You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Motor Behavior

ISSN: 0022-2895 (Print) 1940-1027 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjmb20

Effects of A Dual-Task Intervention in Postural


Control and Cognitive Performance in Adolescents

Pilar Bustillo-Casero, Sara Cebrian-Bou, Carlos Cruz-Montecinos, Alberto


Pardo & Xavier García-Massó

To cite this article: Pilar Bustillo-Casero, Sara Cebrian-Bou, Carlos Cruz-Montecinos,


Alberto Pardo & Xavier García-Massó (2019): Effects of A Dual-Task Intervention in Postural
Control and Cognitive Performance in Adolescents, Journal of Motor Behavior, DOI:
10.1080/00222895.2019.1600467

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2019.1600467

Published online: 15 Apr 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=vjmb20
Journal of Motor Behavior, 2019
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

RESEARCH ARTICLE
Effects of A Dual-Task Intervention in Postural Control and
Cognitive Performance in Adolescents
Pilar Bustillo-Caseroa, Sara Cebrian-Boua, Carlos Cruz-Montecinosb,c , Alberto Pardod, Xavier
Garcıa-Masso  a,e
a
Department of Teaching of Musical, Visual and Corporal Expression, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain.
b
Department of Physical Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, University of Chile, Santiago, Chile. cLaboratory of Biomechanics
and Kinesiology, San Jose  Hospital, Santiago, Chile. dDepartment of Physical and Sports Education, University of
Valencia, Valencia, Spain. eHuman Movement Analysis Group, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain.

ABSTRACT. The aim was to assess dual- versus single-task Balance is the ability to maintain the centre of mass
training for motor performance and cognitive performance in inside the base of support (Yim-Chiplis & Talbot, 2000).
adolescents. Two experiments were performed. In the first, 30 This aspect of physical education has an inestimable
adolescents were randomized to three groups to determine the
effect of dual-task difficulty on postural control: a-scaling and value for young people’s health and motor competence
root mean square (RMS). In the second, 20 adolescents were (Garcıa-Flores, Rivera-Cisneros, Sanchez-Gonzalez,
randomized to two groups to determine the effect of dual-task Guardado-Mendoza, & Torres-Gutierrez, 2016;
practice to improve working memory. RMS in the post-test Granacher, Muehlbauer, Gollhofer, Kressig, & Zahner,
was lower than the pre-test in both dual-task groups, while 2010; Horak, 2006; Maki, Holliday, & Topper, 1994;
a-scaling was lower in post-test than pre-test only in the high-
difficulty dual-task group. A practice effect was observed on Myers et al., 1996; Sherrington et al., 2008; Vellas et al.,
the percentage of correct answers only in the dual-task group 1997). From a health-related point of view, falls are one
(p ¼ 0.035). Thus, dual-task training could enhance motor and of the main causes of unintentional injury in children
cognitive performance more than single-task training. (Moorin & Hendrie, 2008) and a major cause of mortal-
Keywords: Cognitive load; dual task; motor learning; ity and morbidity in older adults (Ambrose, Paul, &
postural control Hausdorff, 2013). As balance training effectively reduces
fall rates (Nagy et al., 2007), improving children’s bal-
ance could reduce falls and the associated injuries in this
population. In terms of motor competence, good balance
is necessary for performing complex actions like locomo-
Introduction
tion (Assaiante, 1998), and it is even important in sports
elping students with a wide range of abilities to like soccer and golf, potentially conferring an advantage
H learn and refine their motor skills is a challenge for
physical education teachers (Obrusnikova & Rattigan,
in performance (Hrysomallis, 2011). Some authors postu-
late that mechanisms related with postural control (e.g.,
2016). However, evidence suggests that developing fun- sensorial reweight) reach maturity in children as young
damental movement skills (i.e. running, balancing, etc.) as 6 to 12 years old (Barela, Jeka, & Clark, 2003;
has important health implications for young people Busquets, Aranda-Garcia, Ferrer-Uris, Marina, &
(Lubans, Morgan, Cliff, Barnett, & Okely, 2010) because Angulo-Barroso, 2018; McKay, Wu, & Angulo-Barroso,
people who see themselves as having good motor com- 2014; Peterson, Christou, & Rosengren, 2006). However,
petence are more likely to be physically active (Hellın, a recent study found that dual-task postural control does
Moreno, & Rodrıguez, 2007). In turn, physical activity not reach an adult level until at least 15 or 16 years of
has been related to greater academic performance age (Estevan, Gandia, Villarrasa-Sapi~na, Bermejo, &
(Morales et al., 2011; Pellicer-Chenoll et al., 2015). In Garcıa-Masso, 2018). Thus, postural control in the ner-
the same way, children and adolescents develop cogni- vous system matures later than previous studies sug-
tive functions and specific competences in school sub- gested, prompting the need to incorporate best practices
jects like Maths or Language during the educational to improve balance at earlier stages.
process. In this regard, working memory capacity has Individuals have little awareness that their balance is
been associated with mathematics or language compre- typically controlled and coordinated very effectively
during daily activities (Canning, 2005; McNevin &
hension (Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Raghubar, Barnes,
Wulf, 2002). That is, people maintain their balance (a
& Hecht, 2010) and is important in the motor learning
postural task) even while performing concurrent phys-
process (Bo, Borza, & Seidler, 2009; Seidler, Bo, &
ical and/or cognitive secondary tasks (Cluff, Gharib, &
Anguera, 2012). Therefore, increasing working memory
(and probably other cognitive functions) could increase
Correspondence address: Xavier Garcıa-Masso,
students’ academic performance. The effect of targeting Departamento de Didactica de la Expresion Musical, Plastica
both movement skills and working memory in educa- y Corporal., Av. Dels Tarongers, 4 (46022), Valencia, Spain.
tional programmes has so far not been studied in-depth. E-mail: xavier.garcia@uv.es

1
P. BUSTILLO-CASERO ET AL.

Balasubramaniam, 2010). This situation is known as a dual-tasks compared to single-tasks in children with
dual-task paradigm. Several studies have investigated spastic hemiparesis (Elhinidi, Ismaeel, & El-Saeed,
interference or performance reduction in primary and 2016). However, there are no studies available on using
secondary activities during the execution of a dual-task this paradigm to enhance cognitive functions in educa-
(Mehdizadeh et al., 2015; Patel & Bhatt, 2015; Reilly, tional programmes in children or adolescents.
van Donkelaar, Saavedra, & Woollacott, 2008; Stins, Thus, evidence on motor performance and cognitive
Ledebt, Emck, Van Dokkum, & Beek, 2009; Vuillerme function enhancement by dual-task practice is scant and
& Vincent, 2006; Weaver, Janzen, Adkin, & Tokuno, mostly limited to elderly and/or injured populations
2012). Three theories have been proposed to explain (Ghai, Ghai, & Effenberg, 2017), so determining the
dual-task interference in both tasks when performed effect of dual-tasking on motor and cognitive perform-
simultaneously. The first theory is the cross-domain ance enhancement in children and adolescents needs fur-
competition model. This postulates that the tasks com- ther consideration. This information could be used in
pete for attentional resources (Lacour, Bernard- schools to improve motor learning in physical education
Demanze, & Dumitrescu, 2008), making both less effi- lessons. Other subjects could also use this paradigm to
cient than in a single-task situation. The second theory is increase cognitive functions or specific skills. The aim of
the U-shaped nonlinear interaction model. This suggests a this study was to assess the effect of dual-task practice
U-shaped relationship between the task demands (Lacour on motor performance and cognitive functions in
et al., 2008), meaning that depending on whether the cog- adolescents.
nitive demand of the secondary task is low or high, motor
performance can be either improved or diminished.
Finally, the task prioritisation model posits that adaptive Experiment 1
responses to age-related declines lead to compensatory
behaviours or strategies like task prioritisation (Lacour This randomised control trial aimed to compare the
et al., 2008). Its main prediction is that older adults priori- effect of easy and hard dual-task practice versus single-
tise motor performance (i.e., postural stability) at the task practice for increasing postural task performance in
expense of cognitive performance in dual-tasking (Lacour adolescents. Thirty participants were randomised into
et al., 2008). three groups: a control group (CG; n ¼ 10) that practiced
Regarding dual-task training or motor practice, recent a single postural task, another group that trained by
studies have pointed out that a limited number of ses- doing an easy dual-task (EDTG; n ¼ 10) and a third
sions practicing dual tasks could increase motor per- group that carried out a hard-cognitive task (HDTG;
formance more effectively than sessions using a single n ¼ 10). All participants performed a pre-test, practice
task (Goh, Lee, & Fisher, 2013; Goh, Sullivan, Gordon, session, post-test and retention test with a single-task
Wulf, & Winstein, 2012; Roche et al., 2007). postural control test, using a Wii Balance Board (WBB)
Silsupadol et al. (2009) studied 23 older adults with on a balance platform.
balance impairment, finding that a 4-week training
period (3 sessions per week) using dual-task exercises Method
was more efficient for improving dual-task performance
than the same training using single tasks. Dual-tasking Participants
could also improve cognitive performance in older Prior to recruitment, sample size was calculated using
adults, regardless of whether they are cognitively GPower 3.1 (University of D€usseldorf, D€usseldorf,
impaired (Delbroek, Vermeylen, & Spildooren, 2017; Germany). A sample size of 9 per group was required
Falbo, Condello, Capranica, Forte, & Pesce, 2016), as to reach a statistical power of 0.95 and an a-level of
well as in people with multiple sclerosis (Sosnoff et al., 0.05 with an estimated effect size of 1.25, based on pre-
2017). Another study that targeted a complex pattern of vious data (Pellecchia, 2005). Thirty 13- and 14-year-
bimanual coordination through dual-tasks, which was old adolescents (14 males and 16 female) volunteered
carried out in women (mean age 23 years) found that for the study. They were randomised to one of the three
dual-task training affected early motor performance due groups (n ¼ 10 each). Inclusion criteria were: i) no prior
to competition for the same neural resources (Remy, experience with the experimental task, ii) 13 or 14 years
Wenderoth, Lipkens, & Swinnen, 2010). In children and old, and iii) no motor control or neural pathology that
adolescents, Torres-Oviedo, Vasudevan, Malone, & could affect balance. Table 1 shows participant
Bastian (2011) found that when training consists only characteristics.
of changing the walking pattern, there is adaptation, but The Institutional Review Board of the University of
retention worsens; however, dual-task training results in Valencia approved the study protocol. Parents/guardians
slower adaptation but improved retention. Finally, more of the participants provided signed, informed consent
improvements in postural control were found using before the tests, and participants gave their assent orally.

2 Journal of Motor Behavior


DUAL-TASK AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE

Wedderkopp, 2014). The data were acquired using


TABLE 1. Subjects’ characteristics of WiiLab software (University of Colorado, Boulder, Co,
experiment 1. USA) for Matlab R2007 (Mathworks Inc, Natick, MA,
USA). Data signals were recorded at a frequency
Variable CG EDTG HDTG of 40 Hz.
Gender
Male 3 6 4 Data Analysis
Female 7 4 6 CoP displacement signals in the medialateral direction
Age (years) 13.1 (0.32) 13.1 (0.32) 13.1 (0.32)
were filtered digitally by a Butterworth low-pass filter
Weight (kg) 54.9 (10.19) 56.8 (14.11) 62.2 (11.76)
Height (m) 1.59 (0.08) 1.62 (1.06) 1.64 (0.59)
with a 12 Hz cut-off frequency. Regarding the spatial
BMI (kgm–2) 21.84 (4.26) 21.59 (4.74) 23.21 (4.11) magnitude of sway, we calculated the root mean square
BMIPercentile 68.9 (23.05) 65.60 (25.3) 78.1 (23.87) (RMS) in the medialateral direction using equations pro-
posed previously (Prieto, Myklebust, Hoffmann, Lovett,
Note. Data are expressed as mean (standard deviation). & Myklebust, 1996). We also assessed temporal sway
BMI ¼ body mass index. dynamics using detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA).
The a-scaling exponent in the medialateral direction was
computed as a dependent variable in an inferential ana-
Procedure lysis. This variable provides information on long-range
All the participants were evaluated at their schools on autocorrelation in the signal (Lin, Seol, Nussbaum, &
two consecutive days. On the first day, they performed a Madigan, 2008). Busa, Ducharme, & Van Emmerik
pre-test, practice session and post-test. They took the (2016) have suggested that a-scaling exponent values
retention test 24 hours later. In the pre-test, post-test and close to 1 are representative of adaptive changes, while
retention tests, the participants only performed a single changes of the a-scaling exponent to values higher than
postural task. This task consisted of maintaining a stabi- 1.6 are representative of maladaptive strategies.
logram (with instability in the medialateral direction) as Therefore, a change close to 1 could indicate adaptive
horizontal as possible during three, 30-s trials (with 30 learning of the task based on the use of a non-predictable
to 60 s rest in between). A reference point was placed at pattern to perform the task, showing a higher adaptability
eye level at 2 m directly in front of the participant. A to the instantaneous conditions.
WBB was placed on the stabilogram to assess centre of
pressure (CoP) displacement; participants were asked to Statistical Analysis
stand on the WBB with their feet shoulder distance apart, SPSS software, version 21 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL,
moving their arms freely to increase stability. USA) was used for the statistical analysis. As the
The intervention consisted of performing a dual-task Shapiro-Wilk test indicated that some variables did not
(EDTG or HDTG) or single task (CG) activity during comply with the normality assumption, we used non-
12, 30-s trials (with 30–60 s rest in between). EDTG and parametric tests, obtaining the median and inter-quartile
HDTG participants performed a cognitive task at the range values by standard statistical methods.
same time as the postural task, while the CG carried out Kruskal-Wallis tests were applied to establish the
the postural task only. The cognitive task consisted of a effects of the group on the dependent variables in pre-
backward digit spam test. EDTG participants performed test, post-test and retention time points. By means of
four sequences of three digits in each 30-s trial, while Friedman’s ANOVAs, we analysed the effect of the
the HDTG completed the same task but with three interventions on the postural control of each group.
sequences of five digits per trial. After each sequence Follow-up assessments used Dunn’s pairwise compari-
appeared on the computer screen at eye level 2 m in sons with Bonferroni correction (two comparisons: pre-
front of the adolescents, they had to repeat the numbers test versus post-test and pre-test versus retention). A p-
value of 0.05 was accepted as the threshold for signifi-
in reverse order. In the EDTG, participants were able to
cance in all statistical analyses.
correctly recite 95.89% of the numbers [95% confidence
interval (CI) 94.5–97.29], compared to 80.23% (95% CI
Results
77.24 – 83.23) in the HDTG.
The Kruskal–Wallis test showed that there were no
Postural Control Measurement differences between the groups in the a-scaling exponent
CoP signals were acquired from a WBB. The WBB or RMS at any time point. Regarding the effect of the
has been validated as an appropriate tool to analyse intervention, no significant differences were observed in
standing postural control in both adults (Clark et al., the CG between any time points in the a-scaling expo-
2010; Koslucher et al., 2012; Park & Lee, 2014) and nent (v22 ¼ 2.60; p ¼ 0.273) or RMS (v22 ¼ 3.80;
children (Larsen, Jørgensen, Junge, Juul-Kristensen, & p ¼ 0.150). In the EDTG, RMS differed according to

3
P. BUSTILLO-CASERO ET AL.

FIGURE 1. Pairwise comparisons of pre-test, post-test and retention test on the postural control variables. A shows the results
of the control group, B the data for the easy dual-task group and C the results for the hard dual-task group. RMS ¼ root mean
square.  indicates significant differences regarding pre-test (p < 0.05).

time point (v22 ¼ 8.60; p ¼ 0.014), but the a-scaling


exponent did not (v22 ¼ 5.4; p ¼ 0.07). Pairwise compar- TABLE 2. Subjects’ characteristics of
isons showed lower RMS at post-test than at pre-test experiment 2.
(z ¼ 2.91; p ¼ 0.008; r ¼ 0.65), although there were no
differences between pre-test and retention. In the HDTG, Variable STG DTG
there was a significant difference in the a-scaling expo- Gender
nent (v22 ¼ 11.4; p ¼ 0.003) and RMS (v22 ¼ 7.20; Male 4 4
p ¼ 0.027) according to time point, and pairwise com- Female 6 6
Age (years) 13.3 (0.48) 13.1 (0.32)
parison showed that the a-scaling exponent (z ¼ 3.35;
Weight (kg) 49.9 (9.75) 53.98 (6.92)
p ¼ 0.002; r ¼ 0.75) and RMS (z ¼ 2.68; p ¼ 0.014; Height (m) 1.65 (0.07) 1.65 (0.08)
r ¼ 0.6) variables were lower at post-test than at pre-test. BMI (kgm–2) 18.33(3.64) 19.87 (1.95)
There were no differences between pre-test and retention BMIPercentile 39.3 (35.26) 61.4 (21.06)
time points in RMS or the a-scaling exponent in the
Note. Data are expressed as mean (standard deviation).
HDTG. Figure 1 shows the median and inter-quartile
BMI ¼ body mass index.
range associated with these comparisons.

Experiment 2
This randomised controlled trial aimed to assess the Method
effect of single- versus dual-task practice on cognitive Participants
performance in adolescents. Twenty participants were Twenty adolescents volunteered to participate in this
divided into two groups using a random sampling study (12 girls and 8 boys). Inclusion criteria were: i) no
method: a single cognitive task group (STG, n ¼ 10), and prior experience with the experimental task, ii) 13 or
a dual-task training group (DTG, n ¼ 10). All participants 14 years old, and iii) no motor control or neural path-
performed a pre-test, practice session, post-test, and ology that could affect balance. Table 2 shows partici-
retention test. pant characteristics.

4 Journal of Motor Behavior


DUAL-TASK AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE

TABLE 3. Effect of practice on working memory.

STG DTG
Variable
Pre-test Post-test Retention Pre-test Post-test Retention
Correct answers (number) 12.17 (2.91) 13.83 (2.33) 13.67 (1.33) 12.83 (1.67) 13.5 (2.75) 14.17 (2.67)
Correct answers (%) 81.17 (19.67) 92.17 (15.33) 91.17 (8.92) 85.67 (11.25) 90.17 (18.33) 94.33 (17.58)

Note. Data are expressed as median (interquartile range).


Significant differences regarding pre-test (p < 0.05).

The Institutional Review Board of the University of parametric tests. The Mann–Whitney U-test was applied
Valencia approved the study protocol. Parents/guardians to compare the number and percentage of correct
of the participants provided signed, informed consent answers between groups at pre-test, post-test and reten-
before the tests, and participants gave their assent orally. tion time points, individually. Two Friedman’s ANOVAs
were performed to establish the effect of the intervention
Procedure on cognitive performance in both groups individually.
All the participants were evaluated at their schools on Follow-up assessments used Dunn’s pairwise compari-
two consecutive days. On the first day, they performed a sons with Bonferroni correction (two comparisons: pre-
pre-test, practice session and post-test. They took the test versus post-test and pre-test versus retention). A p
retention test 24 h later. The pre-test, post-test and reten- value of 0.05 was accepted as the threshold for signifi-
tion assessments consisted of a single cognitive task in cance in all statistical analyses.
three, 30-second trials (with 30–60 s rest in between).
Participants had to memorise the sequences of numbers Results
and then repeat them in reverse order (i.e., backward
digit spam test). The intervention consisted of twelve, The Mann-Whitney U-test test did not yield significant
30-s trials in which the teens performed the same cogni- differences between the groups either on the absolute or
tive task. Both groups performed three sequences of five relative frequency of correct answers during pre-test,
digits in each 30-s trial. The STG only carried out the post-test and retention assessments. Friedman’s
cognitive task, while the DTG performed a cognitive ANOVAs showed an effect for the intervention on the
task and postural task concurrently. In the postural con- number of correct answers both in the STG (v22 ¼ 7;
trol task, participants had to keep a stabilogram (instabil- p ¼ 0.03) and DTG (v22 ¼ 6.06; p ¼ 0.048). However,
ity in the medialateral direction) as horizontal pairwise comparisons did not show significant differen-
as possible. ces in the number of correct answers between pre-test
and post-test or between pre-test and retention assess-
Working Memory Measurement ments in either group. The intervention did show an
Data for the cognitive task were acquired using open- effect on the percentage of correct answers in the DTG
source software; the numbers appeared on the computer (v22 ¼ 6.69; p ¼ 0.035) but not in the STG (v22 ¼ 4.27;
screen, and then participants had to type them in reverse p ¼ 0.12). Pairwise comparisons (Table 3) showed that
order on the keyboard. During the DTG practice session, the DTG obtained a higher percentage of correct answers
adolescents performed the postural task at the same time at the retention assessment than at the pre-test time
as the cognitive task, repeating the numbers out loud so point (p ¼ 0.038).
the researcher could type them on the keyboard. During
the pre-test, post-test and retention assessments, the par- General Discussion
ticipants of both groups typed the numbers directly on
the keyboard. The findings on cognitive and postural control per-
Assessment results were saved on a file, with the formance enhancement obtained in this study provide
absolute and relative frequencies of correct answers serv- important evidence that dual-task practice could be more
ing as a measure of task performance. effective for improving balance and working memory
than single postural tasks in adolescents. After a single
Statistical Analysis training session, the HDTG and EDTG improved their
SPSS software, version 21 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, performance in the postural task in the post-test,
USA) was used for the statistical analysis. As the although this improvement was not retained. Thus, the
Shapiro–Wilk test indicated that some variables did not HDTG and EDTG showed motor adaptation during the
comply with the normality assumption, we used non- postural task, but not permanent changes. However, the

5
P. BUSTILLO-CASERO ET AL.

CG did not make any progress in practice without the exponent, while the EDTG improved only in RMS.
cognitive task. Furthermore, the DTG showed better per- Thus, a high-difficulty secondary task increases cognitive
formance in the working memory task in the retention demands when practicing a motor skill, which is related
test than in the pre-test. Our results suggest that dual- to enhanced motor performance (Lee, Swinnen, &
task practice could be effective in enhancing both motor Serrien, 1994). The cross-domain competition model
and cognitive skills in adolescents. could also explain the improvement in motor perform-
A second finding is that the more difficult the cogni- ance when practicing with dual-tasks. This model affirms
tive task, the higher the postural task adaptation seems to that when two tasks are performed at the same time,
be. In this regard, a difficult cognitive task (i.e., five dig- they compete for attention, diminishing the performance
its in the spam test) significantly reduced RMS and the of both (Lacour et al., 2008). Previously published
a-scaling exponent at the post-test assessment, while results support this model in adolescents (Palluel,
EDTG improved only RMS. High-difficulty dual-tasks Nougier, & Olivier, 2010). In our study, a dual-task was
are thus recommended to improve motor adaptation. performed during training where tasks are continuously
Our first finding corroborates the results of other stud- competing for attention. Later, when the participants per-
ies (e.g., Goh et al., 2012, 2013; Roche et al., 2007; formed the single postural task in the post-test assess-
Silsupadol et al., 2009), which found that dual-tasks are ment, their performance improved considerably, as there
more effective for increasing motor performance than was no other task to compete for attentional resources.
single tasks. However, these studies used dual-tasks in Another possible mechanism involved in the dual-task
the elderly, the ill or adults rather than in adolescents. effect on motor learning could be related to the neural
Differences in postural control during different life stages activation facilitated by the secondary task (Goh et al.,
(i.e., childhood-adolescence, adulthood and old age) 2013). Previous studies have reported that when the sec-
(Hyt€ onen, Pyykk€ o, Aalto, & Starck, 1993) could mean ondary task activates the same neuronal regions as the
that some motor learning techniques are more effective primary task, this facilitates activation, resulting in
at improving postural control in some of these periods higher motor learning (Goh et al., 2013). Moreover,
than others. The results obtained in the present study Rietschel et al. (2012) found that neural activation is cor-
suggest that dual-tasks could be more effective than sin- related with the difficulty of the dual-task, so the more
gle tasks for improving motor performance in difficult the dual-task is during practice, the more active
adolescents. the neuronal areas required to perform the tasks are,
Regarding the effect of dual-task difficulty on motor which probably also leads to higher motor learning.
performance enhancement, Roche et al., (2007) showed These findings suggest that a common neural circuit can
that high-difficulty dual-task conditions improve the pri- be the underlying mechanism for dual-task performance
mary motor task more than easy dual-task conditions. (Goh et al., 2013).
Goh et al. (2012) tested two different hypotheses regard- Finally, the results of the second experiment suggest
ing the characteristics of the dual-task required to that dual-task practice could enhance cognitive perform-
enhance motor performance. The first was the difficulty ance more than practicing single tasks. The fact that par-
hypothesis, supported by the results of Roche et al. ticipants performed better in the retention test than in the
(2007). According to this hypothesis, high-difficulty pre-test indicates a considerable improvement.
dual-tasks are required to improve motor tasks. The Nevertheless, there were no differences between the pre-
second was the similarity hypothesis, which postulates test and post-test assessments. This could be attributed to
that dual-tasking enhances motor performance when both the time-on-task performance decline described in the lit-
tasks use similar processes; their results support this erature (Kirchner & Knopf, 1974; Lim et al., 2010;
hypothesis. The authors recommend carrying out dual- Szalma et al., 2004). That is, engagement in the cogni-
tasks in which the secondary activity involves similar tive task during the training session reduces participants’
processes as the primary motor task to improve the per- cognitive performance (due to fatigue), masking the
formance of the primary action. actual training effect. This result could serve as a basis
In this study, we used a complex primary motor task for strategies facilitating the learning of processes that
and a cognitive task involving working memory. Some involve the working memory (i.e., maths or languages)
studies have found that working memory is involved in in adolescents.
the performance and learning of motor skills such as bal- Both the cognitive and postural performance results
ance (Dault, Frank, & Allard, 2001; Seidler et al., 2012). during dual-task training could be used to extract prac-
Therefore, our tasks could use similar processes, as sug- tical applications for physical education teachers and
gested by Goh et al., (2012). Using the same cognitive other educators. First, to improve motor skills, it would
task in the EDTG and HDTG, our results support the dif- be more appropriate to apply dual-tasks instead of single
ficulty hypothesis, since the HDTG showed improvement tasks. In this regard, cognitive tasks could be added as a
on the same day in both RMS and the a-scaling secondary activity to improve the conditions for

6 Journal of Motor Behavior


DUAL-TASK AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE

enhancing motor performance and carrying out interdis- Experimental Brain Research, 150 (4), 434–442. https://doi.
ciplinary learning projects. The secondary task should be org/10.1007/s00221-003-1441-5
difficult enough to increase the cognitive demands of the Bo, J., Borza, V., & Seidler, R. D. (2009). Age-related declines
task as well as to require a high degree of attention. in visuospatial working memory correlate with deficits in
explicit motor sequence learning. Journal of
This study has several limitations. First, we tested the
Neurophysiology, 102 (5), 2744–2754. https://doi.org/10.
hypothesis with only one day of cognitive and balance
1152/jn.00393.2009
training; the effect of dual-task training could be Busa, M., Ducharme, S., & Van Emmerik, R. (2016). Non-lin-
enhanced with additional sessions. The relatively small ear techniques reveal adaptive and maladaptive postural con-
sample size could also be responsible for the lack of trol dynamics in persons with multiple sclerosis. Journal of
motor task improvement in the retention test. Multiple Sclerosis, 3 (2), 1–3. https://doi.org/10.4172/2376-
Furthermore, the retention results were evaluated at 0389.1000177
24 hours after training, so the long-term effects of train- Busquets, A., Aranda-Garcia, S., Ferrer-Uris, B., Marina, M.,
ing (i.e., months later) are still unknown. In addition, the & Angulo-Barroso, R. (2018). Age and gymnastic experience
assessment included the medio-lateral direction but not effects on sensory reweighting processes during quiet stand.
anterior-posterior displacement, and the experiments Gait & Posture, 63, 177–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gait-
included only 13- and 14-year-olds. Future studies that post.2018.05.009
Canning, C. G. (2005). The effect of directing attention during
assess the transfer between both axes and include other
walking under dual-task conditions in Parkinson’s disease.
age groups would be desirable. More research will also Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 11 (2), 95–99.
be necessary to confirm these results in a more eco- Clark, R. A., Bryant, A. L., Pua, Y., McCrory, P., Bennell, K.,
logical environment. & Hunt, M. (2010). Validity and reliability of the Nintendo
To conclude, motor performance in adolescents could Wii Balance Board for assessment of standing balance. Gait
be enhanced by practicing dual-tasking instead of single- & Posture, 31 (3), 307–310. doi:10.1016/
task training. The difficulty of the secondary task is an j.gaitpost.2009.11.012
important factor facilitating learning; the greater the dif- Cluff, T., Gharib, T., & Balasubramaniam, R. (2010).
ficulty of the secondary task, the greater the motor per- Attentional influences on the performance of secondary
formance enhancement. Finally, adolescents’ working physical tasks during posture control. Experimental Brain
memory capacity could also be enhanced by practicing Research, 203 (4), 647–658.
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and
dual-tasks.
language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 3 (4), 422–433. https://doi.org/10.3758/
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS BF03214546
Dault, M. C., Frank, J. S., & Allard, F. (2001). Influence of a
The authors wish to thank Jose Francisco Garcıa Moreno visuo-spatial, verbal and central executive working memory
for providing the stabilometer used in this study. task on postural control. Gait & Posture, 14 (2), 110–116.
Delbroek, T., Vermeylen, W., & Spildooren, J. (2017). The
effect of cognitive-motor dual task training with the biores-
Disclosure statement
cue force platform on cognition, balance and dual task per-
No potential conflict of interest was reported by formance in institutionalized older adults: A randomized
the authors. controlled trial. Journal of Physical Therapy Science, 29 (7),
1137–1143.
Elhinidi, E. I. M., Ismaeel, M. M. I., & El-Saeed, T. M.
ORCID (2016). Effect of dual-task training on postural stability in
children with infantile hemiparesis. Journal of Physical
Carlos Cruz-Montecinos http://orcid.org/0000-0002-
Therapy Science, 28 (3), 875–880. https://doi.org/10.1589/
3835-3368 jpts.28.875
Estevan, I., Gandia, S., Villarrasa-Sapi~na, I., Bermejo, J. L., &
Garcıa-Masso, X. (2018). Working memory task influence in
REFERENCES postural stability and cognitive function in adolescents.
Motor Control, 22, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1123/mc.2017-
Ambrose, A. F., Paul, G., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2013). Risk fac- 0063
tors for falls among older adults: A review of the literature. Falbo, S., Condello, G., Capranica, L., Forte, R., & Pesce, C.
Maturitas, 75 (1), 51–61. doi:10.1016/ (2016). Effects of physical-cognitive dual task training on
j.maturitas.2013.02.009 executive function and gait performance in older adults: A
Assaiante, C. (1998). Development of locomotor balance con- randomized controlled trial. BioMed Research International,
trol in healthy children. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral 2016, 1.
Reviews, 22 (4), 527–532. Garcıa-Flores, F. I., Rivera-Cisneros, A. E., Sanchez-Gonzalez,
Barela, J. A., Jeka, J. J., & Clark, J. E. (2003). Postural control J. M., Guardado-Mendoza, R., & Torres-Gutierrez, J. L.
in children. Coupling to dynamic somatosensory information. (2016). Correlation between gait speed and muscular strength

7
P. BUSTILLO-CASERO ET AL.

with balance for reducing falls among elderly. Cirugıa y Lin, D., Seol, H., Nussbaum, M. A., & Madigan, M. L. (2008).
Cirujanos, (English Edition), 84 (5), 392–397. https://doi. Reliability of COP-based postural sway measures and age-
org/10.1016/j.circen.2016.08.012 related differences. Gait & Posture, 28 (2), 337–342. doi:
Ghai, S., Ghai, I., & Effenberg, A. O. (2017). Effects of dual 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.005
tasks and dual-task training on postural stability: A system- Lubans, D. R., Morgan, P. J., Cliff, D. P., Barnett, L. M., &
atic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Interventions in Okely, A. D. (2010). Fundamental movement skills in chil-
Aging, 12, 557–577. https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S125201 dren and adolescents: review of associated health benefits.
Goh, H.-T., Lee, Y.-Y., & Fisher, B. E. (2013). Neural corre- Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.), 40 (12), 1019–1035.
lates of dual-task practice benefit on motor learning: A https://doi.org/10.2165/11536850-000000000-00000
repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study. The Maki, B. E., Holliday, P. J., & Topper, A. K. (1994). A pro-
European Journal of Neuroscience, 37 (11), 1823–1829. spective study of postural balance and risk of falling in an
https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12192 ambulatory and independent elderly population. Journal of
Goh, H.-T., Sullivan, K. J., Gordon, J., Wulf, G., & Winstein, Gerontology, 49 (2), M72–M84.
C. J. (2012). Dual-task practice enhances motor learning: A McKay, S. M., Wu, J., & Angulo-Barroso, R. M. (2014).
preliminary investigation. Experimental Brain Research, 222 Effect of Achilles tendon vibration on posture in children.
(3), 201–210. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3206-5 Gait & Posture, 40 (1), 32–37. doi:10.1016/
Granacher, U., Muehlbauer, T., Gollhofer, A., Kressig, R. W., j.gaitpost.2014.02.002
& Zahner, L. (2010). An intergenerational approach in the McNevin, N. H., & Wulf, G. (2002). Attentional focus on
promotion of balance and strength for fall prevention–a supra-postural tasks affects postural control. Human
mini-review. Gerontology, 57 (4), 304–315. Movement Science, 21 (2), 187–202.
Hellın, P., Moreno, J. A., & Rodrıguez, P. L. (2007). Relacion Mehdizadeh, H., Taghizadeh, G., Ghomashchi, H., Parnianpour,
de la competencia motriz percibida con la practica fısico- M., Khalaf, K., Salehi, R., … Sangelaji, B. (2015). The
deportiva. Revista de Psicologia Del Deporte, 15 (2), effects of a short-term memory task on postural control of
219–231. stroke patients. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 22(5),
Horak, F. B. (2006). Postural orientation and equilibrium: what 335–341. doi:10.1179/1074935714Z.0000000039
do we need to know about neural control of balance to pre- Moorin, R. E., & Hendrie, D. (2008). The epidemiology and
vent falls? Age and Ageing, 35 (suppl_2), ii7–ii11. cost of falls requiring hospitalisation in children in Western
Hrysomallis, C. (2011). Balance ability and athletic perform- Australia: A study using linked administrative data. Accident
ance. Sports Medicine, 41 (3), 221–232. https://doi.org/10. Analysis & Prevention, 40 (1), 216–222. https://doi.org/10.
2165/11538560-000000000-00000 1016/j.aap.2007.05.008
Hyt€
onen, M., Pyykk€o, I., Aalto, H., & Starck, J. (1993). Morales, J., Gomis, M., Pellicer-Chenoll, M., Garcıa-Masso,
Postural Control and Age. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 113 (2), X., Gomez, A., & Gonzalez, L.-M. (2011). Relation between
119–122. https://doi.org/10.3109/00016489309135778 physical activity and academic performance in 3rd-year sec-
Kirchner, G. L., & Knopf, I. J. (1974). Differences in the vigi- ondary education students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 113
lance performance of second-grade children as related to sex (2), 539–546. https://doi.org/10.2466/06.11.13.PMS.113.5.
and achievement. Child Development, 45 (2), 490–495. 539-546
https://doi.org/10.2307/1127975 Myers, A. M., Powell, L. E., Maki, B. E., Holliday, P. J.,
Koslucher, F., Wade, M. G., Nelson, B., Lim, K., Chen, F.-C., Brawley, L. R., & Sherk, W. (1996). Psychological indica-
& Stoffregen, T. A. (2012). Nintendo Wii Balance Board is tors of balance confidence: relationship to actual and per-
sensitive to effects of visual tasks on standing sway in ceived abilities. The Journals of Gerontology Series A:
healthy elderly adults. Gait & Posture, 36 (3), 605–608. doi: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 51 (1),
10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.05.027 M37–M43.
Lacour, M., Bernard-Demanze, L., & Dumitrescu, M. (2008). Nagy, E., Feher-Kiss, A., Barnai, M., Domjan-Preszner, A.,
Posture control, aging, and attention resources: Models and Angyan, L., & Horvath, G. (2007). Postural control in eld-
posture-analysis methods. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical erly subjects participating in balance training. European
Neurophysiology, 38 (6), 411–421. Journal of Applied Physiology, 100(1), 97–104.
Larsen, L. R., Jørgensen, M. G., Junge, T., Juul-Kristensen, B., Obrusnikova, I., & Rattigan, P. J. (2016). Using video-based
& Wedderkopp, N. (2014). Field assessment of balance in modeling to promote acquisition of fundamental motor skills.
10 to 14 year old children, reproducibility and validity of the Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance, 87 (4),
Nintendo Wii board. BMC Pediatrics, 14, 144. https://doi. 24–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/07303084.2016.1141728
org/10.1186/1471-2431-14-144 Palluel, E., Nougier, V., & Olivier, I. (2010). Postural control
Lee, T. D., Swinnen, S. P., & Serrien, D. J. (1994). Cognitive and attentional demand during adolescence. Brain Research,
effort and motor learning. Quest, 46 (3), 328–344. https:// 1358, 151–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.
doi.org/10.1080/00336297.1994.10484130 051
Lim, J., Wu, W.-C., Wang, J., Detre, J. A., Dinges, D. F., & Park, D.-S., & Lee, G. (2014). Validity and reliability of bal-
Rao, H. (2010). Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental ance assessment software using the Nintendo Wii balance
workload: an ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. board: usability and validation. Journal of Neuroengineering
NeuroImage, 49 (4), 3426–3435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. and Rehabilitation, 11 (1), 99. https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-
neuroimage.2009.11.020 0003-11-99

8 Journal of Motor Behavior


DUAL-TASK AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE

Patel, P. J., & Bhatt, T. (2015). Attentional demands of per- Sherrington, C., Whitney, J. C., Lord, S. R., Herbert, R. D.,
turbation evoked compensatory stepping responses: Cumming, R. G., & Close, J. C. (2008). Effective exercise
Examining cognitive-motor interference to large magnitude for the prevention of falls: A systematic review and meta-
forward perturbations. Journal of Motor Behavior, 47 (3), analysis. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 56
201–210. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2014.971700 (12), 2234–2243.
Pellecchia, G. L. (2005). Dual-task training reduces impact of Silsupadol, P., Shumway-Cook, A., Lugade, V., van Donkelaar,
cognitive task on postural sway. Journal of Motor Behavior, P., Chou, L.-S., Mayr, U., & Woollacott, M. H. (2009).
37(3), 239–246. doi:10.3200/JMBR.37.3.239-246 Effects of single-task versus dual-task training on balance
Pellicer-Chenoll, M., Garcia-Masso, X., Morales, J., Serra-Ano,
performance in older adults: A double-blind, randomized
P., Solana-Tramunt, M., Gonzalez, L.-M., & Toca-Herrera,
controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine and
J.-L. (2015). Physical activity, physical fitness and academic
Rehabilitation, 90 (3), 381–387.
achievement in adolescents: a self-organizing maps approach.
Sosnoff, J. J., Wajda, D. A., Sandroff, B. M., Roeing, K. L.,
Health Education Research, 30 (3), 436–448. https://doi.org/
10.1093/her/cyv016 Sung, J., & Motl, R. W. (2017). Dual task training in per-
Peterson, M. L., Christou, E., & Rosengren, K. S. (2006). sons with multiple sclerosis: A feasability randomized con-
Children achieve adult-like sensory integration during stance trolled trial. Clinical Rehabilitation, 31 (10), 1322–1331.
at 12-years-old. Gait & Posture, 23 (4), 455–463. https:// https://doi.org/10.1177/0269215517698028
doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.05.003 Stins, J. F., Ledebt, A., Emck, C., Van Dokkum, E. H., &
Prieto, T. E., Myklebust, J. B., Hoffmann, R. G., Lovett, E. G., Beek, P. J. (2009). Patterns of postural sway in high anxious
& Myklebust, B. M. (1996). Measures of postural steadiness: children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5 (1), 1.
Differences between healthy young and elderly adults. IEEE Szalma, J. L., Warm, J. S., Matthews, G., Dember, W. N.,
Transactions on Bio-Medical Engineering, 43 (9), 956–966. Weiler, E. M., Meier, A., & Eggemeier, F. T. (2004). Effects
https://doi.org/10.1109/10.532130 of sensory modality and task duration on performance, work-
Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., & Hecht, S. A. (2010). load, and stress in sustained attention. Human Factors, 46
Working memory and mathematics: A review of develop- (2), 219–233. https://doi.org/10.1518/hfes.46.2.219.37334
mental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Torres-Oviedo, G., Vasudevan, E., Malone, L., & Bastian, A. J.
Learning and Individual Differences, 20 (2), 110–122. (2011). Locomotor adaptation. Progress in Brain Research,
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005 191, 65–74. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-53752-2.00013-8
Reilly, D. S., van Donkelaar, P., Saavedra, S., & Woollacott, Vellas, B. J., Wayne, S. J., Romero, L., Baumgartner, R. N.,
M. H. (2008). Interaction between the development of pos- Rubenstein, L. Z., & Garry, P. J. (1997). One-leg balance is
tural control and the executive function of attention. Journal
an important predictor of injurious falls in older persons.
of Motor Behavior, 40 (2), 90–102.
Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 45 (6), 735–738.
Remy, F., Wenderoth, N., Lipkens, K., & Swinnen, S. P.
doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.1997.tb01479.x
(2010). Dual-task interference during initial learning of a
Vuillerme, N., & Vincent, H. (2006). How performing a mental
new motor task results from competition for the same brain
areas. Neuropsychologia, 48 (9), 2517–2527. https://doi.org/ arithmetic task modify the regulation of centre of foot pres-
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.026 sure displacements during bipedal quiet standing.
Rietschel, J. C., Miller, M. W., Gentili, R. J., Goodman, R. N., Experimental Brain Research, 169 (1), 130–134. doi:
McDonald, C. G., & Hatfield, B. D. (2012). Cerebral-cortical 10.1007/s00221-005-0124-9
networking and activation increase as a function of cogni- Weaver, T. B., Janzen, M. R., Adkin, A. L., & Tokuno, C. D.
tive-motor task difficulty. Biological Psychology, 90 (2), (2012). Changes in spinal excitability during dual task per-
127–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.022 formance. Journal of Motor Behavior, 44 (4), 289–294. doi:
Roche, R. A. P., Commins, S., Agnew, F., Cassidy, S., Corapi, 10.1080/00222895.2012.702142
K., Leibbrand, S., … O’Mara, S. M. (2007). Concurrent Yim-Chiplis, P. K., & Talbot, L. A. (2000). Defining and
task performance enhances low-level visuomotor learning. measuring balance in adults. Biological Research for
Perception & Psychophysics, 69 (4), 513–522. Nursing, 1 (4), 321–331. doi:10.1177/109980040000100408
Seidler, R. D., Bo, J., & Anguera, J. A. (2012). Neurocognitive
contributions to motor skill learning: The role of working Received March 20, 2018
memory. Journal of Motor Behavior, 44 (6), 445–453. Revised January 25, 2019
https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2012.672348 Accepted February 26, 2019

You might also like