You are on page 1of 14

J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

DOI 10.1007/s10956-010-9266-z

A Comparison of Different Conceptual Change Pedagogies


Employed Within the Topic of ‘‘Sound Propagation’’
Muammer Çalik • Murat Okur • Neil Taylor

Published online: 17 November 2010


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Abstract The purpose of this study was to compare dif- that employed the entire suite of conceptual change peda-
ferent conceptual change methods within a topic on ‘sound gogies produced the best learning outcomes.
propagation’. The study was conducted with 80 grade 5
students (aged 11–12 year old) drawn from four cohort Keywords Science education  Conceptual change 
classes in an elementary school on the north coast of Black Alternative conception  Sound propagation
Sea Region in Turkey. While one class was assigned as a
control group, the others formed experimental groups (one
with a conceptual change text, one with analogies pre-
sented as computer animations and one with a combination Introduction
of conceptual change text, analogies and computer ani-
mations). A questionnaire with 10 two-tier questions was The constructivist view of learning has played a significant
administered as a pretest a week before the teaching role in science education for a number of decades (e.g.
intervention, and the same test was re-administered Fensham 1992; Niaz 2008). It incorporates four essential
immediately after the intervention as a post-test. The features: elicitation of prior knowledge, creation of cog-
questionnaire was also employed as a delayed post-test nitive dissonance, application of new knowledge, and
3 weeks after the teaching intervention. The experimental reflection on learning (e.g. Baviskar et al. 2009). Eliciting
groups performed significantly better in the post-test that prior knowledge is first step in this sequence, and has been
the control group (p \ 0.05). Within the experimental well documented in the literature. Prior knowledge may not
groups, the group exposed to a combination of the con- be in accord with the scientific view and has been referred
ceptual change text, analogies and computer animations to in a number of ways in the literature, e.g. ‘alternative
performed best on the post-test and the delayed post-test frameworks or conceptions’, ‘children science’, ‘alterna-
(p \ 0.05). Overall the study indicated that the intervention tive science’, or ‘misconception’ (e.g. Çalık and Ayas
2005; Fensham et al. 1994; Gökdere and Çalık 2010).
Students’ alternative conceptions have been recorded for
most topics within school science e.g. heat and temperature
M. Çalik (&) (e.g. Frederik et al. 1999; Havu-Nuutinen 2007), force and
Department of Primary Teacher Education, Fatih Faculty motion (e.g. Rowlands et al. 2007; Özsevgeç 2006), energy
of Education, Karadeniz Technical University, Trabzon, Turkey (e.g. Ametller and Pinto 2002; Hapkiewicz 1992; Hırça
e-mail: muammer38@hotmail.com; muammer38@ktu.edu.tr
et al. 2008), mechanics (Clement 1987; Oliva 2003),
M. Okur electricity and magnetism (e.g. Choi and Chang 2004;
Department of Science Education, Fatih Faculty of Education, Demirci and Çirkinoğlu 2004), sound and sound propaga-
Karadeniz Technical University, Trabzon, Turkey tion (e.g. Eshach and Schwartz 2006; Hrepic 1998; Merino
1998), light/light source (e.g. Çepni 2009), liquid pressure
N. Taylor
School of Education, University of New England, Arimidale, (Sahin et al. 2009) and mass and weight (Hapkiewicz 1992;
NSW, Australia Moore and Harrison 2004).

123
730 J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

The focus of this research is sound propagation and build on learners’ existing ideas and extend them. Duit
although children experience sound perpetually in the (1994) has referred to these groupings as, respectively
daily lives and as such as a topic it has significant rele- representing discontinuous and continuous pathways
vance, in learning the science of sound many alternative towards conceptual change. The section that follows will
conceptions can be developed. In fact, several studies examine different pedagogical techniques from these two
have focused on students’ alternative conceptions in this categories that were employed in this study.
area (e.g. Beaty 2001; Eshach and Schwartz 2006; Hrepic
1998; Lautrey and Mazens 2004; Menchen and Thompson Analogy and Computer Animation
2004; Merino 1998; Periago et al. 2009), their mental
models (e.g. Coombs 2007; Hrepic 2002, 2004; Hrepic In order to achieve conceptual change in science students
et al. 2002; Lautrey and Mazens 2004), analysis of text- are often required to compare ‘new scientific knowledge’
books (Chambliss 2001), a curriculum design framework with the alternative conception they already hold. With
for science education based on the history of science analogies, students make connections between abstract
(McColl 2003), growth within Vygotsky’s zone of prox- scientific concepts and familiar concrete experiences or
imal development (Jones et al. 1998), and student rea- objects. Coll et al. (2005) suggest that analogies work
soning (Lautrey and Mazens 2004; Mazens and Lautrey because students map the shared attributes of the analogue
2003; Wittmann et al. 2003). The role of the science to the target domain (Duit 1991). Using analogies is now
teacher is to identify these alternative conceptions and ubiquitous in the science classroom (e.g. Brown 1993;
where possible move the students towards an under- Dagher 1995), and avoids students perceiving that their
standing of the scientific view. own ideas are undervalued (Chiu and Lin 2002; Taylor and
There are a number of pedagogical tools that teachers Coll 1997) as there is no direct cognitive conflict involved
can use to help achieve this. These include the use of (Clement 1983; Glynn 1989). There are some significant
analogies, computed based learning, conceptual change advantages in using analogies, e.g. increasing in students’
texts, and so on. Within the topic of sound various authors enthusiasms, making the unfamiliar familiar, making
have used different approach to achieve conceptual change abstract concepts concrete; taking into account students’
in the topic of sound, for example, the learning cycle pre-existing knowledge, however, analogies may have
(Barman et al. 1996), problem based learning and know- disadvantages if they are used without appropriate plan-
want-learn (KWL) (Yurd and Olgun 2008), computer- ning. Therefore, analogies should be used with caution and
based learning (Salgut 2007), hands-on activities (Mueller viewed as a ‘double-edged sword’ (e.g. Harrison and Coll
et al. 2004), worksheets based on 5E model (Sağlam 2006). 2007).
All of these studies reported that the use of conceptual Computer animations can also be used to help students
change methods was more effective in remedying the stu- visualize abstract concepts or sub-microscopic phenomena
dents’ alternative conceptions than traditional approaches. (Russell et al. 1997), because animations help students to
However, none of the studies has focused on whether the imagine how complex/abstract dynamic processes take
teaching intervention helped students to retain their new place at the sub-microscopic level (e.g. Sanger and
conceptions in the long-term, as none of them included a Greenbowe 1997). When an animation is played, students
delayed post test. The present study addresses this issue by can monitor the representations of several dynamic physi-
examining the longer term retention of conceptual change. cal processes (Burke et al. 1998). As with analogies,
Furthermore, the study employs a quasi-experimental animations help make abstract concepts or phenomena
design to compare the effectiveness of different pedagogies ‘concrete’, improve students’ creative thinking, and
e.g. analogies and conceptual change texts, used separately enhance students’ enthusiasms and engagement with the
and in combination as recommended by Çalık et al. (2009). learning of science (e.g. Çalık et al. 2010b; Özsevgeç 2010;
The following section provides a brief review of the liter- Tasker and Dalton 2006). The most significant difference
ature that supports this study. between an analogy and a computer animation is that the
animation is created digitally on a computer. In other
Literature Framework words, analogies may be converted into computer anima-
tions using a computer and generating motion. The authors
Scott et al. (1992) have identified two main groupings of here purpose the use of analogies and animations (1) to
strategies aimed at promoting conceptual change and a make abstract concepts or phenomena ‘concrete’, (2) to
better understanding of science. One grouping is based create an interactive learning environment, (3) to foster
upon cognitive conflict and the resolution of conflicting individual learning, (4) to boost engagement with the
perspectives. The second grouping is of strategies which learning of science.

123
J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742 731

Conceptual Change Texts their physical, mental and moral development in accor-
dance with national objectives. At this level, students are
Conceptual change texts offer more cost- and time- introduced with several courses: Knowledge of life, Turkish
effective strategies by incorporating common alternative language, Visual arts, Social science, Science and tech-
conceptions and comparing these with scientific evidence nology, Physical education, Culture of Religion and Ethics
to produce cognitive conflict. In this way conceptual and so forth. In grade 4 they begin a Science and Tech-
change texts aim to move the learner towards the consen- nology Course taught by elementary school teachers. In
sual scientific view (e.g. Hynd 2001). Several studies have grade 5, the students undertake the following topics within
reported that conceptual change texts are efficient in the ‘Light and Sound’ unit: ‘Sound cannot travel through
achieving conceptual change (e.g. Dole 2000; Murphy space’, ‘Sound propagates at different mediums or mat-
2001). Other researchers suggest that conceptual change ters’, ‘Can sound propagation be prevented?’ and ‘Record
texts should be used in conjunction with other strategies voice and listen’.
e.g. group learning situations (e.g. Guzzetti et al. 1997) or
whole-class discussions (Guzzetti et al. 1997) to produce
Participants
better student understandings. The authors here propose
that the use of conceptual change texts is potentially
Within a non-equivalent pretest–posttest control group
appealing because of its economy and ease of use. Also,
design (Robson 1998), the study was conducted with 80
conceptual change texts not only consist of concise sum-
grade 5 students (aged 11–12 year old) drawn from four
maries that present and compare alternative and scientific
cohort classes in an elementary school on the north coast of
conceptualizations, but are also time-efficient when used
Black Sea Region in Turkey. The students under investi-
within a crowded curriculum (Palmer 2003).
gation had similar educational and socio-economic back-
grounds. Since all elementary school teachers were keen on
participating in the study, the three experimental groups,
The Aim of This Study each comprising 20 students (one with a conceptual change
text, one with analogies and their converted computer
The aim of this study is to compare different conceptual animations and one with a combination of conceptual
change methods with each other and with current teaching change text, analogies and animations) and a control group
methods within the ‘sound propagation’ topic of a ‘light (20 students) were randomly assigned to them. All of the
and sound’ unit. The following four research questions teachers had similar educational backgrounds and teaching
guide this study: experience. The researchers introduced the intervention
1. At which level does a conceptual change text influence materials to the teachers in the experimental groups and
students’ conceptual change of ‘sound propagation’ illustrated their practical applications 1 week before the
concepts? teaching intervention. Furthermore, one of the researchers
2. At which level does analogy/computer animation participated in each intervention and observed how the
influence students’ conceptual change of ‘sound prop- teachers carried them out.
agation’ concepts?
3. Is there any significant difference between the use
Data Collection
of conceptual change methods separately or in
combination?
A questionnaire with 10 two-tier questions (see
4. Do conceptual change methods enhance students’
‘‘Appendix 1’’) was used in the study to identify stu-
retention of their newly developed conceptions?
dents’ conceptions of ‘sound propagation’. The instru-
ment was developed based on related literature and
students’ reported conceptions. For example, interview
Methodology questions used by Eshach and Schwartz (2006) were
changed into two-tier questions. To confirm content
The Context of the Study validity, a science educator and a physics educator
examined the questionnaire and confirmed its appropri-
The Turkish education system involves an 8-year basic ateness in relation to student level, readability and
education cycle catering for the 6–14 year-old age group. It understandability. Further, the questionnaire was pilot-
is compulsory and free in public schools. This system aims tested with twenty-six grade 6 students taught the same
to provide children with basic knowledge and improve topic in the previous year. The alpha reliability

123
732 J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

Table 1 Characteristics of the questionnaire imported into SPSS 13.0. Results of the pre-, post- and
Objective(s) Number of item
delayed post-test tests were compared using a Repeated
Measures Design and One-Way ANOVA. Repeated
To compare sound propagation at phase Item 1 and Item 6 measures designs allow researchers to monitor how the
of solid with that of gases
students’ behaviours or conceptions change over time in
To compare sound propagation at phase Item 2 and Item 7 the much shorter-term case of the teaching intervention.
of liquid with that of gases
In this Repeated Measures Design, the independent vari-
To compare sound propagation at phase Item 3
of gases with that of space (vacuum) able is the teaching intervention, and the measurement of
medium the dependent variable as pre-, post- and delayed post-test
To compare sound propagation at phase Item 9 scores—acting as repeated measures—is the dependent
of solid with that of liquid variable. Furthermore, as a Repeated Measure Design
To investigate whether sound can travel Item 4 and Item 8 may not test the effect of each teaching intervention on
through vacuum/space the students’ conceptions in pre-, post- and delayed post-
To comprehend where sound propagation Item 5 and Item 10 tests. At that point a one-way ANOVA was employed to
occurs rapidly (solid–liquid-gases-
vacuum/space)
test for differences among pre-, post- and delayed post-
test scores within each experimental group and the control
group.

coefficient (KR20) for the pilot study was 0.75, whilst


that for the main study was 0.76. The instrument was Teaching Design
administered as a pre-test a week before the intervention,
and the same test was re-administered as a post-test Each teaching session lasted a two class period (80 min)
immediately after the intervention. Also, the same test (see Appendixes 1, 2, 3, 4 and Figs. 1, 2, 3, 4, 5). The
was employed as a delayed post-test 3 weeks after teaching design for each group is outlined in Table 2.
intervention (see Table 1 for characteristics of the ques- Even though some analogical teaching strategies such as
tionnaire). Since official language in each schooling stage bridging analogy (Brown and Clement 1989), teaching with
in Turkey is Turkish, the instrument and teaching design analogies (Glynn 1989), General Model of Analogy
were administered in Turkish. Then, they were translated Teaching (Zeitoun 1984) and FAR Guide (Treagust et al.
into English by the first two researchers of the current 1998) have generally been used in science education liter-
study who are bilingual. Later, a native English speaker ature, we preferred the FAR Guide to improve the related
and science educator who are also third author of the analogies due to the following reasons. Firstly, the FAR
current study examined them for grammar and clarity. Guide directly enables students to map elements of the
source and target analogues (see Appendix 4 and Table 2).
For example, the following question was planned to
Data Analysis

Before grading the student responses, two of the authors,


who are bilingual, examined draft data superficially and
thematically and created the following criteria: Correct
Choice with Sound Understanding (CSU) (8 points), No
Choice with Sound Understanding (NSU) (7 points),
Incorrect Choice with Sound Understanding (ISU) (6
points), Correct Choice with Alternative Conception
(CAC) (5 points), Correct Choice (CC) (4 points), No
Choice with Alternative Conception (NAC) (3 points),
Incorrect Choice with Alternative Conception (IAC) (2
points), Incorrect Choice (IC) (1 point) and No response
or Irrelevant Responses (NR) (zero point). Then, two of
the authors labelled the student responses separately.
Inter-rater consistency was very high, approximately 90%,
for most items. All disagreements were resolved by
negotiation. Finally, each student score was computed and Fig. 1 Cargo animation for Group A

123
J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742 733

Fig. 2 Cargo animation for Group B Fig. 5 A comparison animation of all cases

stimulate this mapping i.e. ‘Now please compare each case


with each other by taking into account ‘sound propagation’
concept’. Secondly, the FAR Guide not only helps the
teachers consider less operations or steps during the lesson
but also enhances the likelihood that teachers will recognize
and relinquish unsuccessful analogies (Treagust et al.
1998).

Results

From the pre-test scores, a minority of the students’


responses for all items, except for item 4 (60%) and item
8 (25%) in E3, in both the experimental and control
groups fell into CSU category whilst almost none of them
Fig. 3 Cargo animation for Group C
was considered NSU or ISU. Also, a larger proportion of
the students’ responses to the pre-test for the remaining
items were in the CAC or IAC or CC category in regard
to type of group. In the post-test, percentages of the
students’ responses categorized under CSU were virtually
the same for the CG and E1 groups for item 2 (10%),
item 6 (15%), item 7 (20% for the CG and 15% for the
E1), item 8 (20%), item 9 (20%) and item 10 (20% for
the CG and 25% for the E1) whilst most of the students’
responses in the experimental groups fell into this cate-
gory for item 1 (60% for the E1, 85% for the E2, and
95% for the E3), item 3 (75% for the E1, 90% for the
E2, and 70% for the E3) and item 5 (60% for the
E1, 85% for the E2 and the E3). Moreover, a larger
proportion of the students’ responses in the E2 and E3
groups were classified under CSU after the teaching
intervention. In the delayed post-test, majority of the
students’ responses in the E2 and E3 groups were cate-
Fig. 4 Cargo animation for case of ‘No Student’ gorized as CSU. Interestingly, there was an increase in

123
734 J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

Table 2 An outline of the teaching design in each group


Type of group Teacher’s role Students’ role

Control group The teacher asked some curious questions, i.e. how does The students answered these questions using their pre-
sound propagate? Is there any medium where sound existing knowledge
propagation does not take place? to activate the students’ The students carried out hands-on activities such as hitting
pre-existing knowledge two rocks with each other in air medium and in water
Teacher got the students to engage in hands-on activities medium and then listening their sounds, hitting their
such as hitting two rocks with each other in air and in hands to the table and listening its sound, and using
water and then listening their sounds, hitting their hands stethoscope to listen sounds of their hearts. They
to the table and listening its sound, and using stethoscope explained what they observed as a whole-class discussion
to listen sounds of their hearts They took notes scientific explanations given by the teacher
After the students articulated their observations, the teacher
actively presented scientific explanations and required the
students to take notes
Experiment 1 (with The teacher got the students to watch some clips from The students answered these questions using their pre-
conceptual change Starwars film where related heroes can hear sounds of existing knowledge
text) explosions, collisions, space shuttle’s engine at space. The students read conceptual change text (see Appendix 2)
Then, the teacher asked the students about their pre- carefully through 10 min
existing ideas using prompt questions ‘where does sound
propagate rapidly (solid–liquid-gases-vacuum/space)? The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
Can sound travel through space (vacuum)? alternative conceptions as a whole-class discussion
The teacher handed conceptual change text out (see Some of the students attempted to persuade their peers to
Appendix 2) and gave 10 min to read it change their alternative conceptions towards scientific
one
The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
alternative conceptions as a whole-class discussion The students transferred their newly structured knowledge
to different questions
The teacher persuaded the students to change their
alternative conceptions towards scientific one
The teacher asked the students to transfer their newly
structured knowledge to different questions such as ‘how
do astronauts communicate with each other in space’,
‘When we act up at home, our neighbours generally bump
radiators to warn us. Could you explain this by means of
sound propagation?’
Experiment 2 (with The teacher asked the students about their pre-existing ideas The students answered these questions using their pre-
analogy and using prompt questions ‘Does a man learn train’s moving existing knowledge
animation) without seeing it? Please explain your reason’, ‘What The students conducted cargo analogy activity (see
does idiom ‘Many hands make light works’ mean?’ Appendix 3) and examined animations converted from
The teacher handed out worksheet incorporating in the cargo analogy to capture ‘sound propagation’ in cases
directions of cargo analogy activity (see Appendix 3) and of solid, liquid, gases and vacuum (space) (see Figs. 1, 2,
asked them to conduct it by tracking the given directions 3, 4, 5)
The teachers allowed the students to examine animations The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
converted from the cargo analogy activity (see Figs. 1, 2, generated knowledge domains as a whole-class discussion
3, 4, 5) The students watched some clips from Starwars film and
In this process, the teacher sought to clarify parts of the addressed what was wrong in the film
analogy students did not understand, but refrained from The students transferred their newly structured knowledge
giving any clues to the answers to different questions
The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
generated knowledge domains as a whole-class discussion
The teacher got the students to watch some clips from
Starwars where related heroes can hear sounds of
explosions, collisions, space shuttle’s engine at space.
Then, the teacher created an interactive discussion
environment on what was wrong in Starwars film
The teacher confirmed/refuted students’ generated
knowledge domain and presented scientific explanation
The teacher argued where analogy broke down by help of
analogical mapping (shared and unshared domains
between analogue and target) (see Appendix 4)
The teacher asked the students to transfer their newly
structured knowledge to different questions such as ‘how
do astronauts communicate with each other in space’,
‘When we act up at home, our neighbours generally bump
radiators to warn us. Could you explain this by means of
sound propagation?

123
J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742 735

Table 2 continued
Type of group Teacher’s role Students’ role

Experiment 3 (with a The teacher asked the students about their pre-existing ideas The students answered these questions using their pre-
combination of using prompt questions ‘Does a man learn train’s moving existing knowledge
conceptual change without seeing it? Please explain your reason’, ‘What The students conducted cargo analogy activity (See
text, analogy and does idiom ‘Many hands make light works’ mean?’ Appendix 3) and examined animations converted from
animations) The teacher handed out worksheet incorporating in the cargo analogy to capture ‘sound propagation’ in cases
directions of cargo analogy activity and asked them of solid, liquid, gases and vacuum (space) (see Figs. 1, 2,
to conduct it by tracking the given directions 3, 4, 5)
(See Appendix 3) The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
The teachers allowed the students to examine animations generated knowledge domains as a whole-class discussion
converted from the foregoing analogy (see Figs. 1, 2, 3, 4, The students watched some clips from Starwars film and
5). In this process, the teacher sought to clarify parts of addressed what was wrong in the film
the analogy students did not understand, but refrained
from giving any clues to the answers The students read conceptual change text carefully through
10 min
The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
generated knowledge domains as a whole-class discussion The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
alternative conceptions as a whole-class discussion
The teacher got the students to watch some clips from
Starwars film where related heroes can hear sounds of Some of the students attempted to persuade their peers to
explosions, collisions, space shuttle’s engine at space. change their alternative conceptions towards scientific
Then, the teacher created an interactive discussion one
environment on what was wrong in Starwars film The students transferred their newly structured knowledge
The teacher confirmed/disconfirmed students’ generated to different questions
knowledge domain and presented scientific explanation
The teacher argued where analogy broke down by help of
analogical mapping (shared and unshared domains
between analogue and target) (see Appendix 4)
Then, the teacher asked the students some questions ‘where
does sound propagate rapidly (solid–liquid-gases-
vacuum/space)? Can sound travel through space
(vacuum)?
The teacher handed conceptual change text out and gave
10 min to read it
The teacher and the students interactively discussed all
alternative conceptions as a whole-class discussion
The teacher persuaded the students to change their
alternative conceptions towards scientific one
The teacher asked the students to transfer their newly
structured knowledge to different questions such as ‘how
do astronauts communicate with each other in space’,
‘When we act up at home, our neighbours generally bump
radiators to warn us. Could you explain this by means of
sound propagation?

the students’ labelled responses as CSU over time for Results of one-way ANOVA showed significant dif-
item 1 (30%), item 2 (15%), item 4 (25%), and item 5 ferences amongst pre-, post- and delayed post-test for CG
(40%) in CG, and for item 2 (20%), item 4 (50%) and (Sum of Squares: 2166,433; df: 2; Mean Square:
item 5 (65%) in E1, and item 3 (90%) and item 4 (95%) 1083,217; F: 4,309; p \ 0.05), E1 (Sum of Squares:
in E3 whilst there was as expected a decrease for rest of 7147,233; df: 2; Mean Square: 3573,617; F: 25,649;
the items. p \ 0.05), E2 (Sum of Squares: 14764,233; df: 2; Mean
As indicated by Table 3, there were significant differ- Square: 7382,117; F: 72,467; p \ 0.05) and E3 (Sum
ences amongst pre-, post- and delayed post-test scores of Squares: 19182,033; df: 2; Mean Square: 9591,017;
(p \ 0.05) and amongst groups and factor (p \ 0.05). F: 90,949; p \ 0.05).
As can be seen from Table 4, there were significant As indicated in Table 5, there is a statistically significant
difference between CG and E2 (p \ 0.05), and between difference between the pre-test and post-test in each group,
CG and E3 (p \ 0.05) in favour of E2 and E3 whilst there and between the pre-test and delayed post-test scores
was no statistical difference between CG and E1 (p \ 0.05) in each experimental group in favour of the
(p [ 0.05). post-test and delayed post-test scores. However, there are

123
736 J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

Table 3 Results of repeated


Effect Value F Sig.
measure design
Factor (pre-, post- and delayed post-tests) Pillai’s Traca .844 203,329a .000
Wilks’ Lambda .156 203,329a .000
Hotelling’s Trace 5,422 203,329a .000
Roy’s Largest Root 5,422 203,329a .000
Factor (pre-, post- and delayed post-tests) * groups Pillai’s Traca .493 8,283 .000
a a
Exact statistic Wilks’ Lambda .547 8,803 .000
b
The statistic is an upper Hotelling’s Trace .756 9,319 .000
bound on F that yields a lower Roy’s Largest Root .642 16,276b .000
bound on the significance level

Table 4 Results of multiple comparisons (post-hoc tests) Table 5 Results of multiple comparisons for CG, E1, E2, and E3
(I) Group (J) Groups Mean Difference (I-J) Std. error Sig. Group (I) Tests (J) Tests Mean Std. Sig.
difference error
CG E1 -7,2000 3,24251 .127 (I-J)
E2 -21,7833 3,24251 .000
CG Pre-Test Post-Test -14,450* 5,013 .015
E3 -27,0333 3,24251 .000
Delayed Test -9,650 5,013 .141
E1 CG 7,2000 3,24251 .127
Post-Test Pre-Test 14,450* 5,013 .015
E2 -14,5833 3,24251 .000
Delayed Test 4,800 5,013 .607
E3 -19,8333 3,24251 .000
Delayed Test Pre-Test 9,650 5,013 .141
E2 CG 21,7833 3,24251 .000
Post-Test -4,800 5,013 .607
E1 14,5833 3,24251 .000
E1 Pre-Test Post-Test -23,350* 3,732 .000
E3 -5,2500 3,24251 .374
Delayed Test -22,950* 3,732 .000
E3 CG 27,0333 3,24251 .000
Post-Test Pre-Test 23,350* 3,732 .000
E1 19,8333 3,24251 .000
Delayed Test .400 3,732 .994
E2 5,2500 3,24251 .374
Delayed Test Pre-Test 22,950* 3,732 .000
Post-Test -.400 3,732 .994
E2 Pre-Test Post-Test -35,850* 3,191 .000
no statistically significant differences between the post-test Delayed Test -29,900* 3,191 .000
and delayed post-test scores (p [ 0.05) for all groups and Post-Test Pre-Test 35,850* 3,191 .000
between the pre-test and delayed post-test scores in the Delayed Test 5,950 3,191 .158
control group (p [ 0.05). Delayed Test Pre-Test 29,900* 3,191 .000
Post-Test -5,950 3,191 .158
E3 Pre-Test Post-Test -39,450* 3,247 .000
Discussion Delayed Test -36,200* 3,247 .000
Post-Test Pre-Test 39,450* 3,247 .000
After a teaching intervention, it is expected that stu- Delayed Test 3,250 3,247 .579
dents will progress in terms of their conceptual under- Delayed Test Pre-Test 36,200* 3,247 .000
standing of the investigated concepts (Demircioğlu Post-Test -3,250 3,247 .579
et al. 2009; Taber 2001; Teichert and Stacy 2002). In * The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level
the present study the post-test scores indicated that all
groups improved their conceptual understanding. How- post-test scores (see Table 5). Despite the new Turkish
ever, it appeared that for the control group instruction science curricula being based on constructivism, tea-
only temporarily helped the students to improve their cher-centred teaching (didactic teaching) is still very
conceptual understanding and students returned their common (Çalık 2011; Kaya 2008; Widodo et al. 2002)
initial conceptions over time (Taber 2001; Teichert and and many teachers do not implement the new curricula
Stacy 2002). That is, for the control group there was appropriately (Aubusson et al. 1998; Widodo et al.
no significant difference between pre-test and delayed 2002).

123
J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742 737

As seen in Table 4, there was no significant difference cases appeared in the CG and E1 but rarely in E3. This
between the control group and the experimental group suggests that these teaching interventions created a dis-
exposed to a conceptual change text. This indicates that equilibrium environment for students and helped them to
the conceptual change text was less effective in remedi- re-examine their knowledge to reach equilibrium, possibly
ating alternative conceptions or enhancing conceptual stemming from the teaching of the related topics ‘Can
understanding than were analogies/animations or a com- sound propagation be prevented?’ and ‘Record voice’
bination of analogy, animations and the conceptual after the teaching intervention. This may also come from
change text. This claim is inconsistent with related liter- structure of the teaching intervention which is not a
ature which claims that the conceptual change texts are unique element for students’ conceptual understanding.
efficient in moving students’ alternative conceptions That is, this learning process is very intricate and contains
towards scientific ones (e.g. Chambers and Andre 1997; many factors such as environment, family, motivation,
Çalık et al. 2007). This may have resulted from the culture, etc. Therefore, we have little chance to bring
manner and care with which students read the text. Stu- about conceptual change unless a collaborative environ-
dents may also find these texts boring and which would ment is generated.
impede their effectiveness (Dole 2000; Huddle et al.
2000). Furthermore, conceptual change texts may not be
as effective as first-hand experience or engagement Conclusions and Implications for Practice
(Chambers and Andre 1997).
Analogies and animations appeared to allow the The study demonstrated that: (1) all groups showed some
students to construct better scientific explanations than progress in their conceptual understanding after the
did the conceptual change text. Even though the con- teaching (2) instruction with the control group only tem-
ceptual change text challenged the students’ alternative porarily helped the students to improve their conceptual
conceptions directly, it was not as effective as analogies understanding and students returned their initial concep-
and animations at improving their conceptual under- tions over time (3) using a conceptual change text was
standing (see Table 4). This may be because analogies less effective in remediating alternative conceptions and
and animations make the unfamiliar familiar. The fact enhancing conceptual understanding than was using anal-
that the students see familiar features may have ogies/animations or a combination of analogy, animations
increased their enthusiasms and eliminated their preju- and conceptual change text (4) a combination of different
dices (e.g. Harrison and Coll 2007; Huddle et al. 2000; conceptual change methods was the most effective in
Tasker and Dalton 2006). Furthermore, this may be eliminating the students’ alternative conceptions (5) each
because analogies and animations provide more realistic conceptual change method and the combination used here
experiences than the conceptual change (Chambers and resulted in conceptual change being stored in the students’
Andre 1997). long term memory.
As seen in Table 4, a combination of different con- Even though primary teacher training programs
ceptual change methods was the most effective in pro- incorporate some courses on science and technology,
ducing conceptual change. This may be because students i.e. ‘General Biology-Chemistry-Physics’, ‘Science and
have multiple learning styles (e.g. Brinda 2004; Harvey Technology Laboratory I–II’, ‘Science and Technology
and Hodges 1999). Since there was no statistically sig- Teaching I–II’, all subject matter knowledge is concisely
nificant difference between the post- and delayed post-test introduced because of its multidisciplinary feature. Con-
scores in the experimental groups, it seems likely that sequently, science and technology courses in grade 4 and
each conceptual change method and their combination 5 should be taught by science teachers. In fact, English
resulted in conceptual change stored in the students’ teachers, visual arts teachers and physical education
long term memory (e.g. Çalık et al. 2010a; Tsai 1999). teachers teach their own courses in grades 4 and 5, but
Moreover, although there was a decrease in percentages this is not the case with Science and Technology.
of some of the students’ conceptual understanding as Meanwhile, since a combination of different conceptual
might be expected, there were also some increases in their change methods appeared to result in better student
conceptual understanding in delayed post-test. This means achievement, this claim should be tested in other disci-
that even after the teaching intervention, construction and plines with other conceptual change methods, and age
reconstruction of knowledge may have continued. Such groups.

123
738 J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

Appendix 1: Some Examples of Items


in the Questionnaire

Item 1. A man is drilling a hole in the middle of a big empty park. Assume you are standing
far enough away so that you can barely hear the drilling noise. If you put your ear to the
ground, will you hear the drilling
a) better b) worse c) the same d) never
Because…………………………………………………………………………………
Item 2. A boat’s motor can be in or out of the water. Assume that you are in the sea, far
from a boat and that the boat’s motor is in the water. Assume you can hardly hear it when
your head is out of the water. Now, assume that you put your head inside the water (that is,
your ears are under water). Would you be able to hear the boat
a) better b) worse c) the same d) never
Because…………………………………………………………………………………
Item 3. Assume that there is an explosion in sun. How would you be able to hear the
explosion in sun from the world?
a) better b) worse c) the same d) never
Because…………………………………………………………………………………
Item 4. A ringing alarm clock is suspended in the middle of a vacuum bell
as seen from left figure in hand. The clock is not touching the bell’s walls.
Would we still hear the ringing clock if we take the air out of the vacuum
bell?
a) better b) worse c) the same d) never
Because…………………………………………………………………………………
Item 5. Which of the following medium does sound propagate rapidly?
a) Sea water b) Steel oven c) Air d) Space
Because…………………………………………………………………………………………

Appendix 2: Conceptual Change Text

Where does sound propagate rapidly (solid-liquid-gases-

vacuum/space)?

Can sound travel through space (vacuum)?

Mehmet enrolled in Söğütlü Primary School did his falling into sleep, he heart sounds of slippers but everybody
assignments carefully at weekend and looked over pages of was sleeping and all lamps had turned off. He tried to find
scientific news at a well-known newspaper. During this source of sound and acquired that this sound was coming
reading, he noticed interesting news of dolphins which from upstairs neighbours. Even though Mehmet did not hear
propagates sound waves to identify place, distance and size their speaking very well, he was able to hear sounds of their
of their hunts. Further, they can communicate with each slippers. In the morning, as soon as Mehmet went to the
other using sound waves. It is the first time that Mehmet have school, he shared these experiences with his teacher. His
just perceived sound propagation in liquid medium since he teacher stated that sound cannot travel through space (vac-
assumed that sound could not travel through liquid medium. uum) since there is no particle. In other words, sound cannot
Then, he looked for an interesting film in TV Schedule in the propagate without particles, unfortunately, film directors
newspaper and decided to watch ‘Starwars’ because Mehmet generally disregard these features to enhance enthusiasms of
likes space films. When Mehmet was watching the Starwars audience. Also, the teacher pointed out that in the case of the
film, he was impacted from sounds of explosions, collisions, sounds of slippers, the heart sounds of slippers moving on
fighting in space. When Mehmet went to his bed, he was solid medium while he did not hear sounds of upstairs
suddenly confused with a question ‘since there is no particle neighbours talking into gases medium. This means that phase
in space, how did astronauts hear sounds of explosions, of matter influences sound propagation. The more particles
collisions, space shuttle’s engine at space’. When he was are located side by side, the more sound propagation occurs

123
J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742 739

rapidly. Similarly, sounds of dolphins illustrate how to 13) Please explain what happened the number of the
happen sound propagation in liquid medium. Finally, his moved textbooks on two desks where there was no
teacher implied that since distance between particles in student? Defend your response
phases of matter is differ from each other, sound propagation 14) Do you have any idea about what each case meet in
in solid medium is the best in all phases. Then, liquid medium science? Defend your response
and gases medium are lined up consecutively. Because there
is no particle in space (vacuum) medium, no sound propa-
gation takes place in space (vacuum).
Appendix 4: Analogical Mapping of the Cargo Analogy
and Sound Propagation in Different Cases

Appendix 3: Cargo Analogy Activity Analogue Feature Comparison Target Feature

1) Please divide into three groups (Group A with 10 Moving textbooks hand by Compared Sound propagation
hand from one desk to with
students, Group B with 5 students and Group C with another one
2 students) to visualize the cargo analogy activity Slowly vibrations of the Compared Vibrations of solid
2) Locate two desks in mid of the classroom for each students in Group A with particles
student group—distance between desks should be the Student walking step by Compared Behaviours of liquid
same step in Group B with particles
3) Line up in order of side by side for each student Student walking step by Compared Behaviours of gases
group step faster than do with particles
4) Put twenty textbooks on one of the desks Group B
5) You should move them from one desk to the other No moving textbooks Compared Case of space or vacuum
hand by hand from one with where there is no
within 20 s when time commences. desk to another one particles transferring
6) In each group, the students move all textbooks hand where there is no student sound propagation
by hand The number of student in Compared Particles of solid matter
7) In Group A, during moving the textbooks, the students Group A with where they are located
should also vibrate slowly and record how many side by side
textbooks transferred from one desk to another? The number of student in Compared Particles of liquid matter
8) In Group B, during moving the textbooks, the students Group B with where the distance
between particles is
should walk step by step and record how many higher than that of solid
textbooks transferred from one desk to another? matter
9) In Group C, during moving the textbooks, the The number of student in Compared Particles of gases matter
students should walk step by step faster than do Group C with where the distance
Group B and record how many textbooks transferred between particles is the
highest value in phases of
from one desk to another? matter
10) Please locate again two desks where there is no One desk on the textbooks Compared Source of sound
student and record how many textbooks transferred with
from one desk to another? Empty desk Compared Receiver of sound
11) Please fill in the following chart in regard to the with
number of textbooks you moved Student number per Compared Particles number per
volume with volume (particle density).
That is, even though the
Case Second The number of number of particles in
textbooks you moved phases of matter is the
same, particle density
Group A 20 changes in regard to
Group B 20 phase of matter.
Group C 20 Student Does not Particles because we
compare cannot see them by naked
Two desks where there 20
to eyes
is no student
Moving textbooks hand by Does not Sound propagation because
hand from one desk to compare it is more complex than
12) Now please compare each case with each other by another one to the cargo analogy the
students played
taking into account ‘sound propagation’ concept

123
740 J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

continued Çalık M, Kolomuç A, Karagölge Z (2010b) The effect of conceptual


change pedagogy on students’ conceptions of rate of reaction.
Analogue Feature Comparison Target Feature J Sci Educ Technol 19:422–433
Çepni S (2009) Effects of computer supported instructional material
Same distance between Does not Distance between particles (CSIM) in removing students misconceptions about concepts:
desks compare in phases of matter ‘‘Light, light source and seeing’’. Energy Educ Sci Technol Part
to because it changes in B Soc Educ Stud 1:51–83
regard to phase of matter Chambers SK, Andre T (1997) Gender, prior knowledge, interest, and
Student walking step by Does not Behaviours of liquid and experience in electricity and conceptual change text manipula-
step in Group B and C compare gases particles because tions in learning about direct current. J Res Sci Teach
to they also have different 34(2):107–123
motion, i.e. spin and Chambliss MJ (2001) Analyzing science textbook materials to
rotation, in regard to determine how ‘‘persuasive’’ they are. Theory Prac 40(4):255–
phase of matter 264
A decrease in the number Does not The number of particles Chiu MH, Lin JW (2002) Using multiple analogies for investigating
of students in cases of compare because the number of fourth graders’ conceptual change in electricity. Chin J Res Sci
solid, liquid, gases and to particles is the same in Educ 10:109–134
space (vacuum), phases of matter number. Choi K, Chang H (2004) The effects of using the electric circuit
respectively But the distance between model in science education to facilitate learning electricity-
them and particle density related concepts. Journal of the Korean Physical Society
changes in regard to 44(6):1341–1348
phase of matter. Clement J (1983) A conceptual model discussed by Galileo and used
intuitively by physics students. In: Gentner D, Stevens AL (eds)
Mental models. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 325–340
Clement J (1987) The use of analogies and anchoring intuitions to
remediate misconceptions in mechanics. Paper presented at the
References annual meeting of the American Educational Research Associ-
ation, Washington, DC
Ametller J, Pinto R (2002) Students’ reading of innovative images of Coll RK, France B, Taylor I (2005) The role of models/and analogies
energy at secondary school level. Int J Sci Educ 24(3):285–312 in science education: implications from research. Int J Sci Educ
Aubusson P, Watson K, Brown G (1998) Enhancing lower secondary 27(2):183–198
science. University Western Sydney, Penrith, NSW Coombs EC (2007). Investigating student understanding of sound as a
Barman CR, Barman NS, Miller JA (1996) Two teaching methods and longitudinal wave. Unpublished Master Thesis, The University
students’ understanding of sound. School Sci Math 96(2):63–67 of Maine, Orono, ME
Baviskar SN, Hartle RT, Whitney T (2009) Essential criteria to Dagher ZR (1995) Analysis of analogies used by science teachers.
characterize constructivist teaching: derived from a review of the J Res Sci Teach 32(3):259–270
literature and applied to five constructivist-teaching method Demirci N, Çirkinoğlu A (2004) Determining students’ preconcep-
articles. Int J Sci Educ 31(4):541–550 tions/misconceptions in electricity and magnetism concepts.
Beaty WJ (2001). Children’s misconceptions about science. Retrieved J Turkish Sci Educ 1(2):116–138
13 May 2008 from http://www.amasci.com/miscon/opphys.html Demircioğlu H, Demircioğlu G, Çalık M (2009) Investigating
Brinda T (2004) Integration of new exercise classes into the effectiveness of the storylines embedded within context based
informatics education in the field of object-oriented modelling. learning: a case for the periodic table. Chem Educ Res Prac
Educ Info Technol 9(2):117–130 10:241–249
Brown DE (1993) Refocusing core intuitions: a concretizing role for Dole JA (2000) Readers, texts and conceptual change learning. Read
analogy in conceptual change. J Res Sci Teach 30:1273–1290 Writ Q 16:99–118
Brown DE, Clement J (1989) Overcoming misconceptions by Duit R (1991) On the role of analogies and metaphors in learning
analogical reasoning: abstract transfer versus explanatory model science. Sci Educ 75(6):649–672
construction. Instr Sci 18:237–261 Duit R (1994) Research on students’ conceptions-developments and
Burke KA, Greenbowe TJ, Windschitl MA (1998) Developing and trends. In: Pfundt H, Duit R (eds) Bibliography: students’
using conceptual computer animations for chemistry instruction. alternative frameworks and science education, 3rd edn. Univer-
J Chem Educ 75(12):1658–1661 sity of Kiel, Kiel, Germany, pp xxii–xlii
Çalık M (2011) How did creating a constructivist learning environ- Eshach H, Schwartz JL (2006) Sound stuff? Naive materialism in
ment influence my graduate students’ views? Energy Educ Sci middle-school students’conceptions of sound. Int J Sci Educ
Technol Part B Soc Educ Stud 3(1):1–13 28:733–764
Çalık M, Ayas A (2005) A comparison of level of understanding of Fensham PJ (1992) Science and technology. In: Jackson PW (ed)
grade 8 students and science student teachers related to selected Handbook of research on curriculum. Macmillan, New York,
chemistry concepts. J Res Sci Teach 42(6):638–667 pp 789–829
Çalık M, Ayas A, Coll RK (2007) Enhancing pre-service primary Fensham PJ, Gunstone RF, White RT (1994) The content of science:
teachers’ conceptual understanding of solution chemistry with a constructivist approach to its teaching and learning. Falmer
conceptual change text. Int J Sci Math Educ 5(1):1–28 Press, London
Çalık M, Ayas A, Ebenezer JV (2009) Analogical reasoning for Frederik I, Van Der Valk T, Leite L, Thorén I (1999) Pre-service
understanding solution rates: students’ conceptual change and physics teachers and conceptual difficulties on temperature and
chemical explanations. Res Sci Technol Educ 27(3):283–308 heat. Eur J Teach Educ 22(1):61–74
Çalık M, Ayas A, Coll RK (2010a) Investigating the effectiveness of Glynn SM (1989) The teaching with analogies model: explaining
usage of different methods embedded with four-step construc- concepts in expository texts. In: Muth KD (ed) Children’s
tivist teaching strategy. J Sci Educ Technol 19(1):32–48 comprehension of narrative and expository text: research into

123
J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742 741

practice. International Reading Association, Newark, DE, Niaz M (2008) Whither constructivism?—A chemistry teachers’
pp 185–204 perspective. Teach Teach Educ 24(2):400–416
Gökdere M, Çalık M (2010) A cross-age study of Turkish students’ Oliva MJ (2003) The structural coherence of students’ conceptions in
mental models: an ‘‘Atom’’ concept. Didactica Slovenica- mechanics and conceptual change. Int J Sci Educ 25(5):539–561
Pedagoska Obzorja 25(2):185–199 Özsevgeç T (2006) Determining effectiveness of student guiding
Guzzetti BJ, Williams WO, Skeels SA, Wu SM (1997) Influence of material based on the 5E model in ‘‘force and motion’’ unit.
text structure on learning counterintuitive physics concepts. J Turkish Sci Educ 3:121–134
J Res Sci Teach 34(7):701–719 Özsevgeç T (2010) Computer literacy of Turkish preservice teachers
Hapkiewicz A (1992) Finding a list of science misconceptions. in different teacher training programs. Asia-Pacific Education
MSTA Newslett 38:11–14 Review. Published Online First at http://www.springerlink.
Harrison AG, Coll RK (2007) Analogies for science teachers. Corwin, com/content/g1451036k42h520v/fulltext.pdf
Thousand Oaks, CA Palmer DH (2003) Investigating the relationship between refutational
Harvey LC, Hodges LC (1999) The role of multiple teaching text and conceptual change. Sci Educ 87:663–684
strategies in promoting active learning in organic chemistry. Periago C, Pejuan A, Jaén X, Bohigas X (2009) Misconceptions about
Chem Educ 4(3):89–93 the propagation of sound waves. Paper presented at European
Havu-Nuutinen S (2007) Young children’s conceptions of tempera- association for education in electrical and information engineer-
ture and thermometer. Int J Learn 14(9):93–101 ing annual conference, Universitat Politècnica de València,
Hırça N, Çalık M, Akdeniz F (2008) Investigating grade 8 students’ València
conceptions of ‘energy’ and related concepts. J Turkish Sci Educ Robson C (1998) Real word research. Blackwell Publishers Ltd.,
5(1):76–89 Oxford, UK
Hrepic Z (1998) Students’ conceptions in understanding of sound. Rowlands S, Graham T, Berry J, McWilliams P (2007) Conceptual
Unpublished Bachelor’s Thesis, University of Split, Croatia change through the lens of Newtonian mechanics. Sci Educ
Hrepic Z (2002) Identifying students’ mental models of sound 16:21–42
propagation. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Kansas State Univer- Russell JW, Kozma RB, Jones T, Wykoff J, Marx N, Davis J (1997)
sity, Manhattan Use of simultaneous-synchronized macroscopic, microscopic,
Hrepic Z (2004) Development of a real-time assessment of students’ and symbolic representations to enhance the teaching and
mental models of sound propagation. Unpublished Doctoral learning of chemical concepts. J Chem Educ 74:330–334
dissertation, Kansas State University, Manhattan Sağlam M (2006) An investigation of guide material development and
Hrepic Z, Zollman D, Rebello S (2002) Identifying students models its affectiveness according to 5E model for the sound and light
of sound propagation. In: Franklin S, Marx J, Cummings K (eds) unit. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Karadeniz Technical
Proceedings of 2002 physics education research conference. University, Trabzon
PERC Publishing, Boise, Idaho Şahin Ç, Çalık M, Çepni S (2009) Using different conceptual change
Huddle PA, White MW, Rogers F (2000) Simulations for teaching methods embedded within 5E model: a sample teaching of liquid
chemical equilibrium. J Chem Educ 77(7):920–926 pressure. Energy Educ Sci Technol Part B Soc Educ Stud
Hynd C (2001) Persuasion and its role in meeting educational goals. 1(3):115–125
Theory Prac 40(4):270–277 Salgut B (2007) The effects of computer assisted instruction along
Jones MG, Rua MJ, Carter G (1998) Science teachers’ conceptual with internet for 5th grade primary school students acquisition in
growth within vygotsky’s zone of proximal development. J Res science and tecnology lessons light and voice unit. Unpublished
Sci Teach 35(9):967–985 Master Thesis, Çukurova University, Adana
Kaya ON (2008) A student-centered approach: assessing the changes Sanger MJ, Greenbowe TJ (1997) Student misconceptions in
in prospective science teachers’ conceptual understanding by electrochemistry: current flow in electrolyte solutions and the
concept mapping in a general chemistry laboratory. Res Sci Educ salt bridge. J Chem Educ 74(7):819–823
38:91–110 Scott PH, Asoko HM, Driver RH (1992) Teaching for conceptual
Lautrey J, Mazens K (2004) Is children’s naive knowledge consis- change: a review of strategies. In: Duit R, Goldberg F, Niedderer
tent? A comparison of the concepts of sound and heat. Learn H (eds) Research in physics learning: theoretical issues and
Instr 14(4):399–423 empirical studies. Proceedings of an International Workshop.
Mazens K, Lautrey J (2003) Conceptual change in physics: children’s IPN, Kiel, Germany, pp 310–329
naive representations of sound. Cogn Dev 18(2):159–176 Taber KS (2001) The mismatch between assumed prior knowledge
McColl P (2003) A curriculum design framework for science and the learner’s conceptions: a typology of learning impedi-
education based on the history of science. Unpublished PhD ments. Educ Stud 27(2):159–171
thesis, University of Melbourne, Australia Tasker R, Dalton R (2006) Research into practice: visualisation of the
Menchen KVP, Thompson JR (2004) Students understanding of sound molecular world using animations. Chem Educ Res Prac 7(2):
propagation: research and curriculum development. In: Marx J, 141–159
Heron P, Franklin S (eds) Physics education research conference Taylor N, Coll R (1997) The use of analogy in the teaching of
proceedings. American Institute of Physics, New York, pp 81–84 solubility to pre-service primary teachers. Aust Sci Teach J
Merino MJ (1998) Some difficulties in teaching the properties of 43(4):58–64
sounds. Phys Educ 33(2):101–104 Teichert MA, Stacy AM (2002) Promoting understanding of chem-
Moore T, Harrison A (2004) Floating and sinking: everyday science ical bonding and spontaneity through student explanation
in middle school. Retrieved 20 Jan 2008 from http://www. and integration of ideas. J Res Sci Teach 39(6):464–496
aare.edu.au/04pap/moo04323.pdf Treagust DF, Harrison AG, Venville GJ (1998) Teaching science
Mueller A, Le Clair M, Kechaidis M, Swain W, Macdonald J (2004) effectively with analogies: an approach for pre-service and
Playing with pitch: exploring and investigating the science of in-service teacher education. J Sci Teacher Educ 9(2):
sound. Sci Act 40(4):11–20 85–101
Murphy PK (2001) What makes a text persuasive? Comparing Tsai CC (1999) Overcoming junior high school students’ miscon-
students’ and experts’ conceptions of persuasiveness. Int J Educ ceptions about microscopic views of phase change: a study of an
Res 35(7–8):675–698 analogy activity. J Sci Educ Technol 8:83–91

123
742 J Sci Educ Technol (2011) 20:729–742

Widodo A, Duit R, Müller C (2002) Constructivist views of teaching Yurd M, Olgun ÖS (2008) Effect of problem based learning and
and learning in practice: teachers’ views and classroom behav- know-want-learn strategy to remove misconceptions. Hacettepe
iour. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Univ J Educ 35:386–396
Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), New Zeitoun HH (1984) Teaching scientific analogies: a proposed model.
Orleans Res Sci Technol Educ 2(2):107–127
Wittmann MC, Steinberg RN, Redish EF (2003) Understanding and
addressing student reasoning about sound. Int J Sci Educ
25(8):991–1013

123

You might also like