You are on page 1of 7

ZEBRAFISH

Volume 00, Number 00, 2014 Original Article


ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089/zeb.2014.0977

Effects of the Number of Subjects on the Dark/Light


Preference of Zebrafish (Danio rerio)

Bruno de Matos Mansur,1 Bruno Rodrigues dos Santos,1 Cláudio Alberto Gellis de Mattos Dias,2
Marcelo de Sena Pinheiro,1 and Amauri Gouveia Jr.1

Abstract

This research aims to describe the effects of a variable number of Danio rerio fish subjects, ranging from one to
eight, in the light/dark box preference test. Four hundred eighty adult male short-finned phenotype zebrafish
were tested in the light/dark box. There were four groups in this experiment and a different number of subjects
was used in each group: the control group had only one subject, whereas the experimental groups had either
two, four, or eight subjects simultaneously inside the apparatus in every session. The average occurrence (AO)
of subjects in the white side of the aquarium and the first choice average (FC) were recorded. The AO revealed
no difference between the control group and test groups with two and four subjects. The results for the test
group with eight subjects showed significant difference when compared to the control group and from the test
group with two subjects. The FC also showed no difference between the control group and test groups with two
and four subjects. There was significant variation between the control and the test group with eight subjects. The
results reflect a conflict between the animal’s preference for dark places and the innate drive to explore new
environments. Zebrafish are highly social animals, exhibiting preference for swimming in groups and other
patterns of social cohesion. The reduced white avoidance behavior in the test group of eight subjects may
possibly reflect the role of shoaling, which is a defensive behavior, in reducing anxiety and stress. On the other
hand, the absence of difference between the control group and test groups with two and four subjects suggest
that it is feasible to run the light/dark test with up to four subjects, becoming an alternative to streamline and
simplify data collection and test analysis.

Introduction et al.,8 scototaxis can be used as an indicator of stress, fear,


and anxiety in zebrafish, which can be measured and
Z ebrafish (Danio rerio) originated in the valleys of
India and Bangladesh,1 and has been widely used in
behavioral research due to its small size, ease of use, breed-
exploited through the light/dark box test.

Anxiety and the light/dark test


ing, and keeping. It has also shown potential to be used as a
model in social behavior2 research. The zebrafish is well Tests like the light/dark test are based on novelty paradigms
adapted to life in aquariums.3 Diurnal4 fish live in clean and and are commonly used in behavioral neuroscience to study
clear water in ditches or shallow lakes, and in between veg- fear and anxiety.11 According to McNaughton and Corr,12
etation and rice paddies.5 Consequently, the behavior of anxiety is most often generated by concurrent and equivalent
hiding in dark places to avoid predators is a requirement to activation of fear and risk assessment systems. The particular
survive in these well-lit environments. Scototaxis, which is set of responses depends on the perceived threat.13–16 Thus,
the preference for dark environments as opposed to bright risk assessment behaviors, like thigmotaxis (swimming next to
ones,6 is a natural strategy that promotes the survival of an- walls), stretching posture to observe, and different exploratory
imals in this environment, constituting a particular defensive distribution in threatening and safe situations are seen in situ-
pattern of species that exploit concealment or lack of illu- ations where the risk is low.17 This can be observed in tests like
mination in the environment to avoid predators.7,8 This be- the open field, the elevated plus maze, and the light/dark box.
havior may be an indicator of fear and anxiety in several The light/dark test has gained behavioral validity for some
species, including zebrafish.9,10 According to Maximino fish species (Cardinal-tetras Paracheirodon axelrodi; lambaris

1
Universidade Federal do Pará, Belém, Brazil.
2
Universidade Federal do Amapá, Macapá, Brazil.

1
2 MANSUR ET AL.

Astyanax altiparanae; Nile tilapias Oreochromis niloticus; population dynamics that investigate the size and the group’s
guppies Poecilia reticulata; and banded-knife fish Gymnotus displacement as a whole among the environment. Another
carapo), including zebrafish.6 To establish pharmacological way is to use behavioral models, which investigates the
validity, the light/dark test with the Danio rerio8 species used movement of individuals within the group. This study helps
several drugs, such as lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD),18 to elucidate how light variations play a vital role in computer
acute or chronic fluoxetine, buspirone, benzodiazepines, mo- algorithms/simulations that predict movement.
clobemide, caffeine, ethanol, and dizocilpine.19 Other vari- The factors that contribute to the animal’s decision to initiate
ables, like the effects of the forced exposition to a white a movement include momentary contingencies and variables,
chamber, lightness level (250 and 500 lux),20 effects of alarm such as hunger, exposure to predators and temperature, as well
substance,21 methylmercury,22 and the effects of circadian as durable and morphological variables, which include size,
cycle,23 have also been tested. The results have altogether age, lipid levels, parasites, and genetic predisposition.40–46 The
demonstrated that this method can be useful to assess anxiety- position of each individual in schools, seems to be due to the
like behaviors in Danio rerio, showing consistency in the dark relative position of other group members, in an attraction and
preference behavioral pattern, which is responsive to test repulsion chain, in which an individual is attracted to conspe-
manipulations. Such findings render the model a large face, use cific members, unless they get too close, and if so, turning
and predictive validity as an animal model of anxiety for this away.39
species. Behavioral models are used to gather information about
behavior. Moreover, they need to better detect and point out
the changes in brain and behavior. With the study of the light/
Shoal
dark preference test applied to groups, we expect to verify the
Zebrafish are highly social and prefer swimming in groups. influence of conspecifics on the behavior of isolated indi-
Social cohesion in zebrafish is exhibited through social in- vidual group members. In the absence of significant change in
teractions, shoaling, boldness, aggression, and social prefer- behavior, group tests become an alternative to streamline and
ence.24,25 Being part of a group helps to avoid predators, to simplify data collection and test analysis.
travel efficiently, to acquire food,26 and to reproduce more
successfully.27 According to Spence et al.,4 forming schools Objective
is a natural zebrafish behavior and shoaling initiates subse-
This research aims to describe and analyze the effects of a
quently to the eggs hatching.1 In addition, fish created in
variable number of Danio rerio subjects, ranging from one to
isolation quickly form schools when placed in the company
eight, on the light/dark preference test. To develop a more
of other fish28 and show preference for joining others similar
efficient data collection and to achieve an increased
to the ones with which they were raised, even if they have
throughput test, we will also verify the feasibility of running
different phenotypes, just like in an imprinting phenomenon
the light/dark test with more than one subject.
of social preference acquisition during early development.29
The shoaling behavior increases in threatening situations
Materials and Methods
and in response to a pheromone released as a result of injury
to epidermal cells.1,30–32 This shows that shoaling is relevant Subjects
in predator avoidance. According to Rehnberg and Smith,33
Four hundred eighty adult male short-finned phenotype
in the presence of threat there is greater group cohesion, with
zebrafish were acquired from a local pet shop and only ex-
agitated swimming or subject’s immobilization close to the
perimentally naive fish were used in the experiments. They
substrate. These behaviors increase the individual’s survival
were kept in the laboratory for at least 2 weeks before the
probability since fish schools are more likely to detect
experiments, housed in two collective 30 L tanks (240 sub-
predators than an isolated fish.34
jects per tank), measuring 32 · 34 · 67 cm, with controlled
The shoaling behavior seems to have reinforcing proper-
light exposure (12-h light–12-h dark, starting at 6:30 a.m.).
ties,29 reducing anxiety and stress in fish. This is evident in the
The average pH was 6.8 and average temperature was held
effects of different drugs in behavioral tests. In shoaling tests,
constant at 24C – 2C. The fish were fed once a day with
in which the distance between conspecifics are measured,
chow (Color Tetra Tetra GmbH). The experiment was con-
zebrafish exposed to anxiolytic substances, like LSD18 and
ducted in the Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior at the
alcohol,35 had the distance between subjects altered. Alcohol is
Center of Theory and Research of Behavior, Federal Uni-
a stimulating substance in lower doses and an inhibitor in
versity of Pará and the housing conditions met the APA
higher doses. As expected, alcohol reduced the distance be-
Guidelines for Ethical Conduct in the Care and Use of Ani-
tween subjects in lower doses and increased in higher doses.35
mals. The experimental procedures were approved by the
LSD was proven to enhance inter-fish distance in this test.18
ethics committee of UFPA (CEPAE: 138-13).
Meanwhile, the alarm substance, a substance known for its
anxiogenic and stressor effects, has reduced the distance be-
Test apparatus and general procedure
tween subjects.31 Altogether, these results suggest that shoal-
ing behavior reduces anxiety and stress responses in zebrafish, All the experiments are conducted in rectangular acrylic
since social cohesion is an indicator of anxiety.31 opaque tanks (black/white boxes), half side black and half
Computational simulations in laboratory have been suc- side white, each side measuring 15 · 10 · 45 cm, with water
cessful in forecasting migratory movements in groups and column of 10 cm height. The central compartment where the
schools movements based on temperature, salinity, and other fish are placed measures 15 · 10 · 10 cm and it is delimited by
ecological variables.36–38 According to Romey,39 the study of sliding doors that can be pulled up. The sliding doors have the
schools can be done in two ways. One way is to use models of same color and material as the aquarium, one side is black
NUMBER OF SUBJECTS ALTERS DARK/LIGHT PREFERENCE? 3

and the other white. Two lamps illuminate the apparatuses, Table 1. Subjects Per Session Shows How Many
each of them cautiously positioned one meter above the ex- Subjects Were Included in the Apparatus on Each
tremities of the aquarium to keep a uniform illumination, Session; Subjects Per Group Shows the Total Number
avoiding shadow formation. After each session, the tanks are of Subjects on Each Group and Number of Sessions
rotated 180 to prevent effects of spatial orientation (Fig. 1). Shows the Number of Sessions for Each Group
Subjects Subjects Number
Procedure per session per group of sessions
The first subject is placed in the central compartment of the Ctrl 1 32 32
black/white box for a 5-min period (300 s) of habituation, to 2 64 32
diminish effects of manipulation of the subjects. After this 4 128 32
period the sliding doors are carefully removed at the same 8 256 32
time and the subject can freely explore both compartments of
the tank. The subject’s behavior is recorded with a video
camera during 15 min (900 s). Afterward, the video files were
analyzed and the occurrence of subjects on each side of the (AO) of subjects in each side of the aquarium and the first
aquarium was registered every 15 s, resulting in 60 occur- choice average was calculated and compared between
rences per session. Furthermore, the first side chosen by each groups. Since normality was not assumed for the data, the
subject was also recorded. one-way analysis of variance Kruskal–Wallis test was used
There were four groups in this experiment and a different followed by Dunn’s post test. A Pearson correlation test was
number of subjects was used in each group: the control group performed to determine if measuring the number of occur-
had only one subject inside the apparatus during the test, rences every 15 s is as accurate as measuring the total time of
whereas the experimental groups had 2, 4, or 8 subjects permanence.
placed simultaneously inside the apparatus on every session
(As shown on Table 1). Each group was tested 32 times. To Results
control day-to-day variations, each group had three sessions a Validation of measurement methods
day, totaling 12 sessions per day. All group types were tested
each day. The tests were always performed between 8 and 11 Preliminary descriptive statistics was run to compare the
a.m. and the subjects were not used in more than one test. No average total time spent by subjects in the white side of the
animals were hurt or sacrificed as a result of this experiment. apparatus and the average number of occurrences of subjects
in the white side of the apparatus every 15 s, revealing similar
Measurement methods results (as shown in Table 2 and Fig. 2).
The Pearson correlation test comparing the two analysis
The total time spent by each subject on both sides of the methods was statistically significant, with a p-value of
tank is the main standard variable used to analyze dark/light 0.000000000858. Furthermore, a very strong positive corre-
preference. In this study the number of occurrence of subjects lation coefficient r = 0.968 was found.
on each side of the aquarium was registered every 15 s. To
validate this method, 16 sessions of the control group were Dark preference
analyzed using both techniques (total time and number of
occurrences) for further comparison. After data collection and transcription it was possible to
assess and compare the AO of subjects in the white side of the
Statistical analysis apparatus in each group. The Kruskal–Wallis test revealed
significant variation between groups [H(3) = 18.838,
To verify if the dark/light preference pattern changes with p = 0.001] and the Dunn’s post test showed that the test group
the number of subjects in the tank, the average occurrence with eight subjects differs significantly from the control
group and from the test group with two subjects (Fig. 3).
A linear regression test was conducted to verify whether
the AO of subjects has a linear relationship between groups.
The linear regression test has shown a slightly positive linear
relationship. R = 0.370; R2 = 0.137; Coefficient = 2.753;
Standard Error = 0.615; t = 4.474; P = 0.001 (Fig. 4).

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics Comparing


the Techniques of Registering Behavior
(Total Time and Number of Occurrences)
Standard Standard
Variable n Mean deviation error
Total time 16 17,264 13,478 3369
Number of 16 17,111 14,228 3557
occurrences
FIG. 1. Dark/light box setup.
4 MANSUR ET AL.

FIG. 4. Linear regression line and data points of the av-


erage occurrence of subjects in the white side of the appa-
ratus.

FIG. 2. Descriptive statistics graphic comparing the aver- meanwhile, the test group with eight subjects showed statis-
age percentages of total time spent in the white side and tical differences, when compared with the control group and
average occurrence of subjects in the white side on every 15 s. the test group with two subjects. This indicates that the light/
dark test may be done with up to four subjects in the same tank,
during the same session, without affecting the light/dark
First choice preference behavior of individual subjects.
It was also assessed and compared the average of subjects The white side of the apparatus is an aversive stimulus to
that have first chosen the white side of the apparatus within the fish. Given the significant results obtained with the eight
each group. The Kruskal–Wallis test revealed significant subjects group, it is possible to deduce that the shoaling be-
variation between groups [H(3) = 10.167, p = 0.017] and the havior reduces stress and anxiety.31 In the light/dark box
Dunn’s post test showed difference between control and the experiment with more than one subject, there are some
group with eight subjects (Fig. 5). competing stimuli that induce the animal to move to the white
A linear regression test was conducted to verify if the av- side or to the dark side. The animal’s natural tendency to seek
erage of subjects that first chose the white side of the apparatus protection in dark places, avoiding bright environments, and
has a linear relationship between groups. The linear regression the anxiety caused by new environments contribute to the
test did not show any positive or negative relationship between preference for dark places. On the other hand, the innate drive
these variables. R = 0.0416; R2 = 0.00173; Coefficient = 0.364; to explore new places added to a possible anxiolytic effect
Standard Error = 0.779; t = 0.468; P = 0.641 (Fig. 6). caused by the presence of other subjects inside the apparatus
contributed to the subject’s displacement to the white side.
Discussion Zebrafish are a highly social animal, exhibiting preference for
swimming in groups and many other patterns of social co-
The data indicates no significant differences in the light/dark hesion. The reduced white avoidance behavior in the group of
preference test when carried out with as many as four subjects, eight subjects reflects the role of shoaling, which is a defense

FIG. 3. Average occurrence (plus standard error) of sub-


jects in the white side of the apparatus measured on every FIG. 5. Average of subjects (plus standard error) that have
15 s. **Differs from control and from group 2, assuming first chosen the white side of the apparatus. *Differs from
that p < 0.05. The average occurrence is calculated for each control, assuming that p < 0.05. The average occurrence is
group adding up the percentage of occurrence of subjects in calculated for each group adding up the subjects that have
the white side of all sessions (32 per group) and then di- first chosen the white side within the 32 sessions and then
viding the result by the number of sessions (32). dividing this number by the number of sessions.
NUMBER OF SUBJECTS ALTERS DARK/LIGHT PREFERENCE? 5

tion of the test, it would be difficult to measure the total time


of permanence and thigmotaxis.
The results of the research may also have been influenced by
two outside variables, social behavior and/or shoaling, and the
preference for dark places. Social behavior is presented by
the Danio rerio species, which has an established hierarchy in
the shoal, where the subordinated subjects follow the dominant
subjects. In this study, fish of Danio rerio species were placed
to swim in a 15 · 10 · 45 cm dimensions light/dark box with a
10 cm water column, containing 4.5 L. According to Aoki,49
there is a regular distance between individuals in a shoal,
which rises due to the maintenance of a private space for each
individual. Our tests were made successfully with up to four
FIG. 6. Linear regression line and data points of the av- subjects in the same apparatus. The inefficacy in the test with
erage of subjects that have first chosen the white side of the eight subjects was possibly due to the lack of space inside the
apparatus. The numbers beside the dots represent frequency test aquarium that lead the researchers to investigate whether
of the events. the scototaxic behavior is altered or supplanted by a tendency
to maintain the distance between subjects. According to
Westerfield,50 zebrafish is a shoaling fish and can be main-
mechanism, in reducing anxiety and stress. However, the test tained at high concentrations such as 20 subjects per 3 L of
results show that four subjects inside the apparatus is not water at 3 months of age. In our experiment the highest con-
enough to cause the anxiolytic effects of shoaling in the light/ centration of fish was eight subjects per 6.75 m3 of water.
dark test. However, studies using light/dark boxes of different sizes and
Figure 4 shows that the regression line has a mild positive with different water volumes should be further conducted.
tendency for the AO of subjects in the white side of the ap- The test still needs to undergo pharmacological validation
paratus; it might reflect a slight decrease in white avoidance to verify the effectiveness of utilizing more than one subject
even in the group of four subjects. In the novel tank test, for in the presence of drugs, provided that some drugs can
example, four subjects are enough to form a shoal with con- modify both fish and shoal behavior,47,51 resulting in cogni-
sistent performance in the test.47 Perhaps only four subjects in tive impairments, lowering or altering social behavior. It is
a school is not enough to elicit all the benefits observed in possible that such deficit in the subject’s ability to follow
larger schools, such as food location, efficient displacement, conspecifics may harm the group’s movement cohesion,
food acquisition, reproductive improvement and, particularly, causing the absence of shoaling behavior. Nevertheless, even
predator detection and avoidance. It reflects the subject’s without this kind of behavior, it is likely that the results in the
performance on the test, since there is no significant reduction light/dark test remain unmodified in the presence of any
in white avoidance behavior in the groups with two and four drugs, if the fish scototaxis behavior remains intact, because
subjects. These findings support future test with more than one the white avoidance behavior would most likely keep the
subject, since shoal cohesion remains stable and practically subjects in the dark side. This study does not attempt to de-
unaltered and only stimuli of considerable magnitude are able termine if the results are due to social behavior or scototaxis,
to alter its natural condition.48 Therefore, the subjects are therefore, further studies with anxiolytic drugs are necessary
prone to behave accordingly to the scototaxis behavior. to investigate this matter.
The variables commonly measured in the light/dark box
test are the total time in each side of the box; number of Conclusion
midline crossings; latency for the first choice; first choice of
compartment and thigmotaxis. The measurement of the time The reduced white avoidance behavior in the group of
of permanence is related to dark preference/white avoidance eight subjects seems to reflect the role of shoaling, which is a
behavior and can be used as a measure of anxiety, whereas defense mechanism in reducing anxiety and stress. On the
thigmotaxis and first choice are measures of locomotor be- other hand, the absence of significant difference between the
havior and fear.6,8 From the above mentioned variables, this control group and the test groups with two and four subjects
research tested only for first choice behavior. The total time suggest feasibility to run the light/dark test with up to four
of permanence of subjects in each side of the apparatus would subjects, becoming an alternative to streamline and to sim-
be a very difficult task to perform, since there were too many plify the data collection and test analysis. Pharmacological
subjects being observed at the same moment. Therefore, in- tests are still required to determine whether the effect of drugs
stead of the total time of permanence of each subject in each alters the model with enhanced number of subjects.
side of the apparatus, this research measured the occurrence
of subjects in each side of the apparatus every 15 s. This Acknowledgments
measure has proven to be efficient and just as reliable as the This research was supported by grants from CAPES (Co-
measurement of total time (as shown in Table 2 and Fig. 2). ordination for the Improvement of Higher Level or Education
Accordingly to this, the present model with enhanced num- Personnel).
bers of subjects has the potential to provide faster outcomes
and new possibilities for the light/dark box test. However,
Disclosure Statement
researchers are advised to consider the limitations of this
method. Although faster and more convenient, in this varia- No competing financial interests exist.
6 MANSUR ET AL.

References taxis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011;


35:624–631.
1. Engeszer RE, Patterson LB, Rao AA, Parichy DM. Zeb- 20. Maximino C, Brito TM, Colmanetti R, Pontes AAA, Castro
rafish in the wild: a review of natural history and new notes HM, Lacerda RI, Morato S, et al. Parametric analyses of
from the field. Zebrafish 2007;4:21–40. anxiety in zebrafish scototaxis. Behav Brain Res 2010;
2. Paull G, Filby A, Giddins H, Coe T, Hamilton P, Tyler C. 210:1–7.
Dominance hierarchies in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and their 21. Mansur BDM, Rodrigues B, Gouveia A Jr. Efeitos da
relationship with reproductive success. Zebrafish 2010;7: substância de alarme no teste claro/escuro no Zebrafish.
109–117. Biota Amaz 2014;4:87–93.
3. Houart C. Zebrafish as an Experimental Organism. In: eLS. 22. Jesuthasan S. Fear, anxiety, and control in the zebrafish.
John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Chichester. http://www.els.net Dev Neurobiol 2012;72:395–403.
[doi: 10.1038/npg.els.0002094]. 23. Wang J, Liu C, Ma F, Chen W, Liu J, Hu B, et al. Circadian
4. Spence R, Gerlach G, Lawrence C, Smith C. The behaviour clock mediates light/dark preference in zebrafish (danio
and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol Rev Camb rerio). Zebrafish 2014;11:2014.
Philos Soc 2008;83:13–34. 24. Saverino C, Gerlai R. The social zebrafish: behavioral re-
5. Spence R, Fatema MK, Reichard M, Huq KA., Wahab sponses to conspecific, heterospecific, and computer ani-
MA., Ahmed ZF, et al. The distribution and habitat pref- mated fish. Behav Brain Res 2008;191:77–87.
erences of the zebrafish in Bangladesh. J Fish Biol 2006; 25. Pham M, Raymond J, Hester J, Kyzar E, Gaikwad S,
69:1435–1448. Bruce I, et al. Assessing social behavior phenotypes in
6. Maximino C, Brito TM, Dias CAGDM, Gouveia A Jr., adult zebrafish: shoaling, social preference and mirror
Morato S. Scototaxis as anxiety-like behavior in fish. Nat biting tests. Zebrafish Protoc Neurobehav Res 2012;
Protoc 2010;5:209–216. 1–27.
7. Gouveia A Jr., Zampieri RA, Ramos LA, Silva EF, Mattioli 26. Romey WL, Galbraith E. Optimal group positioning after a
R, Morato S. Preference of Goldfish (carassius auratus) for predator attack: the influence of speed, sex, and satiation
dark places. Rev Etol 2005;7:63–66. within mobile whirligig swarms. Behav Ecol 2007;19:
8. Maximino C, Brito TM, Moraes FD, Oliveira FVC, Tac- 338–343.
colini IB, Pereira PM, et al. A comparative analysis of the 27. Engeszer RE, Ryan MJ, Parichy DM. Learned social
preference for dark environments in five teleosts. Int J preference in zebrafish. Curr Biol 2004;14:881–884.
Comp Psychol 2007;20:351–367. 28. Kerr J. Grouping behavior of the zebrafish as influenced by
9. Blaser RE, Chadwick L, McGinnis GC. Behavioral mea- social isolation. Am Zool 1963;2:532–533.
sures of anxiety in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 29. Al-Imari L, Gerlai R. Sight of conspecifics as reward in
2010;208:56–62. associative learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain
10. Egan RJ, Bergner CL, Hart PC, Cachat JM, Canavello PR, Res 2008;189:216–219.
Elegante MF, et al. Understanding behavioral and physio- 30. Wisenden BD, Vollbrecht KA, Brown JL. Is there a fish
logical phenotypes of stress and anxiety in zebrafish. Behav alarm cue? Affirming evidence from a wild study. Anim
Brain Res 2009;205:38–44. Behav 2004;67:59–67.
11. Wong K, Elegante M, Bartels B, Elkhayat S, Tien D, Roy S, 31. Speedie N, Gerlai R. Alarm substance induced behavioral
et al. Analyzing habituation responses to novelty in zebrafish responses in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res
(Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2010;208:450–457. 2008;188:168–177.
12. McNaughton N, Corr PJ. A two-dimensional neuropsy- 32. Pfeiffer W: Pheromones in fish and amphibia. In: Pher-
chology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance. omones. Birch M (ed), vol. 32, pp. 269–296, North-Hol-
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004;28:285–305. land, Amsterdam, 1974.
13. Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Ethoexperimental approaches 33. Rehnberg BG, Smith RJF. The influence of alarm sub-
to the biology of emotion. Ann Rev Psychol 1988;39:43–68. stance and shoal size on the behaviour of zebra danios,
14. Catherall DR. How fear differs from anxiety. Traumatology Brachydanio rerio (Cyprinidae). J Fish Biol 1988;33:
2003;9:76–92. 155–163.
15. Godsil BP, Tinsley MR, Fanselow MS: Motivation. In: 34. Pitcher T, Parrish J: Functions of shoaling in teleosts Be-
Handbook of Psychology: Experimental Psychology. Healy haviour. In: Behaviour of Teleost Fishes. Pitcher T (ed), pp.
AF and Proctor RW (eds), pp. 33–60, John Wiley & Sons, 363–439, Chapman and Hall, London, 1993.
Hoboken, NJ, 2003,. 35. Kurta A, Palestis BG. Effects of ethanol on the shoaling
16. Bolles R, Beecher M: Behavioristic approach to aversively behavior of zebrafish (danio rerio). Dose Response
a functional motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a 2010;8:527–533.
determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. In: 36. Anderson JJAY. A stochastic model for the size of fish
Evolution and Learning. Bolles RC and Beecher MD (eds), schools 1. Fish Bull 1981;79:315–323.
pp. 185–211, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, MI, 2013. 37. Reyes E, Sklar FH, Day JW. A regional organism exchange
17. Maximino C, Brito TM, da Silva Batista AW, Herculano model for simulating fish migration. Ecol Model 1994;
AM, Morato S, Gouveia A Jr. Measuring anxiety in zebra- 74:255–276.
fish: a critical review. Behav Brain Res 2010;214:157–171. 38. Peter Nonacs, Paul E. Smith, Amos Bouskila BL. Modeling
18. Grossman L, Utterback E, Stewart A, Gaikwad S, Chung the behavior of the northern anchovy, Engraulis mordax, as
KM, Suciu C, et al. Characterization of behavioral and a shooling predator exploiting patchy prey. Deep Sea Re-
endocrine effects of LSD on zebrafish. Behav Brain Res search 1994;41:147–169.
2010;214:277–284. 39. Romey WL. Individual differences make a difference in the
19. Maximino C, da Silva AWB, Gouveia A, Herculano AM. trajectories of simulated schools of fish. Ecol Model
Pharmacological analysis of zebrafish (Danio rerio) scoto- 1996;92:65–77.
NUMBER OF SUBJECTS ALTERS DARK/LIGHT PREFERENCE? 7

40. Theodorakis CW. Size segregation and the effects of oddity 48. Miller N, Gerlai R. Quantification of shoaling behaviour
on predation risk in minnow schools. Anim Behav 1989; in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2007;184:
38:496–502. 157–166.
41. Wellington W. Individual differences as a factor in popu- 49. Aoki I. A simulation study on the schoolng mechanism in
lation dynamics: the development of a problem. Can J Zool fish. Bull Jpn Soc Sci Fish 1982;48:1081–1088.
1957;35:293–323. 50. Westerfield M: The Zebrafish Book: A Guide for the La-
42. Slater P: Individual differences in animal behavior. In: boratory Use of Zebrafish (Danio rerio), 4th edition. Uni-
Perspectives in Ethology. Bateson P and Klopfer P (ed), pp. versity of Oregon Press, Eugene, 2000.
35–49, Plenum Press, New York, 1981. 51. Maaswinkel H, Le X, He L, Zhu L, Weng W. Dissociating
43. Clark A, Ehlinger T: Pattern and adaptation in individual the effects of habituation, black walls, buspirone and eth-
behavioral differences. In: Perspectives in Ethology. Bate- anol on anxiety-like behavioral responses in shoaling zeb-
son P and Klopfer P (ed), pp. 1–47, Plenum Press, New rafish. A 3D approach to social behavior. Pharmacol
York, 1987. Biochem Behav 2013;108:16–27.
44. Colgan P: The motivational basis of fish behaviour. In:
Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, 2nd edition. Pitcher T (ed), Address correspondence to:
pp. 23–43, Chapman and Hall, New York, 1993. Bruno de Matos Mansur, BS
45. Magurran A: Individual differences in fish behaviour. In: Universidade Federal do Pará
Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, 2nd edition. Pitcher T (ed), Rua Augusto Corrêa
pp. 441–477, Chapman and Hall, New York, 1986. 01-Guamá
46. Romey W, Rossman D. Temperature and hunger alter CEP 66075-110
grouping trade-offs in whirligig beetles. Am Midl Nat Caixa postal 479
1995;134:51–62. Belém 2012
47. Green J, Collins C, Kyzar EJ, Pham M, Roth A, Gaikwad S,
Pará
et al. Automated high-throughput neurophenotyping of
Brasil
zebrafish social behavior. J Neurosci Methods 2012;210:
266–271. E-mail: bmmansur@hotmail.com

You might also like