You are on page 1of 10

Original Article

Evolutionary Psychology
January-March 2018: 1–10
Exploring the Great Schism in the Social ª The Author(s) 2018
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Sciences: Confirmation Bias and the DOI: 10.1177/1474704917752691
journals.sagepub.com/home/evp
Interpretation of Results Relating to Biological
Influences on Human Behavior and Psychology

Jeffrey Winking1

Abstract
The nature–nurture debate is one that biologists often dismiss as a false dichotomy, as all phenotypic traits are the results of
complex processes of gene and environment interactions. However, such dismissiveness belies the ongoing debate that is
unmistakable throughout the biological and social sciences concerning the role of biological influences in the development of
psychological and behavioral traits in humans. Many have proposed that this debate is due to ideologically driven biases in the
interpretation of results. Those favoring biological approaches have been accused of a greater willingness to accept biological
explanations so as to rationalize or justify the status quo of inequality. Those rejecting biological approaches have been accused of
an unwillingness to accept biological explanations so as to attribute inequalities solely to social and institutional factors, ultimately
allowing for the possibility of social equality. While it is important to continue to investigate this topic through further research
and debate, another approach is to examine the degree to which the allegations of bias are indeed valid. To accomplish this, a
convenience sample of individuals with relevant postgraduate degrees was recruited from Mechanical Turk and social media.
Participants were asked to rate the inferential power of different research designs and of mock results that varied in the degree to
which they supported different ideologies. Results were suggestive that researchers harbor sincere differences of opinion con-
cerning the inferential value of relevant research. There was no suggestion that ideological confirmation biases drive these dif-
ferences. However, challenges associated with recruiting a large enough sample of experts as well as identifying believable mock
scenarios limit the study’s inferential scope.

Keywords
confirmation bias, academia, evolutionary studies, cognitive bias, MTurk

Date received: April 13, 2017; Accepted: December 13, 2017

The nature–nurture debate is one that biologists often dismiss Much of the theory and methods employed by biologically
as a false dichotomy, as all phenotypic traits are the results of oriented social scientists flows from the animal literature.
complex processes of gene and environment interactions When human studies suggest homologous or analogous fea-
playing out across numerous dimensions. Therefore, all traits tures, however, explanations invoking biological pathways or
might be considered to be due to 100% nature and 100% adaptive functions tend to be less readily accepted for human
nurture. However, such dismissiveness belies the long- traits than for animal traits (e.g., for incest avoidance, see
standing debate that is unmistakable throughout the biological
and social sciences concerning the role of biological influ-
1
ences in the development of psychological and behavioral Department of Anthropology, Texas A&M University, College Station,
traits in humans (e.g., Buller, 2006; Horowitz & Yaworsky, TX, USA
2014; Marks, 2012; McKinnon & Silverman, 2005b; Pinker,
Corresponding Author:
2002; Rose & Rose, 2001). Indeed, this debate divides Jeffrey Winking, Department of Anthropology, Texas A&M University,
many disciplines and has at times proven quite contentious 4352 TAMU, College Station, TX 77843, USA.
(Segerstrale, 2000). Email: jwinking@tamu.edu

Creative Commons Non Commercial CC BY-NC: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License
(http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission
provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
2 Evolutionary Psychology

Chapais, 2008; El-Guindi & Read, 2012; Kushnick & Fessler, the sound used to represent the concept of “tree,” the aesthetic
2011; Leavitt, 2007). This is reasonable, given that humans preference for a certain hair length on men, and the feeling of
exhibit an unprecedented level of behavioral plasticity, driven embarrassment when one’s bare legs are seen by others. Obvi-
by uniquely elevated capacities for fluid reasoning and social ously, there are some biological constraints such as the fact that
learning (i.e., culture). Many have argued that such character- humans must be able to produce and hear the sound represent-
istics make it impossible to disentangle the roles of culture and ing “tree,” grow hair to a certain length, and be able to inter-
biology in the phylogenetic story of humans and the ontoge- nalize norms and feel shame when they are broken. But within
netic stories of individuals (Fuentes, 2012; Marks, 2012; those broad constraints, the possibility that any particular pre-
McKinnon & Silverman, 2005a). Under this argument, it is not ference might emerge through a cultural process of historical
simply that such a goal is logistically unattainable, but that such happenstance is indeed real.
endeavors are meaningless: In humans, the biological is inex-
tricably cultural and the cultural biological. In other academic
fields, extreme forms of social constructionism have eliminated
Inferential Power (IP) of Different Study Designs
the consideration of biological factors entirely (Sayer, 1997). If there is fair agreement at the tails, this would suggest that
Obviously, complex phylogenetic and ontogenetic pro- there is some degree of evidence and/or cogency of argument
cesses cannot be captured by the single dimension of nature that satisfies most parties and that the debate likely rests on
versus nurture, genetic versus environment, and so on. For variance in the inferential thresholds demanded before infer-
many phenomena, such a dimension would prove nonsensical ring biological constraints. This raises the question of how we
in its mutually exclusive, linear form. However, the debate that infer a biological influence (Andrews, Gangestad, & Matthews,
divides the social sciences is more specific. 2002). A preference to not be exposed to the arctic cold is
When biologists refer to gene–environment interactions, cross-culturally universal; it appears early in development, can
many types of environmental factors can influence outcomes: be seen in other animals, and serves a clear adaptive purpose.
intrauterine hormonal levels, salinity of interstitial fluids, None of those things can be said for long hair on men. Thus,
energy availability, and so on. However, as the debate has despite not knowing the genetic or biological etiology of a
played out in academic journals and popular publications, cognitive motivation to seek warmth—a common critique of
many of these factors (such as intrauterine hormonal levels) biological explanations—most are comfortable with assigning
would likely be conceptually placed on the biological side of a biological foundation to a preference for warmth over freez-
the ledger. The environmental factors that are relevant are ing. There are a number of research designs (RDs) that are used
typically those relating to experiential influences—those to examine the role of biology in the development of a partic-
derived from what an individual experiences through his or her ular trait. Referenced above, these include explorations of
senses through time. This includes one’s upbringing, schooling, cross-cultural universality (Buss, 1989; Eagly & Wood,
and cultural environment. Even when limiting the discussion to 1999; Henrich et al., 2004), early developmental onset (Cam-
these factors, one’s experiences and biological processes con- pos, Bertenthal, & Kermoian, 1992; Langlois et al., 1987;
tinue to interact to define an intertwined developmental trajec- Thompson & Newton, 2013), animal homologs/analogs (Plav-
tory. However, the overarching spectrum of possibilities can, 2012; Proctor, Williamson, de Waal, & Brosnan, 2013;
becomes limited, and we can more meaningfully characterize Sussman, 2013; Wrangham & Glowacki, 2012), and adaptive
the debate in a more limited question: To what degree, do design (Gould & Lewontin, 1979; Oaten, Stevenson, & Case,
biological processes constrain humans’ remarkable cultural 2009; Winking, Gurven, Kaplan, & Stieglitz, 2009) but also
and behavioral responsiveness to experiential variance? And allelic associations (Hamer, Hu, Magnuson, Hu, & Pattatucci,
the question explored here, therefore, is what explains the 1993; Munafò, Yalcin, Willis-Owen, & Flint, 2008), hormonal
remarkable variation in academics’ interpretations of data associations (Gettler, McDade, Feranil, & Kuzawa, 2011;
relating to this question? Mehta, Jones, & Josephs, 2008), and heritability (Polderman
For those who still question the meaningfulness of suggest- et al., 2015).
ing that a trait is more or less biologically constrained (i.e., There likely exists disagreement concerning the IP of these
environmentally canalized) than another, perhaps a look at the different designs and this might explain some of the variance in
tails would be helpful. There are a number of traits for which the degree to which researchers believe biology plays a role in
there is less doubt that selection played a role in shaping their the development of human traits. Unfortunately, no study to my
expression and that genetically determined biologically pro- knowledge has explored how researchers differ in their inter-
cesses play a role in constraining their development. These pretations according to their outlook on biological influences.
include the aversion to the smell of vomit on one’s food, pre- This is one of the goals of this study.
ferring to be exposed to room temperatures rather than arctic
temperatures, perceiving the act of sex to feel more pleasant
than being stabbed, and the consequent motivation to pursue
Confirmation Bias
sex more than being stabbed. On the other hand, there are many In addition to epistemological differences, researchers on both
traits for which there is little doubt that biological processes do sides of the debate have alleged that the disagreements stem
not play a large role in constraining their development such as from ideological biases. Those who believe biological
Winking 3

influences to be unimportant often allege that the other side is evidential support for competing arguments, and that we are
motivated to rationalize at best, and justify at worst, current and always prepared to change our beliefs upon the presentation of
historical patterns of hegemony, social dominance, and sufficient evidence, a great deal of research suggests that the
inequality. This argument harkens back to the beginning of the cognitive game is fixed (Nickerson, 1998; Snyder & Swann,
sociobiology debates, summarized in the collective response of 1978; Stanovich, West, & Toplak, 2013). Our reasoning is
an early Harvard study group which evaluated the merits of often clouded by a confirmation bias—in its broad definition,
sociobiology, and which included intellectual giants like confirmation bias is the tendency to advantage preexisting
Stephen J. Gould and Richard Lewontin, “[T]hey consistently beliefs through many forms of inferential acrobatics
tend to provide a genetic justification of the status quo and of (Nickerson, 1998). We actively seek confirmatory evidence
existing privileges for certain groups according to class, race, and avoid disconfirmatory evidence (Jonas, Schulz-Hardt,
or sex” (Allen et al., 1975). The argument continues today and Frey, & Thelen, 2001; Taber & Lodge, 2006), and we recall
is evident in writings across multiple disciplines: this confirmatory evidence more readily (Stanovich et al.,
2013). More importantly here, we accept confirmatory evi-
Many researchers use [evolutionary approaches to the study of dence more readily and weight its inferential value more
international relations] to justify the status quo in the guise of heavily, while being overly critical or dismissive of discon-
science. (Lebow, 2013, p. 2) firmatory evidence (often referred to as a “disconfirmation
The totality [of sociobiology and evolutionary psychology] bias”; Lehner, Adelman, Cheikes, & Brown, 2008; Nickerson,
is a myth of origin that is compelling precisely because it reso- 1998; Taber & Lodge, 2006).
nates strongly with Euro American presuppositions about the While it is important to engage in research and debate that
nature of the world. (McKinnon & Silverman, 2005a, p. 4) advances our understanding of how biological factors influence
. . . in the social sciences (with the exception of primatology the development of behavioral and psychological outcomes, it
and psychology) sociobiology appeals most to right-wing social is also important to examine the validity of these alleged biases.
scientists. (Pavelka, 2002, p. 42)
Previous research has shown that those who self-identify as
political radicals or feminists are more likely to reject biologi-
Conversely, those who favor biological approaches have cal influences on human traits (Horowitz & Yaworsky, 2014;
argued that those on the other side are rendered incapable of Sanderson & Ellis, 1992), while others have found academic
objective assessment by their ideological promotion of equal- proponents of biological approaches to be no less liberal on
ity. They are alleged to erroneously reject evidence of biologi- political issues (with both groups being far more liberal than
cal influences because such evidence suggests that social the population at large; Lyle & Smith, 2012; Tybur, Miller, &
outcomes are partially explained by biology, and this might Gangestad, 2007). However, while a finding of an association
inhibit the realization of equality. Their critiques of biological with political outlook might suggest the possibility of an ideo-
approaches are therefore often blithely dismissed as examples logical bias in interpretations, it does not shed light on which
of the moralistic/naturalistic fallacy. This line of reason is side, if either or if both, is indeed biased. To answer this ques-
exemplified in the quote by biologist Jerry Coyne tion, we must examine the interpretations of results that differ
solely on the degree to which they support or fail to support the
If you can read the [major Evolutionary Psychology review
preexisting ideological beliefs that allegedly drive these biases.
paper] and still dismiss the entire field as worthless, or as a
I attempt this here.
mere attempt to justify scientists’ social prejudices, then I’d
suggest your opinions are based more on ideology than judi-
cious scientific inquiry. (Coyne, 2012) Hypotheses
It is entirely possible that confirmation bias plays no role in
Again, such claims are quite common:
driving this disagreement and that the overarching debate in
academia is driven by sincere disagreements concerning the
For critics, the heart of the intellectual problem remains an
ideological adherence to the increasingly implausible view that
inferential value of the RDs informing the debate. Although
human behavior is strictly determined by socialization. . . . - academics might believe that inferential logic is universal and
Should [social] hierarchies result strictly from culture, then the abundantly clear, biological systems are inconceivably com-
possibilities for an egalitarian future were seen to be as open plex, and it is unlikely that we will ever reach a consensus on
and boundless as our ever-malleable brains might imagine. the meaningfulness of large bodies of research. While no pre-
(Horowitz & Yaworsky, 2014, p. 490) dictions can be made concerning which RDs should be per-
Like the Church, a number of contemporary thinkers have ceived as holding more or less IP, we can at least predict that
also grounded their moral and political views in scientific those who believe biology to play a more important role (for
assumptions about . . . human nature, specifically that there isn’t convenience, we will dichotomize this belief and label this
one. (Hagen, 2005, p. 166) group “bioists”) should report that positive results, across a
range of RDs, would carry more inferential weight than that
Although we academics like to believe that we approach reported by participants who believe biology to be less relevant
debates with an open mind, that we objectively weigh the (“skeptics”; Hypothesis 1a; Table 1). Similarly, skeptics should
4 Evolutionary Psychology

Table 1. Hypotheses. frequencies in advantaged versus disadvantaged groups


(Hypothesis 3a), as well as studies that mention that such
Research Design Description
antecedents occur in higher frequencies in advantaged groups
Hypothesis 1a Bioists will assign a higher influential value than (Hypothesis 4a). The opposite effects should occur among
skeptics to positive results of studies exploring skeptics (Hypotheses 3b and 4b, respectively).
biological influences
Hypothesis 1b Skeptics will assign a higher influential value than
bioists to null results of studies exploring
biological influences RD and Methods
Hypothesis 2 Bioists will exhibit a more conservative political
orientation than skeptics A convenience sample was recruited to take part in online
Hypothesis 3a Bioists will rate results suggesting a biological Qualtrics surveys through Amazon’s MTurk and through
antecedent to a socially undesirable trait higher social media sites. Only those who had received a master’s
when that biological antecedent is reported to degree or higher in the biological sciences, social sciences, or
exist in a higher frequency in a nonprivileged relevant humanities were included. Two separate surveys
group than those that do not mention frequencies were conducted, an RD survey and IP (IP) survey. In both,
Hypothesis 3b Bioists will rate results suggesting a biological
those entering through MTurk were offered US$0.10 to
antecedent to a socially undesirable trait higher
when that biological antecedent is reported to answer a series of eligibility questions. If they were deemed
exist in a higher frequency in a nonprivileged eligible, they were allowed to continue and received a
group than those that report the frequency US$1.00 bonus for their participation. Social media
higher in a privileged group announcements were made via Facebook pages of relevant
Hypothesis 4a Skeptics will rate results suggesting a biological groups (Biological Anthropology Society, Evolutionary
antecedent to a socially undesirable trait lower Anthropology Society, and Evolutionary Psychology).
when that biological antecedent is reported to
E-mails were also sent out to eligible acquaintances with the
exist in a higher frequency in a nonprivileged
group than those that do not mention frequencies instructions to forward the e-mail to anyone they thought
Hypothesis 4b Skeptics will rate results suggesting a biological might be eligible. This was done in part because of the lower
antecedent to a socially undesirable trait lower number of biologically oriented researchers on MTurk. Social
when that biological antecedent is reported to media recruits were offered to take part in a lottery in which 1
exist in a higher frequency in a nonprivileged in 50 would win a US$50 Amazon gift certificate. The RD
group than those that report the frequency survey was conducted in June 2015, and the IP survey was
higher in a privileged group
conducted from August 2015 to September 2016.

report that null results carry more inferential weight than bio- Inclusion Criteria
ists report (Hypothesis 1b). Such effects should be evident even Only participants who reported a master’s or PhD in a relevant
when reviewing the IP of generic RD descriptions, without field were included. These fields included the social sciences,
viewing any specific results. biological sciences, gender studies, and philosophy. A US$1.00
If ideological biases are driving this debate, then it must be bonus trends toward the high side of MTurk payouts and there-
the case that bioists and skeptics differ in their ideological fore might lead to higher participation rate of “professional”
outlook. Based on the alleged biases, we can predict that com- MTurkers—those who seek the most lucrative “hits” and mini-
pared to skeptics, bioists should report a more conservative mize the time they spend engaged per hit (Berinsky, Margolis,
orientation, particularly with regard to causes of social strati- & Sances, 2014; Wu, Corney, & Grant, 2014). Furthermore,
fication and efforts to reverse it (Hypothesis 2). That is to say, recruitment materials advertised the fact that only master’s or
compared to skeptics, bioists will report that social outcomes PhD holders were eligible so as to minimize the number of
are based more on inherent differences as opposed to social ineligible participants needlessly beginning the survey. There-
forces and less should be done to reduce inequalities. fore, in addition to the degree and discipline requirements, a
Ideological confirmation biases would be suggested if the number of additional inclusion criteria were used to ensure that
inferential value assigned to the results were associated with participants were completing the survey appropriately.
the degree to which those studies supported a particular ideol- Participants were asked two timed questions (30 s) regard-
ogy, holding all else constant. For instance, results suggesting ing knowledge specific to academia to ensure honesty concern-
that a socially undesirable trait was associated with a biolo- ing reported educational level. One question focused on the
gical antecedent that occurred more frequently in a disadvan- final research product for a master’s degree (a thesis) and the
taged population should evoke a positive ideological other on the meaning of “curriculum vitae.” In a previous sur-
confirmation bias in bioists and a disconfirmation bias in vey of 256 MTurk participants who were not told of any edu-
skeptics. Thus, bioists should rate studies suggesting such a cation requirements, 22 of the 24 (92%) master’s and PhD
scenario as having a higher inferential value than those that holders answered both of these questions correctly, while only
report such an association but do not mention relative 92 of the 232 (40%) participants of other educational levels
Winking 5

answered correctly. Only those who answered both questions Mock Results
correctly were included in the surveys used here.
The original study design included tests of both positive and
Furthermore, MTurk participants were excluded if they fin-
null results. However, the mock null results included psycho-
ished the survey in under 6 min for the RD survey and under 9
logical traits that participants found too different to be mean-
min for the IP survey. This was based on the fastest times (6.7
ingfully comparable and are not used here. Similarly, one
min for RD and 9.3 min for IP) for completion among the social
condition employing mock positive results included a psycho-
media sample, which was expected to not include
logical trait that was deemed to be significantly more complex
“professional” survey takers. An attention check question was
than the others and was excluded from the study. A full descrip-
included; however, 17% of participants who answered incor-
tion of these can be found in the Online Supplemental Material.
rectly did not significantly differ in the amount of time spent
The remaining three conditions are described below.
taking the survey, and it was therefore not used as an exclu-
The creation of the mock results was based on a number of
sionary factor. Finally, Reddit’s “HitsWorthTurkingFor” page
criteria. Psychological traits were considered based on (1) degree
was regularly searched to ensure that inclusion criteria were not
of social preference and (2) familiarity to participants while not
being discussed. The IP survey was discussed on this site 4
being the subject of a large, well-known bodies of literature
times, resulting in drastic increases in the frequency of partici-
focusing on their genetic and biological associates (e.g., general
pants. Participants who completed the survey directly after
intelligence, schizophrenia). Based on the results from the RD
such postings were excluded.
survey, mock results were created using three RDs that were
deemed to have high IP. The designs were presented pseudoran-
Surveys domly (16 sequences). As described above and in the Online Sup-
plementary Material, data from only one RD are reported on here.
The RD and IP surveys (Supplementary Figures S1 and S2)
The mock results suggesting a biological influence were
begin with four identical sections. These include questions
based on a genetic allele design, which was rated as having the
covering participants’ prior beliefs concerning the role of bio-
third highest IP out of eight. Participants were presented with a
logy in influencing human behavior and psychology, political
plot showing a substantial association (Cohen’s d ¼ .86)
beliefs, epistemological approaches, and statistical knowledge.
between a particular allele and appetitive aggression. In the
Specifically, participants were asked where they place them-
first condition, the allele was described as being found in
selves on the “general spectrum that exists in academia con-
higher frequency in populations with African ancestry. In the
cerning the role of biological factors in determining
second condition, this language was absent, and in the third
psychological and behavioral patterns in humans.” Provided
condition, it was described as being found in higher frequency
with a 4-point scale, participants were forced to choose which
in populations with European ancestry. Appetitive aggression
side of the spectrum they placed themselves. For all other
was described as “pro-active and predatory aggres-
questions with ordinal responses, participants chose from a
sion . . . associated with things such as likelihood of committing
7-point scale for scales that had a meaningful center (e.g.,
violence and positive attitudes towards warfare.” By including
extremely liberal–extremely conservative); otherwise, a 5-point
references to both “violence” and “warfare,” such a trait can be
scale was used.
reasonably aligned with negative stereotypes or perceptions
In the RD survey, participants were then presented with
associated with African American culture (Devine, 1989;
eight RDs and asked to rate the inferential value of significant
Dixon, 2008) and European history (Wolfe, 2006).
results regarding the role of biology in influencing psycholo-
After participants rated the IP of each set of results, they
gical and behavioral traits and the influential value of null
were asked how socially preferred they believed the relevant
results regarding the lack of a role of biology. Participants used
trait to be on a scale ranging from 3 to 3. They were also
a sliding scale ranging from 0 to 100.
asked how psychologically complex they believed it to be
In the IP survey, participants were instead presented with
using a 5-point scale.
three mock data plots that were designed to appear, as if they
were scanned from a legitimate academic journal or confer-
ence poster (due to reasons described in the section below, Analysis
data from only one of these proved useful). Participants then Simple comparisons of bioist and skeptic measures are analyzed
read a brief synopsis of the findings and conclusions of the using Wilcoxon rank tests for ordinal data and t tests for
study and were asked to rate the inferential value of the study continuous ratings. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA)
regarding the role of biology in influencing the outcome of the tests are employed for tests involving multiple dependent ratings.
trait in question. Participants used a sliding scale from 0 to All analyses were conducted in SAS Version 9.3.
100 as in the RD survey. Participants were also asked to rate
the psychological complexity of each trait and how socially
preferred they believed the trait to be. At the end of the sur-
vey, participants were asked whether they were suspicious
Results
that the results were forged and whether they believed this Of the 327 participants who began the RD survey, 60 were
influenced their responses. excluded for not having the appropriate educational level,
6 Evolutionary Psychology

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics.

Research Design Inferential Power


Factor
Bioist Skeptic Bioist Skeptic

n 32 39 140 225
Male (%) 15 (47) 20 (51) 91 (65) 149 (66)
Female 17 (53) 19 (49) 49 (35) 76 (34)
Mean age 35.8 34.8 39.0 39.5
MTurk 7 (22) 23 (59) 120 (86) 215 (96)
Social media 25 (78) 16 (41) 20 (14) 10 (4)
Master’s 15 (47) 20 (51) 106 (76) 194 (86)
PhD 17 (53) 19 (49) 34 (24) 31 (14)
Anthropology 21 (66) 14 (36) 37 (14) 17 (8)
Biological sciences 4 (13) 8 (21) 40 (29) 22 (10)
Economics 0 3 (8) 21 (15) 42 (19)
Gender studies 0 0 1 (1) 3 (1)
Philosophy 0 2 (5) 2 (1) 5 (2)
Political science 0 4 (10) 7 (5) 19 (8)
Psychology 5 (16) 3 (8) 16 (11) 43 (19)
Sociology 0 2 (5) 2 (1) 6 (3) Figure 1. Orientation.
Social science, other 2 (6) 3 (8) 31 (22) 68 (30)
Graduate student 6 (19) 7 (18) 11 (8) 19 (8)
Faculty 15 (47) 9 (23) 13 (9) 22 (10)
Research 4 (13) 2 (5) 19 (14) 28 (12)
Academia, other 2 (6) 5 (13) 12 (9) 9 (4)
Nonacademia/research 5 (16) 16 (41) 85 (61) 147 (65)

124 were excluded for working in a field that was not included,
36 were excluded for not answering the academia questions
correctly, 15 were excluded for taking the survey too quickly,
and 21 were excluded for not finishing the survey, leaving 71
included. Of the 2,631 participants who began the IP survey,
248 were excluded for education, 848 for their field, 235 for the
academia questions, 231 for taking it too quickly, 610 for hav-
ing started the survey after a Reddit post, and 94 for not com-
pleting the survey, leaving 365 included. Table 2 presents the
descriptive results for the two data sets. The RD survey
included only one condition and thus required a much smaller
sample size. Most participants could thus be recruited from
social media. The sample also has a higher proportion who
have doctorates, are anthropologists, and are faculty.
When asked to place themselves on the broad spectrum in
academia regarding their opinion on the role of biology in
influencing the development of behavioral and psychological Figure 2. Research designs.
traits, participants appear to have segregated themselves cor-
rectly. Across the four options, there is a linear increase in the Bioists did, however, report a stronger quantitative/empirical
reported degree to which participants believe biology com- orientation as opposed to a qualitative/interpretive orientation
pared to experience to be important in the development of compared to skeptics (on a 5-point scale, bioists x ¼ 2.74,
both general traits and in sex differences (Figure 1, from IP skeptics x ¼ 2.30, Wilcoxon Z ¼ 3.66, p < .001).
survey). Because of the small sample sizes at the tails, the four
groups were collapsed into two groups for both surveys: bio-
ists and skeptics. IP of RDs
Exploring only the IP survey, there was no difference in the Figure 2 presents the reported IP of eight different RDs in
reported familiarity with statistical tests across four areas (gen- descending order of pooled average for both significant (a) and
eral statistical knowledge, t tests, correlations, and p values). null (b) results. The eight RDs are described in Table 3. There
Winking 7

Table 3. Research Designs.

Research Design Description

Cross-cultural A trait or pattern exists universally among


universality cultures . . .
Twin studies Identical twins reared together are . . . compared
to identical twins reared apart . . . [and] are
also compared to adopted siblings reared
together.
Early developmental A trait appears very early in the development of
onset infants . . .
Genetic variants A particular genetic variant is found to be
associated with a trait . . .
Sex hormones within Fetuses of a particular genetic sex are exposed to
the uterus levels of sex hormones within the uterus that
are closer to the exposure levels typical of the
other genetic sex . . .
Adaptive design [A trait that] increases an individual’s ability to
survive and/or reproduce.
Hormone levels A trait is found to be associated with individuals’
circulating levels of a particular hormone. Figure 3. Political orientations.
Animal comparisons A trait is found to have close analogs in other
animal species. the result higher when accompanied with the information that
the allele was found in higher frequencies in populations with
African ancestry (Table 4). The difference actually approached
was good internal agreement among ratings within each parti- significance in the opposite direction than what was predicted
cipant, with a Cronbach’s a of .823 for positive results and .827 (n ¼ 70, t ¼ 1.77, p ¼ .081). Similarly, bioists did not rate the
for null results. For seven of the eight RDs, bioists’ ratings were inferential value of the result differently when the allele was
higher than those of skeptics for positive results. The effect was reported to occur in higher frequencies in populations of
not significant, however, using either a sum of standardized European ancestry (n ¼ 72, t ¼ 0.49, p ¼ .310). Thus,
ratings (bioists x ¼ 0.093, skeptics x ¼ 0.077, t ¼ 1.06, Hypotheses 3a and 3b are not supported.
p ¼ .292) or an MANOVA of standardized ratings (F ¼ Skeptics did not rate the results in the third condition as
0.91, p ¼ .517). Similarly, for seven of the eight RDs, skeptics’ having a higher IP when there was no mention of frequencies
ratings were higher than those of bioists for null results. The (n ¼ 111, t ¼ 0.45, p ¼ .653), nor did they do so when it was
effect attained one-tailed significance as a test of the summed reported that the allele occurred in higher frequencies in popu-
standardized ratings (bioists x ¼ 0.194, skeptics x ¼ 0.113, lations with European ancestry (n ¼ 120, t ¼ 1.57, p ¼ .119).
t ¼ 1.85, p ¼ .069) but did not as an MANOVA of standar- Thus, Hypotheses 4a and 4b were not supported.
dized ratings (F ¼ 1.59, p ¼ .151). Therefore, while both It should be noted that the ratings were quite variable with an
effects are in the predicted direction, Hypothesis 1a was not average coefficient of variation of 51%. As reference, the coef-
supported and there is only tentative support for Hypothesis 1b. ficient of variance for height in U.S. men is 8.5% and for weight,
it is 38% (McDowell, Fryar, Ogden, & Flegal, 2008). Sample
sizes ranged from 33 to 72 for single groups, and consequently,
Political Orientation effect sizes that could be detected with 80% likelihood tended to
The four questions referencing political orientation exhibited be of moderate size (ranging from 0.43 to 0.61). Many of the
fair agreement (a ¼ .79, n ¼ 365). These included the impor- reported relationships, however, were often in the opposite direc-
tance of reducing inequality, whether social position was deter- tion of those predicted, making Type II errors less likely.
mined by inherent traits versus social forces, and where
participants placed themselves on the social and the economic
political spectrum. Bioists and skeptics did not differ substan- Discussion
tially in an index consisting of the sum of these responses (bio- The only hypothesis to receive even marginal support was
ists, n ¼ 139, x ¼ 19.36; skeptics, n ¼ 225, x ¼ 19.64; t ¼ 0.53, Hypothesis 1b that bioists and skeptics differ in their inter-
p ¼ .596). Nor do the two groups differ along any individual pretation of null results of studies exploring biological influ-
response (Figure 3). Thus, Hypothesis 2 is not supported. ences on human psychological and behavioral traits.
Similarly, for seven of the eight study designs, bioists rated
the IP of positive results as being more inferentially powerful,
Confirmation Bias but the effect was not significant and Hypothesis 1a was not
In all three conditions, participants were shown the same supported. In the IP survey, bioists also rated the specific
figure. Bioists, however, did not rate the inferential value of (positive) results as having a higher inferential value overall,
8 Evolutionary Psychology

Table 4. Ratings of Inferential Values.

Condition n Mean SD Direction p Value (Different From Previous)

Bioists
Appetitive aggression higher in nonprivileged 37 53.84 32.45
No mention of frequency 35 71.09 26.01 Opposite .081
Appetitive aggression higher in privileged 35 57.31 27.26 Opposite .310
Skeptics
Appetitive aggression higher in nonprivileged 72 57.60 27.58
No mention of frequency 66 60.36 28.87 Predicted .653
Appetitive aggression higher in privileged 48 49.29 29.51 Opposite .119

after controlling for condition, adding some weight to did not rate the complexity or the social desirability of the
Hypothesis 1a (albeit post hoc weight). While such effects associated psychological traits in ways that allowed for mean-
are indicative of a true disagreement in the inferential value ingful comparisons. Future studies could rely on a pilot study
of such results, it is possible that they result from ontological which would include the ratings of a battery psychological
confirmation biases, as well—that is to say, confirmation traits so that comparable traits could be paired. It would be
biases that exist due to preexisting beliefs, irrespective of necessary to take into account the many factors, described
ideology (e.g., that flossing prevents gingivitis). earlier, that would make a trait more or less appropriate for a
Similar to findings reported in related studies (Lyle & Smith, such a study—psychological complexity, social desirability,
2012; Tybur et al., 2007), bioists and skeptics were not found to presence of existing literature on the topic, believability, and
differ in their political views, failing to support Hypothesis 2. so on. Fortunately, the tests that remained meaningful, those
The remaining hypotheses (Hypotheses 3a, 3b, 4a, and 4b) were focusing on appetitive aggression, were those that provided the
not supported either. Participants’ perceptions of inferential cleanest tests of ideological bias. In these, the only differences
value did not vary by the degree to which results supported a between treatments were small amounts of text that greatly
particular ideology, suggesting that ideological confirmation altered the degree to which the results conformed to the alleged
bias is not affecting participant perceptions of inferential value. ideological biases.
There are a number of potential shortcomings that could be The presentation of the mock results was perhaps more
addressed in future research. The high variance in reported successful. Twenty-nine percent of participants were com-
inferential values and the modest sample sizes lead to relatively pletely unaware that the results were fake, and an additional
low power in these tests, and thus, a null effect cannot be 26% were only a little suspicious. Despite this, only 15% of
declared with great confidence. For most tests, only modest participants believed that their responses were affected more
effect sizes could be detected with an 80% probability. Recruit- than a little by these suspicions. Fewer than 1% believed their
ing a sufficiently large sample of academics is not easy, and suspicions affected their responses “very much.”
while all participants in the present study had earned an Ultimately, this study did not find any evidence of confir-
advanced degree in a relevant field, the majority were not mation bias, despite the common allegations that have been
working in academia. Those who pursue a career path in aca- lodged by both sides for some time. Regardless of one’s posi-
demia might follow the literature more closely and have stron- tion, it is important to remind scholars that if they believe a
ger opinions on the role of biology and on equality, such that group of intelligent and informed academics could be so
ideological confirmation biases become more significant. In unknowingly blinded by ideology that they wholeheartedly
the IP sample, those working in research or teaching reported subscribe to an unquestionably erroneous interpretation of an
being more aware of the research relevant to the debate (5-point entire body of research, then they must acknowledge they
scale, academics: x ¼ 3.14, n ¼ 112, other: x ¼ 2.38, n ¼ 253, themselves are equally as capable of being so misguided. And
Wilcoxon Z ¼ 6.02, p < .001) and were more likely to choose while it is likely that biases do to some degree impact research-
one of the “extreme” options on the 4-point scale (1 or 4) ers’ interpretation of individual results and collective litera-
concerning biological versus experiential orientation (aca- tures, it is also very likely that the overarching debate is also
demics: extreme ¼ 22.32%, other: extreme ¼ 12.65%, w2 ¼ driven by true, lucid, well-informed disagreements about the
5.51, p ¼ .019). A similar RD relying solely on engaged aca- nature by which complex traits develop.
demics might therefore produce different results. For instance,
e-mail mailing lists from relevant academic societies could be Declaration of Conflicting Interests
sampled. These lists tend to number in the thousands, allowing The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
for the creation of a list large enough to overcome the low the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
response rates associated with cold requests.
As described in the Online Supplementary Material, the Funding
survey included assessments of mock results aimed at explor- The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for
ing interpretations of null effects as well. However, participants the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The open
Winking 9

access publishing fees for this article have been covered by the Texas Hagen, E. (2005). Controversial issues in Evolutionary Psychology.
A&M University Open Access to Knowledge Fund (OAK Fund), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 145–176). Hobo-
supported by the University Libraries and the Office of the Vice ken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
President for Research. Hamer, D. H., Hu, S., Magnuson, V. L., Hu, N., & Pattatucci, A. L.
(1993). A linkage between DNA markers on the X chromosome
Supplemental Material and male sexual orientation. Science, 261, 321–327.
Supplementary material for this article is available online. Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., & Gintis, H.
(Eds.). (2004). Foundations of human sociality: Economic experi-
References ments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societ-
Allen, E., Beckwith, B., Beckwith, J., Chorover, S., Culver, D., Dun- ies. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
can, M, . . . Schreier, H. (1975). Against “Sociobiology” (pp. Horowitz, M., & Yaworsky, W. (2014). Whither the blank slate? A
43–44). The New York Review of Books. report on the reception of evolutionary biological ideas among
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W., & Matthews, D. (2002). Adapta- sociological theorists. Sociological Spectrum, 34, 489–509.
tionism, exaptationism, and evolutionary behavioral science. Jonas, E., Schulz-Hardt, S., Frey, D., & Thelen, N. (2001). Confirma-
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 534–553. Retrieved from tion bias in sequential information search after preliminary deci-
<Go to ISI>://WOS:000183687600056 sions: An expansion of dissonance theoretical research on selective
Berinsky, A. J., Margolis, M. F., & Sances, M. W. (2014). Separating exposure to information. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
the shirkers from the workers? Making sure respondents pay atten- chology, 80, 557.
tion on self-administered surveys. American Journal of Political Kushnick, G., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2011). Karo Batak cousin mar-
Science, 58, 739–753. riage, cosocialization, and the Westermarck hypothesis. Current
Buller, D. (2006). Adapting minds: Evolutionary psychology and the Anthropology, 52, 443–448. doi:10.1086/659337
persistent quest for human nature. Cambridge: MIT Press. Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., Casey, R. J., Ritter, J. M., Rieser-
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Danner, L. A., & Jenkins, V. Y. (1987). Infant preferences for
Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and attractive faces: Rudiments of a stereotype? Developmental Psy-
Brain Sciences, 12, 1–14. chology, 23, 363.
Campos, J. J., Bertenthal, B. I., & Kermoian, R. (1992). Early expe- Leavitt, G. C. (2007). The incest taboo? A reconsideration of Wester-
rience and emotional development: The emergence of wariness of marck. Anthropological Theory, 7, 393–419. doi:10.1177/
heights. Psychological Science, 3, 61–64. 1463499607083427
Chapais, B. (2008). Primeval kinship: How pair-bonding gave birth to Lebow, R. N. (2013). You can’t keep a bad idea down: Evolutionary
human society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. biology and international relations. International Politics Reviews,
Coyne, J. (2012). Is evolutionary psychology worthless?. Why Evolu- 1, 2–10.
tion is True. Retrieved from https://whyevolutionistrue.wordpress. Lehner, P. E., Adelman, L., Cheikes, B. A., & Brown, M. J. (2008). Con-
com/2012/12/10/is-evolutionary-psychology-worthless/. firmation bias in complex analyses. IEEE Transactions on Systems,
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic Man, and Cybernetics Part A: Systems and Humans, 38, 584–592.
and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Lyle, H. F., & Smith, E. A. (2012). How conservative are evolutionary
Psychology, 56, 5. anthropologists? Human Nature, 23, 306–322.
Dixon, T. L. (2008). Crime news and racialized beliefs: Understanding Marks, J. (2012). The biological myth of human evolution. Contem-
the relationship between local news viewing and perceptions of porary Social Science, 7, 139–165.
African Americans and crime. Journal of Communication, 58, McDowell, M. A., Fryar, C. D., Ogden, C. L., & Flegal, K. M.
106–125. (2008). Anthropometric reference data for children and adults:
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in United States, 2003–2006. National Health Statistics Reports, 10.
human behavior—Evolved dispositions versus social roles. McKinnon, S., & Silverman, S. (2005a). Introduction. In Complex-
American Psychologist, 54, 408–423. ities: Beyond nature and nurture (pp. 1–20). Chicago, IL: Univer-
El-Guindi, F., & Read, D. (2012). Westermarck hypothesis reconsid- sity of Chicago Press.
ered. Current Anthropology, 53, 134–135. doi:10.1086/663576 McKinnon, S., & Silverman, S. (Eds.). (2005b). Complexities: Beyond
Fuentes, A. (2012). Race, monogamy, and other lies they told you: nature & nurture. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Busting myths about human nature. Berkeley: University of Cali- Mehta, P. H., Jones, A. C., & Josephs, R. A. (2008). The social endo-
fornia Press. crinology of dominance: Basal testosterone predicts cortisol
Gettler, L. T., McDade, T. W., Feranil, A. B., & Kuzawa, C. W. changes and behavior following victory and defeat. Journal of
(2011). Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testoster- Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1078.
one in human males. Proceedings of the National Academy of Munafò, M. R., Yalcin, B., Willis-Owen, S. A., & Flint, J. (2008).
Sciences, 108, 16194–16199. Association of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene and
Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco approach-related personality traits: Meta-analysis and new data.
and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist pro- Biological Psychiatry, 63, 197–206.
gramme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenom-
Sciences, 205, 581–598. enon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175–220.
10 Evolutionary Psychology

Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease- Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2013). Myside bias,
avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303. rational thinking, and intelligence. Current Directions in Psycho-
Pavelka, M. M. (2002). Resistance to the cross-species perspective in logical Science, 22, 259–264.
anthropology. Cambridge Studies in Biological and Evolutionary Sussman, R. W. (2013). Why the Legend of the Killer Ape Never
Anthropology, 25–44. Dies. War, Peace, and Human Nature: The Convergence of Evolu-
Pinker, S. (2002). The blank slate. New York, NY: Viking. tionary and Cultural Views (pp. 97).
Plavcan, J. M. (2012). Sexual size dimorphism, canine dimorphism, Taber, C. S., & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the eva-
and male–male competition in primates. Human Nature, 23, luation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science,
45–67. 50, 755–769.
Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Thompson, R. A., & Newton, E. K. (2013). Baby altruists? Examining
Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., & Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta- the complexity of prosocial motivation in young children. Infancy,
analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of 18, 120–133.
twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47, 702–709. Tybur, J. M., Miller, G. F., & Gangestad, S. W. (2007). Testing the
Proctor, D., Williamson, R. A., de Waal, F. B., & Brosnan, S. F. controversy—An empirical examination of adaptationists’ atti-
(2013). Chimpanzees play the ultimatum game. Proceedings of the tudes toward politics and science. Human Nature—An Interdis-
National Academy of Sciences, 110, 2070–2075. ciplinary Biosocial Perspective, 18, 313–328. doi:10.1007/
Rose, S., & Rose, H. (2001). Alas, poor Darwin: Arguments against s12110-007-9024-y
evolutionary psychology. London, England: Cape. Winking, J., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Stieglitz, J. (2009). The goals
Sanderson, S., & Ellis, E. (1992). Theoretical and political perspec- of direct paternal care among a South Amerindian population.
tives of American sociologists in the 1990s. The American Sociol- American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 139, 295–304.
ogist, 23, 26–42. Wolfe, P. (2006). Settler colonialism and the elimination of the native.
Sayer, A. (1997). Essentialism, social constructionism, and beyond. Journal of Genocide Research, 8, 387–409.
The Sociological Review, 45, 453–487. Wrangham, R. W., & Glowacki, L. (2012). Intergroup aggression in
Segerstrale, U. (2000). Defenders of the truth: The battle for science in chimpanzees and war in nomadic hunter-gatherers. Human Nature,
the sociobiology debate and beyond. New York, NY: Oxford Uni- 23, 5–29.
versity Press. Wu, H., Corney, J., & Grant, M. (2014). Relationship between quality
Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Hypothesis-testing processes in and payment in crowdsourced design. Proceedings of the 2014
social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, IEEE 18th International Conference on Computer Supported
36, 1202. Cooperative Work in Design (CSCWD).

You might also like