You are on page 1of 13

® Academy of Management Journal

1998, Vol. 41, No. 1, 96-107.

BEYOND RELATIONAL DEMOGRAPHY: TIME AND THE EFEECTS OE


SURFACE- AND DEEP-LEVEL DIVERSITY ON WORK GROUP COHESION
DAVID A. HARRISON
KENNETH H. PRICE
MYRTLE P. BELL
University of Texas at Arlington

We examined the impact of surface-level (demographic) and deep-level (attitudinal)


diversity on group social integration. As hypothesized, the length of time group mem-
bers worl^ed together weakened the effects of surface-level diversity and strengthened
the effects of deep-level diversity as group members had the opportunity to engage in
meaningful interactions.

Projected dem^ographic characteristics of the that as work group members continue to interact
U.S. workforce suggest that by the year 2000, ap- with one another, dissimilarity in the typically
proximately 80 percent of its new entrants will studied surface-level dimensions such as sex and
be women and members of ethnic minorities age becomes less important than deep-level attitu-
(Johnston & Packer, 1987). The increasing diversity dinal dissimilarity in, for instance, job satisfaction
of the workforce necessitates a better understand- or organizational commitment. That is, time mod-
ing of how such individual differences affect the erates the relative impact of overt versus underly-
functioning of work groups, as well as which types ing diversity among work group members.
of differences are most consequential. Research ex-
amining the outcomes of diversity has frequently
studied demographic differences among work DIMENSIONS OF DIVERSITY
group members. Consistent with Pfeffer's (1983)
work on demography in organizations, the most Underlying Premises and Assumptions
commonly studied forms of diversity have been The premise that differences among group mem-
heterogeneity in age, sex, race, and to a lesser ex- bers have a negative impact on group functioning is
tent, organizational tenure. The emphasis on those the primary thesis in the relational demography
variables is perhaps due to the ease with which research stream, and it is the backbone of a variety
researchers can measure them and group members of theories in social psychology (Byrne, 1971;
can observe them, and also to the widespread belief McGrath, 1984; Newcomb, 1961) and organiza-
that they are reasonable proxies for underlying psy- tional behavior (Jackson, May, & Whitney, 1995;
chological characteristics (Bantel & Jackson, 1989; Milliken & Martins, 1996; Schneider, 1987). The
Jackson, Stone, & Alvarez, 1993; Pfeffer, 1983; Tsui, main element of group functioning that we address
Egan, & O'Reilly, 1992). Attention to those vari- is what O'Reilly, Caldwell, and Barnett (1989)
ables might also be driven by legislation prohibit- termed social integration: the degree to which
ing employment discrimination and mandating group members are psychologically linked or at-
equal treatment without regard to race, sex, and tracted toward interacting with one another in pur-
age. suit of a common objective. In the present study,
Effects of heterogeneity in these commonly stud- we concentrated on the primary affective dimen-
ied characteristics have, however, been inconsis- sion of social integration, group cohesiveness.
tent across studies. Some of this inconsistency may As we review past work in this section, we em-
have arisen because the connection between overt phasize differences or heterogeneity at both the
demographic differences among employees and the dyad level (i.e., supervisor-subordinate relation-
less obvious, but important, attitudinal differences ships) and the work team or group level. Although
among them is weaker than has been assumed. By there may be important differences between supe-
integrating theory and research from management, rior-subordinate interactions and those between
psychology, and sociology, we further distinguish team members, we have included both dyadic and
between these levels of heterogeneity. We argue team-level diversity in this review for three rea-
sons. First, inclusion of both types of diversity
maintains consistency with the reviews and re-
The authors contributed equally to this article. search published by other diversity researchers
96
1998 Harrison, Price, and Bell 97

(i.e., Jackson et al., 1995; Milliken & Martins, 1996; ceptions of their subordinates' performance or with
Tsui & O'Reilly, 1989; Tsui, Egan, & Porter, 1994). superiors' liking of subordinates. Zalesny and
Secondly, certain basic processes, including face- Kirsch (1989) found that heterogeneity in age was
to-face communication, potential for collaboration unrelated to superiors' ratings of subordinates' per-
and conflict, and influence attempts, characterize formance but negatively related to peers' ratings of
botb dyads and larger groups, althougb the status co-wrorkers' performance. Heterogeneity in age was
differences characteristic of supervisor-subordinate positively associated wi\h team turnover in the
interactions may modify those processes. For ex- Jackson et al. (1991), Wagner, Pfeffer, and O'Reilly
ample, Tsui and colleagues (1994) noted that inter- (1984), and Wiersema and Bird (1993) studies. In
personal liking in dyads could be considered sim- the Tsui et al. (1992) study, group heterogeneity in
ilar to forms of social integration in larger groups. age was negatively associated with organizational
Thirdly, the proposed effects of diversity rest on attachment for some demographic subgroups
assumptions in tbe similarity-attraction and self- (white males) but was not for others (women and
categorization paradigms (Jackson et al., 1993; Tsui minorities). O'Reilly and colleagues (1989) found
et al., 1992). There is little reason to expect that that group heterogeneity with respect to age was
these paradigms would be substantively different associated with lower levels of social integration,
for dyads and larger groups. which in turn was associated with turnover. The
The terminology we use, surface- and deep-level more distant group members were from other mem-
diversity, is similar to that found in recent theoret- bers in terms of age, the more likely they were to
ical work by Jackson and colleagues (1995) and leave.
Milliken and Martins (1996). Both articles describe Sex. Research on the consequences of sex dissim-
diversity in readily detectable attributes (e.g., race/ ilarity has also yielded conflicting results. Tsui and
ethnicity, sex, age) and underlying, deeper-level O'Reilly (1989) reported that subordinates whose
attributes (e.g., attitudes, values). Tbese theorists sex differed from their supervisors' received lower
have agreed on the level (surface or deep) of several performance ratings. Mobley (1982) hypothesized
diversity variables. Our review of the elements of but did not find that supervisor-subordinate simi-
diversity is confined to variables for which there is larity in sex led to higher subordinate performance
such agreement and for which there were available ratings. Pulakos, Oppler, White, and Borman's
data. (1989) analysis of military personnel found that
rater-ratee similarity in sex was not an important
influence on performance ratings. Konrad, Winter,
Surface-Level Diversity and Gutek (1992) found that dissimilarity in sex
Heterogeneity at a surface level can be defined as resulted in isolation and dissatisfaction for women
differences among group members in overt, biolog- in some situations. Finally, Tsui and co-authors
ical characteristics that are typically reflected in (1992) found that the organizational attachment of
physical features. Such characteristics include age, white men decreased as diversity on sex increased,
sex, and race/ethnicity. Following Jackson and col- but that of women (of any racial/ethnic group) did
leagues (1993), we view these characteristics as not.
generally immutable, almost immediately observ- Race/ethnicity. Kraiger and Ford's (1985) meta-
able, and measurable in simple and valid ways (cf. analysis of research from 1966 to 1981 indicated
Jackson et al., 1995; Milliken & Martins, 1996). that rater-ratee ethnic similarity was associated
Social consensus can usually be assumed for each with higher performance ratings. Pulakos and co-
of these demographic attributes (Jackson et al., authors' (1989) meta-analysis indicated very small
1993). That is, one's age (within some range), sex, and inconsistent effects of ratee-rater racial similar-
and race/ethnicity are generally apparent to and ity across performance dimensions. Tsui and
agreed upon by observers. O'Reilly (1989) reported that ethnic heterogeneity
Age. Several studies have focused on the conse- was not associated with superiors' ratings of sub-
quences of group heterogeneity in age. Bantel and ordinates' effectiveness but was marginally related
Jackson (1989) found that heterogeneity in age was to supervisors' liking of subordinates.
not associated with innovations of top management Summary. Of the research that has investigated
banking teams. Judge and Ferris (1993) found that the effects of surface-level heterogeneity, findings
differences in superior-subordinate age signifi- are inconsistent within and across studies for vari-
cantly, negatively influenced supervisors' liking ables such as age, sex, and race. Studies have dif-
and subsequent ratings of subordinates' perfor- fered on whether or not relationships were detected
mance. Tsui and O'Reilly (1989) found that age and, in some instances, on their direction. Other
differences were not associated with superiors' per- researchers have also noted this inconsistency, es-
98 Academy of Management Journal February

pecially for the effects of race and sex dissimilarity attitudes, it becomes less pleasant and more diffi-
(Pulakos et al., 1989). cult for them to work together.

Surface-Level Diversity, Deep-Level Diversity,


Deep-Level Diversity and the Moderating Influence of Time
Heterogeneity at a deep level includes differ- Theoretical perspectives from organizational be-
ences among members' attitudes, beliefs, and val- havior (Schneider, 1987), sociology (Allport, 1954;
ues. Information about these factors is communi- Amir, 1969; Berger, Rosenholtz, & Zelditch, 1980),
cated through verbal and nonverbal behavior and social psychology (Byrne, 1971; Newcomb,
patterns and is only learned through extended, in- 1961) support the idea that group members base an
dividualized interaction and information gather- initial superficial categorization of other group
ing. Jackson and colleagues conceptualized a simi- members on stereotypes and subsequently modify
lar form of diversity, termed diversity in or replace those stereotypes with deeper-level
underlying attributes, which included attitudes, knowledge of the psychological features of the
values, knowledge, and skills. They noted that other individuals. Further, knowledge of attitudi-
these aspects of individuals are more "subject to nal, belief, and value similarity between individu-
construal and more mutable" (Jackson et al., 1995; als forms the basis for continued attraction and
217] than other aspects. Milliken and Martins affiliation. Research generally supports the idea
(1996) made a similar statement. that initial categorizations are accompanied by per-
The few studies that have examined the conse- ceptions of similarity or dissimilarity that are based
quences of similarity in attitudes and values in on surface-level demographic data; these percep-
work groups have found that supervisor-subordi- tions change when deep-level information is ob-
nate attitudinal and value similarity was associated tained (Stangor, Lynch, Duan, & Glass, 1992;
with higher subordinate performance ratings Turner, 1987). For example, Byrne and Wong
(Miles, 1964; Senger, 1971; Turban & Jones, 1988; (1962) found that subjects initially perceived
Zalesny & Kirsch, 1989) and more accurate peer greater attitudinal dissimilarity between them-
ratings (Fox, Ben-Nahum, & Yinon, 1989). One selves and a stranger of another race. When more
study found that attitudinal similarity was associ- details were provided about the stranger's atti-
ated with higher group cohesiveness (Terborg, Cas- tudes, perceptions of attitudinal dissimilarity de-
tore, & DeNinno, 1976). Turban and Jones (1988) creased and interpersonal attraction to the stranger
reported that perceptions of attitudinal similarity increased. Tsui, Egan, and Porter's (1994) summary
were uniquely and positively related to subordi- of the vertical dyad linkage literature noted that
nates' satisfaction, performance ratings, and pay supervisors categorized subordinates as either in-
ratings. Miles (1964) found that attitudinal similar- or out-group members early in relationships, when
ity between supervisors and subordinates was re- there had been little informational exchange be-
lated to higher subordinate ratings; however, nei- tween the two. The authors suggested that "physi-
ther superior-subordinate similarity in age nor cal, observable, and immutable personal and back-
similarity in education had a relationship with sub- ground characteristics . . . play a critical role in the
ordinates' ratings. initial categorization process" of group members;
Unlike the few studies on deep-level diversity in (Tsui et al., 1994: 8; emphasis added). Likewise,
work settings, a number of social psychological Tsui and colleagues (1992) noted this: "The con-
studies have reported that attitude similarity was ceptual foundation for almost all the research on
one of the most important predictors of attraction organizational demography has been the similarity-
and friendship (e.g., Antill, 1984; Byrne, 1971; attraction paradigm (Byrne, 1971). The similarity-
McGrath, 1984; Newcomb, 1961). Similarity in at- attraction hypothesis maintains that similarity in
titudes can ease interpersonal interaction and in- attitudes is a major source of attraction between
crease rewards when relationships are viewed individuals. A variety of physical, social, and sta-
within an exchange theory framework (Thibaut & tus traits can be used as the basis for inferring
Kelley, 1959). Further, attitudinal similarity may similarity in attitudes, beliefs, or personality"
facilitate cominunication; it may also reduce role (1992: 551; emphasis added).
conflict, because people have similar conceptual- Other researchers have noted that over time, as
izations of their organizations and jobs, and it may people acquire more information, their perceptions
reduce role ambiguity, because communication on are based more on observed behavior and less on
the job increases (Tsui & O'Reilly, 1989). It can thus stereotypes prompted by overt characteristics (Jack-
be inferred that as people uncover differences in son et al., 1993; Newcomb, 1961). Further, in re-
1998 Harrison, Price, and Bell 99

search on collegial choices, people have placed Martins (1996) considered it an underlying at-
greater emphasis on similarity in attitudes (beliefs) tribute. Since both agreed that age, race/ethnicity,
than on ethnic similarity (Triandis, 1961), particu- and sex were readily detectable or overt features
larly when information about attitudinal differ- (Jackson et al., 1995; Milliken & Martins, 1996),
ences was available or salient (Brown & Turner, however, we used those three as surface-level vari-
1981). Terborg and colleagues (1976) also noted ables in this study. There is less guidance in the
that the effect of attitude or value compatibility was literature on selecting variables that reflect deep-
not immediate, but took time to unfold. level diversity, which is broadly defined as includ-
Evidence from the sociological literature also ing a variety of psychological characteristics (Jack-
supports the differential contributions of surface- son et al., 1995; Milliken & Martins, 1996). We
and deep-level diversity over time. Allport (1954) specifically chose dimensions of job satisfaction
and Amir (1969) are associated with the "contact and organizational commitment as elements of
hypothesis," which states that under certain condi- deep-level diversity in this study, for a number of
tions, interracial or interethnic contact will result reasons. First, we were limited by the organizations
in positive group outcomes. A key supposition of involved as to what we could measure; v^^e were
this hypothesis is that as people interact to get to restricted from measuring specific work values
know one another, stereotypes are replaced by (e.g., Protestant work ethic) because they had little
more accurate knowledge of each other as individ- face validity in a survey ostensibly asking employ-
uals, which can result in reduced prejudice and ees their opinions about working conditions. Sec-
conflict and greater group cohesiveness (Amir, ond, both satisfaction and commitment are re-
1976). Recent sociological research has supported garded as fundamental work attitudes; they have
the general thrust of the contact hypothesis (Ellison been used in literally thousands of individual-level
& Powers, 1994; Sigelman & Welch, 1993) and, studies, particularly as they relate to role with-
according to Amir, "Work situations provide the drawal and organizational separation (e.g., Locke,
best opportunities for intergroup contact" (1969: 1976; Tett & Meyer, 1993). Third, such deep-level
323). Finally, Milliken and Martins suggested that variables are directly relevant to the pursuit and
"negative affective outcomes of diversity in observ- attainment of a group's goals (McGrath, 1991).
able attributes [surface-level diversity] appear to Members' attitudes toward (1) their tasks, (2) the
decrease with the amount of time that the group supervisors who define and assign tasks, monitor
stays together" (1996: 415-416), an idea that is and assist in task completion, and reward task at-
central to our argument. tainment, (3) their embedding organizations, and
(4) their overall jobs are meaningful and impor-
tant in a work group. Finally, despite some classi-
Hypothesis fication ambiguities, our deep-level variables of
The preceding discussion, involving evidence satisfaction and organizational commitment are
from sociology, social psychology, and organiza- consistent with the Jackson et al. (1995) conceptu-
tional behavior, suggests a fundamental develop- alization of underlying dimensions of diversity.
mental process for diversity in work groups. The
time that members spend urorking together should
Samples
moderate the impact of diversity levels in a partic-
ular pattern (Howell, Dorfman, & Kerr, 1986). We We collected data from two samples. Sample 1
hypothesize the following: involved 39 units (groups) of employees in a medi-
um-sized private hospital in a large southwestern
Hypothesis 1. Time will neutralize, or make
city. The total sample contained 443 people, and
less important, the effects of surface-level di-
groups had from 4 to 25 people. These employees
versity on group cohesiveness, and it will en- included whites (88%), Hispanics (5%), African
hance, or make more important, the effects of Americans (4%), Asians (2%), and Native Ameri-
deep-level diversity. cans (1%). Most employees were women (83%).
The mean age was 37 years (s.d. = 11) and the mean
METHODS organizational tenure was 5 years (s.d. = 4). Four
percent of the employees had not completed high
Selection of Diversity Variahles school, 17 percent were high school graduates, 49
There have been disagreements about the classi- percent had completed some college work, 24 per-
fication of some surface-level variables. Specifi- cent w^ere college graduates, and 6 percent had
cally, Jackson and colleagues (1995) included edu- completed some graduate work or had graduate
cation as a surface-level attribute, but Milliken and degrees. Their jobs varied from professional ones.
100 Academy of Management Journal February

such as administrator, nurse (RN or LPN), pharma- For sample 2, questionnaires were administered
cist, and lab technician or radiologist, to nonpro- over the course of six weeks. Surveys were com-
fessional ones, such as orderly or maintenance pleted "on the clock" in a break room by 94 percent
worker. Work units reflected department, shift, and of the deli-bakery employees. Again, response rates
physical location. For example, one group was did not differ significantly across groups ix^^.^ =
"third floor medical/surgical unit on the day shift." 2.67, p > ,10), In addition, we obtained demo-
Other groups were defined by function, such as the graphic data (sex, age, ethnicity, and tenure) for
"human resources department," because these em- each employee from company records using a spe-
ployees had only one location and worked for only cial code the employees voluntarily provided at the
one shift. In all cases, work groups comprised peo- end of their questionnaires.
ple who worked together daily on tasks in ongoing, Surface-level diversity. Self-report information
face-to-face interactions. There were also institu- regarding each respondent's age, ethnicity, and sex
tionally supported group goals and unit rewards to were collected and then used to calculate a mea-
promote cooperative contact among employees. In sure of group heterogeneity for each variable in
total, these factors suggested that group members both samples. Age was measured in years. Racial/
would, over time, have had the opportunity to ac- ethnic background included the following catego-
quire deep-level information about one another. ries: African American, 'Native American, Asian,
Sample 2 consisted of employees in the deli- Hispanic, and white. For the deli-bakery (sample 2)
bakery sections of 32 grocery stores in a regional employees, self-reports were related to archival
grocery chain. An average of 13 people worked in records for age, sex, and ethnic background. Corre-
each deli-bakery, with group size ranging from 2 to lations of ,99 and ,98 for age and sex, respectively,
22 employees. The mean age was 34 (s,d, = 13), and as well as 98 percent agreement for the ethnicity
the mean organizational tenure was 4 years (s.d, = categories, provided convergent validity for the
3). The racial/ethnic backgrounds of employees in- self-reports. We used the coefficient of variation to
cluded white (68%), African-American (17%), His- index group heterogeneity in age, Blau's (1977) in-
panic (12%), and other (3%), Seventy-four percent dex was used to indicate within-group heterogene-
of the deli-bakery employees were women. Seven- ity in ethnicity and sex. Various diversity research-
teen percent had not completed high school, 42 ers have discussed the advantages of both indexes
percent were high school graduates, 34 percent had (e,g., Bantel & Jackson, 1989; Jackson, Brett, Cessa,
completed some college or the equivalent, and 7 Cooper, Julin, & Peyronnin, 1991).
percent were college graduates. Each deli-bakery Deep-level diversity. We chose three aspects of
had one manager; six larger deli-bakeries also had satisfaction and organizational commitment as
assistant managers. The remainder of the employ- measures of deep-level diversity (recognizing that
ees were clerks, so there were few status differences other beliefs and values might also be relevant, as
among them. Members of each bakery-deli worked discussed above and in "Discussion"), Satisfaction
with each other face-to-face but were relatively iso- with supervision was measured using a subscale of
lated from employees in other parts of the store. the Job Descriptive Index (JDI; Smith, Kendall, &
There was no task substitution between deli-bakery Hulin, 1969), Values of coefficient alpha were ,78
and other store workers, and there was limited and ,83 in samples 1 and 2, respectively. Satisfac-
interaction outside the work group. For the deli- tion with work tasks was measured using another
bakery employees, their group was the most salient JDI subscale (a = .76 in sample 1 and ,79 in sample
feature of their work environment. Again, the co- 2), Overall satisfaction was measured using the
operative and interdependent nature of their tasks, Faces scale (Kunin, 1955) in sample 1 and the Faces
the equality of status, and the average tenure of the scale combined with two items adapted from the
work groups allowed sufficient opportunity for Job Diagnostic Survey (JDS; Hackman & Oldham,
members to learn deeper-level information about 1975) in sample 2 (a = .77). We transformed the
one another over time. data in the latter sample to have the same possible
range as scores in sample 1, Organizational com-
mitment was measured using the Organizational
Data Collection and Measures CommitmeAt Questionnaire (OCQ; Porter & Steers,
For sample 1, questionnaires administered over 1979), Coefficient alpha was,,83 in sample 1 and
the course of three weeks were completed during ,89 in sample 2, In both samples, we indexed deep-
departmental meetings at which we were present to level diversity by the within-group coefficient of
give instructions and answer questions. Response variation.
rates did not differ significantly across work groups Group cohesiveness. The chief affective compo-
[x\a = 12.86, p > ,10). nent of group functioning, group cohesiveness, was
1998 Harrison, Price, and Bell 101

the dependent variable in our investigation. As in sity and cohesiveness were self-reports. To
O'Reilly et al. (1989), it was treated here as a di- examine this issue and an additional question
mension of social integration within a group. The regarding the meaningfulness of our measures of
group cohesiveness measure was taken from Sea- diversity, we conducted preliminary analyses in-
shore (1954). Reliability estimates at the individual volving other data. A small set of additional ques-
level (a) were .68 and .70 for samples 1 and 2, tionnaire items asked respondents their percep-
respectively. tions of ethnic and age differences in their work
We aggregated individual ratings of group cohe- group. Group members were asked questions about
siveness to the group level by calculating the mean . surface-level diversity, such as "How different are
rating of cohesiveness within each group. Although the members of your work group in their ethnic
some researchers have expressed concern about backgrounds?" and "How different are the mem-
such aggregation (e.g., Klein, Dansereau, & Hall, bers of your work group in their age/experiences?"
1994), we made the calculation for several reasons. Responses were made on a five-point scale ranging
First, cohesiveness is conceptually defined at the from 0, "not at all different," to 4, "completely
group level. Its meaning derives from how much different." We aggregated these perceptual mea-
the members of a group "stick together" (Mudrack, sures to the group level and correlated them with
1989). Group-level means of cohesiveness have measures of diversity calculated from demographic
been used as an index of social integration in many data. Perceptual and derived (Blau, 1977) indexes
studies (cf. Gully, Devine, & Whitney, 1995). Sec- of ethnic diversity were significantly correlated
ond, group cohesion is one of the most often stud- (r = .68, p < .01), as were perceptual and derived
ied outcomes in the small groups literature, and it (coefficient of variation) indexes for age (r = .33,
has been assumed to be negatively associated with p < .05). Thus, there was evidence that the ethnic
dissimilarity (Jackson and Associates, 1992). Third, and age differences captured by the demographic
in concluding their meta-analysis of cohesion (and questions were "readily apparent" to employees.
performance) research. Gully and colleagues (1995) However, as with the derived indexes for these
recommended that researchers conceptualize cohe- surface-level dimensions, the correlations of per-
sion as a group-level construct, attending to aggre- ceived ethnic and age differences with cohesive-
gation issues as necessary. One such issue is the ness were nonsignificant (r's = -.14 and .14, re-
requirement for within-group agreement, which we spectively).
calculated for each of the 71 teams in the study Three additional questions measured percep-
using the James, Demaree, and Wolf (1982) index. tions of deep-level differences in personalities, val-
Both the mean and the median agreement indexes ues, and interests. Perceived differences in person-
for cohesiveness were .75. Seventy-five percent of ality were related to the coefficient of variation in
the groups had agreement indexes of at least r.^^ organizational commitment (r = .41, p < .05) and
equals .70. Fourth, the group-level component of job satisfaction (r = .22, p < .05). Perceived differ-
variance in cohesiveness was significant (Fgg 554 = ences in values were related to the coefficient of
1.37, p < .05). Fifth, the internal consistency of the variation for supervisory satisfaction (r = .27 and
group-level measure was .75. Given these justifica- .31, respectively; both p < .05), and perceived dif-
tions, aggregation of individual cohesiveness ferences in interests were related to supervisor sat-
scores to the group seemed reasonable. isfaction (r = .27, p < .05) and work satisfaction
Time spent in the group was measured in a (r = .22, p < .05). Group cohesiveness was signifi-
straightforward way. We asked group members cantly correlated with perceptions of differences in
"How long have you been in your present job?" all three: personality (r = -.27, p < .05), values (r =
(years and months). Because of skewness in the -.25, p < .05), and interests (r = -.27, p < .05). If
individual (within-group) distribution of this vari- our findings had stemmed only from percept-per-
able, we used the median as an index of the typical cept inflation, then both the surface- and deep-level
amount of time that group members had spent sets of relationships should have been significantly
working in their units. and negatively correlated with group cohesiveness.

Main Analysis
RESULTS
The overriding question guiding this research
Preliminary Analyses
was whether surface-level diversity would become
A potential weakness of results might be infla- less important and deep-level diversity more im-
tion of relationships by percept-percept bias, espe- portant in predicting group outcomes as members
cially because measures of both deep-level diver- of work teams spent more time together. To test this
102 Academy of Management Journal February

hypothesis, we conducted a moderated hierarchi- ability to draw conclusions, given the small num-
cal regression analysis at the group level with ber of groups in each sample, we first combined
cohesiveness as the dependent variable. Control data from the groups into one data set. Table 1
variables (group size, mean levels of overall satis- presents means, standard deviations, and correla-
faction, supervisor satisfaction, work satisfaction, tions among all independent, dependent, and con-
and organizational commitment) and independent trol variables in the combined data set. We in-
variables (surface- and deep-level measures of di- cluded group size in the correlation analyses and
versity, and time] were entered first into the regres- included it as a control variable in later analyses to
sion equation as main effects. In the next stage of remove potential confounding effects.
the analyses, we created interaction terms by mul- Results were consistent with both components of
tiplying each of the surface- and deep-level mea- the moderating hypothesis, as shown in Table 2.
sures of diversity by the median time group mem- First, declining effects for surface-level differences
bers had spent together. These interaction terms were indicated by a significant improvement in
were then entered as sets into the regression equa- prediction for the set of the three surface-level in-
tion. Results of this two-stage regression process teractions with time (Ai?^ = .09, p < .10). Most of
are presented below. Because of the inherently low this effect was captured in the sex-by-time interac-
statistical power for detecting moderators in field tion [AR^ = .08, p < .10), which had a sign indi-
data, we relaxed significance levels to p < .10 for cating that the initial negative influence of gender
the interaction terms (McLelland & Judd, 1993). diversity on cohesiveness was being neutralized or
weakened as group members spent more time to-
Descriptive Statistics gether (Darlington, 1990). Split-sample analyses re-
vealed that the product-moment correlation be-
All analyses were conducted at the group level. tween sex diversity and cohesiveness was -.27 for
To increase statistical power and confidence in our groups in the lower half of the time distribution but

TABLE 1
Correlation Matrix and Descriptive Statistics for All Variables"
Variables Mean s.d. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

Control
1. Group size 11.26 4.52
2. Overall job 5.62 0.63 -.08
satisfaction''
3. Supervisory 37.87 4.35 -.07 .31**
satisfaction''
4. Work satisfaction'' 30.17 4.51 -.19 .72** .44**
5. Organizational 48.41 4.42 -.19 .63** .18 .55
commitment''
Surface-level diversity
. 6. Age'^ 0.33 0.09 -.05 .17 .01 .02 .13
7. Sex'' 0.27 0.17 .21 -.05 -.26* .01 -.12 .21
8. Race/ethnicity'' 0.30 0.20 -.14 .24 .07 .28* .25*> ' .26* .16
Deep-level diversity
9. Overall job 0.22 0.12 -.01 -.80** -.33** -.56** -.37** -.02 -.06 -.06
satisfaction"
10. Supervisory 0.23 0.12 .01 -.22 -.86** -.37** -.15 .08 .29* -.05 .36**
satisfaction"
11. Work satisfaction" 0.28 0.11 .14 -.45** -.37** -.63** -.55** -.04 .07 -.08 .41** .31
12. Organizational 0.29 0.13 -.02 -.55** -.16 -.44** -.60** -.24* -.11 -.25* .41** .07 .40*
commitment"
13. Median time in 34.81 22.51 .06 -.04 .04 .05 -.01 -.17 -.01 -.05 -.01 -.05-.11 .23
group"
14. Group cohesiveness 16.59 1.33 -.12 .35** .38** .47** .28 -.02 -.18 .12 -.29** - . 4 0 - . 2 1 -.17 -.10

" JV = 71 (groups).
'' Group means.
" Coefficient of variation.
'' Blau's index.
" In months.
*p < .05
**p < .01
r"*- Y'
1998 Harrison, Price, and Bell 103

TABLE 2
Results of Regression Analyses for Main and Interactive E£fects of Surface- and Deep-Level Diversity on
Work Group Cohesion^
Stage 1: Main Effects Stage 2: Interactions with Time

Unique Unique
Variable P Variance P Variance

Control 0.07
Group size -0.00
Overall satisfaction'' -0.07
Supervisor satisfaction'' -0.00
Work satisfaction'' 0.49*
Organizational commitment'' 0.12
Surface-level diversity 0.04
Age-^ 0.02
Sex" -0.11
Race/ethnicity'' -0.01
Deep-level diversity 0.08
Overall satisfaction" -0.04
Supervisor satisfaction"^ -0.24
Work task satisfaction*^ 0.20
Organizational commitment'^ 0.05
Time 0.01
Median time spent in group" -0.12
Interactions of time with surface-level variables 0.09^
Time X age^ 1.19
Time X sex" 0.78 *
Time X race/ethnicity" -0.33
Interactions of time with deep-level variahles 0.10^
Time x overall satisfaction*^ -0.90 *
Time X supervisor satisfaction*^ -0.46
Time X work satisfaction*^ 0.27
Time X organizational commitment*^ -0.45

Full model F 2.10* 2.08*


Adjusted R^ .17 .24
df 42 49

" N = 71 (groups).-
'' Group mean.
*^ Coefficient of variation.
*' Blau's index.
*" In months.
^ p < .10
* p < .05
** p < .01

.04 for the upper half (groups whose members had DISCUSSION
spent more time together). Including the interactions
of the deep-level diversity variables with time also The existence of different levels of diversity has
had a unique effect (Afl^ = .10, p < .10). Much of this largely been overlooked in previous empirical stud-
effect was conveyed by the overall satisfaction-by- ies of work groups. Examination of these different
time interaction {AR^ = .05, p < .05), which had a levels may help to explain the inconsistent, often
sign indicating that as group members spent more conflicting results of diversity research to date. We
time together, the impact of member differences in proposed that diversity at a deep level has steadily
overall job satisfaction was strengthened (Darlington, stronger consequences for groups than demo-
1990). The correlation between diversity in overall graphic diversity as group members spend more
satisfaction and cohesiveness was .05 for groups in time together. In a sample composed of groups from
the lower half of the time distribution and -.50 for two different populations, we found that surface-
those in the upper half. level differences were less iniportant and deep-
104 Academy of Management fournal February

level differences were more important for groups identifying problems and generating solutions than
that had interacted more often. their homogenous counterparts.
Although time is the variable we examined, the Although we have argued that deep-level vari-
fundamental medium is information. Demographic ables are more important over time than surface-
factors are often a poor surrogate for the deeper- level variables in determining social integration,
level information people need to make accurate one could think of situations where this would not
judgments about similarity of attitudes among be the case. For example, Tsui and colleagues
group members. Time merely allows more informa- (1992) pointed out that demographic variables such
tion to be conveyed. Indeed, it might be more ap- as ethnicity and sex may assume more importance
propriate to think of the richness of interactions as when associated with differences in status. High-
the conduit for information exchange (cf. Daft & status group members (whites and men) may be
Lengel, 1986). That is, time provides the opportu- more likely to withdraw when work groups are first
nity to acquire interpersonal information; the integrated by members of perceived lower status
amount of information acquired is a function of the (nonwhites and women). Similarly, Konrad and
length of shared experience for group members, the colleagues (1992) found that status differences af-
breadth of group activities, the depth of task inter- fected outcomes for minorities and women in
dependence, and other factors. These exchanges group interactions. As equality of member status is
allow group members to learn deeper-level infor- assumed to be an important foundation for infor-
mation about their psychological similarity to or mation exchange, it is possible that inequality of
dissimilarity from their co-workers, where before status may impede communication hetween high-
they would have used surface-level demographic status and low-status group members. Additional
data as information proxies. As previously men-
opportunities for shared experience, broad group
tioned, work in sociology also supports this con-
activities, and task interdependence may be needed
tention, by demonstrating that beneficial conse-
in such cases.
quences of contact among members of overtly
dissimilar groups are most likely to occur under
conditions of equal status and cooperative contact Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research
(Ellison & Powers, 1994; Sigelman & Welch, 1993).
Interpersonal interactions under these conditions This study involved a limited set of deep-level
should allow for more accurate, less stereotypical diversity variables. In addition to our measures,
exchanges as group members get to know one an- variance in individual values regarding working
other over time. per se, working hard (e.g., Protestant work ethic), or
the proper balance between work and family might
Although our results and the proposed model also be important for groups in this context. Includ-
might appear to contradict much of the previous ing such variables would strengthen future studies,
research in this field, a careful examination of re- especially those of extremely potent work teams
sults of that research shows that they do not. For (such as small, family-owned businesses).
example, one might expect that for temporary, ad One of the problems inherent in the selection of
hoc groups, diversity in surface factors would play which deep-level differences to study is that there
a more important role than deep-level heterogene- are literally dozens of conceivably important work-
ity in determining cohesiveness and other forms of related values (e.g., Meglino, Ravlin, & Adkins,
social integration. Had previous research compared 1989), cultural values (e.g., Triandis, 1989), termi-
the relative contributions of surface- and deep-level nal values (e.g., Rokeach, 1973), and work-related
variables, their findings may have been more con- attitudes to pick from. The relevance of each one
sistent with those reported in this study, given sim- likely differs across situations, which is a major
ilar opportunities for rich interactions among group theme in the budding literature on selection for
members. Watson, Kumar, and Michaelsen's (1993) person-organization fit (e.g., Adkins, Russell, &
study of interaction processes and group perfor- Werbel, 1994; Barrett, 1995; Vancouver & Schmitt,
mance within culturally diverse or homogenous (in 1991). Rather than the heterogeneity in job atti-
race/ethnicity) teams provides some converging tudes studied in the present research, level of
support for this idea. Initially, homogenous groups agreement about, say, the proper role of the federal
interacted and performed more effectively than het- government might be a strong element of deep-level
erogeneous groups. Over time, however, interac- diversity for a chapter of local campus Democrats.
tion processes and performance for both groups Differences in attitudes toward enforcing the exist-
improved, with more rapid improvements occur- ing covenants and restrictions on property might be
ring in the diverse groups. At the end of the study, critical to the harmony of a neighborhood home-
the diverse groups had grown more effective in owners' association. In a jazz band or marketing
1998 Harrison, Price, and Bell 105

team, consensus on the value of creative freedom Conclusion


might be paramount. In a day care center, the crit- Given the composition of the workforce today
ical values to share might deal with nurturance and and its projected future composition, understand-
patience. In a sports team, agreement about the ing and predicting employee reactions to all levels
importance of goal directedness, dedication, or of diversity is critical. Most organizational research
achievement might be an overriding concern. In a on these reactions has been gloomy, suggesting that
university department, agreement about the rela- demographic diversity diminishes work group co-
tive values of teaching and research might be most hesion and organizational attachment and height-
significant. The crucial point is that the relevant ens risks of employee withdrawal and turnover.
deep-level variables in any situation are those that Practitioners also see heterogeneity as having neg-
bear directly on the fundamental purposes of the ative outcomes (cf. Campion, Medsker, & Higgs,
group. An important theoretical and research ques- 1993). In view of their positive findings, Watson,
tion, then, is which attitudinal differences are most Kumar, and Michaelsen (1993: 599) suggested the
significant for which types of groups? "dark cloud" of negative outcomes from increasing
Another limitation was our use of a cross-sec- cultural diversity might have a "silver lining." We
tional design. Ideally, any study of the relative in- agree and suggest that research results indicating
fluence of diversity variables on group functioning many negative effects of surface-level diversity may
would follow both the dependent and independent have also contained deeper-level similarities. Ex-
variables over the history of the group. Present plicit recognition of these similarities may have
resulted in different conclusions. Our findings are
theories give little guidance on the proper span of
significant because they focus on the issue of which
time to employ, but recent organizational behavior
levels of differences are the most important in on-
and social psychological research shows that a fine-
going groups. We also hope that our findings are
grained record of membership changes, interper- more optimistic than those of previous work be-
sonal interactions, and performance over time cause they focus on differences that are subject to
can yield entirely new insights about work groups change, rather than those that are immutable.
(Gersick, 1989; McGrath, Arrow, Gruenfeld,
Hollingshead, & O'Connor, 1993).
Common method variance is also a potential
limitation on this type of research. It is important REFERENCES
to note that percept-percept inflation is a linear Adkins, C. L., Russell, C. J., & Werbel, J. D. 1994. Judgments of fit
confound, showing up as an additive bias in a in the selection process: The role of work value congruence.
correlation or regression analysis (Podsakoff & Personnel Psychology, 47: 605-623.
Organ, 1986). That is, it can create spurious main Allport, G. W. 1954. The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA:
effects. However, our hypothesis predicts and the Addison-Wesley.
data reveal important moderating effects. All of Amir, Y. 1969. Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 11: 319-342.
these help to strengthen the argument that the
relations uncovered were a function of the con- Antill, J. K. 1984. Sex role complementarity versus similarity in
married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
structs being studied, rather than a methodolog- chology, 23: 13-37.
ical artifact.
Bantel, K. A., & Jackson, S. E. 1989. Top management and
Finally, some other operational features would innovations in banking: Does the composition of the top
strengthen future studies. A broader and larger team make a difference? Strategic Management Journal,
sampling of work groups (with the caveats given 10: 107-124.
above) would strengthen statistical power. And, Barrett, R. S. 1995. Employee selection vifith the performance
although we "triangulated" surface- and deep-level priority survey. Personnel Psychology, 48: 653-662.
characteristics to some extent, it would have been Berger, J., Rosenholtz, S. J., & Zelditch, M. 1980. Status organiz-
helpful to have had other measures of social inte- ing processes. In A. Inkeles, N. J. Smelser, & K. H. Turner
(Eds.), Annual review of sociology, vol. 6: 479-508. Palo
gration. Such measures might be taken from super- Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.
visory reports on the interpersonal harmony of Blau, P. 1977. Inequality and heterogeneity. New York: Free
work groups, from company archives, or from other Press.
records of membership changes. Consideration of Brown, R. J., & Turner, J. C. 1981. Interpersonal and intergroup
other group-level dependent variables, such as behavior. In J. C. Turner & H. Giles (Eds.), Intergroup be-
communication processes (Gersick, 1989) and per- havior: 33-65. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
formance outcomes (Pelled, 1996), would make Bjnrne, D. 1971. The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic
useful extensions to this model. Press.
106 Academy of Management Joumal February

Byrne, D., & Wong, T. 1962. Racial prejudice, interpersonal theory development, data collection, and analysis. Acad-
attraction, and assumed dissimilarity of attitudes. Journal emy of Management Beview, 19: 195-229.
af Abnormal and Sacial Psychology, 65: 246-253.
Konrad, A. M., Winter, S., & Gutek, B. A. 1992. Diversity in work
Campion, M. A., Medsker, G. J., & Higgs, A. C. 1993. Relations group sex composition: Implications for majority and mi-
between work group characteristics and effectiveness: Im- nority members. In P. Tolbert & S. B. Bacharach (Eds.),
plications for designing effective work groups. Personnel Besearch in the sacialagy of organizatians, vol. 10: 115-
Psychology, 46: 823-850. ' 140. Greenwich, GT: JAI Press.
Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. 1986. Organizational information Kraiger, K., & Ford, J. K. 1985. A meta-analysis of ratee race
requirements, media richness and structural design. Afan- effects in performance ratings. Journal af Applied Psychol-
agement Science, 32: 554-571. ogy, 70: 56-65.
Darlington, R. 1990. Regression and linear madels. New York: Kunin, T. 1955. The construction of a new type of attitude
McGraw-Hill. measure. Personnel Psychology, 8: 65-78.
Ellison, C. C, & Powers, D. A. 1994. The contact hypothesis and Locke, E. A. 1976. The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In
racial attitudes among Black Americans. Social Science M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial organization
Quarterly, 75: 385-400. psychology: 1297-1350. New York: Wiley.
Fox. S., Ben-Nahum, Z., & Yinon, Y. 1989. Perceived similarity McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. 1993. Statistical difficulties of
and accuracy of peer ratings. Journal af Applied Psychol- detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological
ogy, 74: 781-786. Bulletin, 114: 376-390.
Gersick, C. J. G. 1989. Marking time: Predictable transitions in McGrath, J. E. .1984. Groups: Interactian and process. Engle-
work groups. Academy af Management Journal, 32: 274- wood Gliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
309.
McGrath, J. E. 1991. Time, interaction, and performance (TIP): A
Gully, S. M., Devine, D. J., & Whitney, D. J. 1995. A meta- theory of groups. Small Group Besearch, 22: 147-174.
analysis of cohesion and performance: Effects of level of
analysis and task interdependence. Small Graup Be- McGrath, J. E., Arrow, H., Gruenfeld, D. H., Hollingshead, A. B.,
search, 26: 497-520. & O'Gonnor, K. M. 1993. Groups, tasks, and technology: The
effects of experience and change. Small Group Besearch,
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. 1975. Development of the Job 24: 406-420.
Diagnostic Survey. Jaumal af Applied Psychalagy, 60:
Meglino, B. M., Ravlin, E. G., & Adkins, G. L. 1989. A work
159-170.
values approach to corporate culture: A field test of the •
Howell, J. P., Dorfman, P. W., & Kerr, S. 1986. Moderator vari- value congruence process and its relationship to individual
ables in leadership research. Academy af Management outcomes. Jaumal of Applied Psychology, 74: 424-432.
Beview, 11: 88-102.
Miles, R. E. 1964. Attitudes toward management theory as a
Jackson, S. E., & Associates (Eds.). 1992. Diversity in the work- factor in managers' relationships with their superiors.
place: Human resaurce initiatives. New York: Gullford Academy af Management, 7: 303-313.
Press.
Milliken, F. J., & Martins, L. L. 1996. Searching for common
Jackson, S. E., Brett, J. F., Sessa, V. I., Gooper, D. M., Julin, J. A., threads: Understanding the multiple effects of diversity in
& Peyronnin, K. 1991. Some differences make a difference: organizational groups. Academy af Management Beview,
Interpersonal dissimilarity and group heterogeneity as cor- 21: 402-433.
relates of recruitment, promotion, and turnover. Joumal af
Applied Psychology, 76: 675-689. Mobley, W. H. 1982. Supervisor and employee race and sex
effects on performance appraisals: A field study of adverse
Jackson, S. E., May, K. E., & Whitney, K. 1995. Understanding impact and generalizability. Academy of Management
the dynamics of diversity in decision-making teams. In R. A. Journal, 25: 598-606.
Guzzo & E. Salas (Eds.), Team decision-making effective-
ness in organizations: 204-261. San Francisco: Jossey- Mudrack, P. E. 1989. Group cohesiveness: A legacy of confu-
Bass. sion? Small Group Behaviar, 20: 37-49.

Jackson, S. E., Stone, V. K., & Alvarez, E. B. 1993. Socialization Newcomb, T. M. 1961. The acquaintance process. New York:
amidst diversity: Impact of demographics on work team Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
oldtimers and newcomers. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw O'Reilly, G. A., Ill, Galdwell, D. F., & Barnett, W. P. 1989.
(Eds.), Research in organizatianal hehaviar, vol. 15: 45- Work group demography, social integration, and turnover.
109. Greenwich, GT: JAI Press. Administrative Science Quarterly, 34: 21-37.
James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. 1982. Estimating within- Pelled, L. H. 1996. Demographic diversity, conflict, and work
group interrater reliability with and without response bias. group outcomes: An intervening process theory. Organiza-
Jaumal af Applied Psychology, 69: 85-98. tian Science, 7: 615-631.
Johnston, W. B., & Packer, A. H. 1987. Workforce 2000: Work Pfeffer, J. 1983. Organizational demography. In L. L. Gummings
and warkers for the 21st century: Indianapolis: Hudson & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Besearch in organizational behaviar,
Institute. vol. 5: 299-357. Greenwich, GT: JAI Press.
Judge, T. A., & Ferris, G. R. 1993. Social context of performance Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. 1986. Self-reports in organiza-
evaluation decisions. Academy af Management Jaumal, tion research: Problems and prospects. Jaumal af Manage-
36: 80-105. ment, 12: 531-544.
Klein, K. J., Dansereau, F., & Hall, R. J. 1994. Levels issues in Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. 1979. Organizational, work, and
1998 Harrison, Price, and Bell 107

personal factors in employee turnover and absenteeism. different: Relational demography and organizational attach-
Psychological Bulletin. 64: 23-51. ment. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37: 549-579.
Pulakos, E. D., Oppler, S. H., White, L. A., & Borman, W. C. 1989. Tsui, A. S., & O'Reilly, C. A., III. 1989. Beyond simple demo-
Examination of race and sex effects on performance ratings. graphic effects: The importance of relational demography in
Journal of Applied Psychology, 74: 770-780. superior-subordinate dyads. Academy of Management
Journal, 32: 402-423.
Rokeach, M. 1973. The nature of human values. New York:
Free Press. Turban, D. B., & Jones, A. P. 1988. Supervisor-subordinate sim-
ilarity: Types, effects, and mechanisms. Journal of Applied
Schneider, B. 1987. The people make the place. Personnel Psy-
Psychology, 73: 228-234.
chology, 40: 437-453.
Turner, J. C. 1987. Rediscovering the social group: A self-
Seashore, S. 1954. Group cohesiveness in the industrial work
categorization theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
group. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research.
Vancouver, J. B., & Schmitt, N. W. 1991. An exploratory exam-
Sengor, J. 1971. Managers' perceptions of subordinates' compe-
ination of person-organization fit: Organizational goal con-
tence. Academy of Management Journal, 14: 415-423.
gruence. Personnel Psychology. 44: 333-352.
Sigelman, L., & Welch, S. 1993. The contact hypothesis revis-
Wagner, W. G., Pfeffer, J., & O'Reilly, C. A., III. 1984. Organiza-
ited: Interracial contact and positive racial attitudes. Social
tional demography and turnover in top-management
Forces, 71: 781-795.
groups. Administrative Science Quarterly. 29: 74-92.
Smith, P., Kendall, L., & Hulin, C. 1969. The measurement
Watson, W., Kumar, K., & Michaelsen, L. K. 1993. Cultural
of satisfaction in work and retirement. Chicago: Rand
diversity's impact on interaction process and performance:
McNally.
Comparing homogeneous and diverse task groups. Acad-
Stangor, C, Lynch, L., Duan, C, & Glass. B. 1992. Categorization emy of Management Journal, 36: 590-602.
of individuals on the basis of multiple social features. Jour-
Wiersema, M. F., & Bird, A. 1993. Organizational demography in
nal of Personality and Social Psychology. 62: 207-218.
Japanese firms: Group heterogeneity, individual dissimilar-
Terborg, ]. R., Castore, C, & DeNinno, J. A. 1976. A longitudinal ity, and top management team turnover. Academy of Man-
field investigation of the impact of group composition on agement Journal, 36: 996-1025.
group performance and cohesion. Journal of Personality
Zalesny, M. D., & Kirsch, M. P. 1989. The effect of similarity on
and Social Psychology. 34: 782-790.
performance ratings and interrater agreement. Human Re-
Tett, R. P., & Meyer, J. P. 1993. Job satisfaction, organizational lations. 42: 81-96.
commitment, turnover intention, and turnover: Path analy-
ses based on meta-analytic findings. Personnel Psychology.
46: 259-293. David A. Harrison earned his Ph.D. from the University of
Illinois and is currently an associate professor o.f management at
Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. 1959. The social psychology of the University of Texas at Arlington. His current research deals
groups. New York: Wiley. with work role adjustment and withdrawal, temporal issues in
Triandis, H. C. 1961. A note on Rokeach's theory of prejudice. organizations, and measurement.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 16: 81-98.
Kenneth H. Price is an associate professor of management at
Triandis, H. C. 1989. The self and social behavior in differing the University of Texas at Arlington. He received his Ph.D. from
cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96: 506-520. Michigan State University. His current research interests are in
Tsui, A. S., Egan, T. D., & Porter, L. W. 1994. Performance the areas of group dynamics and procedural justice.
implications of relational demography in vertical dyads. Myrtle P. Bell received her Ph.D. from the University of Texas
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Academy of at Arlington, where she is an assistant professor of management.
Management, Dallas. Her current research interests include workforce diversity and
Tsui, A. S., Egan, T. D., & O'Reilly, C. A., III. 1992. Being its consequences and meaning for organization members.

You might also like