You are on page 1of 11

Intellectual and Academic Functioning

of School-Age Children With


Single-Suture Craniosynostosis
Matthew L. Speltz, PhDa,b, Brent R. Collett, PhDa,b, Erin R. Wallace, PhDb, Jacqueline R. Starr, PhDc,d,
Mary Michaeleen Cradock, PhDe,f, Lauren Buono, PhDg, Michael Cunningham, MD, PhDh,i, Kathleen Kapp-Simon, PhDj,k

OBJECTIVE: We compared the developmental status of school-age children with single-suture abstract
craniosynostosis (case group) and unaffected children (control group). Within the case group we
compared the performance of children distinguished by location of suture fusion (sagittal, metopic,
unicoronal, lambdoid).
METHODS: We administered standardized tests of intelligence, reading, spelling, and math to 182 case
participants and 183 control participants. This sample represented 70% of those tested during infancy
before case participants had corrective surgery.
RESULTS: After adjustment for demographics, case participants’ average scores were lower than those of control
participants on all measures. The largest observed differences were in Full-Scale IQ and math computation,
where case participants’ adjusted mean scores were 2.5 to 4 points lower than those of control participants
(Ps ranged from .002 to .09). Adjusted mean case–control differences on other measures of achievement were
modest, although case deficits became more pronounced after adjustment for participation in developmental
interventions. Among case participants, 58% had no discernible learning problem (score ,25th percentile on
a standardized achievement test). Children with metopic, unicoronal, and lambdoid synostosis tended to score
lower on most measures than did children with sagittal fusions (Ps ranged from ,.001 to .82).
CONCLUSIONS: The developmental delays observed among infants with single-suture craniosynostosis are
partially evident at school age, as manifested by lower average scores than those of control participants on
measures of IQ and math. However, case participants’ average scores were only slightly lower than those of
control participants on reading and spelling measures, and the frequency of specific learning problems was
comparable. Among case participants, those with unicoronal and lambdoid fusions appear to be the most
neurodevelopmentally vulnerable.

Departments of aPsychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and hPediatrics, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington; WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: It is unclear
b
Center for Child Health, Behavior and Development, Seattle Children’s Research Institute, Seattle, Washington;
c
Department of Clinical and Translational Research, The Forsyth Institute, Cambridge, Massachusetts; dDepartment of whether developmental delays observed among
Oral Health Policy and Epidemiology, Harvard School of Dental Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts; eDepartment of infants with single-suture craniosynostosis (SSC)
Psychology, St Louis Children’s Hospital, St Louis, Missouri; fDepartment of Pediatrics, Washington University School of
Medicine, St Louis, Missouri; gCenter for Craniofacial Disorders, Children’s Healthcare of Atlanta, Atlanta, Georgia;
persist at school age. Few neurodevelopmental
i
Craniofacial Center, Seattle Children’s Hospital, Seattle, Washington; jDepartment of Surgery, Northwestern studies have examined children with SSC beyond
University, Chicago, Illinois; and kShriners Hospital for Children, Chicago, Illinois
age 3, with most having methodological limitations.
Dr Speltz conceptualized and designed the study, obtained National Institutes of Health funding,
WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: This study is the first to
supervised data collection, assisted with data analyses and interpretation, and drafted the initial
manuscript; Drs Collett, Cradock, Cunningham, and Kapp-Simon assisted in study conceptualization and follow and test infants with SSC and a control
design, participated in data collection, and provided a critical review of the manuscript; Dr Wallace group at school age. Infancy delays among
assisted with conceptualization and study design, particularly data analyses and interpretation, assisted children with SSC persisted at school age in some
with manuscript preparation, and provided a critical review of the manuscript; Dr Starr assisted in
conceptualization and study design, particularly data analysis and interpretation, and provided a critical areas (IQ, math) but not others (reading, spelling).
review of the manuscript; Dr Buono participated in data collection and provided a critical review of the
manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2014-1634
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-1634
Accepted for publication Dec 15, 2014

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 3, March 2015 ARTICLE
Single-suture craniosynostosis (SSC) A demographically matched control #30 months of age at recruitment.
is defined as the premature fusion of group was also followed, with Exclusion criteria included
any of the cranial sutures (eg, sagittal, approximately 70% of case and prematurity (,34 weeks’ gestation),
metopic, coronal, or lambdoid) and control infants retained for major medical or neurologic
occurs in ∼1 in 1700 to 2500 live neuropsychological testing at school conditions (eg, cardiac defects,
births.1,2 Corrective surgery to age. We hypothesized that children seizure disorders, significant health
restore the suture is preferentially with SSC would score lower than conditions necessitating surgical
performed in the first year of life. children without craniosynostosis on correction), presence of $3
Neurodevelopmental impairment has standardized measures of IQ and extracranial minor malformations,16
long been suspected among children academic achievement. or presence of other major
with SSC, because the prematurely malformations. For a subsample of
fused suture is believed to constrain3 178 cases whose parents gave
METHODS
or alter4,5 brain structures and consent, we collected biospecimens
elevate intracranial pressure.6,7 Early Study Design and analyzed genetic data by array
neurodevelopmental investigations We used cross-sectional data from comparative genomic hybridization
were beset with methodological a school-age assessment of children and candidate and gene resequencing
problems and inconclusive with SSC (case group) and unaffected (for details see Cunningham et al
findings,8,9 but in more recent and children (control group) who have 201121). Children with SSC who had
better-designed studies, infants and been followed since infancy in a genetic variant (including a known
a longitudinal study.12 In the original or probable causal mutation for
preschoolers with SSC have
study, we enrolled all eligible case craniosynostosis) were eligible if they
consistently scored lower, on average,
participants between January 2002 had no phenotypic features of
than same-aged children with patent
and September 2006 from Seattle a known syndrome and otherwise
sutures.10–13 In a large multisite
Children’s Hospital, the Cleft Lip and met all inclusion criteria.
study of children in the United States
from infancy to 36 months, we Palate Institute and Northwestern We enrolled 270 case participants
observed children with SSC to score University in Chicago, Children’s (84% of those eligible), 4 of whom
on average about 0.25 to 0.50 SD Healthcare of Atlanta, St Louis were later found to be ineligible (for
lower on standardized tests than Children’s Hospital, and Children’s details about ascertainment, see
a demographically matched control Hospital of Philadelphia (starting in Starr et al 201212). The case sample
group.11 Compared with unaffected January 2006). Unaffected control included 76 children with sagittal
children, case participants had nearly participants were recruited by each synostosis, 48 with metopic, 46 with
twice the odds of scoring in the center and frequency matched to case unicoronal, and 12 with lamboid
delayed range. Similar findings have participants at the time of synostosis. Among the 266 case
been reported in a well-designed recruitment. Participants were infants seen at baseline, 182
study of Australian infants who were assessed at a baseline visit that children (68%) had a school-age
followed with similar developmental occurred, for case participants, before assessment.
measures.14 surgery (mean age = 7.4 months) and
at 3 subsequent visits at which the Control Infants
A key question is whether these average ages were 18 months, 36 Infants were eligible as control
delays persist into the school-age months, and 7 years; the last age participants if they had no known
years and manifest as learning point is the focus of this report. The craniofacial anomaly and met none of
disabilities or other problems in need study was approved by institutional the exclusionary criteria for case
of intervention.15 Few review boards at each participating participants. Control group
neurodevelopmental studies have institution and informed consent participants were recruited through
focused on children with SSC beyond obtained from all parents. pediatric practices, birthing centers,
age 3,16–20 and most of these studies and announcements in publications of
have lacked control groups17,19,20 or Case Infants interest to parents of newborns.
used methods other than testing to Infants with SSC were referred at the Control participants were frequency-
estimate developmental time of diagnosis by a treating matched to case participants on
progress.19,20 In the current study we surgeon or pediatrician. Infants were factors related to
addressed these problems by eligible if they had SSC (isolated neurodevelopmental performance
following to early elementary school sagittal, metopic, unilateral coronal, that may also be potential
age a large sample of infants with SSC or unilateral lambdoid synostosis) confounders: age at enrollment
who were recruited and assessed confirmed by CT scans, had not yet (within 3 weeks), gender, family
before corrective surgery. had cranial vault surgery, and were socioeconomic status (SES) within the

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
e616 SPELTZ et al
same Hollingshead category,22 and Procedures status and matched on the 4
race or ethnicity. After psychometrists completed confounders from the primary
We enrolled 76% of all eligible testing, parents were interviewed to analysis, plus race or ethnicity and
control participants (see Starr et al update the family’s demographic study site. We repeated the primary
201212 for details). Among the 259 information and medical history and linear regression analyses after
control participants seen at baseline, to provide information on any excluding 19 children with SSC in
183 (71%) had a school-age interventions received since the last whom known or probable causal
assessment. assessment at age 3 (eg, speech or mutations for craniosynostosis were
language therapy). Parents who so detected through array comparative
desired were mailed a summary of genomic hybridization and candidate
Measures
their child’s test results, which they and gene resequencing.21
Intelligence was measured by the
were encouraged to share with their To explore potential attrition bias
Full-Scale IQ score from the
child’s pediatrician or teacher. over time, we repeated the analyses
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children, Fourth Edition (WISC- of case–control differences by using
Data Analysis inverse probability weighting
IV).23 The Wide Range Achievement
Test, Fourth Edition (WRAT-4) We examined the distribution of (IPW).33 The predicted probability of
measured reading, spelling, and demographic characteristics and IQ school age participation was
math with a composite or subscale and achievement scores separately estimated with baseline
score representing each of these for case and control participants. characteristics that were known for
areas.24 The Test of Word Reading Visual inspection of boxplots and all participants and included date of
Efficiency (TOWRE) measured histograms confirmed that the birth, gender, race or ethnicity,
reading efficiency, that is, the child’s distributions of each outcome score prematurity (,38 weeks’ gestation,
ability to quickly and accurately were approximately normal in case coded yes/no), case status, suture
decode increasingly difficult words and control participants. We also diagnosis, parents’ marital status,
and nonsense words.25 We used the explored factors associated with maternal IQ, study site, and scores
Comprehensive Test of Phonological attrition by comparing the from the Bayley Scales of Infant
Processing (CTOPP) to assess skills distributions of baseline demographic Development 2 (Psychomotor
that support reading decoding, and neurodevelopmental Development Index) and the
speed, and fluency.26–29 Two CTOPP characteristics of participants seen at Preschool Language Scale 3 (auditory
composite scores were used: school age with those of participants comprehension) completed at
phonological awareness (ie, sound who were lost to follow-up. baseline (see Starr et al 201212).
blending, elision) and rapid naming Unless otherwise specified, all of the Observations from participants
(ie, verbal retrieval of symbols). All following analyses were adjusted for assessed at school age are understood
of these tests have demonstrated 4 potential confounders: child age at to represent participants who had
good to excellent reliability and assessment (in months, continuous), similar baseline characteristics, but
validity.23–29 Age-based child gender, family SES who were lost to follow-up and
standardized scores with a mean of (Hollingshead composite score, assigned weights to represent the full
100 and SD of 15 were used for all continuous), and maternal IQ population.
measures. (continuous, measured at baseline by Intervention services (eg, speech or
All testing sessions were video the Wonderlic Cognitive Ability language therapy, occupational or
recorded and all test Test).30 physical therapy, special education
administrations scored by a second Linear regression with robust SEs services) presumably improved some
psychometrist. Scoring errors were was used to estimate differences children’s outcome scores. To account
recorded and disagreements between case and control participants for these presumed positive effects
between psychometrists resolved by with corresponding 95% confidence and examine their influence on
one of the psychologist investigators intervals (CIs). Because limited case–control differences, we repeated
(K.K.S., B.R.C., or M.L.S.). Resolved sample size precluded direct analyses by using censored normal
scores were used for all analyses. adjustment of all potential regression.34 This method assumes
Among all administrations of the confounders in the same model, we that the scores of children who
4 tests, 94% of WISC-IV test recalculated in secondary analyses received intervention are “left
administrations were error-free, the regression estimates of censored”; that is, although it is
as were 97%, 94%, and 99% of case–control differences after unknown what their scores would
WRAT-4, TOWRE, and CTOPP propensity score matching.31,32 We have been in the absence of the
administrations, respectively. used a logit model that predicted case intervention they received, it is

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 3, March 2015 e617
assumed that the unknown scores are TABLE 1 Demographic Characteristics of Children With and Without SSC at Age 7
at least as low as their observed Characteristic Control Group, N = 183 Case Group, N = 182
scores with intervention.
N % N %
Logistic regression with robust SEs Age, y
was used to compare the proportion ,7.5 144 78.7 124 68.1
of case and control participants with .7.5 39 21.3 58 31.9
learning problems. We used an Gender
empirically derived definition of Female 69 37.7 68 37.4
Male 114 62.3 114 62.6
learning disability: scores below the Grade levela
25th percentile on $1 of the ,1 2 1.1 3 1.7
achievement tests given.35,36 We also 1 130 71.0 110 60.4
examined the distribution of learning 2 37 20.2 56 30.8
problems by case status and test and $3 9 4.9 9 4.9
Race or ethnicityb
calculated the number of tests with Nonwhite or Hispanic 47 25.7 37 20.3
scores ,25th percentile for each White, non-Hispanic 136 74.3 145 79.7
participant. SES
I (highest) 53 29.0 43 23.6
Finally, in secondary analyses II 102 55.7 81 44.5
involving only case participants, we III 15 8.2 36 19.8
used linear and logistic regression to IV 11 6.0 20 11.0
determine whether test scores or the V (lowest) 2 1.1 2 1.1
Site
odds of having a learning problem
Seattle 71 38.8 74 40.7
differed by the site of the affected Chicagoc 76 41.5 66 36.3
suture (sagittal, metopic, unicoronal, St Louis 9 4.9 18 9.9
and lambdoid). Indicators of suture Atlanta 27 14.8 24 13.2
site were included in each model, a Grade level missing for 5 control and 4 case participants.
b Includes Hispanic or Latino ethnicity, Asian or Pacific Islander, black or African American, and mixed races or ethnicities.
with sagittal cases considered the c Includes children from Philadelphia, who were assessed by staff from the Chicago site.
referent, and evidence for overall
group differences evaluated on Wald
tests. proportions of case and control phonological awareness, where case
We performed all analyses by using participants were lost to follow-up participants’ adjusted mean scores
Stata version 12 (StataCorp, College (31% of case and 29% of control were 2.5 to 4 points lower than
Station, TX). participants). Compared with those of control participants (Ps
participants seen at school age, ranged from .002 to .09).
participants lost to follow-up were Case–control differences on other
RESULTS more likely to be of lower SES (44% measures of achievement were
Mean age at the time of the Hollingshead categories III–V vs 24% modest. The lower average IQ scores
assessment was 7.5 years for case in participants), with slightly lower of the case group are also reflected
participants (range 6.9–9.5 years) maternal IQ scores. Children lost to in the distributions of scores relative
and 7.4 years for control participants attrition also had lower Bayley to established clinical classifications
(range 7.0–11.1 years). Sixty-three Psychomotor Development Index (Table 3).
percent of participants were male, scores at study baseline. Differences between case and control
77% identified as white or non- participants were modestly shifted
Hispanic ethnicity, and 76% were of Case–Control Group Differences upward or downward after
middle to upper SES (Hollingshead Case participants’ adjusted mean adjustment with propensity score
categories I–II) (Table 1). Mean scores were lower than those of matching, with a concomitant
maternal IQ was 109.7 (SD 12.7) in control participants on all measures increase in the SEs of the estimates
the control group and 106.2 (SD 13.7) (Table 2). The estimated average compared with direct adjustment.
in the case group. The proportion of difference in standard scores Case deficits increased by 0.6 to 1.8
single-parent families in the control comparing the case and control points for WISC-IV Full-Scale IQ,
and case groups was comparable groups ranged from 0.3 to –4.2 (Ps WRAT-4 reading composite, TOWRE,
(13% and 16%, respectively). ranged from 0.002 to 0.80). The and CTOPP phonological awareness
Parents of 31 case participants largest observed deficits were in (range in case–control differences
reported a history of craniosynostosis WISC-IV Full-Scale IQ, WRAT-4 math 22.7 to 24.6 points). Case–control
in immediate family members. Similar computation, and CTOPP differences were unchanged or

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
e618 SPELTZ et al
TABLE 2 Comparison of Mean IQ and Achievement Scores for Children With and Without SSC
Neurodevelopmental Test Control Group Case Group Case Versus Control Group

Unadjusted Adjusteda

N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) Difference (95% CI) P Difference (95% CI) P
Full-Scale IQ 183 110.2 (15.3) 182 103.2 (16.0) 27.0 (210.2 to 23.8) ,.001 23.9 (26.7 to 21.2) .006
Math computation 182 104.1 (14.6) 180 98.7 (13.4) 25.5 (28.4 to 22.6) ,.001 24.2 (26.8 to, 21.5) .002
Reading composite 183 109.3 (17.2) 180 105.4 (16.5) 24.0 (27.4, 20.5) .03 22.0 (25.2 to, 1.2) .21
Spelling 182 107.2 (14.3) 180 105.2 (16.1) 22.2 (25.3 to 1.0) .18 20.9 (23.8 to 2.0) .54
Total word reading efficiency 179 106.6 (14.4) 180 104.0 (16.1) 22.5 (25.7 to 0.6) .12 20.9 (23.9 to 2.1) .57
Phonological awareness 179 111.1 (14.5) 179 107.1 (15.5) 24.0 (27.1 to 20.9) .01 22.5 (25.4 to 0.4) .09
Rapid naming 172 101.0 (10.9) 167 100.3 (12.2) 20.8 (23.3 to 1.7) .53 0.3 (22.1 to 2.7) .80
a Adjusted for age (continuous), gender, SES (continuous), maternal IQ (continuous).

TABLE 3 Distribution of Full-Scale IQ Scores for Children With and Without SSC participants were classified as having
Classification Control Group Case Group learning problems. After adjustment
for baseline demographics, there was
N % N %
little evidence that case participants
Very superior (130+) 19 10.4 8 4.4
were at higher odds of having
Superior (120–129) 33 18.2 14 7.7
High average (110–119) 46 25.3 44 24.2 learning problems than control
Average (90–109) 66 36.3 81 44.5 participants (adjusted odds ratio
Low average (80–89) 13 7.1 26 14.3 [OR] = 1.4; 95% CI, 0.9 to 2.2).
Borderline (70–79) 4 2.2 3 1.7 However, within each achievement
Extremely low (,69) 1 0.6 6 3.3
test case participants were
Total 182 100 182 100
consistently more likely than control
participants to meet the threshold for
decreased by #0.5 for the other tests Among those for whom we had having a learning problem (Table 5).
(range in case–control differences 0.3 intervention data, 82 case Case participants also met the
to 23.6 points). Case deficits after we participants (46%) received $1 of criterion for having a learning
excluded 19 children with known or the defined intervention services, as problem on a greater number of
probable causal mutations for did 58 (35%) of control participants. tests than did control participants.
craniosynostosis were attenuated by In censored normal regression
0.1 to 0.8 points for all measures with analyses, the differences between Secondary Analyses by Diagnostic
1 exception: CTOPP rapid naming, case and control participants became Subgroup
where the estimate shifted upward by Within the case sample, children with
more pronounced for all measures
0.3 points (range in case–control metopic, unicoronal, or lambdoid
(Table 4). Case deficits increased by
differences 23.4 to 0.3 points). synostosis scored lower on nearly all
2.0 to 3.0 points for each test
Estimates using IPW to gauge measures of achievement and IQ than
attrition bias were of similar (Ps ranged from ,.001 to .24).
did children with sagittal synostosis
magnitude to those from the adjusted Seventy-three case participants (Table 6). Adjusted mean differences
analyses (data not shown). (40%) and 54 (30%) control in scores (Table 7) ranged from 1.0 to
23.2 points for children with metopic
TABLE 4 Differences Between Children With and Without SSC Adjusted for Intervention synostosis, from 21.6 to 211.7 for
Participation children with unicoronal synostosis,
Neurodevelopmental Test Adjusted, With Censored Normal Regressiona,b and from 1.4 to 214.8 for children with
Difference (95% CI) P lambdoid synostosis (Ps for overall
group differences ranged from ,.001 to
Full-Scale IQ 26.5 (210.3 to 22.8) ,.001
Math computation 26.8 (210.4 to 23.1) ,.001 .82). Children with metopic, unicoronal,
Reading composite 25.0 (29.3 to 20.6) .03 and lambdoid synostosis were also
Spelling 22.5 (26.4 to 1.5) .23 more likely to have a learning problem
Total word reading efficiency 23.6 (27.6 to 0.4) .08 than children with sagittal synostosis,
Phonological awareness 25.2 (29.1 to 21.3) .010
although CIs were wide for OR
Rapid naming 21.9 (25.0 to 1.3) .24
a
estimates (metopic vs sagittal adjusted
Adjusted for age (continuous), gender, SES (continuous), and maternal IQ (continuous).
b Among families reporting intervention data, 82/177 case participants (46%) and 58/167 control participants (35%) OR = 1.7; 95% CI, 0.7 to 3.8; unicoronal
received $1 interventions. vs sagittal adjusted OR = 3.2; 95% CI,

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 3, March 2015 e619
TABLE 5 Distribution of Learning Problems for Children With and Without SSC by Test have been 2 to 3 points greater for
Testa
Control Group Case Group each test given (eg, ∼0.5 SD group
difference in IQ and math).
Learning No Learning Learning No Learning
Problem Problem Problem Problem The mean IQ scores for both case and
control participants were in the
N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%)
average range relative to test norms.
Math computation 28 (15.2) 156 (84.8) 36 (19.5) 149 (80.5)
A higher-than-expected proportion of
Reading composite 27 (14.7) 157 (85.3) 41 (22.2) 144 (77.8)
Spelling 19 (10.3) 165 (89.7) 29 (15.7) 156 (84.3) children scored above average,
Total word reading 24 (13.0) 160 (87.0) 36 (19.5) 149 (80.5) probably reflecting that both groups
efficiency had high SES, a low rate of single-
Phonological awareness 5 (2.7) 179 (97.3) 20 (10.8) 165 (89.2) parent families (approximately half
Rapid naming 9 (4.9) 175 (95.1) 29 (15.7) 156 (84.3)
Number of tests ,25%b
the US base rate37), and slightly
1 25 (46.3) —c 28 (38.3) —c above-average mean maternal IQs.
2 14 (25.9) — 14 (19.2) — However, even in this low social risk
3 6 (11.1) — 7 (9.6) — sample, the IQ frequency
4 5 (9.3) — 11 (15.1) —
distributions exhibited important
5 3 (5.6) — 8 (11.0) —
6 1 (1.8) — 5 (6.8) — differences between case and control
Totala 54 (30.9) 121 (69.1) 73 (42.2) 100 (57.8) participants, with more than twice as
a Percentages by row. many control participants as case
b Percentages by column. participants scoring in the 2 highest
c Participants with no learning problem scored $25% on all tests.
categories of WISC-IV Full-Scale IQ.
Conversely, twice as many case as
1.2 to 8.1; lambdoid vs sagittal adjusted with SSC on average scored lower on control participants scored in the low
OR = 3.4; 95% CI, 0.7 to 17.2; P for all measures than demographically average range, and there were 6 case
overall group differences = .09). similar children without SSC. The participants (compared with a single
magnitude of adjusted group control participant) scoring in the
DISCUSSION differences was modest for tests of extremely low or intellectually
spelling and reading achievement disabled category. Although these
This is the first large cohort study to (,0.25 SD). However, case differences are based on small
track the development of children with participants exhibited larger and numbers in some categories, previous
SSC and unaffected control participants educationally meaningful differences studies of younger children with
from infancy to school entry. Only on measures of Full-Scale IQ and SSC have also indicated low
case participants with isolated suture math achievement (∼0.33 SD). rates of accelerated cognitive
fusions, without the phenotypic features Moreover, the effects of development15,38 and higher-than-
of known craniosynostosis syndromes, developmental interventions may expected rates of intellectual
were included in this study. The focus have led to the underestimation of disability.39
on this population increases the utility case deficits. Case participants had
of our study because it is based on In the few previously published
a higher frequency of intervention
phenotype rather than genotype, which studies of older children with SSC,
than control participants, and
may not be available or indicated in participants exhibited elevated rates
censored normal regression analyses
cases of nonsyndromic craniosynostosis. of variably defined problems in
suggested that in the absence of
academic and related skills (∼30% to
As we observed previously at earlier intervention, group differences
50%),16–20 leading several
age points,11–13 school-age children favoring control participants might
investigators to conclude that a large
proportion of children with SSC have
TABLE 6 Distribution of Mean IQ and Achievement Scores for Children With SSC by Suture learning disabilities.9,40,41 Consistent
Neurodevelopmental Test Sagittal Metopic Unicoronal Lambdoid with these previous estimates, 42% of
case participants in the current study
N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD)
scored below the 25th percentile
Full-Scale IQ 76 105.8 (14.7) 48 102.2 (17.8) 46 100.4 (16.3) 12 101.7 (14.3) on $1 academic achievement tests,
Math computation 75 101.1 (12.8) 48 98.7 (14.4) 45 95.7 (13.4) 12 94.5 (10.8)
a commonly used criterion in
Reading composite 75 109.4 (14.7) 48 105.8 (18.4) 45 100.1 (16.0) 12 98.3 (15.4)
Spelling 75 108.4 (13.4) 48 105.0 (17.7) 45 101.5 (17.9) 12 100.0 (15.5) screening for learning problems.35,36
Total word reading efficiency 76 107.3 (13.8) 48 104.0 (17.3) 44 99.8 (17.0) 12 99.6 (19.2) However, nearly one-third of the
Phonological awareness 75 109.3 (14.0) 48 107.0 (17.8) 44 105.4 (14.7) 12 100.3 (15.9) control group also met this criterion,
Rapid naming 71 100.2 (11.5) 45 101.1 (13.3) 41 99.5 (11.6) 10 100.0 (15.2) and after adjustment for demographic

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
e620 SPELTZ et al
TABLE 7 Comparison of Mean IQ and Achievement Scores for Children With SSC by Suture
Neurodevelopmental Test Comparison and Mean Difference

Metopic Versus Sagittal Unicoronal Versus Sagittal Lambdoid Versus Sagittal P for Differences
a a a
Across All Groups
Difference (95% CI) P Difference (95% CI) P Difference (95% CI) P
Full-Scale IQ 23.2 (27.9 to 1.6) .19 27.3 (212.7 to 21.9) .009 24.7 (214.4 to 5.0) .34 .05
Math computation 21.8 (26.3 to 2.7) .44 26.9 (211.7 to 22.1) .006 26.6 (215.1 to 1.9) .13 .04
Reading composite 23.0 (28.4 to 2.4) .27 211.7 (216.8 to, 26.7) ,.001 214.8 (225.8 to 23.7) .009 ,.001
Spelling 22.9 (27.9 to 2.2) .26 210.1 (215.4 to 24.8) ,.001 210.8 (222.2 to 0.6) .06 .002
Total word reading efficiency 22.7 (27.9 to 2.4) .30 210.0 (215.2 to 24.7) ,.001 210.5 (223.1 to 2.0) .10 .002
Phonological awareness 21.5 (27.0 to 4.0) .59 24.7 (29.9 to 0.6) .08 211.6 (221.2 to 21.9) .02 .07
Rapid naming 1.0 (23.5 to 5.5) .65 21.5 (26.2 to 3.1) .51 1.4 (210.2 to 12.9) .82 .82
a Adjusted for age (continuous), gender, SES (continuous), and maternal IQ (continuous).

characteristics, case participants were conclusions here are limited by small CONCLUSIONS
only slightly more likely to exhibit subgroups and wide CIs, the patterns The developmental delays observed
learning problems than control have persisted at every postsurgery among infants and young children
participants. The earlier studies cited, age at which we assessed this cohort with SSC are still partially evident at
most without control groups, may and are similar across outcome school age, as manifested by lower
have overestimated the risk of measures.12 average scores than those of control
learning disabilities in the SSC
Limitations in this research include participants on measures of IQ and
population in relation to unaffected
a sample of case and control math and a downward shift in IQ
children.
participants with higher than score distribution.15 However,
Another long-standing question is average SES, repeated child testing average scores of the case group
whether neurocognitive outcomes in with parent feedback that may have were only slightly lower than those
children with SSC are specific to the elevated referrals for intervention of the control group on reading and
affected suture. In secondary analyses beyond expected levels in the spelling measures, and the
we observed that children with community, and sample attrition frequency of specific learning
metopic, unicoronal, or lambdoid approaching 30%. However, the problems was comparable. Most
synostosis scored lower on nearly all demographic variables for which children with SSC did well
measures of achievement and IQ than we controlled in all regression academically, and 58% had no
did children with the most common analyses at least partially mitigated discernible learning problem;
form of SSC, sagittal synostosis. Group the effect of SES on group however, we would anticipate lower
differences were small between comparisons of outcome measures. levels of achievement in higher
sagittal and metopic case participants Moreover, IPW analyses, in which social risk samples. Among case
(,0.25 SD for all measures). we used baseline data available for participants, those with unicoronal
However, case participants with and lambdoid synostosis appear to
case and control participants to
unicoronal and lambdoid synostosis
evaluate attrition bias, did not be the most neurodevelopmentally
exhibited much larger deficits
greatly alter regression estimates or vulnerable.
(eg, 0.5 SD difference in IQ between
their interpretation. Multidisciplinary care guidelines have
participants with sagittal and
unicoronal synostosis; 0.67 SD It is important to note that the design called for neurodevelopmental
difference in academic achievement of this study does not allow analysis screening in the preschool years for
scores between participants with of the proposed mechanisms by all children with craniosynostosis.44
sagittal and lambdoid synostosis). which SSC or factors associated with Our findings suggest that for patients
Differences of this magnitude are SSC (eg, surgery and anesthesia with single-suture fusions, primary
clinically significant, suggesting that exposure42) might lead to the group attention should be given to those
children with unicoronal and differences we observed. Possible with unicoronal and lambdoid
lambdoid synostosis are more likely causal pathways continue to be synostosis, with more selective
than other children with SSC to need poorly understood and await screening of children with isolated
educational intervention. Although additional study.9,41,43 metopic and sagittal fusions.

Address correspondence to Matthew L. Speltz, PhD, Seattle Children’s Research Institute, 2001 8th Ave, Suite 600, Mailstop CW8-6, Seattle, WA 98121. E-mail: matt.
speltz@seattlechildrens.org

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 3, March 2015 e621
PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).
Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
FUNDING: All phases of this study were supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant R01 DE 13813 (Dr Speltz) and R01 DE 018227 (Dr Cunningham) from the
National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research. Dr Cunningham was also supported by the Jean Renny Endowment for Craniofacial Medicine. Funded by the
National Institutes of Health (NIH).
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

REFERENCES
1. French LR, Jackson IT, Melton LJ III. A suture craniosynostosis. Plast Reconstr scaphocephaly. Pediatrics. 1999;103(4):
population-based study of Surg. 2004;114(4):841–847, discussion 791–795
craniosynostosis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1990; 848–849
19. Kelleher MO, Murray DJ, McGillivary A,
43(1):69–73
11. Starr JR, Kapp-Simon KA, Cloonan YK, Kamel MH, Allcutt D, Earley MJ.
2. Shuper A, Merlob P, Grunebaum M, et al Presurgical and postsurgical Behavioral, developmental, and
Reisner SH. The incidence of isolated assessment of the neurodevelopment of educational problems in children with
craniosynostosis in the newborn infant. infants with single-suture nonsyndromic trigonocephaly.
Am J Dis Child. 1985;139(1):85–86 craniosynostosis: comparison with J Neurosurg. 2006;105(5 suppl):382–384
3. Aldridge K, Marsh JL, Govier D, controls. J Neurosurg. 2007;107(suppl 2):
20. Becker DB, Petersen JD, Kane AA,
Richtsmeier JT. Central nervous system 103–110
Cradock MM, Pilgram TK, Marsh JL.
phenotypes in craniosynostosis. J Anat. 12. Starr JR, Collett BR, Gaither R, et al. Speech, cognitive, and behavioral
2002;201(1):31–39 Multicenter study of neurodevelopment outcomes in nonsyndromic
4. Aldridge K, Kane AA, Marsh JL, et al. in 3-year-old children with and without craniosynostosis. Plast Reconstr Surg.
Brain morphology in nonsyndromic single-suture craniosynostosis. Arch 2005;116(2):400–407
unicoronal craniosynostosis. Anat Rec A Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(6):
21. Cunningham ML, Horst JA, Rieder MJ,
Discov Mol Cell Evol Biol. 2005;285(2): 536–542
et al. IGF1R variants associated with
690–698 13. Speltz ML, Kapp-Simon K, Collett B, et al. isolated single suture craniosynostosis.
5. Aldridge K, Kane AA, Marsh JL, Yan P, Neurodevelopment of infants with single- Am J Med Genet A. 2011;155A(1):91–97
Govier D, Richtsmeier JT. Relationship of suture craniosynostosis: presurgery
22. Hollingshead AB. Four Factor Index of
brain and skull in pre- and postoperative comparisons with case-matched
Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale
sagittal synostosis. J Anat. 2005;206(4): controls. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2007;
University; 1975
373–385 119(6):1874–1881
23. Sattler J, Dumont R. Assessment of
6. Fok H, Jones BM, Gault DG, Andar U, 14. Da Costa AC, Anderson VA, Savarirayan R,
Children: WISC-IV and WPPSI-III
Hayward R. Relationship between et al. Neurodevelopmental functioning of
Supplement. San Diego, CA: Jerome M.
intracranial pressure and intracranial infants with untreated single-suture
Sattler; 2004
volume in craniosynostosis. Br J Plast craniosynostosis during early infancy.
Surg. 1992;45(5):394–397 Childs Nerv Syst. 2012;28(6):869–877 24. Wilkinson G. Wide Range Achievement
Test. 4th ed. Odessa, FL: Psychological
7. Renier D, Sainte-Rose C, Marchac D, 15. Knight SJ, Anderson VA, Spencer-Smith
Assessment Resources, Inc; 1999
Hirsch JF. Intracranial pressure in MM, Da Costa AC. Neurodevelopmental
craniostenosis. J Neurosurg. 1982;57(3): outcomes in infants and children with 25. Torgenson JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte C.
370–377 single-suture craniosynostosis: Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin,
a systematic review. Dev Neuropsychol. TX: Pro-Ed; 1999
8. Lekovic GP, Bristol RE, Rekate HL.
2014;39(3):159–186 26. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA.
Cognitive impact of craniosynostosis.
Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2004;11(4): 16. Chieffo D, Tamburrini G, Massimi L, et al. Comprehensive Test of Phonological
305–310 Long-term neuropsychological Processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: PRO-ED;
development in single-suture 1999
9. Kapp-Simon KA, Speltz ML, Cunningham
craniosynostosis treated early. 27. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK. The nature of
ML, Patel PK, Tomita T.
J Neurosurg Pediatr. 2010;5(3):232–237 phonological processing and its causal
Neurodevelopment of children with
single suture craniosynostosis: a review. 17. Magge SN, Westerveld M, Pruzinsky T, role in the acquisition of reading skills.
Childs Nerv Syst. 2007;23(3):269–281 Persing JA. Long-term Psychol Bull. 1987;101(2):192–212
neuropsychological effects of sagittal
10. Cohen SR, Cho DC, Nichols SL, Simms C, 28. Katzir T, Kim YS, Wolf M, Morris R, Lovett
craniosynostosis on child development.
Cross KP, Burstein FD. American Society MW. The varieties of pathways to
J Craniofac Surg. 2002;13(1):99–104
of Maxillofacial Surgeons outcome study: dysfluent reading: comparing
preoperative and postoperative 18. Virtanen R, Korhonen T, Fagerholm J, subtypes of children with dyslexia at
neurodevelopmental findings in single- Viljanto J. Neurocognitive sequelae of letter, word, and connected text levels of

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
e622 SPELTZ et al
reading. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41(1): learning disabilities in children and 40. van der Meulen J, van der Vlugt J,
47–66 adolescents. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. Okkerse J, Hofman B. Early beaten-
29. Cronin VS. RAN and double-deficit theory. 2005;34(3):506–522 copper pattern: its long-term effect on
J Learn Disabil. 2013;46(2):182–190 36. Lyon GR, Fletcher JM, Barnes MC. intelligence quotients in 95 children with
Learning disabilities. In: Mash EJ, Barkely craniosynostosis. J Neurosurg Pediatr.
30. Matthews TD, Lassiter KS. What does the 2008;1(1):25–30
Wonderlic Personnel Test measure? RA, eds. Child Psychopathology. 2nd ed.
Psychol Rep. 2007;100(3 pt 1):707–712 New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2003: 41. Speltz ML, Kapp-Simon KA, Cunningham
520–586 M, Marsh J, Dawson G. Single-suture
31. Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB. The central craniosynostosis: a review of
role of the propensity score in 37. US Department of Health and Human
Services, Administration for Children neurobehavioral research and theory.
observational studies for causal effects. J Pediatr Psychol. 2004;29(8):651–668
Biometrika. 1983;70(1):41–55 and Families, Office of Planning,
Research, and Evaluation. Temporary 42. Naumann HL, Haberkern CM, Pietila KE,
32. Abadie A, Drukker D, Herr JL, Imbens GW. Assistance for Needy Families. Sixth et al. Duration of exposure to cranial
Implementing matching estimators for annual report to Congress. Chapter VII. vault surgery: associations with
average treatment effects in Stata. Stata Formation and maintenance of married neurodevelopment among children with
J. 2004;4(3):290–311 two-parent families. 2004. Available at: single-suture craniosynostosis. Paediatr
33. Robins JM, Rotnitzky A, Zhao LP. http://archive.acf.hhs.gov/programs/ofa/ Anaesth. 2012;22:1053–1061
Estimation of regression coefficients data-reports/annualreport6/chapter07/ 43. Speltz ML, Birgfeld CB, Starr JR, Collett
when some regressors are not always chap07.htm. Accessed November 3, 2009 BR, Kapp-Simon KA. Comments about
observed. J Am Stat Assoc. 1994;89(427): 38. Da Costa AC, Anderson VA, Holmes AD, Hashim et al. (2014) “The effects of
846–866 et al. Longitudinal study of the whole vault cranioplasty versus strip
34. Tobin MD, Sheehan NA, Scurrah KJ, neurodevelopmental characteristics of craniectomy on long-term
Burton PR. Adjusting for treatment treated and untreated nonsyndromic neuropsychological outcomes in sagittal
effects in studies of quantitative traits: craniosynostosis in infancy. Childs Nerv craniosynostosis.” Plast Reconstr Surg.
antihypertensive therapy and systolic Syst. 2013;29(6):985–995 (in press)
blood pressure. Stat Med. 2005;24(19): 39. Kapp-Simon KA. Mental development and 44. McCarthy JG, Warren SM, Bernstein J,
2911–2935 learning disorders in children with et al Parameters of care for
35. Fletcher JM, Francis DJ, Morris RD, Lyon single suture craniosynostosis. Cleft craniosynostosis. Cleft Palate Craniofac
GR. Evidence-based assessment of Palate Craniofac J. 1998;35(3):197–203 J. 2012;49(suppl):1s–24s

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 3, March 2015 e623
Intellectual and Academic Functioning of School-Age Children With
Single-Suture Craniosynostosis
Matthew L. Speltz, Brent R. Collett, Erin R. Wallace, Jacqueline R. Starr, Mary
Michaeleen Cradock, Lauren Buono, Michael Cunningham and Kathleen Kapp-Simon
Pediatrics 2015;135;e615; originally published online February 23, 2015;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-1634
Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/135/3/e615.full.h
tml
References This article cites 36 articles, 5 of which can be accessed free
at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/135/3/e615.full.h
tml#ref-list-1
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,
tables) or in its entirety can be found online at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xh
tml
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published,
and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk
Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015
Intellectual and Academic Functioning of School-Age Children With
Single-Suture Craniosynostosis
Matthew L. Speltz, Brent R. Collett, Erin R. Wallace, Jacqueline R. Starr, Mary
Michaeleen Cradock, Lauren Buono, Michael Cunningham and Kathleen Kapp-Simon
Pediatrics 2015;135;e615; originally published online February 23, 2015;
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-1634

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is
located on the World Wide Web at:
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/135/3/e615.full.html

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly


publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned,
published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point
Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy
of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at Galter Health Sciences Library on May 18, 2015

You might also like