You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/13946572

Creating False Memories

Article  in  Scientific American · October 1997


DOI: 10.1038/scientificamerican0997-70 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

319 17,703

1 author:

Elizabeth F Loftus
University of California, Irvine
401 PUBLICATIONS   30,204 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Drug effects on false memories in a legal context View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Elizabeth F Loftus on 21 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Creating False Memories
BRYAN CHRISTIE; PHOTOGRAPH COURTESY OF JOSEFA JAKLITSCH

Copyright 1997 Scientific American, Inc.


Researchers are showing how suggestion and imagination
can create “memories” of events that did not actually occur

by Elizabeth F. Loftus

I n 1986 Nadean Cool, a nurse’s aide


in Wisconsin, sought therapy from
a psychiatrist to help her cope with
her reaction to a traumatic event expe-
rienced by her daughter. During thera-
had to resign from his post as a clergy-
man when the allegations were made
public. Later medical examination of
the daughter revealed, however, that she
was still a virgin at age 22 and had never
a stop sign. After the viewing, half the
participants received a suggestion that
the traffic sign was a yield sign. When
asked later what traffic sign they re-
membered seeing at the intersection,
py, the psychiatrist used hypnosis and been pregnant. The daughter sued the those who had been given the sugges-
other suggestive techniques to dig out therapist and received a $1-million set- tion tended to claim that they had seen
buried memories of abuse that Cool tlement in 1996. a yield sign. Those who had not received
herself had allegedly experienced. In the About a year earlier two juries re- the phony information were much more
process, Cool became convinced that turned verdicts against a Minnesota accurate in their recollection of the
she had repressed memories of having psychiatrist accused of planting false traffic sign.
been in a satanic cult, of eating babies, memories by former patients Vynnette My students and I have now conduct-
of being raped, of having sex with ani- Hamanne and Elizabeth Carlson, who ed more than 200 experiments involv-
mals and of being forced to watch the under hypnosis and sodium amytal, ing over 20,000 individuals that docu-
murder of her eight-year-old friend. She and after being fed misinformation ment how exposure to misinformation
came to believe that she had more than about the workings of memory, had induces memory distortion. In these
120 personalities—children, adults, an- come to remember horrific abuse by studies, people “recalled” a conspicuous
gels and even a duck—all because, Cool family members. The juries awarded barn in a bucolic scene that contained
was told, she had experienced severe Hammane $2.67 million and Carlson no buildings at all, broken glass and tape
childhood sexual and physical abuse. $2.5 million for their ordeals. recorders that were not in the scenes
The psychiatrist also performed exor- In all four cases, the women devel- they viewed, a white instead of a blue
cisms on her, one of which lasted for oped memories about childhood abuse vehicle in a crime scene, and Minnie
five hours and included the sprinkling in therapy and then later denied their Mouse when they actually saw Mickey
of holy water and screams for Satan to authenticity. How can we determine if Mouse. Taken together, these studies
leave Cool’s body. memories of childhood abuse are true or show that misinformation can change
When Cool finally realized that false false? Without corroboration, it is very an individual’s recollection in predictable
memories had been planted, she sued the difficult to differentiate between false and sometimes very powerful ways.
psychiatrist for malpractice. In March memories and true ones. Also, in these Misinformation has the potential for
1997, after five weeks of trial, her case cases, some memories were contrary to invading our memories when we talk to
was settled out of court for $2.4 million. physical evidence, such as explicit and other people, when we are suggestively
Nadean Cool is not the only patient detailed recollections of rape and abor- interrogated or when we read or view
to develop false memories as a result of tion when medical examination con- media coverage about some event that
questionable therapy. In Missouri in firmed virginity. How is it possible for we may have experienced ourselves. Af-
1992 a church counselor helped Beth people to acquire elaborate and confi- ter more than two decades of exploring
Rutherford to remember during dent false memories? A growing number the power of misinformation, research-
therapy that her father, a clergy- of investigations demonstrate that under ers have learned a great deal about the
man, had regularly raped her be- the right circumstances false memories conditions that make people susceptible
tween the ages of seven and 14 can be instilled rather easily in some to memory modification. Memories are
and that her mother some- people. more easily modified, for instance,
times helped him by holding My own research into memory dis- when the passage of time allows the
her down. Under her ther- tortion goes back to the early 1970s, original memory to fade.
apist’s guidance, Ruther- when I began studies of the “misinfor-
ford developed memo- mation effect.” These studies show that False Childhood Memories
ries of her father twice when people who witness an event are
impregnating her and
forcing her to abort
the fetus herself
later exposed to new and misleading in-
formation about it, their recollections
often become distorted. In one example,
I t is one thing to change a detail or
two in an otherwise intact memory
but quite another to plant a false mem-
with a coat hang- participants viewed a simulated auto- ory of an event that never happened. To
er. The father mobile accident at an intersection with study false memory, my students and I

FALSE MEMORIES are often created by combining actual memories with suggestions received from
others. The memory of a happy childhood outing to the beach with father and grandfather, for in-
stance, can be distorted by a suggestion, perhaps from a relative, into a memory of being afraid or lost.
False memories also can be induced when a person is encouraged to imagine experiencing specific
events without worrying about whether they really happened or not.

Creating False Memories Copyright 1997 Scientific American, Inc. Scientific American September 1997 71
first had to find a way to plant a pseudo- let, the participants wrote what RECALL AFTER

BRYAN CHRISTIE
memory that would not cause our sub- they remembered about the BOOKLET
jects undue emotional stress, either in event. If they did not remem-
the process of creating the false memory ber it, they were instructed to
or when we revealed that they had been write, “I do not remember RECALL IN
intentionally deceived. Yet we wanted this.” In two follow-up inter- INTERVIEW 1
to try to plant a memory that would be views, we told the participants
at least mildly traumatic, had the expe- that we were interested in ex-
rience actually happened. amining how much detail they RECALL IN
My research associate, Jacqueline E. could remember and how INTERVIEW 2
Pickrell, and I settled on trying to plant a their memories compared with
specific memory of being lost in a shop- those of their relative. The 0 20 40 60 80 100
ping mall or large department store at event paragraphs were not
PERCENT OF SUBJECTS
about the age of five. Here’s how we did read to them verbatim, but
it. We asked our subjects, 24 individuals rather parts were provided as TRUE EVENTS FALSE EVENT
ranging in age from 18 to 53, to try to retrieval cues. The participants REMEMBERED “REMEMBERED”
remember childhood events that had recalled something about 49
been recounted to us by a parent, an old- of the 72 true events (68 per-
er sibling or another close relative. We cent) immediately after the initial read- were to observe many of our partici-
prepared a booklet for each participant ing of the booklet and also in each of pants describe an event, it would be
containing one-paragraph stories about the two follow-up interviews. After difficult indeed to tell whether the ac-
three events that had actually happened reading the booklet, seven of the 24 par- count was of a true or a false memory.
to him or her and one that had not. We ticipants (29 percent) remembered ei- Of course, being lost, however fright-
constructed the false event using infor- ther partially or fully the false event con- ening, is not the same as being abused.
mation about a plausible shopping trip structed for them, and in the two fol- But the lost-in-the-mall study is not
provided by a relative, who also veri- low-up interviews six participants (25 about real experiences of being lost; it is
fied that the participant had not in fact percent) continued to claim that they about planting false memories of being
been lost at about the age of five. The remembered the fictitious event. Statis- lost. The paradigm shows a way of in-
lost-in-the-mall scenario included the tically, there were some differences be- stilling false memories and takes a step
following elements: lost for an extend- tween the true memories and the false toward allowing us to understand how
ed period, crying, aid and comfort by ones: participants used more words to this might happen in real-world settings.
an elderly woman and, finally, reunion describe the true memories, and they Moreover, the study provides evidence
with the family. rated the true memories as being some- that people can be led to remember their
After reading each story in the book- what more clear. But if an onlooker past in different ways, and they can

INTERVIEW 1
BRYAN CHRISTIE

INTERVIEW 2

INTERVIEW 3

0 20 40 60 80 100
PERCENT OF SUBJECTS

TRUE EVENTS FALSE EVENT


REMEMBERED “REMEMBERED

RECALL OF PLANTED CHILDHOOD EVENTS


in this study appears to increase slightly after the
details become familiar to the subject and the
source of the information is forgotten. Ira Hyman
and his colleagues at Western Washington Univer-
sity presented subjects with true events provided
by relatives along with a false event—such as spill-
ing a punch bowl on the parents of the bride at a
wedding. None of the participants remembered
the false event when first told about it, but in two
follow-up interviews, initially 18 percent and later
25 percent of the subjects said they remembered
something about the incident.

72 Scientific American September 1997 Copyright 1997 Scientific American, Inc. Creating False Memories
FALSE MEMORY TOOK ROOT in roughly 25 percent of events in the first interview running around and knocked some-
the subjects in this study by the author and her co-workers. and 88 percent in the sec- thing over like the punch bowl or
The study was designed to create a false recollection of being ond interview. None of something and made a big mess and of
lost at age five on a shopping trip. A booklet prepared for each the participants recalled course got yelled at for it.”
participant included the false event and three events that he the false event during the
or she had actually experienced. After reading the scenarios,
first interview, but 20 per- Imagination Inflation
29 percent of the subjects “recalled” something about being
lost in the mall. Follow-up interviews showed there was little cent said they remembered
variation over time in recalling both the false and true events. something about the false
event in the second inter-
view. One participant who
T he finding that an external sugges-
tion can lead to the construction of
false childhood memories helps us un-
even be coaxed into “remembering” en- had been exposed to the emergency derstand the process by which false
tire events that never happened. hospitalization story later remembered memories arise. It is natural to wonder
Studies in other laboratories using a a male doctor, a female nurse and a whether this research is applicable in
similar experimental procedure have friend from church who came to visit at real situations such as being interrogat-
produced similar results. For instance, the hospital. ed by law officers or in psychotherapy.
Ira Hyman, Troy H. Husband and F. In another study, along with true Although strong suggestion may not
James Billing of Western Washington events Hyman presented different false routinely occur in police questioning or
University asked college students to re- events, such as accidentally spilling a therapy, suggestion in the form of an
call childhood experiences that had been bowl of punch on the parents of the imagination exercise sometimes does.
recounted by their parents. The research- bride at a wedding reception or having For instance, when trying to obtain a
ers told the students that the study was to evacuate a grocery store when the confession, law officers may ask a sus-
about how people remember shared ex- overhead sprinkler systems erroneously pect to imagine having participated in a
periences differently. In addition to ac- activated. Again, none of the partici- criminal act. Some mental health pro-
tual events reported by parents, each pants recalled the false event during the fessionals encourage patients to imag-
participant was given one false event— first interview, but 18 percent remem- ine childhood events as a way of recov-
either an overnight hospitalization for a bered something about it in the second ering supposedly hidden memories.
high fever and a possible ear infection, interview and 25 percent in the third in- Surveys of clinical psychologists reveal
or a birthday party with pizza and a terview. For example, during the first that 11 percent instruct their clients to
clown—that supposedly happened at interview, one participant, when asked “let the imagination run wild,” and 22
about the age of five. The parents con- about the fictitious wedding event, stat- percent tell their clients to “give free rein
firmed that neither of these events actu- ed, “I have no clue. I have never heard to the imagination.” Therapist Wendy

JASON GOLTZ
ally took place. that one before.” In the second inter- Maltz, author of a popular book on
Hyman found that students fully or view, the participant said, “It was an childhood sexual abuse, advocates
partially recalled 84 percent of the true outdoor wedding, and I think we were telling the patient: “Spend time imagin-

Creating False Memories Copyright 1997 Scientific American, Inc. Scientific American September 1997 73
ing that you were sexually abused, with- 40 childhood events, indicating how roll your eyes. During the second ses-
out worrying about accuracy, proving likely it was that these events actually sion, the participants were asked to
anything, or having your ideas make happened to them. imagine some of the actions that they
sense.... Ask yourself...these questions: Consider one of the imagination ex- had not previously performed. During
What time of day is it? Where are you? ercises. Participants are told to imagine the final session, they answered ques-
Indoors or outdoors? What kind of playing inside at home after school, tions about what actions they actually
things are happening? Is there one or hearing a strange noise outside, running performed during the initial session.
more person with you?” Maltz further toward the window, tripping, falling, The investigators found that the more
recommends that therapists continue to reaching out and breaking the window times participants imagined an unper-
ask questions such as “Who would have with their hand. In addition, we asked formed action, the more likely they
been likely perpetrators? When were participants questions such as “What were to remember having performed it.
you most vulnerable to sexual abuse in did you trip on? How did you feel?”
your life?” In one study 24 percent of the partic- Impossible Memories
The increasing use of such imagina- ipants who imagined the broken-win-
tion exercises led me and several col-
leagues to wonder about their conse-
quences. What happens when people
dow scenario later reported an increase
in confidence that the event had oc-
curred, whereas only 12 percent of those
I t is highly unlikely that an adult can
recall genuine episodic memories from
the first year of life, in part because the
imagine childhood experiences that did who were not asked to imagine the in- hippocampus, which plays a key role in
not happen to them? Does imagining a cident reported an increase in the likeli- the creation of memories, has not ma-
childhood event increase confidence hood that it had taken place. We found tured enough to form and store long-
that it occurred? To explore this, we de- this “imagination inflation” effect in lasting memories that can be retrieved
signed a three-stage procedure. We first each of the eight events that participants in adulthood. A procedure for planting
asked individuals to indicate the likeli- were asked to imagine. A number of “impossible” memories about experi-
hood that certain events happened to possible explanations come to mind. An ences that occur shortly after birth has
them during their childhood. The list obvious one is that an act of imagination been developed by the late Nicholas
contains 40 events, each rated on a scale simply makes the event seem more fa- Spanos and his collaborators at Carle-
ranging from “definitely did not hap- miliar and that familiarity is mistakenly ton University. Individuals are led to be-
pen” to “definitely did happen.” Two related to childhood memories rather lieve that they have well-coordinated eye
weeks later we asked the participants to than to the act of imagination. Such movements and visual exploration skills
imagine that they had experienced some source confusion—when a person does probably because they were born in hos-
of these events. Different subjects were not remember the source of informa- pitals that hung swinging, colored mo-
asked to imagine different events. Some- tion—can be especially acute for the dis- biles over infant cribs. To confirm wheth-
time later the participants again were tant experiences of childhood. er they had such an experience, half the
asked to respond to the original list of Studies by Lyn Goff and Henry L. participants are hypnotized, age-re-
Roediger III of Wash- gressed to the day after birth and asked
ington University of re- what they remembered. The other half
IMAGINATION INFLATION cent rather than child- of the group participates in a “guided
FOUND $10 hood experiences more mnemonic restructuring” procedure that
directly connect imag- uses age regression as well as active en-
ined actions to the con- couragement to re-create the infant ex-
BAD HAIRCUT struction of false memo- periences by imagining them.
ry. During the initial ses- Spanos and his co-workers found
sion, the researchers that the vast majority of their subjects
STUCK instructed participants were susceptible to these memory-plant-
IN TREE to perform the stated ing procedures. Both the hypnotic and
action, imagine doing it guided participants reported infant mem-
WON A PET or just listen to the ories. Surprisingly, the guided group did
statement and do noth- so somewhat more (95 versus 70 per-
ing else. The actions cent). Both groups remembered the col-
SAVED were simple ones: knock ored mobile at a relatively high rate (56
BY LIFEGUARD on the table, lift the sta- percent of the guided group and 46 per-
pler, break the tooth- cent of the hypnotic subjects). Many
EMERGENCY pick, cross your fingers, participants who did not remember the
ROOM VISIT
SUBJECTS WHO DID
NOT IMAGINE EVENT
BROKE IMAGINING AN EVENT can increase a person’s belief that the ficti-
WINDOW tious event actually happened. To study the “imagination inflation” ef-
SUBJECTS WHO
fect, the author and her colleagues asked participants to indicate on a
IMAGINED EVENT scale the likelihood that each of 40 events occurred during their child-
CALLED 911 hood. Two weeks later they were given guidance in imagining some of
the events they said had not taken place and then were asked to rate the
BRYAN CHRISTIE

0 20 40 60 original 40 events again. Whereas all participants showed increased


PERCENT OF SUBJECTS WITH INCREASING confidence that the events had occurred, those who took part in active-
CONFIDENCE THAT EVENT OCCURRED ly imagining the events reported an even greater increase.

74 Scientific American September 1997 Copyright 1997 Scientific American, Inc. Creating False Memories
MEMORIES OF INFANCY—such as a mobile hanging over a crib—can be induced
even though it is highly unlikely that events from the first year of life can be recalled. In
a study by the late Nicholas Spanos and his colleagues at Carleton University, “impos-
sible” memories of the first day of life were planted using either hypnosis or a guided
mnemonic restructuring procedure. The mobile was “remembered” by 46 percent of
the hypnotized group and by 56 percent of the guided group.

mobile did recall other things, such as incriminating evidence can induce peo-
doctors, nurses, bright lights, cribs and ple to accept guilt for a crime they did
masks. Also, in both groups, of those not commit and even to develop memo-
who reported memories of infancy, 49 ries to support their guilty feelings.
percent felt that they were real memories, Research is beginning to give us an
as opposed to 16 percent who claimed understanding of how false memories of

CREWS/THE IMAGE WORKS


that they were merely fantasies. These complete, emotional and self-participa-
findings confirm earlier studies that tory experiences are created in adults.
many individuals can be led to construct First, there are social demands on indi-
complex, vivid and detailed false mem- viduals to remember; for instance, re-
ories via a rather simple procedure. searchers exert some pressure on partic-
Hypnosis clearly is not necessary. ipants in a study to come up with mem-
ories. Second, memory construction by the validity of long-buried memories,
How False Memories Form imagining events can be explicitly en- such as repeated trauma, it in no way
couraged when people are having trou- disproves them. Without corroboration,

I n the lost-in-the-mall study, implanta-


tion of false memory occurred when
another person, usually a family mem-
ble remembering. And, finally, individu-
als can be encouraged not to think
about whether their constructions are
there is little that can be done to help
even the most experienced evaluator to
differentiate true memories from ones
ber, claimed that the incident happened. real or not. Creation of false memories that were suggestively planted.
Corroboration of an event by another is most likely to occur when these ex- The precise mechanisms by which such
person can be a powerful technique for ternal factors are present, whether in an false memories are constructed await
instilling a false memory. In fact, merely experimental setting, in a therapeutic further research. We still have much to
claiming to have seen a person do some- setting or during everyday activities. learn about the degree of confidence
thing can lead that person to make a False memories are constructed by and the characteristics of false memo-
false confession of wrongdoing. combining actual memories with the ries created in these ways, and we need
This effect was demonstrated in a content of suggestions received from to discover what types of individuals are
study by Saul M. Kassin and his col- others. During the process, individuals particularly susceptible to these forms
leagues at Williams College, who inves- may forget the source of the informa- of suggestion and who is resistant.
tigated the reactions of individuals false- tion. This is a classic example of source As we continue this work, it is impor-
ly accused of damaging a computer by confusion, in which the content and the tant to heed the cautionary tale in the
pressing the wrong key. The innocent source become dissociated. data we have already obtained: mental
participants initially denied the charge, Of course, because we can implant health professionals and others must be
but when a confederate said that she had false childhood memories in some indi- aware of how greatly they can influence
seen them perform the action, many par- viduals in no way implies that all mem- the recollection of events and of the ur-
ticipants signed a confession, internal- ories that arise after suggestion are nec- gent need for maintaining restraint in
ized guilt for the act and went on to con- essarily false. Put another way, although situations in which imagination is used
fabulate details that were consistent with experimental work on the creation of as an aid in recovering presumably lost
that belief. These findings show that false false memories may raise doubt about memories. SA

The Author Further Reading


ELIZABETH F. LOFTUS is professor of psychology and ad- The Myth of Repressed Memory. Elizabeth F. Loftus and Katherine
junct professor of law at the University of Washington. She re- Ketcham. St. Martin’s Press, 1994.
ceived her Ph.D. in psychology from Stanford University in The Social Psychology of False Confessions: Compliance, Inter-
1970. Her research has focused on human memory, eyewit- nalization, and Confabulation. Saul M. Kassin and Katherine L. Kie-
ness testimony and courtroom procedure. Loftus has pub- chel in Psychological Science, Vol. 7, No. 3, pages 125–128; May 1996.
lished 18 books and more than 250 scientific articles and has Imagination Inflation: Imagining a Childhood Event Inflates
served as an expert witness or consultant in hundreds of trials, Confidence That It Occurred. Maryanne Garry, Charles G. Man-
including the McMartin preschool molestation case. Her book ning, Elizabeth F. Loftus and Steven J. Sherman in Psychonomic Bulletin
Eyewitness Testimony won a National Media Award from the and Review, Vol. 3, No. 2, pages 208–214; June 1996.
American Psychological Foundation. She has received hon- Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory.
orary doctorates from Miami University, Leiden University Edited by David C. Rubin. Cambridge University Press, 1996.
and John Jay College of Criminal Justice. Loftus was recently Searching for Memory: The Brain, the Mind, and the Past. Daniel
elected president of the American Psychological Society. L. Schacter. BasicBooks, 1996.

Creating False Memories Copyright 1997 Scientific American, Inc. Scientific American September 1997 75

View publication stats

You might also like