You are on page 1of 11

REVIEW PAPERS

Personality Dimensions and Depression:


Review and Commentary
Murray W Enns, MD I , Brian J Cox, PhD 2

Objectives: The relationship between dimensionally assessed personality and the onset, features, and course of
depressive illness will be critically examined and considered in relation to 4 hypothesized models: predisposition
or vulnerability: pathoplasty; complication or scar: and spectrum or continuity.
Method: Studies that have used clinically depressed adult patients to explore the relationship between personality
dimensions and depression will he reviewed.
Results: Higher-order personality factors that have shown a significant and consistent association with major
depressive illness include neuroticism, extraversion (negative relationship), and the factors of Cloninger '.I'
Tridimensional Personality Model. Neuroticism appears to be the most powerful predictor of depression.
Lower-orderfactors showing a significant and consistent relationship with depressive illness include dependency,
self-criticism, obsessionality, and perfectionism. The links between depression and dependency and self-criticism
have the strongest empirical support.
Conclusions: Several personality dimensions are significantly associated with depressive illness, hut the evidence
that unequivocally demonstrates a true personality predisposition for depression is modest. Measures ofperson-
ality may prove to he clinically useful for treatment selection.

(Can J Psychiatry 1997;42:274-284)

Key Words: depression, personality, personality factors, dimensions ofpersonality

P ersonality functioning and depressive illness are com-


plexly interrelated. There is an extensive psychiatric lit-
erature on the relationship between personality disorders and
normally distributed traits. such as the continuum of normal
and pathological personality traits, and has advantages in its
precision and its ability to describe "border" cases reliably
depression, and this body of work has been reviewed else- (3,4). The relative advantages and drawbacks of categorical
where (1,2). Psychiatrists may be less familiar, however, with and dimensional personality assessment are described in
personality dimensions or factors that emerging research has more detail by Frances (5), The present review will focus on
identified as important in the etiology and maintenance of studies that have used adult patients with rigorously defined
major depression. major depression (for example, Feighner, Research Diagnos-
tic Criteria [ROC], DSM-I11 and its successors) to examine
In contrast to categorical approaches, dimensional assess- the relationship between personality or temperament and the
ment seems theoretically more applicable in describing occurrence, course, prognosis, and associated features of
depressive illness. The review will begin by examining broad,
empirically derived (higher-order) factors and then review
Manuscript received August 1996,revised December 1996.
I Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry, University of Manitoba,
several specific lower-order factors that have been associated
Winnipeg, Manitoba. with depression.
~As,sistant Professor, Departmentsof Psychology and Psychiatry, University
of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba. Conceptually, 4 kinds of relationship may exist between
Address forcorrespondence: Dr MW Enns, PsycHealthCentre, PZ-430771 personality and depression. These include a vulnerability
BannatyneAvenue, Winnipeg, MB R3E 3N4 model (personality factors predispose to the development of
depression), a pathoplasty model (personality factors affect
Can) PsycWatry, Vol 42, Aprll1997 the expression ofdepression), a complication or "scar" model

274
April 1997 Personality Dimensions and Depression 275

(depression leads to changes in personality function), and a relationships between neuroticism and depression (28-3 I). A
continuity or spectrum model (an underlying process is re- major study by Kendler and others examined the genetics of
sponsible for the occurrence ofboth personality problems and neuroticism and depression in a longitudinal study of 1733
d~pression) (6). As studies are described, their consistency female twin pairs using the Virginia Twin Registry (31). The
with one or more of these models will be presented. twins completed neuroticism ratings (EPI) and were compre-
hensively interviewed on 2 occasions, 14 months apart. In
Neuroticism summary, the study demonstrated the following: neuroticism
was strongly predictive of both lifetime and one-year preva-
. ~euroticism is a major higher-order personality dimension lence of depression (vulnerability model); an episode of de-
ongmally derived by factor analysis from large and diverse pression occurring between assessments increased
samples. (Streiner [7] provides a concise review of factor neuroticism (scar model); a current new episode ofdepression
analysis.) Neuroticism is a broad, general personality trait, the at the second assessment increased neuroticism (continuity
c~re of which is a temperamental sensitivity to negative model or state effect); and the relationship between neuroti-
stimuli. High-neuroticism patients tend to experience a vari- cism and depression was found to be 70% attributable to
ety of negative affective states (8). Studies ofthe relationship shared genetic risk factors for both (continuity) and 10% to
between neuroticism and depression have used a number of depression increasing neuroticism (state effect).
different personality measures, including the Maudsley Per- The broad literature on neuroticism and depression spans
sonality Inventory (MPI) (9), the Eysenck Personality Inven- o.ver3 deca?es m~d provides support for each of the hypothe-
tory (EPI) (10), the Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness .slzed relationships between personality and depression.
Personality Inventory (NEO-PI) (I I), and the Munich Per- ~here_ an~, how.:ver, significant limitations to these impres-
sonality Test (MPT) (12). Because neuroticism measures sive findings. First, the neuroticism measure has been dem-
have been available for many years, a wealth of data on their onstrated in a .number of studies to be significantly
relationship to depression is available. Only an overview of s~ate-depend~nt; It appears to reflect strongly current depres-
the major findings, therefore, will be presented. sion ~nd anxiety symptoms in addition to long-standing per-
Eight longitudinal follow-up studies of depressed patients sonality (31,32). Second, neuroticism is a broad construct that
Used the MPI to measure neuroticism, and thus their findings appears to convey vulnerability in a rather nonspecific man-
can be compared easily across studies (I 3-21). Overall, the ner f~r.a range of"distress disorders" (anxiety and depressive
stUdies show that depressive patients have higher neuroticism conditions) .(~,33). C~inicians and researchers alike may
scores than controls and that chronic or persistent depression therefore be interested 111 personality traits or factors that have
is associated with continued elevation ofneuroticism (consis- greater speci ficity.
tent with the continuity model or state effects). Recovery from
depression is associated with very gradually declining Extraversion
neuroticism (consistent with the scar model). Highly elevated
neuroticism scores during depression are predictive of poor
Extraversion is another major personality dimension that
prognosis; "reactive" depression patients exhibit more
was derived by factor analysis (9, I0). Extraversion is a stable
neuroticism than endogenous depressives (both observations
heritable, and highly general temperamental dimension char~
Consistent with the pathoplasty model). Finally, premorbid
acterized by positive emotionality, energy, and dominance
testing shows greater neuroticism in those who later develop
(8,33,34). The same personality tests described earlier lor
depression than in those who do not (vulnerability model).
neuroticism also contain a measure of extraversion (9-12).
Several other prernorbid assessment studies found that high
neuroticism scores predicted later depression (vulnerability Eight studies of personality and depression that used the
model) (2 I). MPI-N (neuroticism) measure also used the MPI-E (extra-
A number of studies of neuroticism and depression have version) scale (13-2 I). Overall, the effects ofextraversion are
noted a remarkably powerful relationship between neuroti- less robust than the comparable findings for neuroticism. Like
cism and the prognosis of depression (pathoplasty model) neuroticism, extraversion appears to be subject to significant
(22-27). One of the most interesting of these studies was state effects (14,15,18, I9), with depressed subjects showing
reported by Taylor and McLean (26). They studied 155 a significant increase in extraversion with recovery from
depressed patients who were assigned to 4 different treat- depression. Low extraversion was also shown to be associ-
ments (psychotherapy, relaxation. behaviour therapy, or ated with poorer prognosis in some (13, I 5,18), but not all of
amitriptyline); high neuroticism predicted worse outcome these reports (17,20,21) (pathoplasty model). The one
regardless of treatment modality. premorbid assessment study that used MPI-E did not find low
.extraversion scores to be predictive of new onset of depres-
Several studies have examined the familial relationship sion (17). The finding of higher prernorbid extraversion
between neuroticism and depression, and collectively, these scores (18) compared with postmorbid extraversion scores
studies provide evidence for all 4 of the hypothesized ( 13-20) provides evidence of a scar effect.
276 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 42, No 3

Several other studies have evaluated the pathoplastic ef- analysis-based systems in that the personality variables were
fects of extraversion. While neuroticism has been fairly deduced from a synthesis ofinformation from family and twin
consistently associated with depression subtype, reported studies, neuropharmacologic and neurobehavioural studies in
studies ofextraversion have been less remarkable. Two recent humans and animals, and studies oflongitudinal development
studies failed to find a relationship between extraversion and (40,42). The personality dimensions so derived are hypotheti-
situational versus reactive types of depression (30,35). Some cally related to underlying neurotransmitter systems (HA
studies have found lower extraversion in nonmelancholic or relates to serotonergic function, RD to noradrenergic func-
nonendogenous versus melancholic or endogenous depres- tion, and NS to dopaminergic function) (40).
sion (36), but others have not (37). Extraversion scores have The depressed state has been demonstrated to affect TPQ
also been evaluated in relation to the prognosis of depression.
scores. Joffe and others conducted a longitudinal study of
Again, the findings have been mixed, with some studies
patients with major depression who completed the TPQ while
finding that higher extraversion is associated with recovery
depressed and at follow-up (44). Patients who had recovered
(26,37) and others failing to find the relationship (24,25).
from depression after 3 months showed a decrease in HA
In a large sample offemale twins in the general population scores, while those who did not recover had no change in HA
(N = 1733), no evidence ofa significant relationship between scores. NS and RD did not show significant state effects. In
extraversion and major depression was found; extraversion support of the continuity and vulnerabi lity models, the
did not predict new onset, one-year prevalence, or lifetime authors noted that the HA scores of their depressed patients
prevalence of depression, and neither the depressed state nor were substantially higher than published normative values
postdepressive state influenced the level of extraversion (31). regardless of response status (responder versus nonrespon-
der) or clinical state (depressed versus remitted). The scar
The reported findings regarding the relationship between model was supported by the observation that HA scores in
extraversion and depression are less robust and consistent remitted depressives are lower than in the acutely depressed
than the data for neuroticism. Continuity, pathoplasty, and state but still remain substantially higher than norms.
scar models are most strongly supported.
Pathoplastic effects ofTPQpersonalityvariables have also
Recently, Clark and Watson have integrated the data on been demonstrated in a number of studies. The previously
neuroticism, extraversion, and depression in a tripartite model cited study by Joffe and others found that high HA scores
of the relationship between temperament or personality and were indicative of poorer prognosis (44). Joyce and others
the "distress disorders" (33). This model groups anxiety and explored predictors of response to 6 weeks of treatment with
depressive symptoms into 3 subtypes: nonspecific symptoms a tricyclic antidepressant in 84 patients with major depression
of general distress (related to neuroticism), depression-spe- (45). The authors used combinations of "high" versus "low"
cific symptoms of anhedonia, low energy, and low positive scores on the NS, HA, and RD scales to define 8 temperamen-
affect (related to low extraversion), and relatively anxiety- tal types (40) that were strongly predictive of treatment out-
specific symptoms of somatic or autonomic arousal (33,38). come (individually, NS, HA, and RD were not strongly
This empirically supported model has proven to be valuable predictive of outcome). In general, patients with low levels
in organizing research on the relationships among neuroti- of NS, HA, and RD and patients with high HA and RD
cism, extraversion, depression and the comorbidity ofdepres- (regardless ofNS) had a favourable outcome. A later report
sion and anxiety disorders. partially replicated these results in a group of major depres-
sion subjects treated in an 8-week trial of nefazodone (46).
Cloninger Tridimensional Personality Model These reports are impressive in the amount of predictive
power they demonstrate for temperament, but the results are
Based on a general biosocial theory of personality (39), difficult to reconcile with other studies and require
Cloninger has developed a novel system of describing and replication.
studying temperament and personality. Cloninger proposed a
Studies of the relationship ofTPQ-assessed temperament
system ofpersonality description based on 3 independent and
to biological markers of depression have also been published.
heritable higher-order personality dimensions that reflect ba-
Joyce and others studied morning and afternoon cortisol
sic stimulus-response characteristics (40). These 3 major
levels of patients with major depression and controls (47). RD
personality dimensions are termed harm avoidance (HA),
and NS were significantly predictive of morning hypercorti-
reward dependence (RD), and novelty seeking (NS) and are
solemia in the depressed subjects. It was interesting that
measured by the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire
temperament was a more powerful predictor than the severity
('fPQ) (41). The TPQ has more recently been expanded and
of depression or the number of melancholic symptoms.
revised to create the Temperament and Character Inventory
(TCI) (42,43), but the majority ofpublished studies evaluat- The foregoing sections of this review have dealt with
ing the relationship of personality to depression have used the broad, general personality factors that have origins in a larger
earlier instrument (TPQ). Cloninger's system of personality framework of personality theory. These broad personality
description and classification differs from traditional factor dimensions can be described as higher-order factors. Such
April 1997 Personality Dimensions and Depression 277

personality factors have strong empirical foundations, and as not predict new onset of depression, but in "older" subjects
indicated above, some have shown significant relationships (31 to 41 years of age), increased dependency (ERA. LSC)
with depressive illness. These personality factors were not was associated with the onset of depression (vulnerability
developed specifically for the study of depression, however. model). Dependency scores in symptomatically recovered
In their review of the relationship of personality to affec- depression patients were higher than the premorbid scores of
tive disorders, Akiskal and others identified a number of new-onset subjects (scar effect). Another study that tested the
personality associations with depression, including lack of scar hypothesis in older subjects obtained results which were
self-confidence (self-criticism), dependency, obsessionality, more consistent with dependency as a trait marker (orvulner-
and pessimism, that are strongly connected to clinical obser- ability factor) and not consistent with scar effects (53).
vations and explorations (6). To this list we would add the The specificity of interpersonal dependency to depression
cognitively based constructs of sociotropy and autonomy has been questioned. Reich and others conducted a compari-
proposed by Beck and others (48) and perfectionism. (These son of depression, anxiety. and control subjects (54). They
clinically grounded constructs can be described as lower-or-
found no difference in the state effects of depression and
der traits or factors.) Instruments have been derived to meas-
anxiety on ERA and LSC and no differences in interpersonal
ure each of these personality factors, and over the last 20
dependency in panic and depression patients.
years, a significant literature on their relationship to depres-
sive illness has accumulated. The remainder of this review Several other authors have noted pathoplastic effects of
will focus on these lower-order traits and depression. 101 dependency, though the findings have not always been in
.agreement. Frank and others found that slow responders to
Dependency antidepressant treatment were significantly more dependent
(ERA and LSC) than normal responders (13). Boyce and
The role of interpersonal dependency in psychogenetic others found that nonmelancholic patients were more depend-
predisposition to depressive illness has been recognized for ent (ERA and LSC) than melancholic patients, but not signifi-
much of the last century, but systematic study ofthis relation- cantly so (55). Another group of investigators conducted a
ship by operationalizing "dependency" and formally testing large study (N = 569) to characterize a "nuclear depressive
hypotheses has been a more recent development (49). The syndrome" (consisting of patients who commonly meet cri-
basic model of the relationship between dependency and
teria lor multiple definitions of the endogenous/melancholic
depression has been that dependency acts as a vulnerability
subtype of depression). They found that "nuclear" depressed
factor; the dependent individual is vulnerable to depression
patients were significantly more dependent (ERA only) than
When affection, love, or caring from a valued other person is
nonnuclear depressed patients (56).
withheld or lost.
Hirschfeld and others developed the Interpersonal De- Several important observations have been made about the
pendency Inventory (101) based on the understanding of relationship of 101 dependency to depression. The ERA and
interpersonal dependency as a "complex of thoughts, beliefs, LSC subscales have been most fruitful. while the AA subscale
feelings and behaviours which revolve around the need to has not yielded meaningful positive findings. For this reason,
associate, interact with, and rely upon valued other people" it has been suggested that this subscale be abandoned (57).
(49, p 374). The 101 has 3 subscales including emotional
The Interpersonal Sensitivity Measure (IPSM) was spe-
reliance on another person (ERA), lack of social self-confi-
cifically developed as a measure of disposition to depression
dence (LSC), and assertion of autonomy (AA) (50).
(58). The authors defined interpersonal sensitivity as "undue
The effect of the depressed state on the 101 measure is and excessive awareness of. and sensitivity, to the behaviour
robust, with depressed individuals showing a significant de- and feelings of others" (58. p 34 I) . Inspection of the item
cline in both ERA and LSC with recovery (15). Symptomati- content of the IPSM reveals a striking similarity between the
cally remitted depressive subjects, however, continue to have constructs of "dependency" and "interpersonal sensitivity"
ERA and LSC scores above expected baseline levels (consis- (57). A prospective. premorbid assessment study showed that
tent with scar, continuity. and predisposition models) (16,51). high IPSM scores predicted the onset ofdepressive symptoms
A comparison of depressed subjects who eventually remitted following the stressor of childbirth (59.60) (vulnerability
against a matched group who remained ill after a 2-year model). The IPSM has also been shown to distinguish be-
follow-up did not demonstrate pathoplastic effects of tween remitted melancholic and nonmelancholic depressed
interpersonal dependency; ERA, LSC. and AA were not patients; nonmelancholic patients showed higher interper-
different between groups (52). sonal sensitivity than melancholic patients (55), and the IPSM
In a prospective. premorbid assessment study, a group of scores of melancholic patients were not different from those
390 never-ill high-risk patients (relatives of depressed sub- of control subjects (61) (pathoplasty model). High IPSM
jects) were assessed using the 101 at baseline and followed score has also been associated with poor outcome in de-
for 6 years (17). Among younger subjects. dependency did pressed patients (62) (pathoplasty model).
278 The Canadian journal of Psychiatry Vol 42, No 3

Table \. Predisposition to depression: psychoanalytic after 6 or more months, these scores move toward normal,
and cognitive models Fourth, self-criticism scores are associated with depression
Psychoanalytic construct Cognitive construct severity (pathoplasty). Finally, higher baseline DEQ depend-
(measured with Blatt's (measured with Beck's ency and self-criticism scores are associated with nome-
Personality trait DEQ) SAS)
covery (pathoplasty).
Dependency Anaclitic type Sociotropy dimension
_ Selt:':~~:ism ._L~:oje~~ve ~_e ~:onomy dimension __ One of the key predictions of Blatt's model of depressive
vulnerability is a difference in the symptom structure of
anaclitic versus introjective depressed patients (63,66,70).
The data reviewed suggest that there may be a bidirec- The most methodologically sophisticated study to test this
tional relationship between depression and dependency. Ele- prediction in these subjects was able to find only limited
vated dependency appears to act as a vulnerability factor that support for this distinction (67). The specificity of dependent
increases the risk of depression, and the experience of having and self-critical personality dimensions was assessed in a
a depressive episode may increase the individual's interper- comparative study of patients with panic disorder with ago-
sonal dependency. raphobia versus major depression (71). No difference in
DEQ-measured dependency scores was observed in the 2
groups; self-criticism, however, was significantly higher in
Dependency and Self-Criticism the depressed subjects, even after controlling for depressed
mood. (One might also expect to find that self-criticism is
Several authors have conceptualized the vulnerability to nonspecific if social phobia patients were examined, because
depression as consisting of2 lower-order personality factors: self-criticism is a typical clinical feature in socially phobic
dependency and self-criticism. Blatt integrated psychoana- patients.)
lytic and ego psychological theories to propose 2 personality
structures influencing vulnerability, namely anaclitic and A conceptually similar model and line of research comes
introjective types (63). In the more primitive anaclitic (de- from Beck and colleagues. Beck's constructs of sociotropy
pendent) form, the primary concern is interpersonal relation- and autonomy have been studied as personality dimensions
ships, and the features of depression predicted are that differentiate "depressions" according to cognitive
helplessness, tearfulness, mood reactivity, substance abuse, vulnerability. Sociotropy refers to a person's need for positive
and "borderline features." In the introjective (self-critical) interchange with other people; highly sociotropic people are
form, the primary concerns are self-worth and identity issues, very concerned about disapproval from others, and their
and the features of depression predicted are anhedonia, lack behaviour is often intended to please others and secure their
of mood reactivity, social withdrawal, and feelings of guilt attachments. Autonomy refers to an individual's need for
and worthlessness. Personality vulnerability to depression independence and the attainment of meaningful goals; highly
has also been considered from a cognitive perspective, Beck autonomous people are preoccupied with potential personal
offered a formulation using the dual constructs ofsociotropy failure (48). Beck has also postulated that these personality
(dependency) and autonomy (self-criticism) (64). Cognitive dimensions may be related to differing clinical presentations
theorists have also extended these constructs in the Dysfunc- of depression. The highly sociotropic individual is hypothe-
tional Attitudes Scale (DAS), which is intended to measure sized to develop depression around a theme of deprivation
the negative attitudes that organize negative cognitions (65). and display symptoms typically associated with "neurotic/re-
The latter instrument is not clearly intended to measure "per- active" depression (72). The highly autonomous individual is
sonality," per se. It may, however, be quite difficult to sepa- hypothesized to develop depression around a theme of defeat
rate "personality" from "cognition" (Table I). and to show symptoms typically associated with "endogene-
morphic depression" (73). These postulated symptom pat-
Blatt's anaclitic and introjective types were reconstructed terns have been subjected to empirical study.
into dependent and self-critical personality dimensions in the
Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (DEQ) (66). Several Robins, Block, and Peselow (74) examined the relations
notable findings on the relationship between DEQ scores and between levels of sociotropic and autonomous personality
depression have been reported. There are 3 reports on corn- characteristics and depressive symptom clusters using Beck's
parisons of depressed subjects (while acutely depressed and Sociotropy-Autonomy Scale (SAS) in 80 patients with major
remitted) versus controls (67-69). The findings can be sum- depression. They found a strong relationship between socio-
marized as follows. First, depressed patients consistently tropy and proposed sociotropic symptoms, but no relationship
show greater dependency and self-criticism than controls. was observed between autonomy and the proposed
Second, dependency and, to a lesser extent, self-criticism are autonomous symptom cluster. The authors speculated that the
affected by the clinical state; both are more pronounced autonomy measure (rather than the construct) may require
during depression (continuity). Third, shortly after recovery, revision. Similar findings were reported by Moore and Black-
depressed individuals still score higher than controls on both burn (75). Observations such as these have led to refinements
dependency and self-criticism scales (scar hypothesis), but in the psychometric assessment of autonomy (76,77). Using
April 1997 Personality Dimensions and Depression 279

a newer measure of sociotropy and autonomy in a group of In our opinion, there is also a conceptual problem in the
50 depressed inpatients, Robins and Luten (77) were able to diathesis-event-eonsequence models. Researchers have typi-
demonstrate a selective relationship between sociotropy, as cally classified interpersonal events solely in the dependency
well as autonomy, and the predicted sets ofclinical symptoms domain. Could relationship difficulties not provoke depres-
(pathoplasty model). sion in a highly self-critical individual? Such an individual
may blame himself or herself for the relationship problems
The constructs ofsociotropy and autonomy have also been and attribute these difficulties to personal inadequacy.
demonstrated to predict vulnerability to depression in relation
to speci fie kinds of stressful or negative life events. One study
Obsessionality
found that the onset or exacerbation of depressive symptoms,
as well as the total number of symptoms, was more strongly A relationship between obsessionality and depression has
associated with negative interpersonal events in sociotropic been documented in a very large body of literature covering
patients and more strongly associated with negative achieve- a period of several decades (84). In contemporary
ment events in autonomous patients (78). Another study of (psychometric) literature, a critical assessment issue for ob-
highly sociotropic versus highly autonomous depressed pa- sessionality is the distinction between obsessive-compulsive
tients found support for the "personality-event congruence symptoms (typical of obsessive-eompulsive disorder), such
hypothesis" in the sociotropic patients only (79) as genuine obsessive ruminations, rituals, and checking be-
(predisposition). haviours, and obsessional personality traits. such as preoccu-
pation with orderliness, perfectionism, control, and
Important pathoplastic effects (relating to treatment re-
. conscientiousness. Two of the most commonly used dimen-
sponse) have also been reported for the personality constructs
sional assessment instruments for obsessional personality
of sociotropy and autonomy. In one study of SAS-defined
traits are the Leyton Inventory (85) and the obsessional pat-
sociotropy and autonomy and response to pharmacotherapy,
tern of the Lazare-Klerman-Armor inventory (LKA-O) (86).
a regression analysis showed that the sociotropy-autonomy The MPT (12) also measures "rigidity," which is very similar
distinction but not the endogenous-nonendogenous distinc- to the construct ofobsessionality.
tion predicted drug treatment response (80). Subjects with
high autonomy plus low sociotropy scores showed greater A series of papers from the National Institutes of Mental
response to antidepressants and greater drug-placebo differ- Health (NIMH) examining the relationship between a variety
ences. This striking and somewhat surprising finding clearly ofpersonality dimensions and depression was cited earlier in
requires replication. Two studies by the same research group this review (14-17,52). This series of investigations also used
evaluated the response to individual or group cognitive psy- the LKA inventory to measure obsessionality, with entirely
chothcrapy in relation to SAS scores (8 I,82). In the first negative results, Another study that characterized nuclear
study, the authors observed that sociotropic subjects showed (endogenous) and nonnuclear (nonendogenous) types of de-
greater improvement in the more interpersonal context of pression in a large number of subjects with major depression
group therapy, whereas autonomous patients improved more failed to demonstrate differences between these groups in
in individual therapy. In the second study, matching oftrcat- LKA-O scores (56). Also, a study comparing ROC-defined
ment modality (sociotropic subjects in groups, autonomous depressed patients with panic patients found no difference in
subjects in individual therapy) was not associated with better LKA obsessionality between groups (54). Finally, a study of
LKA obsessionality in normal versus slow responders to
treatment outcome but was associated with more sustained
antidepressant treatment found no difference between
improvement at 2-month follow-up. These findings are con-
groups (13).
sistent with Beck's predictions (48).
In contrast to the collection of negative studies summa-
There is mixed support for the dependency and self-
rized above, studies using other instruments to measure ob-
criticism constructs, but the results to date are encouraging.
sessionality have shown positive results. Hirschfeld and
Overall, there appears to be greater support for the depend-
Klerman (14) compared patients with an ROC diagnosis of
ency dimensions. A variety of methodo logic weaknesses that depression with manic patients and controls using the Leyton
may account for the lack ofclear and consistent findings have Obsessional Inventory (LO\). They found higher levels of
recently been identified and reviewed (83). These weaknesses trait obsessionality (obsessional personality traits) in de-
include the use of arbitrary cut-off points to classify partici- pressed than in control subjects and higher levels of state
pants into types and the tendency to exclude those patients obsessionality (obsessive-compulsive symptoms) in de-
who have both high sociotropy and high autonomy scores (a pressed than in either manic or control subjects. Another very
group of important subjects who may be at particularly high long-term (18-year) follow-up study of89 inpatients meeting
risk). Other significant shortcomings in this research area ROC criteria for depression found that high obsessional in-
include the lack of prospective studies and the practice of terference scores on the LOI predicted poor long-term out-
assessing symptom change, when in fact the models are come, impaired social adjustment, and more time spent in
intended to predict the onset of major depressive episodes. hospital (pathoplasty) (22). Maier and others (28) studied
280 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 42, No 3

MPT -defined rigidity in the relatives of depressed subjects A significant relationship between self-oriented and par-
and normal controls. They found higher levels of rigidity in ticularly socially prescribed perfectionism and suicidal idea-
both previously depressed and never-depressed relatives of tion or intent has also been reported in mixed psychiatric
depressed patients compared with control relatives (most samples (96,97). Unfortunately, these interesting findings
consistent with vulnerability and continuity models). have not been replicated in a more homogenous clinical
depression group.
Rumination may be a particularly important aspect of
obsessionality. Researchers such as Morrow and Nolen- The relationship between perfectionism and major depres-
Hoeksema (87) have suggested that individuals differ in their sion has also been explored from the perspective ofa "specific
duration of depressive episode in part because of their ten- vulnerability hypothesis." Hewitt and Flett found that
dency to engage in rumination in reaction to their depressed self-oriented perfectionism interacted specifically with
mood. Preliminary evidence from dysphoric subjects and
achievement-related stresses to predict severity of depres-
bereaved adults have supported this contention (88,89). Un-
sion, and socially prescribed perfectionism interacted specifi-
fortunately, studies of this intriguing hypothesis using clini- cally with interpersonal stresses to predict severity of
cally depressed subjects have not been reported.
depression (the former finding was replicated in a diagnosti-
Overall, it appears that the studies reviewed support a cally heterogeneous sample, while the latter was not) (98).
relationship between obsessionality and depression, but they These observations are most consistent with a pathoplasty
indicate that the LKA-O measure is problematic. Premorbid model (since onset of depression was not considered in this
assessment studies using an instrument other than the LKA study). In a recent report from the NIMH Treatment of
measure of obsessionality have not been reported. Depression Collaborative Research Program, perfectionism
was observed to predict poor outcome consistently, regard-
less of treatment modality (the treatments included interper-
Perfectionism
sonal therapy, cognitive-behavioural therapy, imipramine,
and placebo) (99).
Perfectionism is also thought to confer vulnerability to
depression. The theoretical relationship between perfection-
The dimensional measures of perfectionism have only
ism and depression has been described from both cognitive relatively recently been developed, so the literature on per-
(90) and psychodynamic (91) perspectives. The basic hy-
fectionism and depression is limited. The relationship be-
pothesis is that perfectionistic standards increase the fre-
tween perfectionism and depression appears to be a promising
quency and magnitude of the perceived failures which can
area for further research. Clarification is needed regarding the
provoke depression. Two multidimensional instruments have
conceptual and/or measurement overlap between perfection-
been developed to quantify perfectionism and its personal and ism and other lower-order constructs related to depression.
social aspects (92,93). Only the Multidimensional Perfection-
The most obvious case is self-oriented perfectionism and
ism Scale (MPS) of Hewitt and Flett has been studied in self-criticism. Another possibility is a relationship between
clinically depressed patients. The components of
socially prescribed perfectionism and interpersonal depend-
perfectionism that are identified and measured by the MPS ency. Finally, the evidence reviewed indicates that like
include self-oriented perfectionism (setting and striving for
neuroticism, perfectionism may be linked to a number of
unrealistic standards and focusing on flaws), other-oriented different clinical disorders in addition to depression. It there-
perfectionism (behaviourally similar, but directed toward fore becomes important to demonstrate that perfectionism has
others rather than the self), and socially prescribed perfection- incremental predictive power over higher-order factors
ism (the belief that others hold perfectionistic expectations (neuroticism, extraversion) in major depression.
for oneself) (93). A recent study compared the Hewitt and
Flett MPS with the Frost perfectionism scale in depressed
patients and found that socially prescribed perfectionism was Summary and Conclusions
the strongest predictor of depressed mood (94).
The specificity of these dimensions of perfectionism to There are substantial limitations to the intriguing results
depression was explored in a study of depressed patients, reviewed in this paper, and these limitations present signifi-
matched controls, and anxiety-disordered subjects (95). The cant research challenges. As noted in our review, a major
authors reported that self-oriented perfectionism was ele- shortcoming in many areas of study is the absence of longi-
vated in depressed subjects compared with both controts and tudinal studies in which personality is assessed prior to the
anxiety patients, whereas socially prescribed perfectionism onset ofthe first episode of depression. Such studies provide
was elevated in both depressed and anxious patients relative the greatest assurance that personality is being assessed with-
to controls. These results suggest that higher levels of self- out undue influence of the potentially distorting effects of
oriented perfectionism may be specific to clinical depression, current or past depression. This is particularly important in
while socially prescribed perfectionism may be related to attempting to establish a personality factor or trait as a genu-
various kinds of maladjustment. ine predisposing factor for depression.
Aprll1997 Personality Dimensions and Depression 281

Table 2. Summary of major findings on the relationship between personality and major depression
Personality/depression models
Pathoplasty:
_Personality factors VUlnerability/predisposition Pathoplasty: outcome symptomatology Scar/compiication Continuity/spectrum
Higher order
Neuroticism ++ ++ ++ ++ ++
Extraversion 0 +/- +/- + +
TPQ factors + ++ + + +
Lower order
Dependency (sociotropy) + ++ +/- ++ ++
Self-criticism (autonomy) + ++ +/- + +
Obsessionality +/- + 0 0 +
Perfectionism + + + 0 +

++ =strongly supported (premorbid assessment is required for strong support of the vulnerability model).
+ =moderately supported (studies are smaller. methodologically weaker. or fewer in number).
+/- =mixed positive and negative findings.
()= not yet demonstrated.

The great majority of research studies examining person- career or finances may result in endorsement of items reflect-
ality dimensions and depression have relied on self-report ing self-criticism. Thus the importance of carefully assessing
inventories for the measurement ofpersonality. This presents the social context in which depression and "personality" are
a substantial methodologic issue because comparisons of assessed is clear.
interview and self-report assessments have shown that self-
reports provide more elevated assessments of maladaptive Finally, few studies of the relationships between person-
personality traits, in part because of the effects of depressed ality and depression have used measures of several higher-
mood (100). In contrast, self-report inventories probably and lower-order personality factors simultaneously in at-
show an advantage over interviews in long-term test-retest tempting to explain the onset, features, or course of depres-
reliability (10 1).
sion. Thus opportunities for comparing the validity of
The frequent observation ofstrong effects ofthe depressed competing th,eories and/or demonstrating the additional pre-
state on personality "trait" measurement also presents a chal- dictive value oflower-order factors (over and above neuroti-
lenge. The constructs of personality and mood overlap, and cism) have been limited.
the distinction between personality and depression is not an
absolute trait-state distinction (101). This is most evident in Numerous personality factors that show a substantial
the neuroticism factor, which has been described as a "dimen- relationship with depressive illness have been identified.
sion of stable and pervasive individual differences in mood These relationships are summarized (in a simplified manner)
and self concept" (102, P 465). A significant association in Table 2. The strongest research evidence pertaining to the
between a personality factor and a measure of depressive relationship between personality and depression is available
symptoms suggests that the factor could reflect a predisposi- for neuroticism. Several lower-order personality factors are
tion for depression (103). Excessive overlap between a per- directly connected with clinical formulations ofdepression in
sonality measure and depression symptom measure, the form of "specific vulnerability hypotheses." These in-
however, indicates that the personality measure does not clude the hypothesized specific association between socio-
evaluate personality independently of current symptomatol- tropy or socially prescribed perfectionism and interpersonal
ogy (104). For clinicians, the most important implication is stresses or between autonomy or self-oriented perfectionism
that personality evaluations which are conducted during an and achievement stresses and the onset or exacerbation of
acute depressive episode should be considered with caution. depressive symptoms. These personality factors may be use-
ful to unify the understanding of depression occurring in
Another substantial limitation to the research literature on patients with a wide variety or combination of personality
personality and depression has been emphasized by Coyne disorders as conceptualized in the DSM-IV.
and Whiffen (83). These authors argue that the measured
personality dimensions may reflect current difficult life cir- Evidence is accumulating that measured personality vari-
cumstances in addition to the trait-like dispositions that they ables may be useful to guide clinicians in selecting treatment
are intended to tap. For example, involvement in a deteriorat- approaches. For example, highly sociotropic (dependent) in-
ing intimate relationship might result in endorsement of items dividuals may be treated most effectively in group settings,
reflecting dependency and recent events such as failures in whereas highly autonomous (self-critical) patients may do
282 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 42, No 3

better in individual therapy (81). Also, because of the asso- 14. Hirschfeld RMA, K/erman GL. Personality attributes and affective disorders. Am
J Psychiatry 1979;136:67-70.
ciations of self-criticism with depression severity (67) and 15. Hirschfeld RMA, Klerman GL, Clayton P, Keller M. Personality and depression:
perfectionism with suicidality (105), cognitive or other psy- ernpirical flndings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983;40:993-8.
16. Hirschfeld RMA, Klerman GL, Clayton, P, Keller M, McDonald-Scoll P, Larkin
chotherapeutic approaches that can effectively deal with BH. Assessing personality: effects of the depressive state on trait measurement.
Am J Psychiatry 1983;140:695-9.
self-critical and perfectionistic cognitions and personality
17. Hirschfeld RM, Klerman GL, Lavori P. Keller M, Griffith P, Coryell W. Premor-
styles may be particularly helpful. These examples serve to bid personality assessments of first onset of major depression. Arch Gen Psychia-
try 1989;46:345-50.
highlight the importance of these avenues of research for the
18. KerrTA, Schapira K, Roth M, Garside RF.The relationship between the Maudsley
clinical practice of treating depressive disorders. Personality Inventory and the course of affective disorders. Br J Psychiatry
1970;116:11-9.
19. Liebowitz MR, Stallone F, Dunner DL, Fieve RF. Personality features of patients
with primary affective disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1979;60:214-24.
Clinical Implications 20. Weissman MM, Prusoff BA, Klerman GL. Personality and the prediction of
long-term outcome of depression. Am J Psychiatry 1978;135:797-800.
• There is a complex interaction between dimensionsof person- 21. Gunderson JG, Treibwasser J, Phillips KA, BeginCN. Personalityand vulnerabil-
ality and depression. ity to affective disorders. In: Cloninger CR, editor. Personality and psychopathol-
• Several personalitydimensions have shown a substanti.al rela- ogy. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association Press. Forthcoming.
tionship to the features, course, and treatment responsiveness 22. Duggan C, Lee A, Murray R. Does personality predict long term outcome of
of major depression. depression? Br J Psychiatry 1990;157:19-24.
• Researchfindingsthat support"specific vulnerability hypothe- 23. Duggan C, Lee A, Murray R. Does recurrent depression lead to a change in
ses" are congruentwith some traditional dynamicformulations neuroticism? Psychol Med 1991;2t :985-90.
of depression. 24. Eccleston D, Scott J. Treatment, prediction of relapse and prognosis of chronic
primary major depression. International Clinical Psychopharmacology
Limitations 1991;6(2):3-10.
25. Scott J, Eccleston 0, Boys R. Can we predict the persistence of depression? Br J
• Personalityassessment in patients with depression is problem- Psychiatry 1992;161:633-7.
atic because of the significant effects that current (or past) 26. Taylor S, McLean P. Outcome profiles in the treatment of unipolar depression.
episodes of depressionand current difficult life circumstances Behav Res Ther 1993;31:325-30.
may have on personalitymeasures.
27. Scoll J, Williams JMG, Brittlebank A, Ferrier IN. The relationship between
• The numberof prospectivestudies that have assessed personal- premorbid neuroticism, cognitive dysfunction and persistence of depression. J
ity prior to the onset of major. depressionis limited. AfTect Disord 1995;33:167-72.
• The majority of research in this area has been based on self- 28. Maier W, Lichtermann 0, Minges J, Heun R. Personality traits in subjects at risk
report measures of personality. for unipolar major depression: a family study perspective. J Affect Disord
1992;24:153-64.
29. Duggan C, Sham P, Lee A, Minne C, Murray R. Neuroticism, a vulnerability
References maker for depression: evidence from a family study. J Affect Disord
1995;35:/39--43.
30. Coryell W, WinokurG, Maser10, Akiskal HS, Keller MB, Endicott J. Recurrently
I. Farmer R, Nelson-Grapy RO. Personality disorders and depression: hypothetical situational (reactive) depression: a study of course, phenomenology and familial
relations, empirica/findings and methodologicalconsiderations.Clinical Psychol- psychopathology. J AfTect Disord 1994;31:203-10.
ogy Review 1990;I0:453-76.
31. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. A longitudinal twin
2. Klein MH, Wo~derlich S, Shea MT. Models of relationships between personality
study of personality and major depression in women. Arch Gen Psychiatry
and depression. In: Klein MH, Kupfer DJ, Shea MT, editors. Personality and
1993;50:853-62.
depression: a current view. New York: Guilford; 1993. pi-54.
32. Leibowitz MR, Stallone F, Dunner DL, Fieve RF. Personality features ofpatients
3. Widiger TA, Frances A. The DSM-lII personality disorders, perspectives from
with primary affective disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1979;60:214-24.
psychology. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985;42:615-23.
33. Clark LA, Watson D. Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric
4. Frances A. The DSM-lII personality disorders section: a commentary. Am J
evidence and taxonomic implications. J Abnorm Psychol 1991;100:316-36.
Psychiatry 1980;1.37: I 05~.
34. Viken RJ, Rose RJ, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M. A developmental genetic analysis
5. Frances A. Categorical and dimensional systems of personality diagnosis: a
of adult personality: extraversion and neuroticism from 18 to 59 years of age. J
comparison. Compr Psychiatry 1982;23:516-27. Pers Soc Psychol 1994;66:722-30.
6. Akiskal H, Hirschfeld R, Yerevanian B. The relationship of personality to 35. Joffe RT, Levitt AJ, Bagby RM, Regan JJ. Clinical features of situational and
affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983;40:801-10.
non-situational major depression. Psychopathology 1993;26:138--44.
7. Streiner DL. Figuring out factors: the use and misuse of factor analysis. Can J 36. Benjaminsen S. Primary non-endogenous depression and features attributed to
Psychiatry 1994;39:135-40.
reactive depression. J Affect Disord 1981;3:245-59.
8. Clark LA. Watson 0, Mineka S. Temperament, personality and the mood and 37. Parker G, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Brodaty H, Boyce P, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, and
anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psycho/ 1994;103:103-16. others. Predicting the course of melancholic and non-melancholic depression. J
9. Eysenck HJ. Manual of the Maudsley Personality Inventory. London: University Nerv Ment Dis 1992;60:693-702.
of London Press; 1959. 38. Watson 0, Clark LA, Weber K, Assenheimer JS, Strauss ME, McCormick RA.
10. Eysenck HJ, Eysenck SB. Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. Lpndon: Testing a tripartite model: I and II. J Abnorm Psycho/ 1995;104:3-25.
University of London Press; 1964. 39. Cloninger CR. A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the
1I. Costa PT, McCrae RR. NEO PI professional manual. Odessa (FL): Psychological development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments 1986;3:167-226.
Assessment Resources; 1985. 40. Cloninger CR. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of
12. Von Zerssen 0, Pfister H, Koeller OM. The Munich Personality Test, a short personality variants. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987;44:573-88.
questionnaire for self-rating and relatives' rating of personality traits. European 41. Cloninger CR, PrzybeckTR, Svrakic OM. The Tridimensional Personality Ques-
Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Science 1988;238:73-93. tionnaire: U.S. normative data. Psychol Rep 1991;69:1047-57.
13. Frank E, Kupfer OJ, Jacob M, Jarrell D. Personality featuresand response to acute 42. Cloninger CR, Svrakic OM, PrzybeckTR. A psychobiological model of tempera-
treatment in recurrent depression. Journal of Personality Disorders 1987;I: 14-26. ment and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50:975-90.
April 1997 Personality Dimensions and Depression 283

43. Svrakic DM. Whitehead C, Przybeck TR, Cloninger CR. Differential diagnosis 71. Bagby RM. Cox BJ. Schuller DR. Levitt AJ. Swinson Rp. Joffe RT. Diagnostic
of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. specificity of the dependent and self-critical personality dimensions in major
Arch Gen PsychoI1993;50:991-9. depression. J Affect Disord 1992;26:59-<14.
44. Joffe RT. Bagby M. Levitt AJ. Regan JJ, Parker JD. The Tridimensional Person- 72. Kiloh LG. Garside RF. The independence of neurotic depression and endogenous
ality Questionnaire in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:959-<10. depression. BrJ Psychiatry 1963; 109:451-<13.
45. Joyce PR, Mulder RT. Cloninger CR. Temperament predicts clomipramine and 73. Klein DF. Endogenomorphic depression; a conceptual and terminological revi-
desipramine response in major depression. J Affect Disord 1994;30:35-46. sion. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974;31:447-54.
46. Nelson EC, Cloninger CR. The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire as a 74. Robins CJ. Block 1', Peselow ED. Relations of sociotropic and autonomous
predictor of response to nefazodone treatment of depression. J Affect Disord personality characteristics to specific symptoms in depressed patients. J Abnorm
1995;35:51-7. Psychol 1989;98:86-8.
47. Joyce PR, Mulder RT. Cloninger CR. Temperament and hypercortisolemia in 75. Moore RG. Blackburn 1M. The relationship of sociotropy and autonomy to
depression. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:195-8. symptoms. cognition and personality in depressed patients. J Affect Disord
1994;32:239-45.
48. Beck AT. Epstein N. Harrison RP. Emery G. Development of the sociotropy-
autonomy scales: a measure of personality factors in psychopathology. Philadel- 76. Clark DA, Beck AT. Personality factors in dysphoria: a psychometric refinement
phia: University of Pennsylvania; 1983. of Beck's sociotropy-autonomy scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behav-
ioural Assessment 1991; 13:369-88.
49. Hirschfeld RA. Klerman GL. Chodoff 1', Korchin S, Barrett J. Dependency.
self-esteem, clinical depression. J Am Acad Psychoanal 1976;4:373-88. 77. Robins CJ. Luten AG. Sociotropy and autonomy: differential patterns of clinical
presentation in unipolar depression. J Abnorm PsychoI1991;100:74-7.
50. Hirschfeld RA. Klerrnan GL, Gough HG. Barrett J, Korchin SJ, Chodoff P. A
measure of interpersonal dependency. J Pers Assess 1977;41:61(}-8. 78. Hammen C. Elliott A. Gitlin M. Jamison KR. Sociotropy/autonomy and vulner-
ability to specific life events in patients with unipolar depression and bipolar
51. Hirschfeld RMA. Klerman GL, Clayton PJ. Keller MB. Andreasen NC. Person- disorders. J Abnonn Psychol 1989;98: 154-<10.
ality and gender-related differences in depression. J Affect Disord
1984;7:211-21. 79. Robins CJ. Congruence of personality and life events in depression. J Abnorm
Psycho I 1990;99:393-7.
52. Hirschfeld RMA, Klerman GL. Andreasen NC. Clayton PJ. Keller MB. Psycho-
social predictors of chronicity in depressed patients. Br J Psychiatry 80. Peselow ED, R?bins. CJ. San~lpo MI'. Block P. Fieve RR. Sociotropy and
1986; 148:648-54. autonomy: relationship to antidepressant drug treatment response and endo-
genous-nonendogenous dichotomy. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 10 I :479-86.
53. Rohde 1', Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR. Are people changed by the experience of
having an episode of depression? a further test of the scar hypothesis. J Abnorm 81. Zettle RD. Herring EL. Treatment utility of the sociotropy/autonomy distinction:
Psycho I 1990;99;264-71. implications for cognitive therapy. J Clin Psychol 1995;51 :28(}-9.

54. Reich J. Noyes R. Hirschfeld R. Coryell W. O'Gormon T. State and personality 82. Zettl~ RD. Hallich JL. Reynolds ~A. Responsivity to cognitive therapy as a
in depressed and panic patients. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:181-7. function of treatment format and client personality dimensions. J Clin Psychol
1992;48:787-97.
55. Boyce P, Parker G. llickie I, Wilhelm K. Brodaty H, Mitchell P. Personality
differences between patients with melancholic and non-melancholic depression. 83. Coyne JC, ~hiffen YE. Issues in personality as a diathesis for depression: the
Am J Psychiatry 1990;147:1476-83. case of soclotropy-dependency and autonomy-self criticism. PsychoI Bull
1995;118:358-78,
56. Grove WM, Andreasen NC, Young M. Endicott J, Keller MB, Hirschfeld RM,
and others. Isolation and characterization of a nuclear depressive syndrome. 84. Y~n Zerssen D. Pre morbid personality and affective psychoses. In: Burrows GD.
Psychol Med 1987;17:471-84. editor. Handbook of studies on depression. Amsterdam: Exerpta Medica; 1977.
p 79-103. -
57. Birtchnell J. The measurement of dependence by questionnaire. Journal of Per-
sonality Disorders 1991;5:281-95. 85. Cooper J. The Leyton Obsessional Inventory. Psychol Med 1970; I:48-<14.
58. Boyce P, Parker G. Development of a scale to measure interpersonal sensitivity. 86. Lazare A. Klerman GL, Armor DJ. Oral, obsessive and hysterical personality
Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1989;23:341-51. patterns; replication of factor analysis in an independent sample, J Psychiatr Res
1970;7;275-90.
59. Boyce P. Parker G, Barrett B. Cooney M, Smith F. Personality as a vulnerability
factor to depression. BrJ Psychiatry 1991;159:106-14. 87. Morrow J. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Effects of responses to depression on the reme-
diation of depressive affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 1990;58:519-27.
60. Boyce P, Hickle F. Parker G. Parents. partners or personality: risk factors for
post-natal depression. J Affect Disord 1991;21:245-55. 88. Lyubomirsky S. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric
rumination. J Pers Soc Psychol 1993;65:339-49.
61. Boyce P, Hickle I. Parker G. Mitchell P, Wilhelm K. Brodaty H. Specificity of
interpersonal sensitivity to non-melancholic depression. J Affect Disord 89. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Parker LE. Larson J. Ruminative coping with depressed
1993;27: 101-5. mood following loss. J Pers Soc Psycho I 1994;67:92-104.
62. Boyce P, Hickie I, Parker G. Mitchell P, Wilhelm K. Brodaty H. Interpersonal 90. Beck AT. Depression; clinical. experimental and theoretical aspects. New York:
sensitivity and the one-year outcome of a depressive episode. Aust N Z J Harper & Row; 1967.
Psychiatry 1992;26:156-61. 91. Bibring E. The mechanism of depression. In: Greenacre P. editor. Affective
63. Blatt SJ. Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjeetive depression. disorders. New York: International Universities Press; 1953. p 13-48.
Psychoanal Study Child 1974;24: 107-57. 92. Frost RO. Marten P, Lahart C. Robenblate K. The dimensions of perfectionism.
64. Beck AT. Cognitive therapy of depression: new perspectives. In: Clayton PJ. Cognitive Therapy and Research 1990; 14:449-68.
Barrett JE. editors. Treatment of depression: old controversies and new ap- 93. Hewitt PL. Flett GL. Dimensions of perfectionism and depression; a multidimen-
proaches. New York: Raven; 1983. I' 265-90. sional analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 1990;5:423-38.
65. Mongrain M. ZuroffDC. Cognitive vulnerability to depressed affect in dependent 94. Enns MW. Cox BJ, Guertin J. Ledohowski L. Perfectionism in major depression;
and self-critical college women. Journal of personality Disorders 1989;3:24(}-51. a comparison of multidimensional measures [abstract]. In: Association for Ad-
66. Blatt SJ. D'Affiitti Jp. Quinlan DM. Depressive Experiences Questionnaire. New vancement of Behavior Therapy 30th Annual Meeting; 1996 Nov 21-24;
Haven (CT): Yale University Press; 1976. New York. NY.
67. Klein ON. Harding K. Taylor EB, Dickstein S. Dependency and self-criticism in 95. Hewill PL. Flett GL. Dimensions of perfectionism in unipolar depression. J
depression: evaluation in a clinical population. J Abnorm Psyehol 1988; Abnorm PsychoI1991;100:98-101.
97:399-404. 96. Hewitt PL. Flett GL. Turnbull-Donovan W. Perfectionism and suicide potential,
68. Franche R. Dobson K. Selfcriticism and interpersonal dependency as vulnerability Br J Clin Psychol 1992;31: 181-90.
factors to depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 1992;16:419-35. 97. Hewitt PL. Flett GL. Weber C. Dimensions of perfectionism and suicide ideation.
69. Bagby RM. Schuller DR. Parker JD, Levitt A. Joffe RT. Shafir MS. Major Cognitive Therapy and Research 1994; 18:439-60.
depression and the self-criticism and dependency personality dimensions. Am J 98. Hewitt PL. Flett GL. Dimensions of perfectionism. daily stress and depression: a
Psychiatry 1994; 151:597-9. test of the specific vulnerability hypothesis. J Abnonn Psycho I 1993;102:58-<15,
70. Blatt SJ. Quinlan DM, Chevron E. McDonald C, Zuroff D. Dependency and 99. Blatt SJ. Quinlan OM. Pilkonis PA. Shea MT. Impact of perfectionism and need
self-criticism: psychological dimensions of depression. J Consult Clin Psychol for approval on the brief treatment of depression. J Consult Clln Psychol
1982;50: 113-24. 1995;63:125-32.
284 The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry Vol 42, No 3

100. Hyler 5, Skoda I A, Kellman H, Oldham J, Rosnick L. Validity of the Personality 103. Nietzel MT, Harris MJ. Relationship of dependency and achievement/autonomy
Diagnostic Questionnaire-Revised: comparison with structured interviews. Am J to depression. elin Psycho I Rev 1990;20:279-97.
Psychiatry 1990; I 47: I 043-8.

101. WidigerTA. Personality and depression: assessment issues. In: Klein MH, Kupfer 104. Whiffen VE, Sasserville TM. Dependency, self-criticism and recollections of
OJ, Shea MT, editors. Personality and depression: a current view. New York: parenting: sex ditTerences and the role of depressive affect. J Soc Clin Psycho I
Guilford; 1993. p 77-1 18.
1991;10:121-33.
102. Watson 0, Clark L. Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive
emotional states. Psycho I Bull 1984;96:465-90. 105. Blatt SJ. The destructiveness of perfectionism. Am Psychol 1995; I 2: I 003-20.

Resume
Objectifs : La relation entre la personnalite, dont on a evalue les dimensions. et le debut, les caracteristiques et
l'evolution de la depression fera l'objet d'un examen critique. et elle sera etudiee l 'egard de 4 modeles a
hypothetiques " predisposition ou vulnerabilite. pathophysiologie, complication ou cicatrice et spectre ou con-
tinuite.
Methode: On examinera des etudes portant sur des patients adultes deprimes cliniquement et visant explorer a
la relation entre les dimensions de la personnalite et la depression.
Resultats : Les facteurs de personnalite d 'ordre superieur. qui ont revele un lien significatifet constant avec les
depressions majeures, comprennent les traits nevrotiques, I 'extroversion (relation negative) et les facteurs du
modele de personnalite tridimensionnelle de Cloninger. Les traits nevrotiques semblent constituer Ie predicteur
Ie plus puissant de la depression. Les facteurs d'ordre inferieur, revelant un relation significative et constante
avec la depression, comprennent la dependance, l'autocritique. l'obsessionnalite et le perfectionnisme. Les liens
entre la depression, la dependance et l'autocritique comportent le fondement empirique Ie plus solide.
Conclusions: Plusieurs dimensions de la personnalite sont liees de facon significative a la depression, mais il y
a peu de donnees revelant sans equivoque une veritable predisposition de la personnalite la depression. Les a
mesures de la personnalite pourraient devenir cliniquement utiles dans Ie choix du traitement.In translation

You might also like