You are on page 1of 8

CYBERPSYCHOLOGY, BEHAVIOR, AND SOCIAL NETWORKING

Volume 14, Number 1-2, 2011 RAPID COMMUNICATIONS


ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.
DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0306

Problematic Internet Use Among Greek University Students:


An Ordinal Logistic Regression with Risk Factors
of Negative Psychological Beliefs, Pornographic Sites,
and Online Games

Christos C. Frangos, Ph.D.,1 Constantinos C. Frangos, M.D.,2 and Ioannis Sotiropoulos, Ph.D.3

Abstract

The aim of this paper is to investigate the relationships between Problematic Internet Use (PIU) among uni-
versity students in Greece and factors such as gender, age, family condition, academic performance in the last
semester of their studies, enrollment in unemployment programs, amount of Internet use per week (in general
and per application), additional personal habits or dependencies (number of coffees, alcoholic drinks drunk per
day, taking substances, cigarettes smoked per day), and negative psychological beliefs. Data were gathered from
2,358 university students from across Greece. The prevalence of PIU was 34.7% in our sample, and PIU was
significantly associated with gender, parental family status, grade of studies during the previous semester,
staying or not with parents, enrollment of the student in an unemployment program, and whether the student
paid a subscription to the Internet ( p < 0.0001). On average, problematic Internet users use MSN, forums,
YouTube, pornographic sites, chat rooms, advertisement sites, Google, Yahoo!, their e-mail, ftp, games, and
blogs more than non-problematic Internet users. PIU was also associated with other potential addictive personal
habits of smoking, drinking alcohol or coffee, and taking drugs. Significant risk factors for PIU were being male,
enrolment in unemployment programs, presence of negative beliefs, visiting pornographic sites, and playing
online games. Thus PIU is prevalent among Greek university students and attention should be given to it by
health officials.

Introduction [Internet] use . . . We believe that terms such as ‘excessive,’


‘problematic,’ or ‘maladaptive’ Internet use are most optimal
for describing this behavior as they involve fewer theoretical
T he Internet has become an indispensable tool for
communication, academic research, information, and
entertainment. Research supports that it could fulfill needs for
overtones than terms such as ‘Internet addiction.’’’5 Caplan6
further substantiates this argument and adds the necessary
social recognition, social support, and a sense of belonging not presence of maladaptive cognitions and behaviors involving
currently present in a person’s immediate social world. Internet use that result in negative academic, professional,
Paradoxically, though, heavy users of the Internet are de- and social consequences. Moreover, he supports that ‘‘the
scribed as individuals who lack confident social skills and term problematic refers to usage reflecting a specific cycle of
tend to be isolated. The extreme psychological dysfunction innate dysfunction leading to Internet use that in turn exac-
encountered with Internet use has been labeled Internet ad- erbates the dysfunction.’’6 Although he differentiates it from
diction (IA).1,2 the word pathological, used by Davis,7 he also believes that
There has been a certain amount of literature discussing IA is a misnomer. Thatcher et al.8 discuss the wider generality
whether Problematic Internet Use (PIU) is a more appropriate of PIU over IA rather eloquently:
term to use.3,4 Beard and Wolf 5 define PIU as ‘‘use of the In recent years there have been a wide variety of investiga-
Internet that creates psychological, social, school, and/or tions examining the ‘‘addictive’’ use of the Internet and the
work difficulties in a person’s life.’’ They continue by saying predictors of Internet addiction in studies from around
that ‘‘Internet addiction has been used to describe problematic the world. A great deal of the debate has centred on whether the

1
Department of Business Administration, Technological Educational Institute (TEI) of Athens, Greece.
2
Division of Medicine, University College London, London, United Kingdom.
3
Department of Finance and Auditing, Technological Educational Institute (TEI) of Epirus, Greece.

51
52 FRANGOS ET AL.

Internet is indeed ‘‘addictive’’ given that there is no psychiatric Kapodistrian University of Athens, the Agricultural Uni-
classification for ‘‘addictions’’ in the DSM-IV. Instead, there has versity of Athens, the Athens University of Economics and
been a proliferation of terms to describe the ‘‘overuse’’ or Business, the Technological Educational Institute (TEI) of
‘‘abuse’’ of the Internet. Terms used have included ‘‘unregulated Athens, the TEI of Piraeus, and the TEI of Epirus in Northern
Internet use’’, ‘‘compulsive Internet use’’, ‘‘Internet depen-
Greece. Sixty-five students did not give their age and were
dence’’, ‘‘excessive Internet use’’, ‘‘pathological Internet use’’,
and ‘‘problematic Internet use’’. Alternatively, some researchers
excluded from our analysis, leaving a sample of 2,293 stu-
argue that a person’s overuse or abuse of the Internet is a be- dents. Of these, 46.4% were male and 53.4% were female;
havioural manifestation of other things that may be problematic 35.1% were aged between 18 and 20 years, 37.7% were aged
in their lives.8 between 20 and 22 years, 17.4% were aged between 22 and 24
years, and 9.9% were over the age of 24 years.
In this study, we have selected the term Problematic In-
ternet Use, as it particularly avoids sensitive or controversial
qualities that accompany terms such as addiction and pa- The questionnaire
thology. We shall follow the classification suggested by Sio- Students were asked to complete an anonymous ques-
mos et al.9 and Johannson and Götestam:10 Internet users tionnaire consisting of six parts: (a) demographic elements,
who answer ‘‘Yes’’ to between five and eight questions from (b) general questions on computer and Internet use, (c) time
Young’s Diagnostic Test for Internet Addiction11 are ‘‘Internet spent on general Internet use per week for the past years and
addicted’’ and users who meet between three and four of the for various online applications per week, (d) Problematic
diagnostic criteria are ‘‘at-risk Internet users.’’ PIU is defined Internet Use section (condition of relations with parents,
as IA plus at-risk Internet use (ARIU). teachers, friends, and fellow students, and Young’s Diag-
Several studies have examined the relationship between nostic Test of Internet Addiction [YDTIA]11), (e) psychologi-
PIU and gender, age, academic performance, location of cal beliefs of Internet users, and (f ) additional personal habits
computer, excessive use of Internet, and psychological atti- or dependencies.
tudes.12 University students are considered a high-risk group In the present survey, a priori theoretical assumptions
for PIU.13–15 Possible reasons for this are that: (a) students about the nature of the expected factors of our questionnaire
have huge blocks of unstructured time; (b) schools and uni- were available from previous studies.7,11,17 Items were col-
versities provide free and unlimited access to the Internet; (c) lected from these studies on PIU, and rephrased as outcome
students aged between 18 and 22 years are away from pa- expectations (i.e., ‘‘Assess on a scale from 1–7 to what extent
rental control for the first time without anyone monitoring or the use of the Internet has affected your daily schedule, where
censoring what they say or do online; (d) young students 1 is extremely positive and 7 indifferent’’). These statements
experience new problems adapting to university life and were classified into four factors: Factor 1: Impact of Internet
finding new friends; (e) students receive full encouragement use on daily activities (IMPINT); Factor 2: YDTIA; Factor 3:
from faculty and administrators in using the different Internet Time Scale of Internet Use (TSIU); Factor 4: Negative Psy-
applications; (f ) adolescents are more trained to use the dif- chological Beliefs (NPB). We subjected these to confirmatory
ferent applications of technological inventions and especially factor analysis (CFA) with Amos 16.0. The results of CFA
the Internet; (g) students desire to escape university sources showed that the variables were not orthogonal to each
of stress resulting from their obligations to pass the exams, other, but they were related to some degree. Figure 1 illus-
deliver essays, and fulfill their purpose of getting their de- trates the four-factor CFA model and explanations for all
grees in the prescribed time with reasonable marks; and fi- variables and factors in the figure are given in supplementary
nally, (h) students feel that university life is alienated from Table 1 (see online supplementary material at http://www
social activities, and when they finish their studies, the job .liebertonline.com).
market, with all its uncertainties, is a field where they must The overall model fit was assessed using the root-mean-
participate and succeed in getting a job.16 squared error of approximation (RMSEA) and the compara-
PIU among university students in Greece has not been tive fit index (CFI). The chi-square test was not used as a
studied. The aim of this sample survey was to measure the measure of fit, since it is very sensitive to sample size and
prevalence of PIU, and to identify the risk factors of PIU for almost always indicates a poor fit when a large number of
students who are studying in certain universities in Athens estimated parameters are used. The CFI (0.83) and RMSEA
and other cities in mainland Greece. For this purpose, we (0.051) both indicated a good fit for the model.18
examined whether students’ gender, age, family condition,
academic performance in the last semester of their studies, Problematic Internet use diagnostic test
enrollment in unemployment programs, amount of Internet
use per week (generally and per application), additional per- Thus CFA showed that our hypothesis of the four factors
sonal habits or dependencies (number of coffee or alcoholic created a satisfactory model. This enabled us to construct a
drinks or cigarettes consumed per day and daily frequency of Problematic Internet Use Diagnostic Test (PIUDT), consisting
taking substances), and negative psychological beliefs are as- of the following domains, based on the theoretical assump-
sociated with PIU and are possible risk factors of PIU. tion of PIU in the Introduction:
(a) Problematic Internet Use Domain (PIUD). This domain
Methods consisted of two subparts. The first subpart (Factor 1
from CFA: IMPINT) contained questions in a Likert-
Participants
type scale from 1 to 7, concerning the degree of influ-
The sample survey was conducted among a sample of ence of Internet use in various daily aspects of the
2,358 students, drawn randomly from the National and subject’s personal life (eight items). The second subpart
PIU IN GREEK UNIVERSITY STUDENTS 53

FIG. 1. Confirmatory factor analysis diagram. Chi-square ¼ 4,639.9, degrees of freedom ¼ 659, p < 0.0001. Explanations for
the acronyms are given in supplementary Table 1 (see online supplementary material at http://www.liebertonline.com).

(Factor 2 from CFA: YDTIA) contained the 8-item Di- from good to excellent for PIU and TSIU, and acceptable for
agnostic Test of Internet Addiction developed by NPB.19 The items of each domain are mentioned in supple-
Young.11 mentary Table 1 (see online supplementary material at
(b) Time Scale of Internet Use (TSIU) Domain (Factor 3 http://www.liebertonline.com).
from CFA: TSIU; 14 items).
(c) Negative Psychological Beliefs (NPB) Domain (Factor 4 Statistical analysis
from CFA: NPB; eight items).
Group differences in categorical variables were analyzed
Table 1 contains the coefficients of internal consistency and using the chi-square test. Subsequently, ordinal logistic re-
reliability for the three domains of the PIUDT. These ranged gression with a Logit link was used to analyze the association

Table 1. Coefficients of Internal Consistency and Reliability for the Problematic Internet Use Diagnostic Test

Cronbach’s Standardized item Guttman split-half Spearman–Brown


Domains of PIUDT a coefficient a coefficient coefficient coefficient No. of items

PIU domain 0.817 0.723 0.919 0.919 16


TSIU domain 0.831 0.854 0.825 0.826 14
NPB domain 0.671 0.685 0.700 0.700 8
54 FRANGOS ET AL.

between the multiple risk factors and PIU. Variables with TEI of Epirus, which is located in a remote and one of the
p < 0.05 in the univariate analyses were included in the poorest regions of Europe, the IA percentage was 13.6%.
multivariate models, and we retained those variables that
produced at least one beta estimate significantly different Ordinal logistic regression
from zero. Ordinal logistic regression was chosen because
The results of ordinal logistic regression are shown in
PIU classification consisted of three categories, with increas-
Table 2. All models presented a good fit ( p < 0.0001): the test
ing severity of values (non-problematic Internet users ¼ 0, at-
of parallel line could not reject the null hypothesis ( p > 0.05)
risk Internet users ¼ 1, Internet-addicted users ¼ 2). Logit is
and the Pearson chi-square goodness-of-fit measure was al-
the most commonly used link, and it allows us to produce
ways non-significant. These summary measures suggest sat-
odds ratios by exponentiation of the model estimates. Nega-
isfactory ordinal logistic regression models.20
tive estimates of the independent variables show that one
Significant risk factors of PIU were gender and enroll-
value of an independent variable compared to its following
ment in the unemployment STAGE program. From the
value is more likely to receive lower values on the ordinal
amount of Internet use, a significant risk factor was daily
dependent variable, and vice versa for positive estimates.20
hours of Internet use. After a certain degree of hours, the
The predefined control group was the Internet-addicted
p values are above 0.05; we see that beta estimates tend to
group.
increase, although the evidence is not sufficient to say that
they are different from zero. YouTube use was a risk factor
Results along with online games, pornographic sites, and chatting.
Demographics For online games, the estimate for its fifth value (7  h < 9;
beta estimate ¼ 0.961, p ¼ 0.003) is significantly different
All percentages and chi-squares with p values for demo- from zero, showing that this value is associated with higher
graphic variables are presented in supplementary Table 2 (see values on the scale of PIU (virtually, at-risk Internet users and
online supplementary material at http://www.liebertonline Internet-addicted users). Significant risk factors of PIU were
.com). All demographic variables (gender, parental marital all negative psychological beliefs, which presented negative
status, mark of studies during the previous semester, staying estimates, showing that No answers (x ¼ 0) are associated
with parents or not, enrollment in an unemployment pro- more with lower values in the scale of PIU, which are non-
gram,a personal payment of Internet subscription) correlated problematic Internet users (x ¼ 0). Hence, Yes answers (x ¼ 1)
with PIU ( p < 0.0001). A greater percentage of men were for the presence of negative psychological beliefs are associ-
problematic Internet users than women (42.1% vs. 27.7%); ated with higher levels of PIU. Finally, two additional risk
more divorced parents spawned problematic Internet users factors of PIU are drinking coffee and the number of cigarettes
than married or unmarried parents, while a greater percent- consumed daily.
age of students not living with their parents were more In-
ternet addicted than those living with their parents. Discussion
Moreover, people enrolled in the unemployment programs
(STAGE) were more likely to be problematic Internet users We performed a cross-sectional study of university stu-
than students who were not enrolled in these programs, and dents in Greece to estimate the prevalence of PIU. Our most
this occurred with students who personally paid for their important result was that the prevalence of IA and PIU was
Internet subscriptions. 12.0% and 34.7% respectively. Additionally, PIU was associ-
The amount of weekly use of certain Internet Web sites and ated with being of male gender, divorced parental condition,
applications correlated significantly with PIU ( p < 0.0001; low marks during the previous semester, not staying with
supplementary Table 3 [see online supplementary material parents, enrollment of the student in an unemployment
at http://www.liebertonline.com]). On average, we can see program, and the student paying for his own subscription to
that problematic Internet users use MSN, forums, YouTube, the Internet ( p < 0.0001). On average, we can see that prob-
pornographic sites, chat rooms, advertisement sites, Google, lematic Internet users use MSN, forums, YouTube, porno-
Yahoo!, their e-mail, ftp, games, and blogs more than non- graphic sites, chat rooms, advertisement sites, Google,
problematic Internet users. Moreover, PIU was significantly Yahoo!, their e-mail, ftp, games, and blogs more than non-
associated with other personal habits/dependencies of problematic Internet users. And finally, PIU was associated
smoking, drinking alcohol or coffee, and taking drugs (sup- with other potential addictive personal habits like smoking,
plementary Table 4 [see online supplementary material at drinking alcohol or coffee, and taking drugs, as well as neg-
http://www.liebertonline.com]). On average, Internet- ative psychological beliefs. We performed ordinal regression
addicted users and at-risk Internet users would consume models to examine if PIU could be predicted by the variables
larger quantities of alcoholic drinks, coffee, and cigarettes, presenting a significant association with it in univariate an-
and would use drugs three times more often than non- alyses. Because of the cross-sectional nature of this study, our
problematic Internet users. results should be interpreted more as associations rather than
causal relationships, something that could be deducted from
an experimental prospective study of university students.
Problematic Internet use
Nevertheless, this study adds to the growing body of evi-
The prevalence rate of IA in our sample of 2,293 students dence on PIU, and we would like to suggest some explana-
was 12.0%, while at-risk Internet users made up 22.7%; thus tions for our observed results.
34.7% were problematic Internet users and 65.3% were non- Ordinal regression showed that being of male gender was
problematic Internet users. The prevalence of IA for students a risk factor for PIU. This observation corroborates the find-
at the TEI of Athens was 10.9%, whereas for students of the ings of others.11,21–23 Tsai et al.23 give a satisfactory explana-
PIU IN GREEK UNIVERSITY STUDENTS 55

Table 2. Ordinal Regression Results and the Risk Factors of Problematic Internet Use

Variables Logistic coefficient Standard error P value Odds ratio 95% CI

Demographics Model fit: chi-square ¼ 73.01 ( p < 0.0001); Test of parallel lines: chi-square ¼ 5.013 ( p ¼ 0.082);
Goodness-of-fit: (i) Pearson chi-square ¼ 6.151 ( p ¼ 0.188) and (ii) Deviance chi-square ¼ 6.251
( p ¼ 0.181)
Gender
Female (x ¼ 0) 0.689 0.094 p < 0.0001 0.50 0.42–0.60
Male (x ¼ 1) Ref
Unemployment programs
No (x ¼ 0) 0.713 0.160 p < 0.0001 0.49 0.36–0.67
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Internet use Model fit: chi-square ¼ 367.17 ( p < 0.0001); Test of parallel lines: chi-square ¼ 40.69 ( p ¼ 0.114);
Goodness-of-fit: (i) Pearson chi-square ¼ 1670.87 ( p ¼ 0.031) and (ii) Deviance chi-square ¼
1626.49 ( p ¼ 0.136)
Daily hours of Internet use
h < 0.5 (x ¼ 1) 1.188 0.272 p < 0.0001 0.30 0.18–0.52
0.5  h < 1 (x ¼ 2) 1.184 0.263 p < 0.0001 0.30 0.18–0.51
1  h < 2 (x ¼ 3) 0.837 0.269 p ¼ 0.002 0.43 0.26–0.73
2  h < 3 (x ¼ 4) 0.921 0.269 p ¼ 0.001 0.40 0.23–0.68
3  h < 4 (x ¼ 5) 0.546 0.272 p ¼ 0.045 0.58 0.34–0.99
4  h < 5 (x ¼ 6) 0.405 0.281 p ¼ 0.149 0.67 0.38–1.16
5  h < 6 (x ¼ 7) 0.240 0.295 p ¼ 0.415 0.79 0.44–1.40
6  h < 7 (x ¼ 8) 0.532 0.341 p ¼ 0.119 0.59 0.30–1.15
7  h < 8 (x ¼ 9) 0.126 0.360 p ¼ 0.728 1.13 0.56–2.30
8  h < 9 (x ¼ 10) 0.320 0.498 p ¼ 0.521 0.73 0.27–1.93
9  h < 10 (x ¼ 11) 0.958 0.520 p ¼ 0.065 2.60 0.94–7.23
h  10 (x ¼ 12) Ref
Pornographic sites (h/week)
0  h < 1 (x ¼ 1) 0.887 0.233 p < 0.0001 0.41 0.26–0.65
1  h < 3 (x ¼ 2) 0.514 0.260 p ¼ 0.048 0.60 0.36–0.99
3  h < 5 (x ¼ 3) 0.325 0.320 p ¼ 0.309 1.38 0.74–2.59
5  h < 7 (x ¼ 4) 0.364 0.387 p ¼ 0.347 1.44 0.67–3.07
7  h < 9 (x ¼ 5) 0.341 0.407 p ¼ 0.402 1.41 0.63–3.12
h  9 (x ¼ 6) Ref
Online games (h/week)
0  h < 1 (x ¼ 1) 0.182 0.243 p ¼ 0.453 0.83 0.52–1.34
1  h < 3 (x ¼ 2) 0.131 0.252 p ¼ 0.603 1.14 0.70–1.87
3  h < 5 (x ¼ 3) 0.332 0.267 p ¼ 0.213 1.39 0.83–2.35
5  h < 7 (x ¼ 4) 0.005 0.295 p ¼ 0.987 0.99 0.56–1.78
7  h < 9 (x ¼ 5) 0.961 0.326 p ¼ 0.003 2.61 1.38–4.96
h  9 (x ¼ 6) Ref
Chatting (h/week)
0  h < 1 (x ¼ 1) 1.003 0.277 p < 0.0001 0.37 0.21–0.63
1  h < 3 (x ¼ 2) 0.724 0.290 p ¼ 0.013 0.49 0.27–0.86
3  h < 5 (x ¼ 3) 0.433 0.309 p ¼ 0.161 0.65 0.35–1.19
5  h < 7 (x ¼ 4) 0.293 0.336 p ¼ 0.383 0.75 0.39–1.44
7  h < 9 (x ¼ 5) 0.305 0.369 p ¼ 0.410 1.36 0.66–2.80
h  9 (x ¼ 6) Ref
YouTube (h/week)
0  h < 1 (x ¼ 1) 0.618 0.234 p ¼ 0.008 0.54 0.34–0.85
1  h < 3 (x ¼ 2) 0.332 0.226 p ¼ 0.142 0.72 0.46–1.12
3  h < 5 (x ¼ 3) 0.294 0.234 p ¼ 0.209 0.75 0.47–1.18
5  h < 7 (x ¼ 4) 0.265 0.244 p ¼ 0.279 0.77 0.48–1.24
7  h < 9 (x ¼ 5) 0.382 0.287 p ¼ 0.183 0.68 0.39–1.20
h  9 (x ¼ 6) Ref
Negative psychological Model fit: chi-square ¼ 386.49 ( p < 0.0001); Test of parallel lines: chi-square ¼ 4.951 ( p ¼ 0.763);
beliefs Goodness-of-fit: (i) Pearson chi-square ¼ 466.61 ( p ¼ 0.108) and (ii) Deviance chi-square ¼ 518.90
( p ¼ 0.002)
Do you feel lonely and you need protection?
No (x ¼ 0) 0.661 0.122 p < 0.0001 0.52 0.41–0.65
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Whenever you face difficult problems, do you find that you don’t insist in solving them and leave them?
No (x ¼ 0) 0.561 0.117 p < 0.0001 0.57 0.45–0.72
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
(continued)
56 FRANGOS ET AL.

Table 2. (Continued)

Variables Logistic coefficient Standard error P value Odds ratio 95% CI

Do you believe that the purpose of life is enjoyment?


No (x ¼ 0) 0.522 0.104 p < 0.0001 0.59 0.48–0.73
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Do you find that you don’t want to work?
No (x ¼ 0) 0.433 0.103 p < 0.0001 0.65 0.53–0.79
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Do you find that you don’t have objectives in your life?
No (x ¼ 0) 0.489 0.115 p < 0.0001 0.61 0.49–0.77
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Do you believe that everything in life is trivial?
No (x ¼ 0) 0.546 0.130 p < 0.0001 0.58 0.45–0.75
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Do you believe that you have been abandoned by everybody?
No (x ¼ 0) 0.464 0.154 p ¼ 0.003 0.63 0.46–0.85
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Do you have a deep disappointment from your life?
No (x ¼ 0) 0.470 0.136 p ¼ 0.001 0.62 0.48–0.82
Yes (x ¼ 1) Ref
Other personal Model fit: chi-square ¼ 163.26 ( p < 0.0001); Test of parallel lines: chi-square ¼ 17.37 ( p ¼ 0.067);
habits or dependencies Goodness-of-fit: (i) Pearson chi-square ¼ 60.99 ( p ¼ 0.301) and (ii) Deviance chi-square ¼ 62.86
( p ¼ 0.246)
Cups of coffee drunk per day
0 to 1 (x ¼ 1) 1.450 0.325 p < 0.0001 0.23 0.12–0.44
2 to 3 (x ¼ 2) 1.252 0.319 p < 0.0001 0.29 0.15–0.53
3 to 4 (x ¼ 3) 0.941 0.330 p ¼ 0.004 0.39 0.20–0.75
4 to 5 (x ¼ 4) 0.069 0.367 p ¼ 0.850 0.93 0.45–1.91
6 to 7 (x ¼ 5) 0.000 0.399 p ¼ 0.999 1.00 0.46–2.18
More than 7 (x ¼ 6) Ref
Cigarettes smoked per day
0 to 1 (x ¼ 1) 0.832 0.230 p < 0.0001 0.44 0.28–0.68
2 to 3 (x ¼ 2) 0.595 0.251 p ¼ 0.018 0.55 0.34–0.90
3 to 4 (x ¼ 3) 0.650 0.243 p ¼ 0.008 0.52 0.32–0.84
4 to 5 (x ¼ 4) 0.215 0.239 p ¼ 0.369 0.81 0.50–1.29
6 to 7 (x ¼ 5) 0.395 0.260 p ¼ 0.129 0.67 0.40–1.12
More than 7 (x ¼ 6) Ref

tion for this gender difference, and denote that the more vi- eventually bad marks during the exam period.11,16,28 Con-
sual nature of men compared to women—with respect to cerning the use and time patterns of Internet applications
sexual fantasies—leads them to see more pornographic sites. (games, chatting, e-mail), frequency of their use by prob-
This has been facilitated by a significant decrease in band- lematic Internet users is higher than the frequency of use by
width costs over the last few years. Truly, we found that healthy Internet users; in particular, viewing sex pages,
pornographic sites were significantly associated with PIU, chatting, and viewing YouTube were risk factors for
and ordinal regression showed that it was a risk factor for PIU.11,22,29 Negative psychological beliefs (feelings of loneli-
PIU. Moreover, many other researchers have repeatedly ness, deep disappointment in life, being abandoned, a sense
written that women use the Internet mostly for social pur- that everything in life is trivial, an absence of objectives in life,
poses, and men for downloading programs, getting infor- whether the only purpose in life is enjoyment, the tendency of
mation, and for sex purposes.16,24–26 The other possible factor giving up on solving problems, and a weak will to work)
increasing the percentage of PIU among males is the in- were all significant risk factors for PIU. It is a question,
creased frequency of previous online gaming and online though, whether negative psychological beliefs cause PIU or
gaming addictions in male adolescents compared to that are a result of it. The cognitive behavioral model suggests that
among females.27 The lower percentage of PIU among female they preexist PIU as a distal cause7 and the appearance of PIU
college students is explained by the fact that they often re- causes them to become more intense (rich-get-richer model).
ceive more family supervision than males, preventing them Kim et al.30 suggest, however, that the relationship between
from spending as much time as men on the Internet.23 loneliness and PIU is possibly bidirectional, since excessive
Bad academic performance was also significantly associ- and compulsive use of the Internet can cause psychosocial
ated with PIU. This probably occurs because of lack of sleep, dysfunctions as well.
since the user stays awake during late-night hours in order to Finally, two new important results arise from our analysis.
surf through various web pages. The lack of sleep causes lack Ordinal regression showed that enrollment in unemployment
of concentration and loss of interest in everyday lectures, programs (STAGE) and the presence of other potentially
resulting in reduced reading of the course material and addictive habits (smoking and drinking coffee) are risk
PIU IN GREEK UNIVERSITY STUDENTS 57

factors for PIU. Moreover, chi-square tests showed that these 7. Davis RA. Cognitive-behavioral model of pathological In-
factors, along with drinking alcohol and taking drugs, were ternet use. Computers in Human Behavior 2001; 17:187–95.
significantly associated with PIU. Binge drinking has been 8. Thatcher A, Wretschko G, Fridjhon P. Online flow experi-
associated with unemployed males,31 and this could be the ences, problematic Internet use and Internet procrastination.
link between PIU and unemployment. Possible explanations Computers in Human Behavior 2008; 24:2236–54.
involve either common antecedent factors that promote vul- 9. Siomos KE, Dafouli ED, Braimiotis DA, et al. Internet
nerability to the coexistence between drug use or the presence addiction among Greek adolescent students. CyberPsychol-
of other personal habits/dependencies and PIU, and that they ogy & Behavior 2008; 11:653–7.
represent different conditions along a spectrum of related 10. Johansson A, Götestam KG. Internet addiction: Characteristics
of a questionnaire and prevalence in Norwegian youth (12–18
disorders of addiction. However, these possible antecedent or
years). Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 2004; 45:223–9.
vulnerability factors linking PIU and substance use disorders
11. Young KS. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clin-
still remain elusive. This coexistence of PIU with other per-
ical disorder. CyberPsychology & Behavior 1998; 1:237–44.
sonal habits/dependencies is a similar pattern to that ob- 12. Byun S, Ruffini C, Mills JE, et al. Internet addiction: Meta-
served in the coexistence of pathological gambling and synthesis of 1996–2006 quantitative research. CyberPsy-
substance abuse. Although this parallel might be unequal, it chology & Behavior 2009; 12:203–7.
is useful to draw upon it as an example, in order to seek 13. Nalwa K, Anand AP. Internet addiction in students: A cause
aetiological factors.32 of concern. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2003; 6:653–6.
In conclusion, our study presents limitations in that it does 14. Kandell JJ. Internet addiction on campus: The vulnerability of
not elucidate the mechanism of risk factors associated with college students. CyberPsychology & Behavior 1998; 1:11–7.
PIU. Thus a causal relationship cannot be confirmed but only 15. Chen YF, Peng SS. University students’ Internet use and its
hypothesized. However, a possible profile of the PIU student relationships with academic performance, interpersonal re-
stems from this study: a male subject who spends increased lationships, psychosocial adjustment, and self-evaluation.
hours daily on chatting, gaming, and pornographic sites re- CyberPsychology & Behavior 2008; 11:467–9.
sults in significant psychological distress and functional im- 16. Young KS. Internet addiction: A new clinical phenomenon
pairment. and its consequences. American Behavioral Scientist 2004;
48:402–15.
Notes 17. Weiser EB. The functions of Internet use and their social and
psychological consequences. CyberPsychology & Behavior
a. In Greece, governmental unemployment programs are 2001; 4:723–43.
called STAGE. Thus enrolment in these programs is 18. Byrne M. (2001) Structural equation modeling with AMOS:
considered as an objective measure of unemployment in Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Hillsdale, NJ:
Greece, and we included the question ‘‘Are you incor- Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 79–86.
porated in a STAGE program or in a subsidized seminar 19. George D, Mallery P. (2003) SPSS for Windows step by step: A
for unemployed?’’ in our questionnaire. simple guide and reference. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, p. 231.
20. O’Connell AA. (2006) Logistic regression models for ordinal
Acknowledgments response variables. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
21. Greenfield DN. Psychological characteristics of compulsive
We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for Internet use: A preliminary analysis. CyberPsychology &
useful comments on our paper. Behavior 1999; 2:403–12.
22. Özcan NK, Buzlu S. Internet use and its relation with the
Disclosure Statement psychosocial situation for a sample of university students.
No competing financial interests exist. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2007; 10:767–72.
23. Tsai HF, Cheng SH, Yeh TL, et al. The risk factors of Internet
addiction—A survey of university freshmen. Psychiatry
References
Research 2009; 167:294–9.
1. Kim HK, Davis KE. Toward a comprehensive theory of 24. Kraut R, Kiesler S, Boneva B, et al. Internet paradox re-
problematic Internet use: Evaluating the role of self-esteem, visited. Journal of Social Issues 2002; 58:49–74.
anxiety, flow, and the self-rated importance of Internet ac- 25. Ybarra M, Mitchell KJ. Exposure to Internet pornogra-
tivities. Computers in Human Behavior 2009; 25:490–500. phy among children and adolescents: A national survey.
2. Beard KW. Internet addiction: A review of current assess- CyberPsychology & Behavior 2005; 8:473–86.
ment techniques and potential assessment questions. Cy- 26. Tsitsika A, Critselis E, Kormas G, et al. Adolescent porno-
berPsychology & Behavior 2005; 8:7–14. graphic Internet site use: A multivariate regression analysis
3. Yellowlees PM, Marks S. Problematic Internet use or Internet of the predictive factors of use and psychosocial implica-
addiction? Computers in Human Behavior 2007; 23:1447–53. tions. CyberPsychology & Behavior 2009; 12:545–50.
4. Morahan-Martin J. Internet abuse: Addiction? Disorder? 27. Lin SSJ, Tsai CC. Sensation seeking and internet dependence
Symptom? Alternative explanations? Social Science Com- of Taiwanese high school adolescents. Computers in Human
puter Review 2005; 23:39–48. Behavior 2002; 18:411–26.
5. Beard KW, Wolf EM. Modification in the proposed diag- 28. Kubey RW, Lavin MJ, Barrows JR. Internet use and colle-
nostic criteria for Internet addiction. CyberPsychology & giate academic performance decrements: Early findings.
Behavior 2001; 4:377–83. Journal of Communication 2001; 51:366–82.
6. Caplan SE. Preference for online social interaction: A theory 29. Morahan-Martin J, Schumacher P. Incidence and correlates
of problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being. of pathological Internet use among college students. Com-
Communication Research 2003; 30:625–48. puters in Human Behavior 2000; 16:13–29.
58 FRANGOS ET AL.

30. Kim J, LaRose R, Peng W. Loneliness as the cause and Address correspondence to:
the effect of problematic Internet use: The relationship be- Dr. Constantinos C. Frangos
tween Internet use and psychological well-being. Cyber- University College London
Psychology & Behavior 2009; 12:451–5. Ifor Evans Hall Site
31. Crawford A, Plant MA, Kreitman N, et al. Unemployment 109 Camden Road
and drinking behaviour: Some data from a general popula- London
tion survey of alcohol use. British Journal of Addiction 1987; NW1 9HZ
82:1007–16. United Kingdom
32. Hall GW, Carriero NJ, Takushi RY, et al. Pathological
gambling among cocaine-dependent outpatients. American
E-mail: Constantinos.Frangos.09@ucl.ac.uk
Journal of Psychiatry 2000; 157:1127–33.

You might also like