Develop Med Child Neuro - 2014 - Jenni - Stability of Cognitive Performance in Children With Mild Intellectual Disability

You might also like

You are on page 1of 7

DEVELOPMENTAL MEDICINE & CHILD NEUROLOGY ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Stability of cognitive performance in children with mild


intellectual disability
OSKAR G JENNI 1,2 | SYLVIA FINTELMANN 1 | JON CAFLISCH 1 | BEATRICE LATAL 1,2 | VALENTIN ROUSSON 3 |
AZIZ CHAOUCH 3

1 Child Development Center, University Children’s Hospital Z€urich, Z€urich; 2 Children’s Research Center, University Children’s Hospital Z€urich, Z€urich; 3 Centre
Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois and University of Lausanne, Statistical Unit, Institute of Social and Preventive Medicine, Lausanne, Switzerland.
Correspondence to Oskar G Jenni at Department of Pediatrics, Child Development Center, University Children’s Hospital, Steinwiesstrasse 75, CH-8032 Z€urich, Switzerland.
E-mail: oskar.jenni@kispi.uzh.ch

PUBLICATION DATA AIM Longitudinal studies that have examined cognitive performance in children with
Accepted for publication 19th September intellectual disability more than twice over the course of their development are scarce. We
2014. assessed population and individual stability of cognitive performance in a clinical sample of
Published online 2nd November 2014. children with borderline to mild non-syndromic intellectual disability.
METHOD Thirty-six children (28 males, eight females; age range 3–19y) with borderline to
ABBREVIATIONS mild intellectual disability (Full-scale IQ [FSIQ] 50–85) of unknown origin were examined in a
FSIQ Full-scale IQ retrospective clinical case series using linear mixed models including at least three
SES Socio-economic status assessments with standardized intelligence tests.
RESULTS Average cognitive performance remained remarkably stable over time (high
population stability, drop of only 0.38 IQ points per year, standard error=0.39, p=0.325)
whereas individual stability was at best moderate (intraclass correlation of 0.58), indicating
that about 60% of the residual variation in FSIQ scores can be attributed to between-child
variability. Neither sex nor socio-economic status had a statistically significant impact on
FSIQ.
INTERPRETATION Although intellectual disability during childhood is a relatively stable
phenomenon, individual stability of IQ is only moderate, likely to be caused by test-to-test
reliability (e.g. level of child’s cooperation, motivation, and attention). Therefore, clinical
decisions and predictions should not rely on single IQ assessments, but should also consider
adaptive functioning and previous developmental history.

Cognitive performance tests are widely used in the clini- references see Conley1 and a recent twin study considering
cal evaluation of children with intellectual disabilities. genetic and environmental contributions2) and in children
Medical diagnostic and therapeutic procedures as well as with genetic syndromes such as Fragile X, Down syn-
the need for special educational services are often decided drome, or Williams syndrome,3–7 only few studies exist
on the magnitude of intellectual impairment of these indi- that include children with mild intellectual disability of
viduals. For example, special needs services are offered unknown origin (i.e. non-syndromic or unspecific intellec-
depending on individual IQ test scores (e.g. children with tual disability). For instance, Naglieri and Pfeiffer8 studied
an IQ<70 or 75 will receive support, while others will 54 children with a mean Full-scale IQ (FSIQ) of 75 at age
not). Moreover, physicians may initiate further diagnostic 9 years, re-tested them 3 years later using the Wechsler
procedures (such as genetic testing, magnetic resonance Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-R) and found a cor-
imaging, and so on) if an IQ is below a certain cut-off relation coefficient of 0.56. Similarly, Friedman et al.9
value or if cognitive decline is occurring. In this context, assessed 44 individuals with intellectual disability with a
a critical question is whether cognitive functions remain mean FSIQ of 80 at age 9 years, examined them 1.5 years
stable over time and, thus, are valid measures of the true later and found a correlation coefficient of 0.68 on the
abilities of the individual and therefore support further WISC FSIQ. In 2008, Whitaker10 summarized all available
clinical decisions. One approach to address this question studies in a meta-analysis on the stability of IQ in patients
is to study children with intellectual disability several with mild intellectual disability of unknown origin. Includ-
times during their development. ing 17 studies in his analysis, he found a weighted mean
While there is an abundance of earlier studies about the correlation coefficient of 0.82 for FSIQ over a mean test–
stability of IQ test scores in children with typical develop- retest interval of 2.8 years. He concluded that IQ remains
ment (for an older, but comprehensive review and relatively stable over time in children and adolescents with

© 2014 Mac Keith Press DOI: 10.1111/dmcn.12620 463


intellectual disability, although IQ assessments in these What this paper adds
children should be taken with care because some individu- • Knowledge about stability of cognitive performance in children with intellec-
als may change in their cognitive abilities more than others tual disability.
(i.e. show an increase or decrease of IQ>10 points). Whi- • Better clinical understanding of cognitive improvements or declines in intel-
taker stated, however, that only few studies are available lectual disability.
for individuals with three or more tests and an initial age
• Intellectual disability is a relatively stable phenomenon, but with moderate
test-to-test IQ stability.
of <10 years (see also Table I in Whitaker10). In fact, lon- • Sixty per cent of the intellectual disability variation is explained by
gitudinal studies that have assessed children with intellec- between-child variability.
tual disability several times (i.e. more than three times) in • Interpretation of cognitive profiles must not only include IQ, but also judg-
the course of their development are practically non-existent ment of adaptive functioning and previous developmental history.
(but see as an exception Clare et al.11).
HAWIK-R15 or HAWIK-III16), and the German version
The stability of longitudinal profiles of children with
of the Kaufman-Assessment Battery for Children (3–12.5y,
mild intellectual disability can be described with two dif-
K-ABC17). Only FSIQ was considered, because indices and
ferent concepts. First, one may examine how the popula-
subtest scores are known to be less reliable than FSIQ (see
tion globally evolves over time. In this context, the
Whitaker10). All testers were certified fellows or consul-
population mean is of particular interest. Thus, a constant
tants in developmental pediatrics with experience in the
mean would refer to a stable situation (i.e. ‘population sta-
psychological testing of children with developmental disor-
bility’), whereas a positive or negative trend in the mean
ders. Individual testers did not examine children and ado-
over time would be synonymous with changing conditions
lescents more than once.
(or ‘population instability’). Second, it might be of interest
to quantify how individuals track their own centile, that is,
Participants
whether their longitudinal profile follows the same pattern
Using a retrospective case series methodology we analyzed
as that of the population mean (‘individual stability’ or
the database of our outpatient clinic which included
reproducibility of the measurement) or whether future IQ
13 569 patients younger than 20 years evaluated between
measurements of a child are largely unpredictable given
1990 and 2006 for developmental disorders (global devel-
the past values (‘individual instability’).
opmental delays, learning disorders, intellectual disabilities,
In this study we assessed population and individual sta-
neurodevelopmental impairments, genetic syndromes,
bility by using FSIQ in a group of children with borderline
inborn errors of metabolism, and endocrine diseases) as
to mild non-syndromic intellectual disability referred to
well as children with behavioral disorders (e.g. sleep prob-
our clinical center.
lems, attention-deficit–hyperactivity disorder, etc.). The
clinical database was screened for individuals with border-
METHOD line to mild intellectual disability at the first visit (as
Measures defined by a FSIQ score below 85 [1SD below the mean]
Children were included in the analysis when valid FSIQ
and above 50) and having at least three cognitive assess-
test scores of the following standardized instruments were
ments. Although the children were selected based on their
obtained: the Snijders-Oomen Nonverbal Intelligence tests
IQ, they were all suffering from a number of functional
(3–7y, SON-Y12 or SON-R13), the German version of the
deficits (in language, memory, school and social abilities,
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence
self-management, etc.), which was the reason for referral
WPPSI (4y 5mo–7y, HAWIVA14), the German version of
to our center for further evaluation and therapeutic man-
the Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children WISC (6–18y,
agement. Children with genetic syndromes, abnormal pre-
natal/perinatal history, identifiable neurological disorders,
autism, psychosocial deprivation, drug exposure and any
Table I: Time interval between two successive assessments for each IQ other known aetiology for the disability were excluded
test from the analysis. The final sample consisted of 36 individ-
uals (28 males and eight females; age range 3–19y) with
Time between successive assessments
isolated non-syndromic (non-specific) mild intellectual dis-
Test Minimum (y:mo) Maximum (y:mo) ability or borderline intelligence of unknown origin (i.e. no
confirmed aetiology of the disability). These children were
HAWIK-III 0:9 4:9
HAWIVA 0:9 5:0 all born between 1984 and 1997 (median year of birth:
HAWIK-R 0:8 6:2 1989). Nineteen children had three to four IQ measure-
K-ABC 0:10 3:11 ments, 13 had five to six data points, and four children had
SON-R 1:2 1:11
SON-Y 0:6 2:0 seven to nine tests (164 FSIQ assessments with irregularly
spaced observations). The FSIQ at first evaluation was
K-ABC, German version of the Kaufman-Assessment Battery for
Children; HAWIK-R or HAWIK-III, German version of the Wechsler
measured between 51 and 83 scores with a median of 73
Intelligence Test for Children WISC; HAWIVA, German version of scores. The age at the first evaluation ranged from 3 to 8
the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; SON-R years 2 months, while the time between two successive
or SON-Y, Snijders-Oomen Nonverbal Intelligence test.

464 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2015, 57: 463–469


assessments performed on the same child ranged from individual FSIQ scores occurred between tests, although
1 month up to 8 years and 4 months. When considering most individuals remained in the low IQ range (50–85).
assessments performed using the same test, this time was Table II presents estimates and 95% CIs for model
quite variable (see Table I), but no shorter than 6 months parameters. We note that the test means at study entry
(SON-Y). The time between the first and the last assess- (a values) vary. However, these coefficients should be inter-
ment on the same child (duration of follow-up) ranged preted with caution as they may encompass a possible con-
from 2 years and 2 months up to 14 years and 2 months founded effect of age at entry. The cohort effect was not
with a median of 8 years. To accommodate these varying significant (p=0.721) which is not surprising since all chil-
lengths of follow-up and the developmental age of the dren were born within a period of only 13 years. On the
child, individuals were tested using different tests over other hand, for children tested with the same assessment
time. Most of the children (70%) had two or three differ- test within 1 year, a practice effect (i.e. IQ gain caused by
ent tests during their follow-up. A single child was assessed taking the same test at two consecutive assessments) was
three times using the same test (K-ABC). On the other observed, with children scoring on average 5.26 points
hand, two children with a long follow-up time (>7y) were higher (p=0.001) at the second occasion. Neither sex nor
tested with five different tests. SES had a statistically significant impact on FSIQ,
Information about parental occupation was gathered at although the eight females in our sample had a slightly
study entry and converted into the International Socio- lower average FSIQ compared to the 28 males (b1=2.31).
Economic Index.18 The higher International Socio-Eco- The evolution of FSIQ in the population was found to be
nomic Index score of the father or mother was used as the barely negative with an estimated drop of 0.38 points of
socio-economic status (SES) of the child. The SES ranged IQ per year (standard error=0.39), but this result was again
from 21 to 88 with a median of 40 points on the Interna- not statistically significant in our limited sample (p=0.325).
tional Socio-Economic Index scale. We note that the The intraclass correlation was estimated at 0.58 (95% CI
International Socio-Economic Index score in our study 0.46–0.74) indicating that about 60% of the residual varia-
sample was lower than in the representative Swiss PISA tion in FSIQ scores of the participants can be attributed to
study (n=1190, mean 52.9 [SD 16.4], unpublished data between-child variability (individual stability).
from Urs Moser, PhD, 2012).
The institutional review board confirmed that the study DISCUSSION
was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki, Clinicians are frequently asked by parents and other pro-
and conformed to legal and ethical norms. fessionals to make predictions for the developmental course
of children with intellectual disability. While a prediction
Statistical analysis can be made or is more robust in children with moderate
Our modeling approach addressed both definitions of sta- to severe intellectual disability that is often associated with
bility, namely the evolution of the population mean over a genetic syndrome, the prediction for children with bor-
time in a group of children with mild intellectual disability derline to mild intellectual disability is far more difficult.
(population stability) and how these individuals track their Our knowledge about the longitudinal course of these chil-
own centile given that average evolution in the population dren is based on studies which have included only two data
(individual stability). Stability of longitudinal profiles of points. Although the exisiting literature proposes a ‘high’
FSIQ among children with mild intellectual disability was correlation between initial and subsequent IQ testing in
analyzed using a linear mixed model (see Appendix S1, sup- children with non-syndromic intellectual disability,10 we
porting information published online for more details of are relatively often faced with the clinical situation where
the statistical analysis). In this model the FSIQ of an indi- children have made improvements or have declined
vidual is adjusted for the test, sex, and SES at study entry. between assessments. Thus, more data on the longitudinal
Cohort and practice effects have also been included in the course of these children are needed.
model as potential confounders (Appendix S1). The mean In this study including a true clinical sample, we have
evolution (population stability) is summarized by the coeffi- used a linear mixed model to study the stability of individ-
cient b5 whereas the intraclass correlation defined on the ual profiles of FSIQ among a group of children with bor-
interval [0,1] quantifies how children globally track their derline to mild non-syndromic intellectual disability. We
own centile (individual stability or reproducibility), with used the time since the first evaluation as the metric for
low values (close to 0) referring to poor tracking or low sta- recording time while adjusting for the IQ test for two rea-
bility and large values (close to 1) indicating a strong track- sons. First, in IQ testing current age and tests are partly
ing or high stability. Confidence intervals (CIs) for model collinear because tests change with increasing age. Second,
parameters were calculated using 2000 bootstrap-t samples children referred to our center for suspected intellectual
(see Davison and Hinkley19 [p. 194] and Appendix S1). disability and with a number of impairments in adaptive
functioning had to score between 50 and 85 at their first
RESULTS FSIQ assessment to enter the study. Thus, it was not
Figure 1 illustrates individual longitudinal profiles of all 36 appropriate to use age for recording time because new par-
individuals included in the study. Substantial changes in ticipants entering the study would have constantly pulled

Cognition in Children with Intellectual Disability Oskar G Jenni et al. 465


IQ Test
HAWIK-III K-ABC
HAWIVA SON-R
HAWIK-R SON-Y

5 10 15 20 5 10 15 20 5 10 15 20

100

85

70

55

40
100

85

70

55

40
100

85
Full-scale IQ

70

55

40
100

85

70

55

40
100

85

70

55

40
100

85

70

55

40

5 10 15 20 5 10 15 20 5 10 15 20
Age (years)

Figure 1: Illustration of the individual longitudinal profiles of all 36 individuals. K-ABC, German version of the Kaufman-Assessment Battery for Children;
HAWIK-R or HAWIK-III, German version of the Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children; HAWIVA, German version of the Wechsler Preschool and Pri-
mary Scale of Intelligence; SON-R or SON-Y, Snijders-Oomen Nonverbal Intelligence test.

466 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2015, 57: 463–469


Table II: Parameter estimates with 95% confidence intervals

Coefficient Description Estimate Standard error p 95% Bootstrap CI

a1 SON-Y test mean 73.77 2.58 – 69.88; 77.91


a2 SON-R test mean 69.48 3.37 – 62.22; 75.84
a3 HAWIVA test mean 67.48 2.53 – 62.34; 70.54
a4 K-ABC test mean 67.81 2.93 – 62.03; 73.08
a5 HAWIK-R test mean 63.35 3.47 – 57.38; 69.45
a6 HAWIK-III test mean 72.73 3.94 – 66.09; 78.94
b1 Cohort effect 0.19 0.52 0.721 0.85; 1.21
b2 Practice effect (IQ gain if two consecutive assessments 5.26 1.61 0.001 2.80; 7.66
separated by 1y use the same test)
b3 FSIQ difference for females (compared to males) 2.31 4.11 0.577 11.95; 9.43
b4 FSIQ change associated with an increase of 1 pt SES 0.07 0.09 0.445 0.16; 0.29
b5 FSIQ difference per year of follow-up 0.38 0.39 0.325 1.18; 0.40
q Intraclass correlation 0.58 – – 0.46; 0.74

Bootstrap-t using 2000 replicates (resampling of individuals) were used to construct 95% CI. A variance stabilizing transformation was used
to compute the CI of the intraclass correlation. Note that a 95% CI that does not include zero indicates a statistically significant result
(p<0.050). FSIQ, Full-scale IQ; K-ABC, German version of the Kaufman-Assessment Battery for Children; HAWIK-R or HAWIK-III, German
version of the Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children WISC; HAWIVA, German version of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of
Intelligence; SON-R or SON-Y, Snijders-Oomen Nonverbal Intelligence test.

down the average FSIQ score at a given age thus generat- explained by the increasing weaknesses in abstract reason-
ing biased estimates. We note that neither year of birth, ing and higher symbolic language skills, which are part of
sex nor SES at study entry were found to have a statisti- the cognitive testing procedures of these individuals.4
cally significant impact on FSIQ scores, which is in line Regarding individual stability, the estimated intraclass
with earlier studies (e.g. SES was not associated with any correlation coefficient indicates that the average correlation
IQ stability measures in children with mild intellectual dis- between two FSIQ measurements made on the same indi-
ability20). On the other hand, the data did support the exis- vidual is 0.58 (CI 0.46–0.74). Thus, the correlations from
tence of a practice effect. test to test are rather moderate. In fact, the figure illustrates
In terms of population stability, we observed that after individual profiles, which reflect ‘bouncing and rebouncing
adjusting for tests, practice and cohort effects, SES at study trends’ rather than steady patterns of improvement or
entry, and sex, the FSIQ of the children tended to decrease decline (as expressed by Moffitt et al.21). We have recently
over time, although this result was not statistically signifi- investigated the intraindividual stability of neuromotor and
cant (0.38 points of IQ per year, p=0.325). In contrast, if IQ scores in a sample of 256 healthy children of the Zurich
one would select a subset of healthy children scoring below Longitudinal Studies and reported a correlation between
85 at their first visit, one would expect their average FSIQ two FSIQ measurements in these healthy individuals of
to improve over the next few visits. Some children with 0.72 (CI 0.68–0.77) for measurements taken 4 years apart.22
average or above-average individual performances may This finding may suggest that long-term individual stability
score below 85 at some point only because of random of FSIQ in children with borderline to mild intellectual dis-
measurement errors. At the next visits these children would ability is lower than that of children with typical develop-
likely score higher thus inducing a general improvement in ment. In fact, cooperation, motivation, fatigue, and
population IQ performances (regression to the mean). The attention may play a more important role in the clinical
magnitude of that regression to the mean depends on the population of children with intellectual disability during the
proportion of variance related to measurement errors; a test situation than in typically developing participants. Of
larger measurement error variance would induce a larger note, our estimated intraclass correlation is lower than the
potential improvement in the population mean. Although average correlation of 0.82 found in the meta-analysis
the confidence interval for b5 (Table II) cannot rule out a reported by Whitaker.10 This may be because the follow-
positive trend in the evolution of population IQ score over up was longer in our study with more changes of tests along
time, the fact that the regression to the mean was not the years. However, recall that even a correlation of 0.8 as
observed on our sample suggests that, on average, cogni- found by Whitaker10 cannot be considered as high when
tive performance of clinically referred children with bor- the focus lies in predicting future IQ levels of a child. In
derline to mild intellectual disability should not be fact, with such a correlation a 95% CI for the difference
expected to improve over time, but rather remain relatively between two IQ measurements performed on the same
pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
stable. This high population stability in non-syndromic child would lie within 1:96  2  ð1  qÞ ¼ 1:24 standard
intellectual disability stands in contrast to the cognitive deviations. This means that despite such a ‘high’ correlation
decline of children with specific syndromes such as Fragile one may still observe a difference of up to 1.24915=18.6
X, Down syndrome, or Williams syndrome3–7 where a pro- IQ points between two repeated measurements (Whitaker
gressive IQ decline has been consistently reported. The IQ actually found that 14% of children had scores that chan-
drop in these clinical populations across age may be ged by more than 10 points10).

Cognition in Children with Intellectual Disability Oskar G Jenni et al. 467


Several limitations must be kept in mind when interpret- delay does not regularly occur (at least for the population
ing the results of this study. While it might appear desir- as a whole). We stress, however, that IQ stability of individ-
able to adjust the FSIQ of a child for his/her age at entry, uals from test to test is only moderate. Therefore, we may
an inherent difficulty pertaining to the study of longitudinal reach the same conclusions as Whitaker10 and note that
IQ measurements is that IQ tests change with age. In fact, although some children will get similar scores, others will
in clinical practice different test instruments are often used show substantial changes between successive assessments
during different times and by different examiners which (see Fig. 1). The relatively large changes may be explained
reflects the true clinical situation. Thus, systematic differ- in part by the degree of the child’s cooperation, motivation,
ences between score means of specific tests may be attrib- attention, and practice effects as well as by the discontinuity
uted to test effects. Furthermore, the FSIQ scores of the of test items and different examiners, rather than because of
children in our study may be influenced both by the test true change of IQ.
used during the assessment and the age at which the test We believe that, despite the high population stability of
was performed. These partial effects, however, are difficult children with non-syndromic intellectual disability, deci-
to disentangle because of changes in the testing procedures. sions about diagnostic, therapeutic, and educational proce-
In other words, the effects of age and tests remain con- dures as well as the later prediction of development should
founded. Therefore, one should not over interpret the coef- not be solely made on the basis of individual IQ profiles,
ficients a1–a6 presented in Table II. Individual stability is but must also include careful clinical judgment of the
also affected by changing tests and although this cannot be severity of impairments in adaptive behaviors of the child
avoided in the context of IQ measurements, changing tests and the previous developmental history. Thus, our findings
generate some instability in the score, which in turns lowers are in line with the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Sta-
the estimated intraclass correlation. We note that the intra- tistical Manual of Mental Disorders, where individual IQ
class correlation is a measure of average individual stability test scores are given less weight for the diagnosis of intel-
over the period for which measurements are available. The lectual disability, but also to focus on individual’s adaptive
correlation between any two IQ measurements of an indi- functioning.23
vidual is fixed in our model and does not depend on age or
on the time lag separating these two measurements (no ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
serial correlation). These simplifications resulted from the This study was supported by the Swiss National Science
limited sample size and the fact that only few individuals Foundation, grant no 3200B0-112324 and the Anna Muller
had more than four or five data points in their individual Grocholski Foundation.
profiles, preventing the fit of more complex models.
Several conclusions may be drawn from this study based DISCLOSURE
on a clinical sample of children with mild to borderline The authors have stated that they had no interests that
intellectual disability of unknown origin. The FSIQ of chil- might be perceived as posing a conflict of interest.
dren with borderline to mild non-syndromic intellectual
disability remains remarkably stable over time (high popu- SUPPORTING INFORMATION
lation stability) suggesting that intellectual disability is not The following additional material may be found online:
a transitory phenomenon and catch up of the cognitive Appendix S1: Supporting statistical information.

REFERENCES
1. Conley JJ. The hierarchy of consistency: a review and 6. Melyn MA, White DT. Mental and developmental mile- study of children with developmental delays and their
model of longitudinal findings on adult individual differ- stones of noninstitutionalized Downs-syndrome chil- families. Am J Ment Retard 1998; 103: 117–29.
ences in intelligence, personality and self-opinion. Pers dren. Pediatrics 1973; 52: 542–5. 12. Snijders JT, Snijders-Oomen N. Snijders-Oomen nicht-
Individ Dif 1984; 5: 11–25. 7. Udwin O, Davies M, Howlin P. A longitudinal study verbale Intelligenztestreihe S.O.N. 2.5-7. Groningen:
2. Franic S, Dolan CV, van Beijsterveldt CE, Hulshoff Pol of cognitive abilities and educational attainment in Wolters-Noordhoff, 1997.
HE, Bartels M, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmen- Williams syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol 1996; 38: 13. Snijders JT, Tellegen PJ, Winkel M, Laros J, Wijnberg-
tal stability of intelligence in childhood and adolescence. 1020–9. Williams B. SON-R 2½ – 7 Snijders-Oomen Non-verb-
Twin Res Hum Genet 2014; 17: 151–63. 8. Naglieri JA, Pfeiffer SI. Reliability and stability of the aler Intelligenztest – Revidierte Version. Frankfurt:
3. Carr J. Six weeks to 45 years: a longitudinal study of a WISC-R for children with below average IQs. Educ Psy- Swets & Zeitlinger, 1997.
population with Down syndrome. J Appl Res Intellect chol Res 1983; 3: 203–8. 14. Eggert D, Schuck KD. HAWIVA: Der Hannover-
Disabil 2012; 25: 414–22. 9. Friedman R. Reliability of the Wechsler Intelligence Wechsler-Intelligenztest f€
ur das Vorschulalter. Bern:
4. Hagerman RJ, Schreiner RA, Kemper MB, et al. Longi- Scale for children in a group of mentally retarded chil- Hans Huber Verlag, 1975.
tudinal IQ changes in fragile X males. Am J Med Genet dren. J Clin Psychol 1970; 26: 181–2. 15. Tewes U. HAWIK-R: Der Hamburg-Wechsler-
1989; 33: 513–8. 10. Whitaker S. The stability of IQ in people with low Intelligenztest f€
ur Kinder. Bern: Hans Huber Verlag,
5. Hall SS, Burns DD, Lightbody AA, Reiss AL. Longitu- intellectual ability: an analysis of the literature. Intellect 1983.
dinal changes in intellectual development in children Dev Disab 2008; 46: 120–8. 16. Tewes U, Schallberger P, Rossmann U. Hamburg-
with Fragile X syndrome. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2008; 11. Clare L, Garnier H, Gallimore R. Parents’ developmen- Wechsler-Intelligenztest f€
ur Kinder III (HAWIK-III).
36: 927–39. tal expectations and child characteristics: longitudinal Bern: Hans Huber Verlag, 1999.

468 Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2015, 57: 463–469


17. Melchers P, Preuss U. K-ABC. Amesterdam: 20. Bernheimer LP, Keogh BK, Guthrie D. Young children 22. Jenni OG, Chaouch A, Locatelli I, et al. Intra-individual
Swets&Zeitlinger, 1991. with developmental delays as young adults: predicting stability of neuromotor tasks from 6 to 18 years: a longi-
18. Ganzeboom HBG, De Graaf PM, Treiman DJ. A stan- developmental and personal-social outcomes. Am J Ment tudinal study. Hum Mov Sci 2011; 30: 1272–82.
dard international socio-economic index of occupational Retard 2006; 111: 263–72. 23. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statis-
status. Soc Sci Res 1992; 21: 1–56. 21. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Harkness AR, Silva PA. The natu- tical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). Arlington,
19. Davison AC, Hinkley DV. Bootstrap Methods and ral history of change in intellectual performance: who VA: American Psychiatric Association, 2013.
Their Application. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univer- changes? How much? Is it meaningful? J Child Psychol
sity Press, 1997. Psychiatry 1993; 34: 455–506.

Book Review: Central Nervous System Infections in Seventeen disease-/syndrome-specific chapters by a range
Childhood of experts form the bulk of the work, each providing a con-
Edited by Pratibha Singhi, Diane E Griffin and Charles R Newton cise, up-to-date summary of one or more important CNS
International Review of Child Neurology Series infections, with in each case appropriate attention paid to
London: Mac Keith Press, 2014
£95.00 (Hardback) pp 378
practical matters of diagnosis and specific management.
ISBN: 978-1-909962-44-6 Notable among many excellent, well-referenced contribu-
tions are Whitley’s presentation of a lifetime of experience
in his chapter on herpes simplex CNS infection; Kumar and
The burden of central nervous system (CNS) infections Singh’s, and Kanta’s chapters on Japanese B encephalitis and
in children – and particularly in children in resource poor rabies; the Singhis’ expositions of their wealth of experience
settings – is staggeringly high. Doctors caring directly for of the management of bacterial and fungal brain infections,
acutely ill children, and neurologists from whom special- neurocysticercosis and the life-threatening bacterial exo-
ized opinions are sought, need a practical resource to toxin-mediated syndromes; Schoeman and van Toorn’s
keep abreast of developments in this fast-moving field. authoritative summary of CNS pathology caused by tuber-
This was attested to in a poll of members of the Interna- culosis, with their starkly juxtaposed reminders that while
tional Child Neurology Association that identified the early diagnosis and treatment is usually completely curative,
topic of children’s CNS infections as high on their wish in most settings (resource poor and rich alike) diagnosis is
list for an up-to-date textbook. In Central Nervous System long delayed and disability or death are common outcomes;
Infections in Childhood, the team of Singhi, Griffin, and and Sladky and Willison’s comprehensive discussion of
Newton have, in my opinion, produced a valuable, acute disseminated encephalomyelitis, Guillain-Barre syn-
scholarly resource for paediatricians and paediatric neurol- drome, and other post-infectious neurological disorders.
ogists. All three editors are eminent in the field compris- The editors’ intention is that an electronic version of the
ing that small zone of trisection in the Venn diagram book, available online, will be periodically updated, when
that represents the bodies of knowledge making up paedi- no doubt – for example – the dramatic impact of Meningo-
atrics, infectious diseases, and neurology. As well as coccal A conjugate vaccine (MenAfriVac), developed for
bringing a wealth of practical clinical experience and use in sub-Saharan Africa’s meningitis belt, can be pre-
complementary research to their project, they have sented in some detail. In this way too perhaps one unac-
recruited an ‘A list’ of international contributors, each countable omission can soon be addressed: perhaps
with personal experience and expertise in the areas of representing the triumph of hope for the eradication of
their contributions, to produce a remarkably well-bal- measles by vaccination over the general experience of fal-
anced multi-author textbook. tering progress, there is at present no chapter on this terri-
Scene-setting chapters provide a background in epidemi- bly important viral infection of the brain in this otherwise
ology, bacterial and viral pathogenesis, general principles of comprehensive book.
the management of CNS infections and their acute compli- At close to half the length of Bell and McCormick’s
cations, and neuroimaging, with a further useful contribu- Neurologic Infections in Children (2nd edition, 1981; digi-
tion by Neville on the important topic of febrile seizures. tized, 2008), Central Nervous Systems Infections in Childhood
Khandelwal and Rumboldt’s chapter on neuroimaging, with nevertheless provides a valuable, well-referenced textbook
its emphasis on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), is per- for paediatricians and neurologists everywhere, and, with
haps inevitably presented in highly technical language. It regular updates, has every chance of becoming an endur-
reminds the reader of the rapid, transformative impact on ing, standard text.
neurology of computed tomography and MRI, and fore-
shadows exciting technological developments to be antici- J Simon Kroll
pated with, for example, diagnostic magnetic resonance Department of Medicine, Imperial College London, London, UK.
spectroscopy. The chapter represents a valuable primer text
to assist discussion with neuroradiologist colleagues. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12586

Cognition in Children with Intellectual Disability Oskar G Jenni et al. 469

You might also like