some, but not all studies nevertheless unclear to what extent the reported
measuring effort–reward imbalance at work: contextual psychometric properties hold true for other professional
validation; adjustment for a specific response style groups. Comparable factor analyses of the scales were
(negative affectivity); repeated measurement and differ- conducted in samples of specific professional groups
ent techniques of data collection (computer-assisted such as physicians (Calnan, Wainwright, & Almond,
interview vs. self-administered questionnaire). Contex- 2000), nurses (Bakker, Killmer, Siegrist, & Schaufeli,
tual validation was performed in studies on blue collar 2000), dental technicians (Tsutsumi et al., 2001) and
workers and middle managers where objective indicators software specialists (Hanson et al., 2000). Moreover,
(job instability, blocked mobility, inequality in wages) these and other studies indicate that the scales can be
were analyzed in relation to information obtained from applied to sociocultural populations that differ from
the questionnaire (Siegrist, 1996b). In a recent Japanese those in western and northern European countries, such
study, sensitivity to change over time of the scales was as countries in Central and Eastern Europe (Pikhart
documented in a company that was faced with a et al., 2001), China (Xu, Cao, Lee, & Critchley, 2000),
significant economic crisis (Tsutsumi, Nagami, Mori- Japan (Tsutsumi et al., 2001, 2002), the United States
moto, & Matoba, 2002). Two studies that included the (Rugulies & Krause, 2000) and Canada (Kerr, Ibrahim,
overcommitment scale in intervention trials aiming at a Peter, & Sullivan, 2000).
reduction of work-related stress and overcommitment A further limitation of this study relates to the
also indicated that the scales were sensitive to change statistical analyses that were applied to test the model’s
over time (Aust, Peter, & Siegrist, 1997; Puls, Inhester, & assumptions. As imbalance between effort and reward is
Wienhold, 2002). assumed to be of crucial importance, a ratio of the effort
Several investigations adjusting for negative affectiv- and reward scales is constructed. In several studies
ity demonstrated the persistent significance of the effects of the ratio on health were tested taking into
model’s scales on health indicators (Bosma et al., 1998; account main effects and interaction terms of the two
Hanson et al., 2000; Joksimovic et al., 2002; Stansfeld, scales. Evidence so far indicates that effects based on
Fuhrer, & Shipley, 1999). Repeated measures were continuous information on the ratio are more powerful
obtained from the longitudinal blue collar study than effects based on other formulations (Kuper et al.,
(Siegrist, 1996b). To date, the consistency of informa- 2002; Niedhammer et al., 2003; Pikhart, 2002). There-
tion obtained from repeated measurement has been fore, using the ratio as a continuous log-scaled variable
satisfactory. Similarly, comparison of computer-assisted is proposed as a most convenient statistical approach in
interview data with data received from self-administered future studies unless more sophisticated methods have
questionnaires did not reveal remarkable differences been developed. One interesting refinement concerns
with the exception of less missing values with computer- the test of separate effects on health produced by the
assisted data. An additional argument supporting the three reward components in relation to effort. Estimat-
validity of the measures is provided by the numerous ing the relative contribution of reward components is of
investigations that use medically diagnosed diseases, particular interest for intervention studies (Vegchel,
biomedical risk factors and continuously recorded Jonge, Bakker, & Schaufeli, 2002).
physiological parameters (ambulatory blood pressure, Likewise, improved statistical modeling may also
heart rate, heart rate variability) as outcome measures apply to the combined analysis of the two components
(Kivim.aki et al., 2002; Kuper, Singh-Manoux, Siegrist, (ratio, overcommitment) of the model. In this study,
& Marmot, 2002; for review of earlier findings see separate effects were only analyzed. Other investigations
Siegrist, 2002). have applied a linear structural equation modeling
The cross-sectional design of this study points to a (Siegrist, 1996b) or analyzed effect modification (over-
substantial limitation, and that of reverse causation and commitment modifies the effect of the ratio on health;
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1496 J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

Jonge, Bosma, Peter, & Siegrist, 2000; Kuper et al., participating civil service departments and their
2002). Additional statistical analyses should explore welfare, personnel, and establishment officers; the
whether ‘effort’, ‘reward’ (and its subscales) and ‘over- Occupational Health and Safety Agency; the Council
commitment’ are really separate constructs by conduct- of Civil Service Unions; all participating civil servants in
ing an overall confirmatory factor analysis on all items the study; and all members of the Whitehall II Study
simultaneously. In fact, such an approach has been Team.
successfully tested with different data sets, including the
Somstress Study data (see also Hanson et al., 2000). In
the Somstress data, the overall model fit (GFI) was Appendix
0.923, and the RMSEA measure was 0.065. The
standardized regression weights of the latent construct Effort
on ‘effort’ were 0.62, on ‘overcommitment’ 0.86, and on ERI1 I have constant time pressure due to a heavy
‘reward’ –0.45. This information indicates that, indeed, work load.
the theoretically assumed structure of the proposed ERI2 I have many interruptions and disturbances in
theoretical model fits the data. my job.
In conclusion, a psychometrically well-justified ERI3 I have a lot of responsibility in my job.
measure of work-related stress (ERI) grounded in ERI4 I am often pressured to work overtime.
sociological theory is available for comparative socio- ERI6 Over the past few years, my job has become
epidemiologic investigations. In the light of the im- more and more demanding.
portance of work for adult health such investigations are
critical in advanced societies within and beyond Europe. Reward
Component esteem
ERI7 I receive the respect I deserve from my
Acknowledgements superiors.
ERI8 I receive the respect I deserve from my
The collaborative research presented in this paper was colleagues.
supported by the European Science Foundation’s ERI9 I experience adequate support in difficult
Scientific Programme on ‘Social Variations in Health situations.
Expectancy in Europe’. Authors thank Craig Pollack ERI10 I am treated unfairly at work.
and Andreas Roedel for their help in finalizing the ERI15 Considering all my efforts and achievements, I
paper. Further acknowledgement concerns the single receive the respect and prestige I deserve at
studies. SOMSTRESS: This study was supported by the work.
Federal Office for Scientific, Technical and Cultural
Affairs (SSTC) in Belgium. GAZEL and WOLF-Norr- Component job promotion
land: The authors would like to thank the Medical ERI11 My job promotion prospects are poor.
Committee of EDF-GDF, all the workers of the ERI14 My current occupational position adequately
GAZEL cohort, and the members of the GAZEL study reflects my education and training.
team, especially Marcel Goldberg and Annette Leclerc, ERI16 Considering all my efforts and achievements,
who made the GAZEL study possible. The same holds my work prospects are adequate.
true for the workers of the WOLF cohort, the WOLF ERI17 Considering all my efforts and achievements,
study group, especially Lars Alfredsson, Anders Knuts- my salary/income is adequate.
.
son, Tores Theorell and Peter Westerholm. Public
Transport Study: Authors thank all study participants Component job security
and the collaborators Ljiljana Joksimovic and Olaf von ERI12 I have experienced or I expect to experience an
dem Knesebeck. The study was supported by the undesirable change in my work situation.
German Ministry of Research and Technology (C1 ERI13 My job security is poor.
Public Health). Whitehall II: The Whitehall II Study has
been supported by grants from the Medical Research Overcommitment
Council; British Heart Foundation; Health and Safety OC1 I get easily overwhelmed by time pressures at
Executive; Department of Health; National Heart Lung work.
and Blood Institute (HL36310), US, NIH: National OC2 As soon as I get up in the morning I start
Institute on Aging (AG13196), US, NIH: Agency for thinking about work problems.
Health Care Policy Research (HS 06516); and the John OC3 When I get home, I can easily relax and ‘switch
D and Catherine T MacArthur Foundation Research off’ work.
Networks on Successful Midlife Development and OC4 People close to me say I sacrifice too much for
Socioeconomic Status and Health. We also thank all my job.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499 1497

OC5 Work rarely lets me go, it is still on my mind Imbernon, E. (2001). Socioeconomic, demographic, occupa-
when I go to bed. tional and health factors associated with participation in a
OC6 If I postpone something that I was supposed to long-term epidemiologic survey. A prospective study of the
do today I’ll have trouble sleeping at night. French GAZEL cohort and its target population. American
Journal of Epidemiology, 154, 373–384.
Goldblatt, P. (1990). 1971–1981 longitudinal study. Mortality
and social organization. London: Her Majesty’s Stationery
References Office.
Gouldner, A. (1960). The norm of reciprocity. A preliminary
statement. American Sociological Review, 25, 161–179.
Amick, B. C., McDonough, P., Chang, H., Rogers, W. H.,
Hanson, E. K. S., Schaufeli, W., Vrijkotte, T., Plomp, N. H., &
Pieper, C. F., & Duncan, G. (2002). Relationship between
Godaert, G. L. R. (2000). The validity and reliability of the
all cause mortality and cumulative working life course—
Dutch effort–reward imbalance questionnaire. Journal of
Psychosocial and physical exposures in the United States
Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 142–155.
labor market from 1968–1992. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64,
Hayduk, L. A. (1987). Structural equation modelling with Lisrel.
370–381.
Aust, B., Peter, R., & Siegrist, J. (1997). Stress management in Essentials and advances. Baltimore, London: Johns Hopkins
bus drivers. A pilot study based on the model of effort– University Press.
reward imbalance. International Journal of Stress Manage- Hobfall, S. E. (1998). Stress, culture, and community. New
ment, 4, 297–305. York: Plenum Press.
Backhaus, K., Erichson, B., Plinke, W., & Weiber, R. (2000). Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic
Multivariate Analyse-methoden. Berlin: Springer. regression. New York: Wiley.
Bakker, A. B., Killmer, C., Siegrist, J., & Schaufeli, W. B. Idler, E. L., & Benyamini, Y. (1997). Self rated health and
(2000). Effort–reward imbalance and burnout among mortality: A review of 27 community studies. Journal of
nurses. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 31, 884–891. Health and Social Behavior, 34, 21–37.
Bosma, H., Peter, R., Siegrist, J., & Marmot, M. (1998). Two Joksimovic, L., Starke, D., von dem Knesebeck, O., & Siegrist,
alternative job stress models and the risk of coronary heart J. (2002). Perceived workstress, overcommitment and self-
disease. American Journal of Public Health, 88, 68–74. reported musculoskeletal pain: A cross-sectional investiga-
Brugiavini, A. (2001). Early retirement in Europe. European tion. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 9,
Review, 9, 501–510. 122–138.
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural Equation Modelling with Amos. Jonge, J. de., Bosma, H., Peter, R., & Siegrist, . J. (2000). Job
Basic Concepts Application and Programming. Mahwah, strain, effort–reward imbalance and employee well-being: A
London: Erlbaum. large-scale cross-sectional study. Social Science & Medicine,
Calnan, M., Wainwright, D., & Almond, S. (2000). Job strain, 50, 1317–1327.
effort–reward imbalance and mental distress: A study of Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (1999). Well
occupations in general medical practice. Work and Stress, being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York:
14, 211–297. Russel Sage Foundation.
Cooper, C. L. (Ed.). (1998). Theories of organizational stress. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An
Oxford: Oxford University Press. analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.
Dunham, J. (2001). Stress in the workplace. Past, present and Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and
future. London: Whurr. mental strain: Implications for job redesign. Administration
Edwards, J. R., Caplan, R. D., & van Harrison, R. (1998). Science Quarterly, 24, 285–307.
Person–environment fit. In C. Cooper (Ed.), Theories of Karasek, R. A., & Theorell, T. (1990). Healthy work. Stress,
organizational stress (pp. 28–67). Oxford: Oxford University productivity, and the reconstruction of working life. New
Press. York: Basic Books.
Ferrie, J., Marmot, M., Griffiths, J., & Ziglio, E. (Eds.). (1999). Kelloway, E. K. (1998). Using LISREL for structural equation
Labour market changes and job insecurity: A challenge for modelling. A researcher’s guide. Thousand Oaks, London:
social welfare and health promotion. Copenhagen: WHO Sage Publications.
Regional Publications. Kerr, M. S., Ibrahim, S., Peter, R., & Sullivan T, J. (2000).
Fox, A. J., & Adelstein, A. M. (1978). Occupational mortality: Frequency of musculoskeletal pain and the effort–reward
Work or way of life? Journal of Epidemiology and imbalance model [Abstract]. International Journal of Beha-
Community Health, 32, 73–78. vioral Medicine, 7, 161.
Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal Kitagawa, E. M., & Hauser, P. M. (1973). Differential mortality
cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and in the united states: A study in socio economic epidemiology.
losses. Science, 295, 2279–2282. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Godin, I., & Kittel, F. (2003). Differential economic stability Kivim.aki, M., Leino-Arjas, P., Luukonen, R., Riihim.aki, H.,
and psychosocial stress at work: Associations with psycho- Vahtera, J., & Kirjonen, J. (2002). Work stress and risk of
somatic complaints and absenteeism. Social Science & cardiovascular mortality: Prospective cohort study of
Medicine, doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00345-9. industrial employees. British Medical Journal, 325, 857–863.
Goldberg, M., Chastang, J. F., Leclerc, A., Zins, M., Bone- Kuper, H., Singh-Manoux, A., Siegrist, J., & Marmot, M.
nfant, S., Bugel, I., Kaniewski, N., Schmaus, A., Niedham- (2002). When reciprocity fails: Effort–reward imbalance in
mer, I., Piciotti, M., Chevalier, A., Godard, C., & relation to CHD and health functioning within the
ARTICLE IN PRESS
1498 J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499

Whitehall II Study. Occupation and Environmental Medicine, Pikhart, H., Bobak, M., Siegrist, J., Pajak, A., Rywik, S.,
59, 777–784. Kyshegyi, J., Gostautas, A., Skodova, Z., & Marmot, M.
LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon & (2001). Psychosocial work characteristics and self-rated
Schuster. health in four post-communist countries. Journal of
Marmot, M. G., Adelstein, A. M, Robinson, N., & Rose, G. Epidemiology and Community Health, 55, 624–630.
(1978). The changing social class distribution of heart Puls, W., Inhester, M. L., & Wienhold, H. (2002). Stress
disease. British Medical Journal, 2, 1109–1112. management trainings as a component of work
Marmot, M. G., Bosma, H., Hemingway, H., & Stansfeld, S. A. place prevention of substance use disorders. Sucht, 48,
(1997). Contribution of job control and other risk factors to 271–283.
social variations in coronary heart disease incidence. Lancet, Rugulies, R., & Krause, N. (2000). The impact of job
350, 235–239. stress on musculoskeletal disorders, psychosomatic
Marmot, M. G., Davey-Smith, G., Stansfeld, S. A., Patel, C., symptoms and general health in hotel room cleaners
North, F., Head, J., White, I., Brunner, E., & Feeney, A. [Abstract]. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
(1991). Health inequalities among British civil servants: The 7(1), 16.
Whitehall II Study. Lancet, 337, 1387–1393. Schnall, P. L., Belkic, K., Landsbergis, P., & Baker, D. (2000).
Marmot, M. G., Fuhrer, R., Ettner, S. L., Marks, N. F., The workplace and cardiovascular disease. Occupational
Bumpass, L. L., & Ryff, C. D. (1998). Contribution of Medicine: State of the Art Reviews, 15, 1–334.
psychosocial factors to socio economic differences in health. Schrijvers, C. T. M., van de Mheen, H. D., Stronks, K., &
The Milbank Quarterly, 76, 403–448. Mackenbach, J. P. (1998). Socio economic inequalities in
Marmot, M. G., Shipley, M., Brunner, E., & Hemingway, H. health in the working population: The contribution of
(2001). Relative contribution of early life and adult socio working conditions. International Journal of Epidemiology,
economic factors to adult morbidity in the Whitehall II 27, 1011–1018.
Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 55, Semmer, N., Zapf, D., & Greif, S. (1996). ‘Shared job strain’. A
301–307. new approach for assessing the validity of job stress
Marmot, M. G., Shipley, M. J., & Rose, G. (1984). Inequalities measurements. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
in death-specific explanations of a general pattern. Lancet, i, Psychology, 63, 293–310.
1003–1006. Siegrist, J. (1996a). Adverse health effects of high effort–low
Marmot, M. G., Theorell, T., & Siegrist, J. (2002). Work and reward conditions at work. Journal of Occupational Health
coronary heart disease. In S. A. Stansfeld, & M. G. Marmot Psychology, 1, 27–43.
(Eds.), Stress and the heart (pp. 50–71). London: BMJ Siegrist, J. (1996b). Soziale Krisen und Gesundheit (Social crises
Books. .
and health). Gottingen: Hogrefe.
Niedhammer, I., & Siegrist, J. (1998). Facteurs psychosociaux Siegrist, J. (2000). Place, social exchange and health: Proposed
au travail et maladies cardiovasculaires. Revue d’Epid- sociological framework. Social Science & Medicine, 51,
e!miologie et Sant!e Publique, 46, 398–410. 1283–1293.
Niedhammer, I., Siegrist, J., Landre, M. F., Goldberg, M., & Siegrist, J. (2002). Effort–reward imbalance at work and health.
Leclerc, A. (2000). Etudes de qualit!es psychom!etriques de la In P. L. Perrowe, & D. C. Ganster (Eds.), Historical and
version francaise du mod!ele du Des!equilibre Efforts/ current perspectives on stress and health (pp. 261–291).
R!ecompenses. Revue d’Epid!emiologie et de Sant!e Publique, Amsterdam: JAI Elsevier.
48, 419–437. Siegrist, J., Starke, D., Knesebeck, O. V. d., Joksimovic, L.,
Niedhammer, I., Tek, M. L, Starke, D., & Siegrist, J. (2003). Dragano, D., & Larisch, M. (2001). Soziale Reziprozitat . und
Effort–reward imbalance model and self-reported health: Gesundheit-eine exploratorische Studie zu beruflichen und
cross-sectional and prospective findings from the GAZEL auX erberuflichen Gratifikationskrisen. Unpublished Re-
cohort. Social Science & Medicine, this issue. search Report. Department of Medical Sociology, Univer-
Paoli, P., & Merlli!e, D. (2001). Third European Survey on sity of Duesseldorf, Duesseldorf.
working conditions 2000. Luxembourg: European Founda- Spector, P. E., Zapf, D., Chen, P. Y., & Frese, M. (2000).
tion for the Improvement of Living and Working Why negative affectivity should not be controlled in
Conditions. job stress research: Don’t throw out the baby with the
Peter, R., Alfredsson, L., Hammar, N., Siegrist, J., Theorell, T., bath water. Journal of Organisational Behaviour, 21,
& Westerholm, P. (1998). High effort, low reward, and 79–95.
cardiovascular risk factors in employed Swedish men and Stansfeld, S., Fuhrer, R., Shipley, M. J., & Marmot M, G.
women: Baseline results from the Wolf Study. Journal of (1999). Work characteristics predict psychiatric
Epidemiology and Community Health, 52, 540–547. disorder: Prospective results from the Whitehall II
Peter, R., Siegrist, J., Hallqvist, J., Reuterwall, C., Theorell, T., Study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 56,
& the SHEEP Study Group (2002). Psychosocial work 302–307.
environment and myocardial infarction: improving risk Tsutsumi, A., Ishitake, T., Peter, R., Siegrist, J., & Matoba, T.
estimation by combining two alternative job stress models in (2001). The Japanese version of the Effort–Reward-Imbal-
the SHEEP Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community ance Questionnaire: A study in dental technicians. Work and
Health 56, 294–300. Stress, 15, 86–96.
Pikhart, H. (2002). Social and psychosocial determinants of Tsutsumi, A., Nagami, M., Morimoto, K., & Matoba, T.
self rated health in Central and Eastern Europe. Boston: (2002). Responsiveness of measures in the effort–reward
Kluwer. imbalance questionnaire to organizational changes: A
ARTICLE IN PRESS
J. Siegrist et al. / Social Science & Medicine 58 (2004) 1483–1499 1499

validation study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 52, Walster, E., Walster, G. W., & Berscheid, E. (1978). Equity:
249–256. Theory and research. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Vegchel, N. van, Jonge, J. de, Bakker, A. B., & Schaufeli, W. B. Xu, L. Y., Cao, J. W. U., Lee, L. M., & Critchley, J. A. J. H.
(2002). Testing global and specific indicators of rewards in (2000). Psychosocial stress and hypertension among work-
the Effort–Reward Imbalance Model: Does it make any ing women in Beijing [Abstract]. International Journal of
difference? European Journal of Work and Organizational Behavioral Medicine, 7(Suppl. 1), 10.
Psychology, 11, 403–421.

View publication stats

You might also like