You are on page 1of 10

European Journal of Dental Education ISSN 1396-5883

Psychometric properties of the Greek version of the Toronto


Composite Empathy Scale in Greek dental students
D. Tsiantou1, D. Lazaridou1, T. Coolidge2, K. N. Arapostathis1 and N. Kotsanos1
1
Department of Paediatric Dentistry, Faculty of Dentistry, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece,
2
Department of Oral Health Sciences, School of Dentistry, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, USA

Keywords Abstract
empathy; dental students; education research;
psychometrics. Introduction: Empathy levels of health practitioners are related to patient satisfaction
and treatment outcomes. The Toronto Composite Empathy Scale (TCES) was recently
Correspondence developed to assess cognitive and emotional empathy levels in both professional and
Dimitra Tsiantou personal spheres, and tested in an English-speaking sample of dental students. The aim
Department of Paediatric Dentistry of this study was to examine the psychometrics of the Greek version of the TCES.
Faculty of Dentistry, Aristotle University of
Thessaloniki
Materials and Methods: The TCES was translated into Greek and administered to
Thessaloniki 54124 , Greece
all of the dental students at Aristotle University of Thessaloniki. A random subset of
Tel: +306973052955
Fax: +302310999582
students completed the questionnaire twice for test–retest analysis.
e-mail: tsiantoudim@yahoo.gr
Results: Nearly all (96.5%) of the students completed the questionnaire. The internal
Accepted: 29 January 2013 consistencies of each of the four subscales were generally acceptable (Cronbach’s
alphas: 0.676–0.805), and the scale showed good discriminant and convergent validities
doi: 10.1111/eje.12037
(r’s for discriminant validity: 0.217 and 0.103; r’s for convergent validity: 0.595 and
0.700). Test–retest reliabilities ranged from 0.478 to 0.779. After eliminating items that
fell on both cognitive and emotional factors, a rotated factor analysis indicated that
the items loaded on two cognitive and three emotional factors.

Discussion: Our results indicate that the Greek version of the TCES has good psycho-
metric properties. The factor analysis indicates that the emotional and cognitive aspects
of empathy are distinct, supporting the need to address both aspects in studies of
empathy.

Conclusions: The Greek version of the TCES is a reliable and valid tool for the mea-
surement of cognitive and emotional empathy, in both professional and personal life,
in Greek dental students.

communication abilities are considered to be central to the suc-


Introduction cess of healthcare providers as they operate within a particular
Empathy has been defined as the ability to ‘perceive the internal type of social relationship, namely the doctor–patient relation-
frame of reference of another with accuracy as if one were the ship (5). In this relationship, healthcare providers must be able
other person but without ever losing the ‘as if’ condition’ (1; p. to focus on patients, listen carefully to their medical history
210). This ability allows humans to recognise and understand and understand exactly how they possibly feel (6). The impor-
the internal thoughts and feelings of others, facilitating social tance of these skills in dentistry is highlighted by their inclusion
relationships in general (2–4). Whilst scientific advances con- in the set of competencies required for dentists practising in
tinue to improve our understanding of disease (including the European Union (7) and other areas, such as the United
detection, prevention and treatment), empathy and associated States (8).

208 ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Tsiantou et al. Greek Version of TCES

Greater levels of empathy in physicians result in more questionnaire was to incorporate the cognitive and emotional
favourable treatment outcomes (9, 10), better adherence to dimensions of empathy, in both personal and professional life.
treatment (11) and closer monitoring of the patient (12). In This scale combined questions from four scales related to
dentistry, empathy has been correlated with greater satisfaction empathy: JSPE-HP (45), Interpersonal Reactivity Index (19),
and decreased dental fear (13), greater levels of cooperation in E-Scale (50) and the short form of the Empathy Quotient (51).
children (14), more satisfied patients in aesthetic dentistry (15) The TCES consists of 52 questions, 26 for the professional set-
and prosthodontics (16), better compliance in orthodontics ting and another 26 for personal life, equally divided into cog-
(17) and better treatment outcomes in myofascial pain (18). nitive and emotional empathy in each case. This results in four
Whilst a number of definitions or descriptions of empathy subscales consisting of 13 items each: the Personal Cognitive
have been suggested, Davis points out that some have argued subscale, the Personal Emotional subscale, the Professional
that empathy is primarily cognitive (involving accuracy in Cognitive subscale and the Professional Emotional subscale.
understanding someone else’s mental state through perspective- The personal and professional subscales ask the respondent to
taking) and others have argued that it is primarily emotional rate how often he/she responds in certain ways in situations
or affective (involving physiological responses in the observer, involving friends or people in general (personal subscales), or
such as experiencing the same emotion that the other is feeling) patients (professional subscales). For example, an item in the
(19). Still others consider empathy to include both cognitive Personal Emotional subscale reads: ‘I tend to get emotionally
and emotional aspects, arguing that the ability to understand involved with a friend’s problems’, whilst the corresponding
another’s perspective requires an emotional response to observ- item in the Professional Emotional subscale reads: ‘I tend to
ing the other, whilst merely experiencing the same emotion as get emotionally involved with my patient’s problems’. Similarly,
the other can result in emotional contagion, rather than empa- an item in the Personal Cognitive subscale reads: ‘I try to think
thy. Evidence supporting the view that there are cognitive and like my friends in order to help them more’, whilst the corre-
emotional aspects of empathy comes from a variety of fields. sponding item in the Professional Cognitive subscale reads: ‘I
For example, some forms of psychopathology are associated try to think like my patients in order to help them more’. Each
with deficits in cognitive empathy (e.g. autism and bipolar dis- item is answered on a 6-point scale, where 1 = ‘At no time’
order), whilst others are associated with deficits in emotional and 6 = ‘All the time’. Possible scores for each subscale range
empathy (e.g. alcoholism and schizophrenia) (20, 21). In addi- from 13 to 78, where higher scores refer to greater levels of
tion, individuals without psychiatric problems but who have empathy. Yarascavitch et al. found that the TCES showed
greater emotional empathy are more successful on experimental acceptable internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha values of
tasks that require emotional understanding, whilst those with 0.759 for the Personal Cognitive subscale, 0.765 for Personal
greater cognitive empathy are more successful on tasks requir- Emotional, 0.814 for Professional Cognitive and 0.768 for Pro-
ing cognitive understanding (22). Third, developmental evi- fessional Emotional. The authors did not report whether the
dence indicates that emotional empathy begins to develop in alphas would be affected by deleting any item. The team also
the child earlier than cognitive empathy (23). Finally, a growing found evidence for convergent validity, measured by higher
body of literature indicates that individual differences in emo- correlations between the two cognitive subscales (r = 0.646)
tional and cognitive empathies are associated with differences and the two emotional subscales (r = 0.691), and discriminant
in brain structures; furthermore, in addition to brain regions validity, measured by lower correlations between the two per-
that appear to be involved in all types of tasks requiring empa- sonal subscales (r = 0.190) and the two professional scales
thy, different specific regions are associated with tasks involving (r = 0.066). However, the authors did not report the scale’s
emotional vs. cognitive empathies (20, 24, 25). test–retest reliability, nor did they conduct a factor analysis of
Researchers have assessed psychometrically strong measure- the items to examine whether they performed as expected (49).
ment tools for empathy, including those appropriate for medi- Finally, although other authors have typically found that
cal students (26–35), pharmacy students (36), nursing students females score higher on empathy than males (30, 42), Yarascav-
(35, 37, 38) and physicians (27, 29, 35, 39–41). Most of the itch et al. (49) did not report whether gender was related to
studies in health profession students (27, 29, 34–37, 42–44) scores on the TCES.
measure empathy using the same self-report scale: the Jefferson Whilst Greek dental patients who were interviewed about
Scale of Physician Empathy Health Professionals [JSPE-HP their expectations and perceptions towards dental care stated
(45)]. This scale has been shown to have good psychometric that dentists’ empathy was foremost in their expectations (52),
properties in dental students (42); however, it does not clearly to date studies in Greece have not assessed the empathy levels
identify emotional and cognitive aspects of empathy. To pro- of dentists. Whilst other Greek studies have used scales for
vide the emotional context of empathy, some studies with describing the empathic style and caring behaviour of some
health professionals (33, 46–48) have measured empathy in health professionals [nurses and physicians; (52–55)], these
professional life with emotional intelligence tools. Furthermore, have not strictly measured the levels of healthcare practitioners’
the JSPE-HP measures students’ empathy in professional set- empathy.
tings only and thus does not refer to students’ empathy in their It seems therefore that, although there are valid self-report
personal lives. The latter may be important in contributing to scales in English, there is no empathy scale with established
understanding whether students’ empathies are trait-like, or psychometric properties for Greek dental students or dentists.
differ in professional and personal situations. Thus, developing and testing the Greek version of the TCES in
Yarascavitch et al. (49) designed a new questionnaire, the dental students would be useful. The aim of this study is to
Toronto Composite Empathy Scale (TCES). The purpose of the report on the development and psychometric properties of the

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 209
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Greek Version of TCES Tsiantou et al.

Greek version of TCES in dental students, including its internal The questionnaires were distributed to all students of the
consistency, test–retest reliability and construct validity. In Dental School, AUTH, over a 2-month period between October
addition, we report on gender differences found on the mea- and November 2010. The questionnaires were administered at a
sure. predetermined time and in the classrooms in the Dental
School. Students had been given 30 min at the beginning of
tutorial courses to answer the questionnaires without any time
Materials and methods
pressure or postponement of their training. The questionnaires
The study took place in the Faculty of Dentistry, Aristotle Uni- were collected at the same time. To check the test–retest
versity of Thessaloniki (AUTH), and was approved by the Insti- reliability, the questionnaire was given again for a second time
tutional Review Board at Aristotle University, Greece (Code No (2–4 weeks interval) to a random sample of the 25% of all par-
215/29-9-2010). Faculty of Dentistry, AUTH, is one of the two ticipants in each training year. After the initial and the retest
dental schools in Greece, enrolling about 500 dental students, questionnaires were matched, all personal data were erased.
almost equally divided over the five training years. All under- Questionnaires were coded and data were entered into an
graduate dental students were judged capable to participate in Excel file and checked for accuracy. Analyses were carried out
the study (whole sample population). Nineteen additional stu- with SPSS version 16.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL,
dents, selected randomly from a pool that had completed the USA). Participants who omitted 5% or more of the TCES items
fifth year assignments after the deadline for the regular gradua- were excluded. Amongst those who were retained for the analy-
tion date and were waiting for the next graduation date, partic- ses, chi-square test was used to compare students who had
ipated in the pilot study. completed the TCES and those who had not on gender and
The English version of the TCES was translated into the Greek year of study, whilst Mann–Whitney test was used to compare
language. The modified guidelines of the American Association the age of these two subgroups. In addition to descriptive sta-
of Orthopedic Surgeons (AAOS) to health and quality of life tistics, t-tests were used to examine gender differences on the
(56) were followed in order to translate the TCES. The AAOS empathy scores, whilst Pearson’s r was used to determine how
guidelines involve the translation and cultural adaptation of the similar the personal and professional dimensions were in both
questionnaire and include six stages. According to these steps, the cognitive and emotional subscales. Cronbach’s alpha was
two bilingual researchers independently translated the scale from used to measure internal consistency, and Pearson’s r was used
English to Greek (forward translation). Second, both transla- for examining the test–retest reliability and convergent and dis-
tions were synthesised to create a uniform version. The new criminant validities, as the distributions of the TCES scores
translation was independently translated back into English by were normal. Factor analysis using varimax rotation was used
two new translators who were unaware of the original version to examine the factor structure of the TCES.
(back translation). In the fourth stage, all translators together
came to a consensus about the pre-final version of the question-
Results
naire that would be used in the pilot study. Item 15 [‘When I
see a hurt stranger/patient, I tend to remain calm’ (reversed)] is The questionnaire was distributed to all of the 490 students of
ambiguous with regard to whether the situation refers to is an all training years who were present during the tutorial courses
emotional or physical one. The back translation revealed that in October/November 2010, and 469 students agreed to partici-
the original Greek translation was construed to be ‘wounded’, pate in the study (response rate 96.5%). Nearly all of the stu-
which is more likely to refer to a physical experience. Therefore, dents completed the entire set of questionnaires (including the
the Greek wording was changed to ‘When I see a stranger/ TCES) in 10–12 min, and all students finished in less than
patient in pain…’. The pre-final version was then piloted with 20 min. Nine of the 469 students omitted 5% or more of the
the 19 dental students who had completed training but not yet TCES items and were therefore excluded. Amongst the 460
graduated. The piloting revealed that items 25 [‘I find myself retained, the distribution in training years was almost equally
unaffected by other people’s/my patients’ misfortunes’ divided (from the first to the fifth year, there were 82, 82, 99,
(reversed)] and 26 [‘I find it hard to feel sorry for other people/ 93 and 104 participants, respectively). The mean age of the par-
my patients when they are having problems’ (reversed)] were ticipants was 20.70 years (SD = 2.56, median = 20.00,
somewhat confusing. On the other hand, slightly simplified revi- range = 18–45), and 65.2% were female. Of the 460 who were
sions (‘I am not affected by…’ and ‘It is hard for me to feel retained, 405 completed the TCES and 55 omitted up to 5% of
sorry…’) were more acceptable, and therefore, these revised the items. Neither gender nor training year was related to TCES
items were used in the final questionnaire. (The Greek version completion. However, those who completed the TCES were
of the TCES is available from the first author on request.) older than those who did not (median ages 20.50 vs. 20.00;
The final form of the questionnaire consisted of two sections. Mann–Whitney U-test = 9156.000, P = 0.036).
The first one was a cover letter aimed to inform students of Overall, the mean score on the Personal Cognitive subscale
their voluntary participation in the study; they were asked to was 56.13 (SD = 6.85), the mean score on the Personal Emo-
provide their names (to allow comparison of the TCES scores tional subscale was 47.60 (SD = 8.67), whilst the means and
for test–retest purposes), gender and similar personal data, SDs on the Professional Cognitive and Emotional subscales
highlighting that their personal data were confidential and were 57.31 (7.71) and 40.13 (9.56), respectively. The means and
accessible only by the research team. The second section was standard deviations of the four subscales by gender are shown
the Greek version of the TCES and other questionnaires not in Table 1. Females had significantly higher empathy scores on
reported on here. the Personal Cognitive, Personal Emotional and Professional

210 ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Tsiantou et al. Greek Version of TCES

Emotional subscales. For males, the correlation for the Personal to the alphas obtained in our sample revealed that eliminating
and Professional Cognitive subscales was 0.681 (P < 0.001), any item in the Personal Emotional subscale would decrease this
and the correlation for the Personal and Professional Emotional value. The alpha of the Professional Emotional subscale would
subscales was 0.739 (P < 0.001). For females, the corresponding increase from 0.805 to 0.811 if one item was removed [Profes-
correlation coefficients were 0.542 and 0.675 (both P’s <0.001). sional item 25: ‘I am not affected by my patient’s misfortunes’
To check the test–retest reliability, the questionnaire was ran- (reversed)]. The alpha of the Personal Cognitive subscale would
domly administered to 115 students (25%) 3 weeks (1 week) increase from 0.676 to 0.683 if one item was removed (Personal
after the first administration. Of the 115, 110 agreed to com- item 23: ‘It is hard for me to see why some things upset people
plete the questionnaire a second time (response rate 95.65%). so much’); removing the corresponding item from the Profes-
One student omitted more than 5% of the TCES items in the sional Cognitive subscale (Professional item 23: ‘It is hard for
second administration and was excluded, leaving 109 who com- me to see why some things upset patients so much’) would
pleted the questionnaire both times. As seen in Table 2, all four increase the subscale’s alpha from 0.759 to 0.761. Significant
empathy subscale scores in the test (time 1) and retest distribu- positive correlations were found between the two cognitive
tion (time 2) were significantly correlated. (r = 0.595, P < 0.001) and the two emotional (r = 0.700,
The internal consistencies of each of the four subscales were P < 0.001) subscales, as well as between the two personal
generally acceptable, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.676 (r = 0.127, P < 0.001) and the two professional (r = 0.103,
to 0.805. Examination of the importance of the individual items P = 0.028) subscales (Table 3).
Initial inspections of the data indicated that they were appro-
priate for factor analysis [i.e. there were correlations >0.3 in
TABLE 1. Means and standard deviations of the Greek version of Toronto the correlation matrix, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure
Composite Empathy Scale (TCES) by gender of sampling adequacy was 0.823, and the value for the Bartlett’s
test of sphericity was significant (approximate chi-square
Males mean Females mean test = 7747.061, df = 1326, P < 0.001)]. The unrotated factor
Subscale (SD) (SD) t analysis indicated that there were 15 components with eigen-
Personal cognitive 54.90 (7.35) 56.79 (6.48) 2.842**
values >1.0. The scree plot was examined in order to reduce
Personal emotional 43.85 (8.60) 49.60 (8.04) 7.126*** the number of potential factors. As a result, five components,
Professional cognitive 56.73 (7.77) 57.62 (7.67) 1.182 which together accounted for 40.00% of the variance, were
Professional emotional 38.16 (9.04) 41.18 (9.67) 3.255** selected for the rotated analysis. Table 4 contains the factor
loadings for each item on the five factors; loadings >0.3 are
SD, standard deviation. shown in boldface. The table also indicates whether the item
df = 458. was taken from the cognitive or emotional subscales. A visual
*P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001. comparison of the personal and professional sections indicates
that almost all pairs of items loaded on the same scale(s) in
both sections of the questionnaire. (For example, item #1
TABLE 2. Test–retest reliability of the Greek version of Toronto Composite
Empathy Scale (TCES)
loaded on Factor 2 in both sections.) Across the TCES, all but
one of the emotional items loaded on Factors 1 and/or 5 (four
Time 1 mean Time 2 mean items loaded on both factors), whilst all of the cognitive items
Subscale (SD) (SD) r loaded on Factors 2, 3 and/or 4 (two items loaded on two of
the factors). The single emotional item that did not load at 0.3
Personal cognitive 56.13 (6.85) 56.39 (7.39) 0.669***
or higher on any factor loaded 0.298 on both Factor 1 and Fac-
Personal emotional 47.60 (8.67) 45.79 (9.27) 0.779***
tor 5. Six of the emotional items (Personal items 18, 25 and
Professional cognitive 57.31 (7.71) 56.70 (7.45) 0.478***
26; Professional items 16, 18 and 26) also loaded positively on
Professional emotional 40.13 (9.56) 40.03 (9.84) 0.657***
Factors 2, 3 or 4, and two of the cognitive items (Personal and
SD, standard deviation. Professional items 19) also loaded positively on Factor 1. One
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. cognitive item also had a high negative (> 0.3) loading on an

TABLE 3. Internal reliability and construct validity of the Greek version of Toronto Composite Empathy Scale (TCES)

Subscale Personal cognitive Personal emotional Professional cognitive Professional emotional


1
Personal cognitive 0.676
Personal emotional 0.2172*** 0.7601
Professional cognitive 0.5953*** 0.7591
Professional emotional 0.700 ***
3
0.1032* 0.8051
1
Cronbach’s alpha.
2
Discriminant validity.
3
Convergent validity.
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 211
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Greek Version of TCES Tsiantou et al.

TABLE 4. First rotated factor analysis of the Greek version of the Toronto Composite Empathy Scale (TCES)

Factors

Items I II III IV V Scale assignment

Personal items
1 0.020 0.515 0.074 0.039 0.240 Cognitive
2 0.068 0.199 0.458 0.221 0.292 Cognitive
3 0.061 0.079 0.071 0.477 0.198 Cognitive
4 0.245 0.389 0.273 0.073 0.327 Cognitive
5 0.464 0.218 0.112 0.050 0.139 Emotional
6 0.067 0.021 0.639 0.020 0.185 Cognitive
7 0.206 0.161 0.223 0.230 0.576 Emotional
8 0.154 0.028 0.019 0.350 0.152 Cognitive
9 0.162 0.023 0.181 0.630 0.079 Cognitive
10 0.516 0.078 0.035 0.116 0.129 Emotional
11 0.158 0.512 0.021 0.101 0.087 Cognitive
12 0.440 0.196 0.077 0.003 0.207 Emotional
13 0.141 0.159 0.034 0.186 0.656 Emotional
14 0.282 0.465 0.098 0.144 0.118 Cognitive
15 0.497 0.136 0.078 0.258 0.024 Emotional
16 0.305 0.269 0.047 0.062 0.282 Emotional
17 0.046 0.020 0.717 0.143 0.098 Cognitive
18 0.362 0.211 0.379 0.132 0.073 Emotional
19 0.399 0.453 0.140 0.171 0.022 Cognitive
20 0.654 0.172 0.108 0.082 0.260 Emotional
21 0.329 0.087 0.025 0.093 0.544 Emotional
22 0.133 0.137 0.550 0.036 0.016 Cognitive
23 0.015 0.031 0.080 0.512 0.196 Cognitive
24 0.298 0.055 0.182 0.267 0.298 Emotional
25 0.379 0.013 0.002 0.477 0.128 Emotional
26 0.341 0.082 0.105 0.454 0.077 Emotional
Professional items
1 0.050 0.630 0.215 0.014 0.052 Cognitive
2 0.068 0.322 0.515 0.031 0.072 Cognitive
3 0.168 0.083 0.116 0.577 0.012 Cognitive
4 0.038 0.643 0.254 0.174 0.123 Cognitive
5 0.517 0.241 0.110 0.131 0.172 Emotional
6 0.069 0.097 0.697 0.066 0.087 Cognitive
7 0.322 0.213 0.381 0.201 0.439 Emotional
8 0.111 0.095 0.029 0.521 0.009 Cognitive
9 0.073 0.087 0.323 0.550 0.114 Cognitive
10 0.648 0.027 0.054 0.120 0.188 Emotional
11 0.099 0.667 0.066 0.081 0.079 Cognitive
12 0.555 0.221 0.043 0.029 0.144 Emotional
13 0.368 0.070 0.116 0.346 0.497 Emotional
14 0.026 0.738 0.179 0.186 0.183 Cognitive
15 0.487 0.172 0.251 0.118 0.026 Emotional
16 0.470 0.327 0.082 0.169 0.082 Emotional
17 0.137 0.099 0.691 0.103 0.027 Cognitive
18 0.426 0.324 0.245 0.006 0.120 Emotional
19 0.427 0.528 0.041 0.056 0.038 Cognitive
20 0.724 0.112 0.063 0.045 0.236 Emotional
21 0.505 0.029 0.060 0.176 0.474 Emotional
22 0.012 0.152 0.567 0.104 0.001 Cognitive
23 0.223 0.111 0.009 0.599 0.096 Cognitive
24 0.404 0.054 0.107 0.392 0.027 Emotional
25 0.359 0.108 0.050 0.297 0.037 Emotional
26 0.351 0.152 0.047 0.455 0.052 Emotional
Variance accounted for 11.32% 8.09% 7.72% 7.58% 5.29%

Values in bold face are greater than ± 0.3.

212 ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Tsiantou et al. Greek Version of TCES

emotional factor, whilst 3 emotional items had high negative Whilst the internal consistency analyses yielded values that
(> 0.3) loadings on a cognitive factor. were very similar to those reported by Yarascavitch et al. (49),
A second factor analysis was carried out after omitting the the highest value was 0.805. The cognitive subscales could be
eight items that had loaded positively on both emotional and improved somewhat by eliminating item 23, but our results do
cognitive factors. These data also showed suitability for factor not indicate that a single item would improve both of the emo-
analysis [i.e. correlations above 0.3 in the correlation matrix, a tional subscales if it was removed. To date, ours is the only
KMO value of 0.809 and a significant value for Bartlett’s test of sample that explored item deletion as a method to improve the
sphericity (approximate chi-square test = 6119.890, df = 946, alpha values, and it would be useful to administer the scale to
P < 0.001)]. Twelve components with eigenvalues >1.0 were other samples to look for general patterns of item performance.
identified in the unrotated analysis, whilst the scree plot indi- Especially as the TCES has only been tested in two samples to
cated that five of these were optimal for inclusion. Together, date, it would also be desirable to include other measures
these five factors accounted for 41.63% of the variance. The designed to assess convergent and/or divergent validity more
results of the rotated analysis are presented in Table 5. All of fully. Further, it might be fruitful to compare the measure with
the emotional items loaded on Factors 1 and/or 5 (four items judges’ ratings of the participants’ empathic behaviours and
loaded on both), and all of the cognitive items loaded on Fac- communications, as a way of assessing criterion validity.
tors 2, 3 and/or 4 (2 items loaded on two of these). There were Whilst we found that every item selected by Yarascavitch
no emotional items that loaded positively on Factors 2, 3 and/ et al. (49) to be an emotional item fell on at least one of the
or 4, nor were there any cognitive items that loaded positively two emotional factors (with the exception of item 24, which
on Factors 1 and/or 5. There were 3 emotional items that also loaded at 0.298), and each item selected by that team to be a
loaded negatively on Factors 2, 3 or 4, whilst 4 cognitive items cognitive item fell on at least one of the three cognitive factors,
also loaded negatively on Factor 5. Factors 2 and 3 were several items loaded on both cognitive and emotional factors in
reversed in the second factor analysis (i.e. items that had our original analysis. These items differed from those identified
loaded on Factor 2 in the initial analysis now comprised Factor in the internal consistency analyses as possible contenders to be
3, and vice versa). According to the item loadings of the second omitted to increase the alpha values. It is also of interest that
analysis, Factor 1 appears to focus on emotional involvement we found two emotional and three cognitive factors, rather
in general (sample items: ‘I tend to get emotionally involved than one of each type. This may be related to the fact that the
with a friend’s/my patient’s problems’), whilst Factor 5 focuses scale’s items were drawn from a variety of sources. Additional
on helplessness and emergencies [sample item: ‘In emergency factor analyses (including confirmatory factor analysis) would
situations I feel calm and composed (reversed)’]. Factor 2 need to be conducted in other samples to determine whether
focuses on using visual cues to determine the thoughts and our findings that some items loaded on both emotional and
feelings of others (sample items: ‘I notice if someone/a patient cognitive factors and that there are several emotional and cog-
appears interested or bored with what I’m saying’), Factor 3 nitive factors within the scale are related to our sample in some
centres around taking the cognitive perspective of others (sam- way, or are related to the scale itself. In general, the second fac-
ple items: ‘I try to think like my friends/patients in order to tor analysis indicates that the emotional and cognitive aspects
help them more’), whilst Factor 4 taps judging and understand- of empathy are distinct, supporting the need to address both
ing the thoughts and feelings of others [sample items: ‘When aspects in studies of empathy.
someone/a patient is offended by a remark, I find it hard to As is typical in research on empathy (30, 42), we found
understand why’ (reversed)]. that females scored higher than males did on three of the
four subscales. This is likely to be reflecting the fact that the
TCES items were based on those found in other empathy
Discussion scales (i.e. JSPE-HP (45), Interpersonal Reactivity Index (19),
Our results indicate that the Greek version of the TCES has E-Scale (50) and the short form of the Empathy Quotient
good psychometric properties. All subscales demonstrated (51), and therefore, they would be answered in similar ways
acceptable or good internal consistency, with alphas very simi- as participants rate the items or similar items on other empa-
lar to those reported by Yarascavitch et al. (49). The high cor- thy scales.
relations found for the pairs of two cognitive and two Looking at the various subscales in more detail, we found
emotional subscales suggest good convergent validity. In addi- that the mean levels of personal and professional cognitive
tion, the lower correlations found for the two personal and the empathy were the same for males and females. However, for
two professional subscales suggest good discriminant validity. both genders, the levels of emotional empathy were higher on
We also found good test–retest reliability for the scale; to the personal subscales, compared with the professional sub-
our knowledge, this is the first time that test–retest reliability scales. This may be due to the relatively short period of time
has been measured with the TCES. Whilst still highly significant that dental students have been interacting professionally with
(P < 0.001), the test–retest coefficient for professional cognitive patients, compared with peers in their personal lives.
empathy was somewhat lower compared with the other three The males rated both their cognitive and emotional empa-
subscales. Future studies might elucidate whether this is related thies similarly in both the personal and professional domains
to the scale, or an aspect of our particular sample (e.g. the (r’s = 0.681 and 0.739). On the other hand, the females’ ratings
redistribution of the questionnaires coincided with the timing of their cognitive empathies in the two domains are somewhat
of semester examinations, which might have influenced the rat- lower than the corresponding ratings of their emotional empa-
ings of professional empathy). thies (r’s = 0.542 vs. 0.675). Because their mean scores are

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 213
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Greek Version of TCES Tsiantou et al.

TABLE 5. Second rotated factor analysis of the Greek version of the Toronto Composite Empathy Scale (TCES)

Factors

Items I II III IV V Scale assignment

Personal items
1 0.001 0.033 0.495 0.037 0.395 Cognitive
2 0.024 0.411 0.185 0.191 0.430 Cognitive
3 0.056 0.088 0.036 0.530 0.161 Cognitive
4 0.192 0.233 0.359 0.078 0.525 Cognitive
5 0.427 0.111 0.168 0.056 0.266 Emotional
6 0.032 0.644 0.045 0.006 0.248 Cognitive
7 0.383 0.201 0.106 0.207 0.491 Emotional
8 0.145 0.012 0.031 0.407 0.183 Cognitive
9 0.132 0.198 0.006 0.623 0.152 Cognitive
10 0.566 0.070 0.087 0.125 0.065 Emotional
11 0.154 0.029 0.490 0.145 0.015 Cognitive
12 0.458 0.059 0.242 0.054 0.089 Emotional
13 0.281 0.029 0.240 0.273 0.553 Emotional
14 0.284 0.059 0.440 0.121 0.313 Cognitive
15 0.489 0.100 0.128 0.271 0.060 Emotional
16 0.376 0.047 0.286 0.071 0.138 Emotional
17 0.004 0.721 0.050 0.129 0.038 Cognitive
20 0.699 0.080 0.207 0.029 0.042 Emotional
21 0.454 0.019 0.137 0.151 0.405 Emotional
22 0.111 0.561 0.137 0.021 0.043 Cognitive
23 0.045 0.057 0.005 0.545 0.109 Cognitive
24 0.366 0.192 0.062 0.285 0.162 Emotional
Professional items
1 0.098 0.189 0.639 0.016 0.096 Cognitive
2 0.057 0.478 0.346 0.034 0.161 Cognitive
3 0.137 0.153 0.082 0.588 0.091 Cognitive
4 0.018 0.221 0.689 0.165 0.013 Cognitive
5 0.556 0.077 0.194 0.127 0.046 Emotional
6 0.073 0.715 0.094 0.074 0.125 Cognitive
7 0.466 0.361 0.182 0.200 0.346 Emotional
8 0.115 0.021 0.107 0.548 0.015 Cognitive
9 0.030 0.355 0.063 0.541 0.167 Cognitive
10 0.693 0.050 0.014 0.132 0.001 Emotional
11 0.119 0.074 0.653 0.079 0.017 Cognitive
12 0.555 0.048 0.224 0.003 0.030 Emotional
13 0.479 0.088 0.092 0.377 0.351 Emotional
14 0.100 0.175 0.752 0.177 0.067 Cognitive
15 0.532 0.237 0.219 0.166 0.079 Emotional
17 0.134 0.706 0.118 0.092 0.019 Cognitive
20 0.767 0.088 0.080 0.066 0.037 Emotional
21 0.619 0.038 0.045 0.199 0.291 Emotional
22 0.016 0.567 0.163 0.122 0.039 Cognitive
23 0.204 0.010 0.118 0.624 0.005 Cognitive
24 0.391 0.060 0.035 0.355 0.103 Emotional
25 0.313 0.023 0.121 0.245 0.067 Emotional
Variance accounted for 12.58% 8.40% 7.90% 7.83% 4.92%

Values in bold face are greater than ± 0.3.

similar (56.79 vs. 57.62), this suggests that the females draw on highest scores and the Personal Emotional subscale fell in
different aspects of cognitive empathy when dealing with between. By contrast, Yarascavitch et al. (49) found highest
patients vs. people in their personal life. On the other hand, scores for the two emotional subscales and lowest scores for
the males are more likely to use the same empathy tools in the two cognitive subscales in their study of Canadian dental
both professional and personal domains. students (whilst the authors did not provide overall mean
Our results indicate that the Professional Emotional subscale scores, based on information presented in Figure 1 of their
had the lowest mean score, the two cognitive subscales had the paper, we estimate that the mean emotional sum scores ranged

214 ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Tsiantou et al. Greek Version of TCES

from 40.95 to 49.40 across the years of dental training and the Our data indicate that the Greek version of the TCES is a
mean cognitive sum scores ranged from 27.30 to 33.80). It is reliable and valid tool to measure the levels of emotional and
possible that these differences are related to sampling differ- cognitive empathies in both the clinical and personal contexts.
ences. Although the TCES consists of 52 questions, the measure can
Although we report on a single sample of dental students, easily be answered in a relatively short period of time. Given
our results are strengthened by our very high response rate that the JSPE-HP has been used with a variety of health profes-
(96.5%) compared with the rates of other studies (42). In sionals, we believe that the TCES could similarly have wide
particular, the response rate was higher than the 36.5% usage across health professions.
reported for the English version of the TCES (49). In the
present study, the questionnaires were distributed during tuto-
Conclusions
rials, whilst distribution of questionnaires in other surveys
was via email, and this difference may explain the different The present study is the first to assess the psychometric proper-
response rates (42, 49). ties of the Greek version of the Toronto Composite Empathy
Given the importance of empathy to the dentist–patient Scale in dental students, as well as the first to report on this
relationship, one may ask whether empathy can be increased scale’s test–retest reliability, factor analytic structure and gen-
through training. Whilst some authors have found that den- der-related results. The Greek version of TCES displayed good
tal and other health professional students can be taught test–retest reliability and acceptable internal consistency, good
methods to increase empathy (e.g., ref. 57), others have convergent and divergent validity and factor analytic results
found that increases may be short-lived (e.g., ref. 58). Still consistent with the claim that the scale measures both emo-
others have argued that empathy cannot be taught (59). It is tional and cognitive aspects of empathy. Taken together, our
possible that the variety of definitions of empathy and/or results indicate that the Greek version of the TCES is a reliable
measures of empathy used in the various studies contribute and valid tool for the measurement of cognitive and emotional
to this mixed literature. For example, Nash (4) asserts that empathy, in both professional and personal life, in Greek dental
cognitive empathy can be increased, but emotional empathy students.
is developed in childhood and therefore unlikely to change
in adulthood.
References
As noted previously, healthcare professionals draw upon
scientific knowledge of diagnosis and disease prevention and 1 Rogers C. A theory of therapy: personality and interpersonal
treatment, as well as empathy and other communication relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. New
skills, in their professional lives. A recent study of medical York: McGraw Hill, 1959.
students found that factors such as college grades and test 2 Davis MH. Empathy: a social psychological approach. Boulder, CO:
scores were more important predictors of success earlier in Westview Press, 1996.
3 Reniers RLEP, Corcoran R, Drake R, Shryane NM, V€ ollm BA. The
the didactic portion of their training, whilst factors such as
QCAE: a questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. J Pers
empathy and personality were more important predictors of Assess 2011: 93: 84–95.
success once the students began clinical interactions with 4 Nash DA. Ethics, empathy, and the education of dentists. J Dent
patients, suggesting that the two kinds of skills were differen- Educ 2010: 74: 567–578.
tially important in gaining knowledge of disease and subse- 5 Glass RM. The patient-physician relationship: JAMA focuses on the
quently putting into practice that knowledge via the center of medicine. JAMA 1996: 275: 147–148.
healthcare professional–patient relationship (60). Drawing 6 Stepien KA, Baernstein A. Educating for empathy. A review. J Gen
upon the results of this study, dental school admissions crite- Intern Med 2006: 21: 524–530.
ria might include indications that the applicant has high lev- 7 Council of European Dentists. Competencies required for the
els of both kinds of skills (4). practice of dentistry in the European Union. 2009. Available at:
http://www.eudental.edu/index.php?ID=2741. (Accessed 20
In an intriguing study of practising physicians, Hojat and
November 2012).
colleagues found significant differences in empathy level by 8 American Dental Association. Accreditation standards for dental
specialty (45). For example, psychiatrists, paediatricians and education programs. 2010. Available at: http://www.ada.org/316.
physicians whose specialty was internal medicine had the aspx. (Accessed 20 November 2012).
highest levels of empathy, whilst neurosurgeons, orthopaedic 9 Squier RW. A model of empathic understanding and adherence to
surgeons and anaesthesiologists had the lowest levels. These treatment regimens in practitioner-patient relationships. Soc Sci
cross-sectional data might be explained by the relatively Med 1990: 30: 325–339.
greater amount of ‘empathy training’ that psychiatrists and 10 Hojat M, Louis DZ, Markham FW, Wender R, Rabinowitz C,
others with the higher levels would have received, compared Gonnella JS. Physicians’ empathy and clinical outcomes for diabetic
with anaesthesiologists and others with the lower levels. On patients. Acad Med 2011: 86: 359–364.
11 Burns DD, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Therapeutic empathy and recovery
the other hand, perhaps medical students with different levels
from depression in cognitive-behavioral therapy: a structural
of empathy were differentially drawn to different specialties. It equation model. J Consult Clin Psychol 1992: 60: 441–449.
would be informative to see whether dental students’ empathy 12 Hojat M, Mangione S, Gonnella JS, Nasca T, Veloski JJ, Kane G.
levels were related to their selection of specialties. Perhaps Empathy in medical education and patient care. Acad Med 2001:
empathy level might be a useful way of helping students 76: 669.
determine which area(s) of dentistry they might be best suited 13 Corah NL, O’Shea RM, Bissell GD, Thines TJ, Mendola P. The
for. dentist-patient relationship: perceived dentist behaviors that reduce

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 215
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Greek Version of TCES Tsiantou et al.

patient anxiety and increase satisfaction. J Am Dent Assoc 1988: psychometrics and score differences by gender and level of medical
116: 73–76. education. Acad Med 2009: 84: 1192–1197.
14 Weinstein P, Getz T, Ratener P, Domoto P. Dentists’ responses to 35 Kliszcz J, Nowicka-Sauer K, Trzeciak B, Nowak P, Sadowska A.
fear- and nonfear-related behaviors in children. J Am Dent Assoc Empathy in health care providers-validation study of the Polish
1982: 104: 38–40. version of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy. Adv Med Sci 2006: 51:
15 Marzola R, Derbabian K, Donovan TE, Arcidiacono A. The science 219–225.
of communicating the art of esthetic dentistry. Part I: patient- 36 Fjortoft N, Van Winkle LJ, Hojat M. Measuring empathy in
dentist-patient communication. J Esthet Dent 2000: 12: 131–138. pharmacy students. Am J Pharm Educ 2011: 75: 109.
16 Sondell K, Soderfeldt B, Palmqvist S. Dentist-patient 37 Fields SK, Mahan P, Tillman P, Harris J, Maxwell K, Hojat M.
communication and patient satisfaction in prosthetic dentistry. Int J Measuring empathy in healthcare profession students using the
Prosthodont 2002: 15: 28–37. Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy: health provider-student
17 Sinha PK, Nanda RS, McNeil DW. Perceived orthodontist behaviors version. J Interprof Care 2011: 25: 287–293.
that predict patient satisfaction, orthodontist-patient relationship, 38 Ward J, Schaal M, Sullivan J, Bowen ME, Erdmann JB, Hojat M.
and patient adherence in orthodontic treatment. Am J Orthod Reliability and validity of the Jefferson Scale of Empathy in
Dentofacial Orthop 1996: 110: 370–377. undergraduate nursing students. J Nurs Meas 2009: 17: 73–88.
18 Laskin DM, Greene CS. Influence of the doctor-patient relationship 39 Passalacqua SA, Segrin C. The effect of resident physician stress,
on placebo therapy for patients with myofascial pain-dysfunction burnout, and empathy on patient-centered communication during
(MPD) syndrome. J Am Dent Assoc 1972: 85: 892–894. the long-call shift. Health Commun 2012: 27: 449–456.
19 Davis M. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for 40 Kliems H, Witt CM. The good doctor: a qualitative study of
a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 1983: 44: 113–126. German homeopathic physicians. J Altern Complement Med 2011:
20 Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, 17: 265–270.
Kelly C. The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and 41 Di Lillo M, Cicchetti A, Lo Scalzo A, Taroni F, Hojat M. The Jefferson
cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional Scale of Physician Empathy: preliminary psychometrics and group
dynamics. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2012: 7: 727–737. comparisons in Italian physicians. Acad Med 2009: 84: 1198–1202.
21 Maurage P, Grynberg D, No€el X, et al. Dissociation between affective 42 Sherman JJ, Cramer A. Measurement of changes in empathy during
and cognitive empathy in alcoholism: a specific deficit for the dental school. J Dent Educ 2005: 69: 338–345.
emotional dimension. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2011: 35: 1662–1668. 43 Hojat M, Mangione S, Kane GC, Gonnella JS. Relationships
22 Gilin D, Maddux WW, Carpenter J, Galinsky AD. When to use between scores of the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy (JSPE)
your head and when to use your heart: the differential value of and the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). Med Teach 2005: 27:
perspective-taking versus empathy in competitive interactions. Pers 625–628.
Soc Psychol Bull 2012: 39: 3–16. 44 Tavakol S, Dennick R, Tavakol M. Psychometric properties and
23 Decety J. The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Dev confirmatory factor analysis of the Jefferson Scale of Physician
Neurosci 2010: 32: 257–267. Empathy. BMC Med Educ 2011: 11: 54.
24 Banissy MJ, Kanai R, Walsh V, Rees G. Inter-individual differences 45 Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Nasca TJ, Mangione S, Vergare M, Magee
in empathy are reflected in human brain structure. Neuroimage M. Physician empathy: definition, components, measurement, and
2012: 62: 2034–2039. relationship to gender and specialty. Am J Psychiatry 2002: 159:
25 Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, Northoff G. Is there a core 1563–1569.
neural network in empathy? an fMRI based quantitative meta- 46 Borges NJ, Stratton TD, Wagner PJ, Elam CL. Emotional
analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011: 35: 903–911. intelligence and medical specialty choice: findings from three
26 Hemmerdinger JM, Stoddart SD, Lilford RJ. A systematic review of empirical studies. Med Educ 2009: 43: 565–572.
tests of empathy in medicine. BMC Med Educ 2007: 7: 24. 47 Pau AK, Croucher R. Emotional intelligence and perceived stress in
27 Neumann M, Edelhauser F, Tauschel D, et al. Empathy decline and dental undergraduates. J Dent Educ 2003: 67: 1023–1028.
its reasons: a systematic review of studies with medical students and 48 Hannah A, Lim BT, Ayers KM. Emotional Intelligence and clinical
residents. Acad Med 2011: 86: 996–1009. interview performance of dental students. J Dent Educ 2009: 73:
28 Tavakol S, Dennick R, Tavakol M. Empathy in UK medical 1107–1117.
students: differences by gender, medical year and specialty interest. 49 Yarascavitch C, Regehr G, Hodges B, Haas DA. Changes in dental
Educ Prim Care 2011: 22: 297–303. student empathy during training. J Dent Educ 2009: 73: 509–517.
29 Hojat M, Spandorfer J, Louis DZ, Gonnella JS. Empathic and 50 Leibetseder M, Laireiter A-R, K€ oller T. Structural analysis of the
sympathetic orientations toward patient care: conceptualization, E-scale. Pers Indiv Differ 2007: 42: 547–561.
measurement, and psychometrics. Acad Med 2011: 86: 989–995. 51 Wakabayashi A, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, et al. Development
30 Hojat M, Vergare MJ, Maxwell K, et al. The devil is in the third of short forms of the Empathy Quotient (EQ-Short) and the
year: a longitudinal study of erosion of empathy in medical school. Systemizing Quotient (SQ). Pers Indiv Differ 2006: 41: 929–940.
Acad Med 2009: 84: 1182–1191. 52 Karanikola MN, Papathanassoglou ED, Kalafati M, Stathopoulou H.
31 Hojat M, Mangione S, Nasca TJ, Gonnella JS, Magee M. Empathy Exploration of the association between professional interactions and
scores in medical school and ratings of empathic behavior in emotional distress of Intensive Care Unit Nursing Personnel.
residency training 3 years later. J Soc Psychol 2005: 145: 663–672. Dimens Crit Care Nurs 2012: 31: 37–45.
32 Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J. A cross-sectional 53 Tsimtsiou Z, Benos A, Garyfallos AA, Hatzichristou D. Predictors
measurement of medical student empathy. J Gen Intern Med 2007: of physicians’ attitudes toward sharing information with patients
22: 1434–1438. and addressing psychosocial needs: a cross-sectional study in
33 Quince TA, Parker RA, Wood DF, Benson JA. Stability of empathy Greece. Health Commun 2012: 27: 257–263.
among undergraduate medical students: a longitudinal study at one 54 Tsimtsiou Z, Kerasidou O, Efstathiou N, Papaharitou S,
UK medical school. BMC Med Educ 2011: 11: 90. Hatzimouratidis K, Hatzichristou D. Medical students’ attitudes
34 Kataoka HU, Koide N, Ochi K, Hojat M, Gonnella JS. toward patient-centred care: a longitudinal survey. Med Educ 2007:
Measurement of empathy among Japanese medical students: 41: 146–153.

216 ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217
Tsiantou et al. Greek Version of TCES

55 Papastavrou E, Karlou C, Tsangari H, et al. Cross-cultural 58 Van Winkle LJ, Fjortoft N, Hojat M. Impact of a workshop about
validation and psychometric properties of the Greek version of the aging on the empathy scores of pharmacy and medical students.
Caring Behaviors Inventory: a methodological study. J Eval Clin Am J Pharm Educ 2012: 76(1): 9.
Pract 2011: 17: 435–443. 59 Davis CM. What is empathy, and can empathy be taught? Phys
56 Beaton DE, Bombardier C, Guillemin F, Ferraz MB. Guidelines for Ther 1990: 70: 32–36.
the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. 60 Haight SJ, Chibnall JT, Schindler DL, Slavin SJ. Associations of
Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2000: 25: 3186–3191. medical student personality and health/wellness characteristics with
57 Beattie A, Durham J, Harvey J, Steele J, McHanwell S. Does their medical school performance across the curriculum. Acad Med
empathy change in first-year dental students? Eur J Dent Educ 2012: 87: 476–485.
2011: 16: e111–e116.

ª 2013 John Wiley & Sons A/S. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 217
Eur J Dent Educ 17 (2013) 208–217

You might also like