You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Business Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbusres

Representing another nation: The influence of foreign citizen ambassadors T


on product evaluations

Nathalie Spielmanna, , Elizabeth A. Mintonb
a
Marketing, NEOMA Business School, 59 rue Pierre Taittinger, 51100 Reims, France
b
Marketing, College of Business, University of Wyoming, Dept. 3275, 1000 E University Ave, Laramie, WY 82071, USA

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Through two studies in two different nations, this research examines the nationality of non-celebrity spokes-
National identity people (i.e., a citizen ambassador) and answers the question, how does the nationality of a citizen ambassador
Advertising influence attitudes toward a product being advertised? Merging meaning transfer and schema congruity theories,
Spokesperson effects the results show that the use of citizen ambassadors of a different (same) nationality as the consumer leads to
Product attitudes
higher product attitudes, but only if the product is of the same (different) nationality as the consumer (Study 1).
Citizen ambassadors
Consumers low in identity salience experience these effects the most, such that the citizen ambassador is more
influential on perceptions of nationally representative foods, in comparison to consumers high in identity sal-
ience (Study 2). Moderate incongruity (as opposed to mild or high incongruity) appears to be a more potent
explanation for the effects of citizen ambassadors on product attitudes toward nationally representative pro-
ducts, especially in online advertising.

1. Introduction country) (Braun, Kavaratzis, & Zenker, 2013; Rehmet & Dinnie, 2013).
We build on the research supporting meaning transfer as explaining
Consuming certain products helps consumers affirm, confirm, and spokesperson effects (Kamins & Gupta, 1994), to show that meaning
signal identity positions, including national identity (Aronczyk, 2013). transfer research should also consider schema congruity (Mandler,
As such, marketers may use native spokespeople in their advertising to 1984; Meyers-Levy & Tybout, 1989) as a more comprehensive theore-
render national identity more salient for consumers. For example, tical framework explaining spokesperson effects when nationality is the
Brazilian supermodel Gisele Bündchen is the spokesperson for the em- focal characteristic. Specifically, this research examines how meaning
blematic Brazilian sandal brand Ipanema. Yet the market is also full of transfer occurs as a result of perceived congruity, similar to the pro-
examples of non-native endorsers of nationally representative products. cedure used by Misra and Beatty (1990). Stated simply, we merge
For example, American astronaut Buzz Aldrin is a spokesperson for meaning transfer and schema congruity theories together to provide a
Swiss tourism. Much of the past research on nationality effects in ad- more thorough understanding of spokesperson effects when nationality
vertising examines the influence of national identity in advertising is a focal characteristic of the spokesperson and/or product. Ad-
(e.g., Western ads in China; Zhao & Belk, 2008), but provides little ditionally, this research postulates and demonstrates consumer pre-
insight on the role of a spokesperson in supporting (vs. not supporting) ference for moderate incongruity in their evaluation of citizen ambas-
national identity representations of products in advertising. Ad- sador advertising. Consequently, this research adds to the existing
ditionally, prior research shows that explicit identity marketing can stream of research on the value of moderate incongruity in advertising
backfire (Bhattacharjee, Berger, & Menon, 2014), providing an avenue and product evaluation (Campbell & Goodstein, 2001; Mandler, 1984;
for marketers to use subtle nationality references by citizen ambassa- Meyers-Levy & Tybout, 1989) by extending it into the spokesperson
dors. domain.
This research examines the case of nationality as an interpreted Meaning transfer theory (McCracken, 1989) suggests attitudes are
identity cue in spokesperson advertising, with a specific focus on non- influenced by cognitive assimilation because symbolic properties of a
celebrity spokespersons, which are referred to here as citizen ambas- spokesperson “move from celebrity to consumer good and from good to
sadors. Citizen ambassadors are conceptualized as residents of a place consumer” (p. 310). As such, congruent symbolic properties, for ex-
who can become spokespersons for their brand (i.e., their city or ample between a spokesperson and a brand (c.f., Misra & Beatty, 1990),


Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: nathalie.spielmann@neoma-bs.fr (N. Spielmann), eminton@uwyo.edu (E.A. Minton).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2018.05.035
Received 11 January 2018; Received in revised form 23 May 2018; Accepted 24 May 2018
Available online 30 May 2018
0148-2963/ © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

result in more positive marketing outcomes. The research herein posits 2.2. Citizen ambassador effects in advertising
a moderating effect to this theoretical approach, such that when the
focal feature of the citizen ambassador is their nationality, consumer Currently, most advertising research focuses on the importance of
attitudes may be improved when moderate incongruity is present due to celebrity spokespeople in general advertising (c.f., Erdogan (1999) for a
cognitive assimilation. In other words, consumers may report greater review of celebrity endorsers). Celebrity spokesperson use in adver-
satisfaction for a product after they must cognitively process dissim- tising is prevalent because use of these spokespeople is expected to add
ilarity between a product's national origin and that of their citizen credibility to products (Bartz, Molchanov, & Stork, 2013; Petroshius &
ambassador. For example, Canadian consumers may expect a Canadian Crocker, 1989). Additionally, celebrity endorsers have a social influ-
to speak of maple syrup in advertising, but they may also be more re- ence that affects message acceptability (Grewal, Gotlieb, &
ceptive if the spokesperson is Japanese because this requires more Marmorstein, 1994; Kelman, 1961; Marshall, Na, & Deuskar, 2008).
cognitive processing due to the unexpected pairing of the two nation- Taken together, the results on celebrity spokespeople suggest that their
alities. influence is rooted in their superior social status, social capital, or
In order to show this effect, the research herein examines how the visibility.
nationalities of a product, citizen ambassador, and consumer interact. However, while celebrities carry their own appeal, they may not be
Building on previous literature regarding congruence between the accessible to all advertisers. As well, qualifying the effects that occur
product/brand and spokesperson characteristics (Choi & Rifon, 2012; when spokespeople are celebrities also brings to light situations when
Miller & Allen, 2012), this research seeks to incrementally add to the spokespeople are not outright celebrities. Depending on the product
message source-recipient literature in terms of (a) focusing on national type or the brand positioning, it may be more appealing for consumers
identity as a source characteristic, (b) testing the influence of citizen to see “regular people” as spokespeople. This is the case for brands like
ambassador nationality on product attitudes, and (c) distinguishing TJ Maxx in the USA that in 2014 decided to feature actual shoppers in
nationality congruence effects from ethnocentrism (which could be their advertising campaign (Feldman, 2014) or the ‘Unlimited You’
considered an alternative mechanism). Additionally, identity threat is campaign by Nike featuring regular men and women in their adver-
examined as a mediator and identity salience as a moderator to these tising (Verry, 2016). There are many types of spokespeople a company
relationships, testing the important role that identity plays in under- may use in advertising; we focus in particular on citizen ambassadors,
standing consumer response to citizen ambassador use in advertising. or rather non-celebrity spokespeople that represent a particular nation
Consequently, this research sheds light on the implications of nation- or place. This is most prevalent in place brand marketing (e.g., using
ality as a potentially important characteristic, thereby reorienting the residents as spokespeople in city advertising; Rehmet & Dinnie, 2013).
discussion from product to ambassador origin and demonstrating the The use of such spokespeople (versus celebrities) is meant to add
necessity of this area of research. credibility to brand messages via perceived congruence between the
consumer and the spokesperson (i.e., both residents of the same place)
or between the spokesperson and the product (i.e., a resident promoting
2. Literature review their own city) (Braun et al., 2013). It is expected that congruence
between a citizen ambassador and a product would result in favorable
2.1. National identity salience in advertising marketing outcomes because congruence between the consumer and
the brand (Misra & Beatty, 1990) or between a spokesperson and a
The influence of identity on consumption behavior is particularly product (Garretson & Niedrich, 2004; Kamins & Gupta, 1994) result in
strong when one's identity is made salient via market cues, such as more favorable marketing outcomes. However, Heider's (1958) balance
symbols on products and in advertising (Elias & Greenspan, 2007; theory also stipulates that attributional processing can result when
Forehand, Deshpandé, & Reed II, 2002). The same can be said for na- tension, such as incongruent stimuli, is presented. In particular, con-
tional identity, which is often made salient via consumer practices sumers may seek to understand why a source may endorse a message
(Lalonde, 2002) or national cues and symbols on products (Spielmann, incongruent with a consumer's expectations. This is interesting as while
2016). For instance, the US beverage manufacturer, Musselman's, af- it might be common logic to use a national spokesperson to promote a
fixes an “American Grown Apples” label on its bottles of apple cider to national product, there may be more marketing benefits if a national
remind American consumers of the importance of sustaining American product is promoted by a non-national spokesperson. The moderate
companies. National symbols can be implicit or explicit in advertising, incongruity generated by the aforementioned scenario may lead to in-
but nonetheless aim to arouse the national identity of consumers and creased processing that then sparks assimilation and thus more positive
help consumers maintain a distinct connection to their nation (Carvalho attitudes (as per Mandler, 1984; Meyers-Levy & Tybout, 1989 and
& Luna, 2014; Dimofte, Forehand, & Deshpande, 2003; Yoo & Lee, Campbell & Goodstein, 2001).
2016). However, overtly explicit identity references can also backfire, Yet discussion about in/congruence based on nationality or how
leading consumers to feel less agency in identity expression spokespeople can promote products carrying national value is notice-
(Bhattacharjee et al., 2014). ably absent from the spokesperson literature. For example, national
The more an identity is important or rendered important to a con- culture is shown to moderate the way individuals and firms use in-
sumer, the more appealing consumers will find products related to that formation from others (Money, Gilly, & Graham, 1998), and consumers
identity (Forehand & Deshpandé, 2001; Kleine, Kleine, & Kernan, tend to donate larger sums of money in response to donation requests
1993), thereby provoking homophily perceptions. Homophily is a ten- from domestic rather than international spokespeople (Einolf,
dency by individuals to prefer and trust what is similar to them Philbrick, & Slay, 2013). However, little research specifically examines
(McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2011). Often modeled in social how congruence between the nationality of a citizen ambassador, the
networks and online contexts (e.g., word of mouth online), homophily consumer, and the product influences marketing outcomes. Stated
can be conceptualized as congruence between an individual and an- simply, little research examines congruency effects between the iden-
other individual based on socio-demographic characteristics such as tities of citizen ambassadors, consumers, and products. At the same
age, gender, education, or nationality (Davis, 1999; Nitzan & Libai, time, conceptual work hints at the importance of source credibility in
2011). Consequently, the meaning of nationality and the salience of motivating consumers to process incongruent information (Lee &
national identity should depend on a certain perceived level of con- Schumann, 2004). In other words, if a credible source promotes a
gruence between a sender and a receiver (e.g., between a citizen am- message, a brand, or a product that is different from a consumer's ex-
bassador and a consumer). pectations, then the consumer may be more inclined to process the
advertising message in order to uncover why the source is willing to

410
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

promote it. Specifically, patriotic consumers feel a sense of pride when they are
In light of the literature on spokesperson effects and source cred- primed to think about the traditions, history, and cultural symbols of
ibility, we now focus on the features of a spokesperson that can make their nation (Kemmelmeier & Winter, 2008; Mummendey, Klink, &
them seem credible, beyond celebrity status. While some work has been Brown, 2001). By priming national and cultural symbols, but in a
conducted on brand ambassadors (c.f., Kalandies, Andersson, & Ekman, slightly incongruent or unexpected fashion (e.g., with citizen ambas-
2009) and place ambassadors (Braun et al., 2013), little work focuses sadors from a different national background than a product), consumers
on product ambassadors. Places can be conceptualized as products, but are reminded of what they stand for as well as encouraged to maintain a
there are also products that are symbolic of places (e.g., fine watches connection to their culture. Finally, when there are too many incon-
and Switzerland; wine and France, etc.). Product ambassadors, like gruities, such as when the nationality between a product, a citizen
market mavens (Feick & Price, 1987), are consumers, not celebrities, ambassador and a consumer are all different, then major incongruity
who have additional information and exceptional knowledge about would occur. This could in turn increase resistance by consumers, such
products that they willingly share with other consumers. Often, product as when market offerings are incongruent to a consumer's self-concept,
ambassadors and/or market mavens acquire knowledge given personal leading consumers to potentially reject the incongruity all together
motivation and involvement in a product category (Zaichkowsky, (Galvagno, 2011; Lee & Schumann, 2004).
1994). For example, a consumer who appreciates wine and consumes a In this research, we make a distinction between ethnocentrism (i.e.,
lot of it might over time and via regular wine-related activities become how one evaluates another culture in relation to their own culture) and
a market maven for wine within their social group. identity salience as the vector of incongruity assimilation for citizen
However, when the product is also representative of a social group, ambassadors. Whereas product cues and spokespeople can stimulate
such as being nationally representative, one might consider the con- nationality cognitions, these are explicit cues that are made salient and
sumer to be a citizen ambassador. Such consumers have a connection to primed through marketing communications, such as advertisements.
the place and are able to promote the image and features of a place Pride encourages involvement and therefore desire to resolve incon-
(Kalandies et al., 2009). Thus, citizen ambassadors can be recognized gruity because the cues are personally relevant to the consumer
for their representation of a place/nation or from their ability to market (Kemmelmeier & Winter, 2008; Lee & Schumann, 2004). Alternatively,
a product symbolic of a place. As such, these consumers become cred- ethnocentrism is an implicit processing mechanism related to social
ible sources of information (Wilton & Myers, 1986). Yet, the influence behavior that results in automatic attitudes. In contrast to implicit
of these citizen ambassadors with an identified nationality remains processing that can occur without the consumer's awareness, the con-
under-examined. To address this gap, the research herein examines two sumer is often engaged and aware of explicit processing. An example of
issues: a) the use of citizen ambassadors with an identified nationality this is the explicit processing that activated by nationality-related cues
and their influence on product attitudes, and b) the role of perceived in advertising and can be controlled by the consumer, thereby being
identity threat as well as nationality identity prominence in explaining sufficiently strong to trump implicit processing mechanisms, such as
the influence on citizen ambassadors. ethnocentrism (MacDonald, 2008). In particular, explicitly represented
As such, this research moves away from the already researched goals can control implicit negative attitudes (Wheeler & Fiske, 2005).
domain of celebrity spokespeople and focuses exclusively on “regular As such, we posit that when nationality cues are explicit (e.g., stating an
people” as spokespeople (i.e., citizen ambassadors) in addition to ex- origin, stating a nationality, clearly outlined country cues, recognizable
amining how their nationality can influence product attitudes. The accents), they activate the more explicit-processed and response of
studies herein examine citizen ambassador effects using two different pride rather than the more implicit-processed response of ethno-
advertising contexts - online video blogs and social media posts. The centrism.
context of online advertising is interesting for two reasons. First, be- The country of origin literature supports this theory of moderate
cause of increased spending in online media, and second, because of the incongruity among brands, manufacturing origins, and brand cues in
ease with which citizen ambassadors of varying nationalities can in- showing that such incongruity positively influences attitudes (Carvalho,
teract with each other using this advertising medium. Samu, & Sivaramakrishnam, 2011). Past advertising research suggests
that some congruency between endorsers and a product is necessary for
3. Conceptual framework improved product evaluations (Wright, 2016). Thus, product-consumer
congruity with a native citizen ambassador should lead to assimilation
In accordance with meaning transfer theory (McCracken, 1989), this whereas a citizen ambassador with a differing national identity than the
research suggests that nationality is a source of perceived homophily product or consumer should create contrast. This contrast should then
between a citizen ambassador and a consumer. Thus, if a product is of lead to more positive attitudes toward the product, inclusive of heigh-
the same national background as a consumer and the citizen ambas- tened pride, due to the greater processing involved. Likewise, moderate
sador, then shared identity should be salient to the consumer, making incongruity with shared national identity between the citizen ambas-
the product more attractive (Forehand et al., 2002). This scenario re- sador and the consumer partnered with a product of a different na-
presents assimilation (Mandler, 1984) where the nationality schema of tionality should result in cognitive assimilation as well as more positive
the citizen ambassador, the product, and the consumer is congruent for product evaluations.
the consumer. However, assimilation stimulates lower levels of in- Stated another way, moderate incongruity should lead consumers to
formation processing in comparison to nationality scenarios that are greater processing of advertising, necessitating cognitive assimilation,
slightly incongruent (Mandler, 1984; Meyers-Levy & Tybout, 1989), and this presumably greater level of central processing (Meyers-Levy &
such as when a citizen ambassador and product are representative of Tybout, 1989) should produce more positive attitudes. Such incon-
different nationalities. When faced with moderate nationality incon- gruity can occur in two forms. First, it can be in the form of a citizen
gruity between a citizen ambassador and a product or between a citizen ambassador of a different nationality in comparison to the consumer
ambassador and a consumer, consumers have to cognitively assimilate, promoting a nationally-representative product (i.e., incongruity be-
which requires greater processing and results in potentially higher tween a citizen ambassador and a consumer's nationality). Second,
product attitudes. moderate incongruity can be in the form of a citizen ambassador of the
This greater processing should lead to more positive affect because same nationality as the consumer promoting a non-nationally re-
processing incongruity stimulates a consumer's central processing me- presentative product (i.e., incongruity between the nationalities of the
chanism due to the source credibility of a spokesperson or personal citizen ambassador/consumer and the product's origin). This theorizing
relevance of a product (Lee & Schumann, 2004), particularly when builds off Spielmann's (2016) approach in examining the number of
consumers value the nationality of the spokesperson or product. elements that are congruent or incongruent in identifying the effects of

411
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

origin cues on product attitudes, with the moderate levels of (in)con- To address these hypotheses, Study 1 examines the influence of
gruity between cues having the strongest effects on product attitudes. congruent and incongruent citizen ambassadors for national products
Thus: (H1) and non-national products (H2). Study 2 then examines the
moderating role of nationality identity prominence (H3) and the med-
H1. Citizen ambassadors representing a different nationality as a
iating role of identity threat (H4).
consumer produce more positive product attitudes toward a national
product, in comparison to citizen ambassadors representing the same
4. Study 1
nationality as a consumer.
H2. Citizen ambassadors representing a different nationality as a Study 1 tests hypotheses H1 and H2 using a between-subjects de-
consumer produce less positive product attitudes toward a non- sign. In this study, the citizen ambassadors' nationality (native versus
national product, in comparison to citizen ambassadors representing non-native) as well as product nationality (national versus non-na-
the same nationality as a consumer. tional) is empirically manipulated. Ethnocentrism was also included as
an alternative mechanism potentially moderating product attitudes.
As previously mentioned, identity likely plays an important role in
Ethnocentrism is the tendency for individuals of one culture or nation
how consumers respond to citizen ambassadors. Prior research has
to view their own nation as central and all others to be of less im-
shown that identity effects are particularly strong when one's identity is
portance and relevance. This effect extends to products and services as
made salient via market cues, such as via symbols on products and in
well as to all symbols associated with a consumer's nation, with re-
advertising (Elias & Greenspan, 2007; Forehand et al., 2002). Building
sulting outcomes influencing product attitudes (Huddleston, Good, &
on this, a consumer's nationality identity prominence (i.e., the im-
Stoel, 2001).
portance of a consumer's national identity to his or her total identity)
To manipulate incongruity, we followed suit with the literature on
should moderate effects of identity priming on evaluations of adver-
operationalizing schema incongruity (Meyers-Levy & Tybout, 1989).
tisements featuring citizen ambassadors. While nationality references
Specifically, the congruent (incongruent) scenario represented the ex-
should increase connection to one's nationality (Carvalho & Luna, 2014;
pected (not expected) schema in the consumer's mind for the config-
Dimofte et al., 2003; Yoo & Lee, 2016), these effects may be most likely
uration of the consumer-spokesperson-product. Stated another way, a
to occur for those that already have pre-existing high levels of nation-
match in these elements (i.e., same nationality of the consumer,
ality identity prominence. Additionally, consumers lower in nationality
spokesperson, and product) is a congruent scenario, and a mismatch in
identity prominence are likely to be more open to different references
these elements (i.e., different nationalities among the consumer,
to nationality in advertising because incongruent nationalities (between
spokesperson, and product) is an incongruent scenario.
that of the consumer and that of the product) are less likely to lead to a
threat to one's core identity (Kruglanski & Ajzen, 1983; Kruglanski &
4.1. Pre-test
Webster, 1996). Thus:
H3. A consumers' nationality identity prominence moderates the effect In an online questionnaire, French respondents rated how nationally
of citizen ambassadors on product attitudes, such that consumers high representative of France and America they considered a series of ran-
(low) in nationality identity prominence are more likely to desire domly-presented products (e.g. Bourbon, movies, wine, foie gras,
congruent (incongruent) citizen ambassadors. cheese, BBQ sauce, etc.). These two countries were selected because
they were also used in prior cross-cultural research on product at-
In addition to the moderating role of nationality identity promi-
tachment (Overby, Gardial, & Woodruff, 2004). Respondents indicated
nence, perceived identity threat should mediate the relationship be-
the degree to which they perceived the list of products to be re-
tween exposure to citizen ambassadors of different nationalities and
presentative of the USA and France. A Qualtrics panel of French re-
resulting product attitudes. Social identity threat occurs when “people's
spondents completed the pretest (n = 49, 47% men, 82% between the
interests are jeopardized because their group has to compete with other
ages of 18–49). Paired t-tests revealed that foie gras was more re-
groups for scarce resources” (Branscombe, Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje,
presentative of France than of the US (MFrance = 5.49, MUS = 0.67,
1999, p. 36). When consumers feel their social group's values, distinc-
t = 15.77, p < .001), and BBQ sauce was more representative of the
tiveness, or overall identity is being challenged, they may react nega-
US than France (MUS = 4.69, MFrance = 1.49, t = 6.96, p < .001).
tively to the stimulus that creates this tension. As such, and in light of
incongruent nationality-based stimuli, it is plausible that national
4.2. Experimental manipulations and questionnaire design
identity threat may moderate these effects. Thus, nationality identity
prominence is treated as a moderator rather than a mediator because it
A 2 (citizen ambassador-consumer nationality: congruent or
is a stable trait representing how important nationality is to a con-
not) × 2 (product: nationally representative or not) between-subjects
sumer's total identity (Kruglanski & Ajzen, 1983; Kruglanski & Webster,
design was established. Four videos using a bilingual French-American
1996); therefore, nationality identity prominence should not be influ-
actor (who had resided in both the US and France for many years) were
enced by exposure to advertising featuring nationally-representative
produced. This actor had not acted in any major roles (acting is not
products or spokespeople. In contrast, perceived threat to one's identity
even his main job) and was unknown and unrecognizable to our panel.
should be influenced by exposure to identity-relevant advertising,
In the French condition, the actor spoke French with a Parisian-French
thereby serving as a mediator in the proposed model.
accent. In the American condition, the actor spoke French with a strong
Consumers that are exposed to advertising should be more open-
American accent (i.e., dragging out vowels and pronouncing all con-
minded in evaluation of the corresponding product when advertisement
sonants such as ‘traays’ instead of ‘treh’ for the French word très). To
elements (e.g., the citizen ambassador) are not perceived as a threat to
manipulate product nationality, videos positioned as vlogs (i.e., video
one's core identity (Kruglanski & Ajzen, 1983; Kruglanski & Webster,
blogs) were created. The actor presented how he preferred to prepare
1996). Since consumers respond more positively when their core
and use either foie gras (nationally-representative of France) or BBQ
identity is not threatened (Hildebrand, DeMotta, Sen, & Kongsompong,
sauce (not nationally-representative of France). In all the vlogs, the
2013), it is expected that identity threat negatively influences citizen
actor identified himself as either French or American in addition to
ambassador effects. Thus:
using the adapted accent when speaking. Additionally, vlogs did not
H4. Perceived identity threat mediates the relationship between citizen specify any celebrity status of the citizen ambassador.
ambassador use and product attitudes, such that high levels of identity After watching the videos, participants completed several measures
threat lead to lower product attitudes. assessing their product attitudes (α. = 0.987), compatibility between

412
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

the product and the citizen ambassador (α. = 0.969), and the CETSC- demonstrate the process mechanism that drives these positive evalua-
ALE (α. = 0.962). See Table 1 for scale items. We used the French tions, which Study 2 seeks to uncover. Thus, Study 2 seeks to fill this
translation of the CETSCALE as per Netemeyer, Durvasula, and gap by examining how nationality identity prominence moderates and
Lichtenstein (1991). perceived identity threat mediates the relationship between citizen
Respondents were randomly assigned to view one of the four videos ambassador use and additional product evaluations.
before answering the dependent measures. The survey was sent to a
Qualtrics panel of 200 French adults (n = 175 after manipulation 5. Study 2
checks, 49% men, MAge = 40.1 years, all with French citizenship).
Manipulation checks were included at the end of the survey to: (a) 5.1. Pre-test
identify those who did not remember the nationality of the citizen
ambassador, (b) ensure respondents saw and heard the videos properly, A sample (n = 51, 46% men, M age = 35.40, SD = 10.65) of
(c) assess demand effects and filter out those who guessed the purpose American adults from Amazon's Mechanical Turk completed the same
of the study, and (d) eliminate those who felt that the actor was not pretest questionnaire as used in Study 1 in exchange for a small cash
authentically French or American (depending on the condition). incentive. Participants evaluated products as which were most re-
presentative of the US and France. For French foods, crepes (M = 6.02,
4.3. Results SD = 1.19) were seen as more uniquely French than apple pie
(M = 2.61, SD = 1.67), t(50) = 11.77, p < .001. In contrast for
Ethnocentrism did not differ between the four groups (F(1, American foods, apple pie (M = 6.00, SD = 1.09) was seen as more
171) = 0.42, p > .05), ruling out the possibility that the results were uniquely American than crepes (M = 2.58, SD = 1.44), t(50) = 12.03,
driven by protectionist national sentiments. p < .001.
First, a two-way ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between
product nationality and citizen ambassador nationality on perceived 5.2. Experimental manipulations and questionnaire design
compatibility (F(1, 171) = 29.38, p < .001). Pairwise comparisons
showed that when a product was nationally representative, a non-na- Two-hundred and eighty-two American adults (Mage = 40.04,
tive citizen ambassador led to significantly higher perceptions of SD = 15.45, 68.08% female) from a Qualtrics panel participated in this
compatibility (MNON-NATIVE = 4.33) than a native citizen ambassador study in exchange for a small cash incentive. A 2 (citizen ambassador-
(MNATIVE = 2.82) (F(1, 171) = 23.55, p < .001). In contrast, when a consumer nationality: congruent or not) × 2 (product: nationally re-
product was not nationally representative, a native citizen ambassador presentative or not) between-subjects factorial design was created. The
led to significantly higher perceptions of compatibility same protocol and questionnaire from Study 1 was used in Study 2;
(MNATIVE = 4.23) than a non-native spokesperson (MNON-NATIVE = 3.52; however, fictitious Facebook posts rather than vlogs as well as different
F(1, 171) = 7.88, p < .05), supporting our moderate incongruity hy- products (crepes – French, apple pie – American) were used for Study 2
pothesis. (see Fig. 2 for an example). Posts did not specify any celebrity status of
Second, a separate two-way ANOVA revealed a significant interac- the citizen ambassador.
tion between product nationality and citizen ambassador nationality on The same product attitude measure (α. = 0.971) and manipulation
product attitudes (F(1, 171) = 15.15, p < .001). Pairwise comparisons checks as used in Study 1 were used again here in Study 2. Additionally,
showed that when a product was nationally representative, a non-na- more product evaluation measures were included to assess purchase
tive citizen ambassador led to significantly higher product attitudes intentions (α = 0.983) and likelihood of sharing the Facebook post
(MNON-NATIVE = 5.05) than a native citizen ambassador (α = 0.984). Lastly, the moderator of nationality identity prominence
(MNATIVE = 4.49) (F(1, 171) = 4.54, p < .05). In contrast, when a (α = 0.918) and mediator of perceived identity threat (α = 0.871)
product was not nationally representative, a native citizen ambassador were measured. See Table 1 for all items.
led to significantly higher product attitudes (MNATIVE = 4.64) than a
non-native spokesperson (MNON-NATIVE = 3.75; F(1, 171) = 24.48, 5.3. Results
p < .001). These results support hypotheses H1 and H2 (see Fig. 1).
Four participants failed the manipulation check and were removed
4.4. Discussion from the dataset, leaving the data from 278 participants for further
analysis. Hierarchical linear regression was used to assess the influence
These results suggest that for domestic advertising, a non-native of conditions and the moderator of nationality identity prominence on
citizen ambassador garners higher perceived compatibility and product perceived identity threat and product attitudes. The three-way inter-
attitudes than a native citizen ambassador. These results align perfectly action of citizen ambassador condition, food condition, and nationality
with schema congruity theory and extend prior research on schema identity prominence had a directional direct effect on purchase inten-
incongruity between product cues (Carvalho et al., 2011) to spokes- tions, t(270) = 1.71, p = .089, with the overall model being significant,
person effects, such that moderate incongruity likely leads to greater F(7,270) = 5.50, p < .001. All other direct effects on product attitudes
processing and as a result of such, produces higher product attitudes. were non-significant. However, there was a significant three-way in-
The aforementioned past research demonstrates that changes to one teraction on the mediator of perceived identity threat, t(270) = −2.21,
product feature versus many would qualify as moderate versus major p = .028, with the full model also being significant, F(7,270) = 3.47,
incongruity. The results from Study 1 also show that this slight incon- p = .001.1
gruity results in positive perceptions of compatibility. Thus, changing To further examine mediation effects through perceived identity
one feature (e.g., a product's nationality or a spokesperson's nationality threat, Hayes' (2013) PROCESS macro with 10,000 bootstrapped sam-
but not both) results in moderate incongruity, thereby explaining our ples and bias-corrected confidence intervals (model 4) was used for
results. As per the previously presented theorizing, citizen ambassador-
consumer nationality incongruence can be balanced out with product-
1
consumer congruity, resulting in moderate incongruity and more po- A follow-up analysis using the same items from Study 1 for product attitudes was
conducted. A similar pattern of effects (although non-significant) was found. Here, apple
sitive product attitudes. Likewise, product-consumer nationality in-
pie with a French ambassador (i.e., incongruent, M = 5.79) and crepes with an American
congruity can be balanced out by citizen ambassador-consumer con- ambassador (i.e., incongruent, M = 4.98) produced the greatest overall attitudes
gruity. However, the results of Study 1 do not identify the nature of the (p = .166). Lower attitudes occurred for apple pie with an American ambassador (i.e.,
positive evaluations (i.e., boundary conditions) nor do they congruent, M = 5.44) or crepes with a French ambassador (i.e., congruent, M = 4.77).

413
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

Table 1
Scale items.
Construct Items

Product Attitudes (all items measured on a 7-point bipolar scale) What is your attitude toward the product that was presented?

Adapted from Batra and Stayman (1990) 1. Bad – Good


2. Dislike – Like
3. Unpleasant – Pleasant
4. Unfavorable – Favorable
5. Negative - Positive
Product & Citizen Ambassador Compatibility (all items measured on 6-point bipolar scale) What do you think about the product and the person presenting the product?

Self-generated scale 1. The product and this person are not at all in concordance – The product and this
person are in perfect concordance
2. It is completely inappropriate that this person presents this product – It is
completely appropriate that this person present this product
3. It is completely illogical that this person presents this product – It is completely
logical that this person present this product
4. The product and this person seem completely incompatible – The product and this
person seem completely compatible
CETSCALE (all items measured on a 1, not at all, to 7, very much, scale; note that Study 1 Please indicate your agreement with the following statements:
mentioned French people and Study 2 mentioned American people given the different
nationalities tested between these studies) 1. French (American) people should always buy French (American)-made products
instead of imports.
Full scale from Shimp and Sharma (1987) 2. Only those products that are unavailable in France (the U.S.) should be imported.
3. Buy French (American)-made products. Keep France (America) working.
4. French (America) products, first, last, and foremost.
5. Purchasing foreign-made products is un-French (American).
6. It is not right to purchase foreign products because it puts the French (Americans)
out of jobs.
7. A real French person (American) should always buy France (American)-made
products.
8. We should purchase products manufactured in France (America) instead of
letting other countries get rich off us.
9. It is always best to purchase French (American) products.
10. There should be very little trading of purchasing of goods from other countries
unless out of necessity.
11. French people (Americans) should not buy foreign products because this hurts
French (American) business and causes unemployment.
12. Curbs should be put on all imports.
13. It may cost me in the long-run, but I prefer to support French (American)
products.
14. Foreigners should not be allowed to put their products on our markets.
15. Foreign products should be taxed heavily to reduce their entry into France (the
U.S.).
16. We should buy from foreign countries only those products that we cannot obtain
within our own country.
17. French (American) consumers who purchase products made in other countries
are responsible for putting their fellow French people (Americans) out of work.
Purchase Intentions (all items measured on a 7-point bipolar scale) How likely would you be to buy the product discussed in the post?

Self-generated scale 1. Unlikely – Likely


2. Definitely would not – Definitely would
3. Not probable - Probable
Likelihood of Sharing Facebook Post (all items measured on a 7-point bipolar scale) How likely would you be to share this Facebook post with your friends if it was on
your timeline?
Self-generated scale
1. Unlikely – Likely
2. Definitely would not – Definitely would
3. Not probable - Probable
Nationality Identity Prominence (all items measured on a 1, none at all, to 5, a great deal, Please indicate how much you feel each of these statements are true. How much…
scale)
1. Is your nationality a key component of your identity?
Self-generated scale 2. Does your nationality reflect who you are?
3. Is your nationality a part of you as a person?
Perceived Identity Threat (all items measured on a 1, strongly disagree, to 7, strongly agree, When I think about this Facebook post, it makes me think that…
scale)
1. American is not well represented.
Self-generated scale 2. America is not be respected.
3. Posts like this put into question America's identity.

each product evaluation variable (product attitude, purchase inten- share intentions (CI: 0.0129 to 0.4408). Specifically, the three-way in-
tions, share intentions). Confidence intervals (CIs) not containing zero teraction significantly influenced perceived identity threat, t
represent significant at a 95% confidence level. Significant indirect (270) = −2.21, p = .028, and then perceived identity threat sig-
effects through perceived identity threat emerged for product attitudes nificantly and negatively influenced product attitudes (t
(CI: 0.0292 to 0.6112), purchase intentions (CI: 0.0309 to 0.6209), and (269) = −6.02, p < .001), purchase intentions (t(269) = −5.02,

414
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

5.06
5 4.64
4.48

Product attitudes 4 3.75

0
French product (Foie gras)* Non-French product (BBQ sauce)*

Fig. 1. Product attitudes, Study 1.

p < .001), and share intentions (t(269) = −2.63, p = .009). nationality congruence within the consumer-spokesperson-product
These interaction effects were probed using a spotlight analysis at triad and provides support for the positive influence of moderate
one standard deviation above and below the mean for perceived iden- schema incongruity on product evaluations and attitudes. The results
tity threat (M = 3.25, SD = 1.13). These analyses revealed that con- make several contributions. First, the results suggest that citizen am-
sumers low in nationality identity prominence experienced higher bassadors, just like celebrities, can influence product attitudes. Second,
perceived identity threat when the Facebook post featured a French the results suggest that the positive influence of citizen ambassadors in
(American) citizen ambassador with a congruent product, crepes advertising depends, in part, on cognitive assimilation. Specifically,
(versus incongruent product, apple pie). Opposing effects were found citizen ambassadors can be of the same or a different nationality than
for consumers high in nationality identity prominence, such that higher consumers, or they may present a national or non-national product, but
perceived identity threat occurred when the Facebook post featured a they must present one facet in common with the consumers' nationality.
French (American) citizen ambassador with an incongruent product, Third, we find this effect to be particularly stronger when a consumer's
apple pie (versus congruent product, crepes); see Fig. 3. nationality is not a prominent component of their identity. Thus, it is
possible to conceive that consumers may trust an independent source
(e.g., citizen ambassadors, friends, consumer reports, etc.) more than a
5.4. Discussion
marketer or celebrity because this source is perceived to be less biased.
Furthermore, consumers may judge a non-native speaker (versus a
Similar to Study 1, the nationality of a citizen ambassador influ-
native speaker) to have more expertise in a product category, which
ences product evaluations for a nationally-representative in comparison
explains why they are a spokesperson for the product. This in contrast
to a non-nationally-representative product. Specifically, we found di-
to a native speaker, who may simply have a country association and no
rectionally significant evidence for hypotheses H1 and H2. Further-
expertise. Stated otherwise, to be a spokesperson, the non-native
more, results show that consumers' nationality identity prominence
speaker would have to work harder to be an expert, versus a native
moderates the influence of citizen ambassadors on product evaluations,
speaker who has simply been exposed to a product.
thereby supporting H3, and perceived identity threat mediates this re-
Finally, the results also reveal that the positive effects of moderate
lationship, thereby supporting H4. These results are particularly inter-
incongruity in consumer-spokesperson-product based national adver-
esting in that incongruent citizen ambassador effects produce the most
tising are driven by perceived threat to the consumer's identity. We
positive product evaluations for consumers low in nationality identity
argue these positive effects of moderate incongruency are due to the
prominence. These consumers perceive less threat to their identity from
fact that consumers engage in greater processing to understand the
incongruent citizen ambassadors likely because nationality is not a
incongruity, thereby leading to more positive product attitudes as a
prominent feature of their identity (Kruglanski & Ajzen, 1983;
result.
Kruglanski & Webster, 1996). Thus, Study 2 provides an interesting
boundary condition to citizen ambassador effects, such that incon-
gruent ambassadors should be used for consumers low in nationality
6.1. Implications for theory and research
identity prominence.
Source attributes are important in determining the credibility of a
6. General discussion citizen ambassador (Gilly, Graham, Wolfinbarger, & Yale, 1998) as well
as in influencing product perceptions (McCracken, 1989). The results of
As prior research suggests, schema congruence or expected simi- the studies herein suggest that nationality is an especially important
larity does not always result in more positive consumer evaluations characteristic influencing product attitudes. When a citizen ambassador
(Bhatnagar & Wan, 2011). This research examines the effects of is of a different (versus same) nationality than the consumer, the

415
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

French Ambassador, Apple Pie

American Ambassador, Crepes

Fig. 2. Example Facebook post stimuli.

consumer has more (versus less) favorable attitudes toward a national connection between nationality references and their own nationality
product, particularly when nationality is not a prominent component of rather than overtly expressing the connection to personal identity.
the consumer's identity. Again, we argue these effects are due to in- Thus, our findings support the use of citizen ambassadors for commu-
creased processing in incongruent situations. Consequently, this re- nicating explicit nationality cues as well as elucidate the appropriate
search answers questions regarding who can best sell national products contexts for making such nationality cues salient.
as well as what role a citizen ambassador has in augmenting or em- In addition, while research has focused extensively on celebrities
phasizing national attachment to products. Thus, further research is (e.g., Erdogan, 1999; McCracken, 1989), only a small body of research
needed examining the complex relationship between citizen ambassa- has focused on non-celebrity citizen ambassadors. This research con-
dors and national products as well as how citizen ambassadors can tributes to the literature by examining non-celebrities (i.e., citizen
shape or modify national/non-national product attachment. For ex- ambassadors), including those positioned as experts versus non-expert
ample, examining how a national citizen ambassador can attenuate (similar to Wang (2005)) and consequently reveals the value of na-
negative stereotypes or how foreign (even expatriated) citizen ambas- tionality in influencing product attitudes. For national products, it ap-
sadors can augment positive product stereotypes is a worthy area for pears that a non-native citizen ambassador may be more effective,
future research. whereas for a non-national product a native citizen ambassador may be
The results are consistent with research proposing that explicit na- more effective.
tionality cues trigger more positive consumer evaluations (Dimofte This research also shows the importance of understanding the pro-
et al., 2003). The results specifically demonstrate that explicit nation- minence of nationality in a consumer's identity as well as the role of
ality cues are relevant in messaging featuring citizen ambassadors. perceived identity threat in a consumer's response to companies using
More specifically, explicit statements of nationality can activate ste- citizen ambassadors in marketing communications. Thus, the studies
reotypes and stereotype schemas, making it easier for identities to be- herein contribute to the literature on citizen ambassadors and spokes-
come salient. While prior research has shown that explicit identity cues figures (c.f., Choi & Rifon, 2012; Miller & Allen, 2012) to show how
can backfire for marketers (Bhattacharjee et al., 2014), the studies nationality identity prominence is a boundary condition to moderate
herein show that identity cues can allow consumers to make the incongruity effects.

416
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

schemas about product nationality (e.g., maple syrup is Canadian, pasta


Low Nationality Identity Prominence is Italian), but they may also have a harder time identifying product and
7 6.29 brand origins for other products like chicken or oatmeal (Samiee,
Perceived Identity Threat

6 Shimp, & Sharma, 2005). A citizen ambassador's nationality may be a


5 means to emphasize a real or even fictitious brand origin. As well,
4.34
perhaps explicit and implicit nationality cues conveyed by citizen am-
4
2.7 bassadors can serve to identify or confirm brand nationality. Nation-
3 ality cues by entities other than citizen ambassadors should also be
2 1.66
explored to see if moderate incongruity effects from nationality pervade
1 beyond people (e.g., partnered charities that influence consumer per-
0 ceptions, such as Food4Africa or American Red Cross; Minton &
Crepes Apple Pie Cornwell, 2016).
We also acknowledge that we did not measure involvement or
American Citizen-Ambassador French Citizen-Ambassador processing style, which prior research has posited may influence the
effect of incongruity in advertising (Lee & Schumann, 2004). Further-
more, we did not test perceived compatibility in Study 2, given that we
had already shown this effect in Study 1, preferring instead to follow up
with other analyses in Study 2. We, like much of the prior research
High Nationality Identity Prominence outlined in this paper, assumed that respondents used central proces-
7 sing when evaluating the study advertising stimuli. We also only
Perceived Identity Threat

6 measured nationality identity prominence in Study 2. As such, future


5 4.28 research should examine the effect of nationality-derived incongruity
4.26
and measure the processing style used by consumers at the time of ad
4
2.71 evaluation as well continually measure perceived compatibility and
3
nationality identity prominence in order to explicitly test schema in-
2 1.48 congruity mechanisms. Future research should also measure the
1 amount of processing that occurs at different levels of nationality
0 congruence between the consumer, spokesperson, and product. Finally,
Crepes Apple Pie Study 2 used Facebook posts while Study 1 used videos. Consequently,
consumers may have spent less time viewing the Facebook posts than
American Citizen-Ambassador French Citizen-Ambassador
the videos and thereby engaged in less processing which is what led to
only directional effects in Study 2 compared to Study 1.
Fig. 3. Spotlight analysis for perceived identity threat, Study 2.
Additionally, future researchers are encouraged to consider the in/
congruence of a citizen ambassador's nationality with brand nation-
6.2. Implications for practitioners ality, particularly for products that are perceived as native versus non-
native. It would also be interesting to replicate and extend the studies
Marketers should continue using citizen ambassadors in marketing herein by including branding cues to see how these might interact with
communications, particularly when national identity is not a core the product category as well as the spokesperson's nationality to in-
component of a consumer's identity. More specifically, marketers fluence other marketing outcomes, such as brand attitudes. For ex-
should seek ways to produce moderate incongruity to increase con- ample, running shoes are not uniquely American (e.g., Nike and Reebok
sumers' evaluations of corresponding products. For example, using a are American brands, but Adidas and Puma are German and Lacoste is
French citizen ambassador for a new burger restaurant (since burgers French). However, the use of “regular” people of multiple nationalities
are associated with the USA), an American citizen ambassador for in Nike advertising may make the brand seem more global but local at
quality French wine (since wine is traditionally associated with France: the same time and thus more attractive. Interesting further research
Bastien, Dubourdeau, & Leclère, 2011), or a Brazilian spokesperson for could identify how brands benefit from using such types of citizen
sushi (since Sushi is associated with Asia) could actually produce higher ambassadors.
product evaluations than using citizen ambassadors from the nation- Further research might also explore the use of self-referencing lan-
ally-representative product. guage on the effectiveness of citizen ambassadors. For example, a ci-
A citizen ambassador may also evoke a consumer's national identity tizen ambassador may describe a product as “the product” to not re-
via implicit cues, such as accents (Mai & Hoffmann, 2014) or even body ference personal relations to the product. Alternatively, the citizen
language. This area of implicit nationality cues remains an interesting ambassador could describe a product as “our product” or “your pro-
and under-examined area of research and an area where marketers duct” making the consumer's relationship with the product more pro-
could benefit from further understanding. For example, an American minent. Such self versus group-referencing language (e.g., through the
with a French accent or a South African wearing Indian attire could be use of personal pronouns) has been shown to influence consumer's
enough to produce moderate incongruity effects. Additionally, more product evaluations and brand commitment (Sela, Christian Wheeler, &
subtle manipulations of the nationality of a citizen ambassador may be Sarial-Abi, 2012).
able to reduce the perceived identity threat from incongruent citizen More research also needs to be conducted to elucidate the peer-to-
ambassadors of consumers high in nationality identity prominence, peer effects of nationality congruence. With the advent of electronic
thereby leading to moderate and positive incongruity effects for all communication and social media, the importance of non-celebrity ci-
consumers. tizen ambassadors has increased, in particular because online settings
facilitate peer-to-peer exchanges (Roed, 2003). Likewise, another lim-
6.3. Future research and limitations itation of this research is that it focused on positively framed messages.
Negative information has more influence on shaping beliefs and atti-
This research focused on national products, and specifically food tudes, since it is perceived as diagnostic (Skowronski & Carlston, 1987).
products, and as such limits the conclusions to these categories. Thus, future research should examine negatively framed citizen am-
Furthermore, in the global marketplace, consumers may have fixed bassador advertising (e.g., for preventative health-related messages)

417
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

and how this interacts with the national identity of a product and ci- of Israeli advertising. Journal of Advertising Research, 47(1), 113–122.
tizen ambassador. Also, future research should examine how such va- Erdogan, B. Z. (1999). Celebrity endorsement: A literature review. Journal of Marketing
Management, 15(4), 291–314.
lenced messages can change stereotypes associated with products or Feick, L. F., & Price, L. L. (1987). The market maven: A diffuser of marketplace in-
nations via a citizen ambassador's attractiveness and expertise. formation. Journal of Marketing, 51(1), 83–97.
Feldman, J. (2014). TK Maxx casts ‘regular’ people in ad, and the results are great.
HuffPost. Retrieved from http://www.huffingtonpost.com/2014/03/21/tk-maxx-ad_
Funding n_5008609.html, Accessed date: 3 July 2017.
Forehand, M. R., & Deshpandé, R. (2001). What we see makes us who we are: Priming
This work was supported by a grant from Reims Métropole [RM- ethnic self-awareness and advertising response. Journal of Marketing Research, 38(3),
336–348.
2017]. Forehand, M. R., Deshpandé, R., & Reed, A., II (2002). Identity salience and the influence
of differential activation of the social self-schema on advertising response. Journal of
Appendix 1 Applied Psychology, 87(6), 1086–1099.
Galvagno, M. (2011). The intellectual structure of the anti-consumption and consumer
resistance field: An author co-citation analysis. European Journal of Marketing, 45(11/
American National, French Product 12), 1688–1701.
Garretson, J. A., & Niedrich, R. W. (2004). Spokes-characters: Creating character trust and
Hello, my name is Michel Roger, and I'm American. I enjoy foie gras positive brand attitudes. Journal of Advertising, 33(2), 25–36.
Gilly, M. C., Graham, J. L., Wolfinbarger, M. F., & Yale, L. J. (1998). A dyadic study of
even if I don't know much about it. Here is how I like to enjoy foie gras. interpersonal information search. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 26(2),
I like to serve it at room temperature because when it's too cold I find it 83–100.
doesn't have as much taste. When I am ready to serve it, I use a smooth Grewal, D., Gotlieb, J., & Marmorstein, H. (1994). The moderating effects of message
framing and source credibility on the price-perceived risk relationship. Journal of
knife because a serrated knife does not make for nice slices. Normally, a Consumer Research, 21(1), 145–153.
portion of foie gras is between 50 and 150 g, but that will depend on if I Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis:
am serving it as a starter or a main dish. I like to serve foie gras with a A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.
sweet wine. Hildebrand, D., Demotta, Y., Sen, S., & Kongsompong, K. (2013). In-group and out-group
influences on the consumption behavior of minority groups: The case of gay men.
French National, American Product Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 32(special issue), 70–78.
Huddleston, P., Good, L. K., & Stoel, L. (2001). Consumer ethnocentrism, product ne-
cessity and Polish consumers' perceptions of quality. International Journal of Retail &
Hello, my name is Michel Roger, and I'm French. I enjoy BBQ sauce Distribution Management, 29(5), 236–246.
even if I don't know much about it. Here is how I use BBQ sauce. First, I Kalandies, A., Andersson, M., & Ekman, P. (2009). Ambassador networks and place
branding. Journal of Place Management and Development, 2(1), 41–51.
buy it because I don't know how to make it. I do not marinate my meat
Kamins, M. A., & Gupta, K. (1994). Congruence between spokesperson and product type:
in it because I find it tends to burn if I do that. I prefer to baste it on my A matchup hypothesis perspective. Psychology and Marketing, 11(6), 569–586.
food after it has been cooked so I get all the good flavors out of it. I Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of opinion change. Public Opinion Quarterly, 25(1), 57–78.
really like the rich texture and the smoky, sweetness of BBQ sauce. I Kemmelmeier, M., & Winter, D. G. (2008). Sowing patriotism, but reaping nationalism?
Consequences of exposure to the American flag. Political Psychology, 29(6), 859–879.
could put BBQ on anything, but usually I prefer BBQ sauce on pork ribs, Kleine, R. E., Kleine, S. S., & Kernan, J. B. (1993). Mundane consumption and the self: A
chicken, and even shrimp. social-identity perspective. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2(3), 209–235.
Kruglanski, A. W., & Ajzen, I. (1983). Bias and error in human judgment. European Journal
of Social Psychology, 13(1), 1–44.
References Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “seizing” and
“freezing”. Psychological Review, 103(2), 263–283.
Aronczyk, M. (2013). Branding the nation: The global business of national identity. Oxford: Lalonde, R. N. (2002). Testing the social identity-intergroup differentiation hypothesis:
Oxford University Press. we're not American eh!. British Journal of Social Psychology, 41(4), 611–630.
Bartz, S., Molchanov, A., & Stork, P. A. (2013). When a celebrity endorser is disgraced: A Lee, E.-J., & Schumann, D. W. (2004). Explaining the special case of incongruity in ad-
twenty-five-year event study. Marketing Letters, 24(2), 131–141. vertising: Combining classic theoretical approaches. Marketing Theory, 4(1/2), 59–90.
Bastien, V., Dubourdeau, P. L., & Leclère, M. (2011). La Marque France. Paris: Presses de MacDonald, K. B. (2008). Effortful control, explicit processing, and the regulation of
l'Ecole des Mines. human evolved predispositions. Psychological Review, 115(4), 1012.
Batra, R., & Stayman, D. M. (1990). The role of mood in advertising effectiveness. Journal Mai, R., & Hoffmann, S. (2014). Accents in business communication: An integrative model
of Consumer Research, 17(2), 203–214. and propositions for future research. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(1), 137–158.
Bhatnagar, N., & Wan, F. (2011). Is self-character similarity always beneficial? Journal of Mandler, G. (1984). Consciousness, imagery and emotion - with special reference to
Advertising, 40(2), 39–50. automatic imagery. Journal of Mental Imagery, 8(4), 87–94.
Bhattacharjee, A., Berger, J., & Menon, G. (2014). When identity marketing backfires: Marshall, R., Na, W., & Deuskar, S. (2008). Endorsement theory: How consumers relate to
Consumer Agency in Identity Expression. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(2), celebrity models. Journal of Advertising Research, 48(4), 564–572.
294–309. McCracken, G. (1989). Who is the celebrity endorser? Cultural foundations of the en-
Branscombe, N. R., Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (1999). The context and content dorsement process. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(3), 310–321.
of social identity threat. Social Identity: Context, Commitment, Content, 35–58. McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2011). Birds of a feather: Homophily in
Braun, E., Kavaratzis, M., & Zenker, S. (2013). My city–my brand: The different roles of social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415–444.
residents in place branding. Journal of Place Management and Development, 6(1), Meyers-Levy, J., & Tybout, A. M. (1989). Schema congruity as a basis for product eva-
18–28. luation. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(2), 243–252.
Campbell, M. C., & Goodstein, R. C. (2001). The moderating effect of perceived risk on Miller, F. M., & Allen, C. T. (2012). How does celebrity meaning transfer? Investigating
consumers' evaluations of product incongruity: Preference for the norm. Journal of the process of meaning transfer with celebrity affiliates and mature brands. Journal of
Consumer Research, 28(3), 439–449. Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 443–452.
Carvalho, S. W., & Luna, D. (2014). Effects of national identity salience on responses to Minton, E. A., & Cornwell, T. B. (2016). The cause cue effect: Cause-related marketing and
ads. Journal of Business Research, 67(5), 1026–1034. consumer health perceptions. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 50(2), 372–402.
Carvalho, S. W., Samu, S., & Sivaramakrishnam, S. (2011). The effect of country-related Misra, S., & Beatty, S. E. (1990). Celebrity spokesperson and brand congruence: An as-
brand associations and product attributes on attitude toward unfamiliar foreign sessment of recall and affect. Journal of Business Research, 21(2), 159–173.
brands: A schema congruity perspective. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, Money, R. B., Gilly, M. C., & Graham, J. L. (1998). Explorations of national culture and
23(2), 1–16. word-of-mouth referral behavior in the purchase of industrial services in the United
Choi, S. M., & Rifon, N. J. (2012). It is a match: The impact of congruence between States and Japan. Journal of Marketing, 62(4), 76–87.
celebrity image and consumer ideal self on endorsement effectiveness. Psychology and Mummendey, A., Klink, A., & Brown, R. (2001). Nationalism and patriotism: National
Marketing, 29(9), 639–650. identification and out-group rejection. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(2),
Davis, T. C. (1999). Revisiting group attachment: Ethnic and national identity. Political 159–172.
Psychology, 20(1), 25–47. Netemeyer, R. G., Durvasula, S., & Lichtenstein, D. R. (1991). A cross-national assessment
Dimofte, C. V., Forehand, M. R., & Deshpande, R. (2003). Ad schema incongruity as of the reliability and validity of the CETSCALE. Journal of Marketing Research, 28(3),
elicitor of ethnic self-awareness and differential advertising response. Journal of 320–327.
Advertising, 32(4), 7–17. Nitzan, I., & Libai, B. (2011). Social effects on customer retention. Journal of Marketing,
Einolf, C. J., Philbrick, D. M., & Slay, K. (2013). National giving campaigns in the United 75(6), 24–38.
States entertainment, empathy, and the national peer group. Nonprofit and Voluntary Overby, J. W., Gardial, S. F., & Woodruff, R. B. (2004). French versus American con-
Sector Quarterly, 42(2), 241–261. sumers' attachment of value to a product in a common consumption context: A cross-
Elias, N., & Greenspan, L. (2007). The honey, the bear, and the violin: The Russian voices national comparison. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 32(4), 437–460.

418
N. Spielmann, E.A. Minton Journal of Business Research 121 (2020) 409–419

Petroshius, S. M., & Crocker, K. E. (1989). An empirical analysis of spokesperson char- country of origin products. Journal of Business Research, 69(3), 1130–1137.
acteristics on advertisement and product evaluations. Journal of the Academy of Verry, P. (2016). How regular people outshined pros in Nike's best ads of 2016. Retrieved
Marketing Science, 17(3), 217–225. from http://footwearnews.com/2016/focus/athletic-outdoor/nike-fnaa-marketer-of-
Rehmet, J., & Dinnie, K. (2013). Citizen brand ambassadors: Motivations and perceived the-year-award-winner-lebron-james-kobe-bryant-unlimited-280246/, Accessed date:
effects. Journal of Destination Marketing & Management, 2(1), 31–38. 3 July 2017.
Roed, J. (2003). Language learner behaviour in a virtual environment. Computer Assisted Wang, A. (2005). The effects of expert and consumer endorsements on audience response.
Language Learning, 16(2–3), 155–172. Journal of Advertising Research, 45(4), 402–412.
Samiee, S., Shimp, T. A., & Sharma, S. (2005). Brand origin recognition accuracy: Its Wheeler, M. E., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Controlling racial prejudice: Social-cognitive goals
antecedents and consumers' cognitive limitations. Journal of International Business affect amygdala and stereotype activation. Psychological Science, 16(1), 56–63.
Studies, 36(4), 379–397. Wilton, P. C., & Myers, J. G. (1986). Task, expectancy, and information assessment effects
Sela, A., Christian Wheeler, S., & Sarial-Abi, G. (2012). We are not the same as you and I: in information utilization processes. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(4), 469–486.
Causal effects of minor language variations on consumers' attitudes toward brands. Wright, S. A. (2016). Reinvestigating the endorser by product matchup hypothesis in
Journal of Consumer Research, 39(3), 644–661. advertising. Journal of Advertising, 45(1), 26–32.
Shimp, T. A., & Sharma, S. (1987). Consumer ethnocentrism: Construction and validation Yoo, J. J., & Lee, W. (2016). Calling it out: The impact of national identity on consumer
of the CETSCALE. Journal of Marketing Research, 24(3), 280–289. response to ads with a patriotic theme. Journal of Advertising, 45(2), 24–255.
Skowronski, J., & Carlston, D. E. (1987). Social judgment and social memory: The role of Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1994). The personal involvement inventory: Reduction, revision, and
cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases. Journal of Personality application to advertising. Journal of Advertising, 23(4), 59–70.
and Social Psychology, 52(4), 689–699. Zhao, X., & Belk, R. W. (2008). Advertising consumer culture in 1930s shanghai:
Spielmann, N. (2016). Is it all or nothing? Testing schema congruity and typicality for Globalization and localization in Yuefenpai. Journal of Advertising, 37(2), 45–56.

419

You might also like