You are on page 1of 5

Developmental Neurorehabilitation, August 2010; 13(4): 234–238

Developmental profiles from the Battelle developmental inventory:


A comparison of toddlers diagnosed with Down Syndrome, global
developmental delay and premature birth

JOHNNY L. MATSON, JULIE A. HESS, MEGAN SIPES, & MAX HOROVITZ

Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, USA

(Received 23 February 2010; accepted 28 February 2010)


Dev Neurorehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Universitat de Girona on 11/20/14

Abstract
Objective: Developmental profiles and milestone attainment have been examined for children suffering from various
developmental disabilities. However, research comparing the same across numerous developmental disabilities is scant.
Method: Therefore, the purpose of the current study was to examine the developmental profiles of toddlers (i.e. aged 17–34
months) who were premature, diagnosed with Down Syndrome, or diagnosed with Global Developmental Delay. A total of
28 toddlers met inclusion criteria for the study.
Results: Those diagnosed with Global Developmental Delay or Down Syndrome scored significantly lower on the Battelle
Developmental Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-2), compared to those who were born premature. More specifically,
differences emerged on the BDI-2 domains of personal-social and motor.
Conclusion: Implications of these findings and directions for future research are discussed.

Keywords: BDI-2, atypically developing, toddlers, developmental milestones, Down Syndrome, premature
For personal use only.

Resumen
Objetivo: Se han examinado los perfiles de desarrollo y de adquisición de hitos para niños que sufren de múltiples
discapacidades del desarrollo. Sin embargo, las investigaciones comparando lo mismo a través de múltiples discapacidades
del desarrollo son escasas.
Método: Por lo tanto, el propósito del presente estudio fue examinar los perfiles de desarrollo de niños (esto es, 17 a 34
meses de edad) prematuros, diagnosticados con sı́ndrome de Down, o diagnosticados del retraso global del desarrollo. Un
total de 28 niños cumplieron con los criterios de inclusión para el estudio.
Resultados: Aquellos niños diagnosticados con retraso global de desarrollo o con sı́ndrome de Down arrojaron calificaciones
más bajas en el Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-2), comparados con aquellos que nacieron
prematuramente. Especı́ficamente, las diferencias emergieron en los dominios motor y personal-social del BDI-2.
Conclusión: Se discuten las implicaciones de estos hallazgos y direcciones para futuras investigaciones.

Palabras clave: BDI-2, desarrollo atı́pico, niños, hitos del desarrollo, sı́ndrome de Down, prematurez

Introduction Developmental assessment should play a useful role


in identifying children who are at risk and for
Developmental disabilities are life-long and affect all
identifying specific skills that may require early
aspects of the person’s life [1–3]. The identification
intervention [11–13].
and prioritizing of skill deficits in very young
Considerable literature has emerged looking at
children remains one of the greatest challenges [4,
developmental milestones across a host of develop-
5]. As a result, detecting the signs of neurological
mental disabilities. These disorders include genetic
delay as early as possible has become a priority
forms of intellectual disability, seizures, Down syn-
among researchers and clinicians [6]. Some drome, and general global delays. However, typically
researchers have demonstrated that some beha- only one disability is examined in a particular study
vioural deficits can be identified at birth to 20 vs comparing multiple disabilities, and most of the
weeks of age [7]. However, behaviour does not research tends to focus on newborns or school-aged
develop in a uniform fashion. Thus, a continuous children [14–16]. In addition, researchers
and finer grained analysis should be ongoing [8–10]. have examined development across different

Correspondence: Johnny L. Matson, Ph.D., 324 Audubon Hall, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA. E-mail: johnmatson@aol.com
ISSN 1751–8423 print/ISSN 1751–8431 online/10/040234–5 ß 2010 Informa UK Ltd.
DOI: 10.3109/17518421003736032
Developmental profiles 235

developmental disability groups; however, this developmental disability. EarlySteps is Louisiana’s


research has tended to focus on only one or a few Early Intervention System under the Individuals
areas of development (e.g. language, social skills with Disabilities Education Act, Part C, which
and/or communication) [17]. provides services to infants and toddlers and their
The present study is an extension of the existing families from birth to 36 months. Children qualify
literature by comparing the developmental strengths for these services if they have a developmental delay
and deficits across a number of developmental or a medical condition likely to result in a develop-
disabilities in toddlers, 17–34 months of age. mental delay as identified by their family paediatri-
Comparisons among toddlers with Down syndrome, cian. Exclusion criteria for the current study
global developmental delay and premature birth eliminated any toddlers who had a diagnosis of an
were made using a well established scale, the BDI-2 Autism Spectrum Disorder.
[18]. Thus, the aim of this study was exploratory and
conducted to determine how the above-mentioned Measures
group of toddlers develop in relation to each
Battelle Developmental Inventory, 2nd Edition (BDI-2)
Dev Neurorehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Universitat de Girona on 11/20/14

other across multiple domains of development


[18]. The BDI-2 is used to assess developmental
(i.e. personal/social, adaptive, motor, communica-
skills in children from birth to 7 years 11 months.
tion and cognitive).
Using observation and informant report, the 450
items assess five domains of development: personal/
social, adaptive, motor, communication and cogni-
Method tive. Items are answered on a three-point scale:
Participants 0 ¼ ‘no ability in this skill’, 1 ¼ ‘emerging ability’ or
2 ¼ ‘ability at this skill’. Each domain in the BDI-2
Twenty-eight participants met inclusion criteria for yields a developmental quotient (DQ) for that area.
this study. The participants in this study (i.e. infants The five domain areas are summed to yield a total
For personal use only.

and toddlers) ranged in age from 17–34 months DQ. The total DQ has a mean of 100 and a standard
(M ¼ 24.93 months, SD ¼ 5.20). Within the sample, deviation of 15. For this study, the DQ’s for each
57.1% was male. The ethnicity of the participants domain and the total DQ were of interest. The
was 42.9% African American and 57.1% Caucasian. BDI-2 has shown acceptable test–re-test reliability
Based on their diagnosis, participants were and excellent internal consistency. In addition, the
sectioned into three groups: Group 1, Down BDI-2 has content validity and criterion validity at
syndrome (n ¼ 8); Group 2, Global Developmental acceptable levels [18].
Delay (n ¼ 10); and Group 3, Premature (n ¼ 10).
No participant had more than one of the above
Procedure
conditions (e.g. Down Syndrome and premature).
Diagnoses were reported by informants (i.e. parents For each participant, parents or caretakers were
or guardians) during the assessment process. Table I interviewed by qualified personnel who provide
includes demographic information for each of the services for the State of Louisiana’s Early Steps
three groups utilized in this study. Program. The assessors, occupational therapists,
All participants for this study were obtained physical therapists, social workers, speech-language
through their enrolment in a state-funded early pathologists, educators or psychologists were certi-
intervention programme for children at-risk for a fied or licensed in their respective disciplines. The
BDI-2 and other measures were administered to
the caregivers in a quiet area with the child present.
Table I. Demographic information. For the current study, non-identifying demographic
information, as well as BDI-2 scores were used. This
Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 study was approved by the state of Louisiana’s Office
Age for citizens with Developmental Disabilities
M (SD) 25.12 (6.03) 26.10 (5.51) 23.60 (4.35) (OCDD) and by the Louisiana State University
Range 17–33 19–34 18–33 Institutional Review Board.
Gender
Male 50.0% 80.0% 40.0% Research design
Female 50.0% 20.0% 60.0%
Ethnicity In order to ensure groups did not differ significantly
Caucasian 87.5% 30.0% 60.0% on demographic variables, Chi square analyses were
African-American 12.5% 70.0% 40.0% employed. The three groups did not differ signifi-
Group membership is as follows: Group 1, Downs Syndrome; cantly in regard to gender, 2(2) ¼ 3.50, p ¼ 0.17;
Group 2, global developmental delay; Group 3, premature. but differed significantly in regards to ethnicity,
236 J. L. Matson et al.

2(2) ¼ 6.05, p ¼ 0.049. To ensure age did not All three groups had total DQs significantly
significantly differ between groups, an analysis of less than the mean total DQ of the BDI-2.
variance (ANOVA) was conducted. Groups did not Therefore, analyses were conducted to determine if
significantly differ on age, F(2, 25) ¼ 0.567, p ¼ 0.57. the three group’s total DQs differed significantly
Next, three separate one sample t-tests were per- from each other. An ANCOVA was conducted to
formed to determine if each groups mean total DQ examine group differences on the BDI-2 Total
significantly differed from the mean total DQ of the Developmental Quotient. A significant difference
BDI-2 (i.e. 100). For these three t-tests, alpha was between groups was found, F(2, 24) ¼ 8.15,
set to 0.017 to control for multiple tests. Third, an p 5 0.05. Post-hoc analyses revealed that the
Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) was employed Premature group (M ¼ 90.90, SD ¼ 7.14) differed
to examine if there was a difference between groups significantly from both the Down Syndrome group
on the total DQ. Group membership was utilized as (M ¼ 72.50, SD ¼ 10.53) and the Global
the independent variable, BDI-2 total DQ as the Developmental Delay group (M ¼ 71.20, SD ¼
dependent variable and ethnicity as the covariate. 15.85). That is, participants with Down Syndrome
Dev Neurorehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Universitat de Girona on 11/20/14

Post-hoc analyses were utilized to determine which and Global Developmental Delays had significantly
groups differed significantly. lower total DQs when compared to those who
Lastly, to determine if groups varied significantly were Premature. Those diagnosed with Global
on the specific domains of the BDI-2 (personal/ Developmental Delay and Down syndrome did not
social, adaptive, motor, communication and cogni- significantly differ from each other on their total
tive), a multivariate analysis of covariance DQ score.
(MANCOVA) was conducted using the DQ from Next, a MANCOVA was conducted to determine
the five BDI-2 domains as dependent variables, if the five groups differed in regards to scores on the
group membership as the independent variable and five domains of the BDI-2. There was a significant
ethnicity as the covariate. By ensuring the no group difference between groups in regards to the sub-scale
was 1.5-times larger than the smallest group;
For personal use only.

scores, F(10, 40) ¼ 2.23, p 5 0.05. Table II displays


assumptions of the MANCOVA were controlled the mean score for each group on each sub-scale of
for [19]. An a priori power analysis using GPOWER the BDI-2. This significant result was followed up
was conducted to determine the required sample size with an ANCOVA for each of the five domain areas.
[20]. Based on this analysis, using a medium effect Bonferroni correctional procedures were used to
size with the level of significance set at ¼ 0.05 and control for the inflation of Type I errors. First,
power at 0.80 a sample size of 24 was required. To groups differed on the Personal-Social Domain
follow up significant results of the MANCOVA, Developmental Quotient, F(2, 24) ¼ 5.88, p 5 0.01.
individual ANCOVAs were used to assess group The Premature group (M ¼ 99.60, SD ¼ 10.95) had
differences on each domain of the BDI-2. In order to a significantly higher Developmental Quotient than
control for inflated alpha error due to running the Global Developmental Delay group (M ¼ 82.80,
multiple ANCOVAs, a Bonferroni correction was SD ¼ 8.79). The Down Syndrome group was not
used. Thus, the alpha level was set at 0.01 for each of significantly different from either the premature or
the five ANOVAs conducted. Global Developmental Delay group. Secondly, sig-
nificant differences were found between groups on
the Motor Domain Developmental Quotient,
Results F(2, 24) ¼ 9.56, p 5 0.01. The Premature group
First, three separate one sample t-tests were per- (M ¼ 103.70, SD ¼ 6.60) had a significantly higher
formed and compared against a test value of 100 (i.e. Developmental Quotient than both the Down
BDI-2 DQ, M ¼ 100). These analyses were per- Syndrome group (M ¼ 76.63, SD ¼ 15.31) and the
formed to determine if each of the three groups total
mean DQ differed significantly from mean DQ for
Table II. Mean (SD) DQ per group.
the BDI-2 (i.e. 100). The total mean DQ of all three
groups differed significantly from the mean of the Group 1 Group 2 Group 3
BDI-2. That is, all groups had a significantly lower
Total DQ score 72.50 (10.53) 71.20 (15.85) 90.90 (7.14)
total DQ when compared to the mean DQ of the Sub-scales
BDI-2 (Groups Means are displayed in Table II). Adaptive 72.50 (12.94) 72.30 (17.90) 88.70 (16.03)
Results of the one sample t-tests are as follows: Personal–Social 87.38 (12.63) 82.80 (8.79) 99.60 (10.95)
Communication 67.63 (7.17) 74.60 (15.44) 79.50 (15.32)
Group 1 (Down Syndrome), t(7) ¼ 7.387, Motor 76.63 (15.31) 81.50 (18.06) 103.70 (6.60)
p 5 0.017; Group 2 (Global Developmental Cognitive 80.88 (12.08) 72.00 (13.41) 86.20 (6.53)
Delay), t(9) ¼ 5.745, p 5 0.017; and Group 3 Group membership is as follows: Group 1, Downs Syndrome;
(Premature), t(9) ¼ 4.030, p 5 0.017. Group 2, global developmental delay; Group 3, premature.
Developmental profiles 237
Dev Neurorehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Universitat de Girona on 11/20/14
For personal use only.

Figure 1. Mean developmental quotient for each group across each of the five domains of the BDI. Domains are as follows: (1) Adaptive,
(2) Personal-Social, (3) Communication, (4) Motor, and (5) Cognitive.

Global Developmental Delay group (M ¼ 81.50, childhood. However, differences between the three
SD ¼ 18.06). However, the Down syndrome and groups surfaced when examining the developmental
Global developmental delay groups were not signif- quotient in the Motor Domain and the Personal-
icantly different from each other. Figure 1 displays Social Domain.
the developmental quotient of each sub-scale for As evinced by these results, those diagnosed with
each group. Global Developmental Delay or Down syndrome
had significantly more problems in motor develop-
ment compared to those who were premature. Also,
Discussion those with Global Developmental Delay had the
greatest deficits in the personal-social domain com-
The current study confirmed that toddlers with pared to the other two groups. Thus, on both of
various forms of developmental disabilities experi- these domains, those who were premature had
ence delays in the attainment of developmental significantly higher DQs, indicating fewer problems
milestones. However, the toddlers most affected in with attaining milestones in these areas. This is
terms of development were those diagnosed with a consistent with other research, which has shown that
Global Developmental Delay or Down syndrome. 2-year-olds who were born premature were compa-
Although toddlers who were born premature had rable in their development to children who were full
significant deficits in development (i.e. when com- term [21]. However, the same authors indicated that
pared to the mean BDI-2 score), they were not as at 1-year of age; the above-mentioned does not
developmentally delayed when compared to toddlers hold true.
with concomitant problems (i.e. diagnosis of Down Therefore, future studies would benefit from
syndrome and experiencing developmental delays) examining developmental profiles of those diagnosed
or those with a diagnosis of Global Developmental with Down syndrome or Global Developmental
Delay. Furthermore, no significant differences were Delay at specific ages. Conducting such an investi-
found when examining the domains of adaptive, gation would allow for researchers to determine
communication and cognitive skills; thus, differences whether differences in development (i.e. across
within these developmental domains may not be as personal/social, adaptive, motor, communication
pronounced at this age and simply emerge later in and cognitive domains) emerge at sensitive time
238 J. L. Matson et al.

periods. For example, specific language impairments research in a diverse preterm group. European Journal of
in those diagnosed with Down syndrome primarily Paediatric Neurology 2010;14:131–137.
7. American Academy for Pediatrics. Screening for develop-
emerge from the pre-school to school-age years [22]. mental disabilities. Pediatrics 1986;78:526–528.
Knowing this information would help establish 8. Diamond KE. The role of parents’ observations and concerns
intervention services prior to these susceptible ages. on screening for developmental delays in young children.
Lastly, weeks or months premature were not Topics in Early Special Education 1993;13:68–81.
accounted for in the current study for Group 3 9. Frankenburg WK. Preventing developmental delays: Is
developmental screening sufficient? Pediatrics
(premature group). An earlier birth (e.g. months 1994;93:586–593.
premature vs weeks premature) could possibly 10. Tsao R, Kindelberger C. Variability of cognitive development
account for within-group differences regarding in children with Down syndrome: Relevance of good reasons
achieving developmental milestones. Thus, effects for using the cluster procedure. Research in Developmental
on development from those born ‘more’ premature Disabilities 2009;30:426–432.
11. Berls AT, McEwen IR. Batelle developmental inventory.
could be negated when combining all toddlers born Physical Therapy 1999;79:776–783.
premature into one group. Future research of this 12. Matson JL, Boisjoli JA. An overview of developments in
Dev Neurorehabil Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Universitat de Girona on 11/20/14

sort may benefit from partitioning out those born research on persons with intellectual disabilities. Research in
‘more’ or ‘less’ prematurely than others and includ- Developmental Disabilities 2009;30:587–591.
ing them in the analyses as separate groups. 13. Matson JL, Mahan S, LoVullo SV. Parent training: A
review of methods for children with developmental disabil-
ities. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2009;30:
961–968.
14. Msall ME, Rogers BT, Ripstein H, Lyon N, Wilczenski F.
Declaration of Interest: The authors report no Measurements of functional outcomes in children with
cerebral palsy. Mental Retardation and Developmental
conflicts of interest. The authors alone are respon-
Disabilities Research 1998;3:194–203.
sible for the content and writing of the paper. 15. Shevell M, Majnemer A, Platt RW, Webster R, Birnbaum R.
Developmental and functional outcomes in children with
For personal use only.

global developmental delay or developmental language


impairment. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology
References
2005;47:678–683.
1. Matson JL, Baglio CS, Smiroldo BB, Hamilton M, 16. Weitzner-Lin B. A comparison of intentional communication
Packlowskyj T, Williams D, Kirkpatrick-Sanchez S. in children who have Down syndrome with typical children
Characteristics of autism as assessed by the Diagnostic matched for developmental and chronological age. Infant-
Assessment for the Severely Handicapped-II (DASH-II). Toddler Intervention 1997;7:123–132.
Research in Developmental Disabilities 1996;17:135–143. 17. Rodrigue JR, Morgan SB, Geffken GR. A comparative
2. Nieuwenhuis-Mark RE. Diagnosing Alzheimer’s evaluation of adaptive behavior in children and adolescents
dementia in Down syndrome: Problems and possible solu- with autism, Down syndrome, and normal development.
tions. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2009;30: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1991;21:
827–838. 187–196.
3. Rose E, Bramham J, Young S, Paliokostas E, 18. Newborg J. Battelle developmental inventory-second
Xenitidis K. Neuropsychological characteristics of adults edition. Itasca, IL: Riverside; 2005.
with comorbid ADHD and borderline/mild intellectual 19. Tabachnick BC, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics.
disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2009;30: 5th ed. Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc; 2007.
496–502. 20. Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A. GPOWER: A general power
4. Fodstad JC, Matson JL, Hess J, Neal D. Social and commu- analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments,
nication behaviours in infants and toddlers with autism and & Computers 1996;28:1–11.
pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified. 21. Ouden LD, Rijkin M, Brand R, Verloove-Vanhorick SP,
Developmental Neurorehabilitation 2009;12:152–157. Ruys JH. Is it correct to correct? Developmental milestone in
5. Matson JL, Wilkins J. Psychometric testing methods for 555 ‘normal’ preterm infants compared with term infants.
children’s social skills. Research in Developmental The Journal of Pediatrics 1991;118:399–404.
Disabilities 2009;30:249–274. 22. Chapman RS. Language development in children and ado-
6. Kodric J, Sustersic B, Paro-Panjan D. Assessment of general lescents with Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and
movements and 2.5 years developmental outcomes: Pilot Developmental Disabilities 1997;3:307–312.

You might also like