You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/263918219

Creative Mindsets: Measurement, Correlates, Consequences

Article  in  Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts · February 2014


DOI: 10.1037/a0034898

CITATIONS READS
145 11,487

1 author:

Maciej Karwowski
University of Wroclaw
184 PUBLICATIONS   3,096 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Creativity and Domains View project

Social consequences of COVID-19 View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Maciej Karwowski on 01 December 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2013 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 8, No. 1, 62–70 1931-3896/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0034898

Creative Mindsets: Measurement, Correlates, Consequences

Maciej Karwowski
Academy of Special Education

This article describes a research program exploring the structure, correlates, and consequences of creative
mindsets, defined as beliefs about the fixed-versus-growth nature of creativity. In the first study, I
reported on the development of the Creative Mindset Scale. Exploratory factor analysis demonstrated a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

two-factor structure with growth- and fixed-creative mindsets forming two relatively independent, yet
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

negatively correlated scales, rather than two ends of one continuum. I then verified this structure by
confirmatory factor analysis using a larger sample. In the second study, the growth mindset and the
creative self-concept (creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity) were strongly positively
associated. In the third study, the fixed-creative mindset was negatively related to efficiency in solving
insight problems, and an association between the growth mindset and problem solving was moderated by
the fixed mindset.

Keywords: creative mindsets, fixed and growth mindset, creative self-efficacy, creative personal identity,
item response theory

Was Albert Einstein creative because of an innate gift or did his goals, and school satisfaction (Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995;
creativity develop as a result of his well-known curiosity? Were Dweck & Leggett, 1988). Even young children were identified as
Edison’s patents possible due to his innate talent or to his famous having a fixed-versus-a-growth perception of their abilities, which
“99% percent of perspiration?” Is it possible to develop creative influences their functioning (Mueller & Dweck, 1998).
potential, as intervention studies suggest (Dziedziewicz, Oledzka, Analyses of mindsets are rarely provided in the creativity liter-
& Karwowski, 2013), or must one be born with creative qualities ature (Makel, 2008; O’Connor, Nemeth, & Akutsu, 2013). Al-
to achieve greatness? These questions are central to a discussion of though there are findings implicitly connected with this research
the roots and nature of creativity, but have been relatively rarely area, only individual studies directly measuring creative mindsets
posited in the creativity literature to date, which is surprising. have been presented to date; similarly, the correlates or conse-
Although there are dozens of studies involving implicit theories of quences of different creative mindsets have been studied only
creativity (see Andiliou & Murphy, 2010, for a review), these sparsely. Such studies have revealed people’s beliefs about the
theories usually are not analyzed by creativity researchers due to fixed or growth nature of creativity in their implicit theories;
the perceived nature of creativity—the so-called creative mindset, several have provided arguments that creativity can be perceived
defined as a set of beliefs associated with the nature of creativity, as a malleable construct (Maksić & Pavlović, 2011; Seng, Keung,
particularly beliefs about the stable-versus-malleable character of & Cheng, 2008; Sen & Sharma, 2011).
creativity. The main aim of this paper was to fill the above-mentioned gap,
People perceive the nature of their own characteristics differ- specifically by: (a) developing an instrument to measure creative
ently, viewing abilities, motivations, and personality traits either as mindsets; (b) exploring whether growth and fixed mindsets are two
fixed (stable) or malleable (able to grow or to decline). Since the extremes of the same continuum (i.e., one factor), or whether they
1950s (Heider, 1958; Kelly, 1955), social– cognitive and educa- could possibly be held simultaneously (i.e., two factors); and (c)
tional psychology have demonstrated that holding a fixed-versus- showing selected and potentially theoretically sound correlates of
a-growth mindset regarding one’s abilities translates into the ef- these mindsets, and the consequences of holding different mind-
fectiveness of functioning, well-being, attributional styles, learning sets, as deduced from the existing literature. I was, therefore,
especially interested here in the structure of the creative mindset,
the correlates of different mindsets with creative self-concept
variables, creative self-efficacy (CSE) and creative personal iden-
This article was published Online First December 23, 2013. tity (CPI), and the relationships between mindsets and solving
Maciej Karwowski, Creative Education Lab, Department of Educational insight problems.
Studies, Academy of Special Education, Warsaw, Poland.
I thank Vlad Glăveanu for his helpful comments on the previous version
of this article. Creative Mindset
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Maciej
Karwowski, Creative Education Lab, Department of Educational Studies, I have defined the creative mindset as beliefs about the stable-
Academy of Special Education, Szczesliwicka Street, 40, 02-353 Warsaw, versus-malleable character and the nature of creativity. Although
Poland. E-mail: maciek.karwowski@gmail.com there is a wide range of work on implicit theories of creativity from
62
CREATIVE MINDSETS 63

both experts (Elsbach & Kramer, 2003; Glück, Ernst, & Unger, and possible different levels of analysis (little-c, mini-c, pro-c,
2002; Spiel & von Korff, 1998; Sternberg, 1995) and laypeople Big-C, etc.), it is likely that people can hold both an entity and an
(Chan & Chan, 1999; Runco & Johnson, 2002), these implicit incremental view of creativity. On the one hand, they may be
theories have rarely been studied in terms of the perceived genesis convinced that famous creators with great achievements are stim-
of creative abilities and the possibility of the development of ulated by an inborn power; on the other, they may see putting in
creativity (Maksić & Pavlović, 2011). Given that creativity is a effort as a way to possibly increase their own creative potentials.
complex phenomenon usually analyzed from different perspec- Hence, one of the aims of the studies presented here is to consider
tives, such as the four Ps (Rhodes, 1961), the five As (Glăveanu, whether fixed- and growth-creative mindsets (or incremental- and
2013), and the various levels between little-c and Big-C creativity entity-creativity theories) are different parts of the same contin-
(Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009), it is worthwhile to test how people uum, or whether it is possible to adhere to both an incremental and
perceive its nature and roots. an entity theory of creativity.
It has been demonstrated in several studies that even laypeople The hypothetical reasons for expecting two separate creative
are able to spontaneously distinguish between Big-C and little-c mindsets (growth and fixed) rather than growth and fixed ends
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

creativity. Karwowski (2009) has demonstrated that laypeople of a continuum of one mindset stem from differences in the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

often describe themselves as “little-c creative,” but not “Big-C perceived importance of creativity. People who treat creativity
creative.” The correlation between such self-descriptions, although as unimportant to their self-description and identity, and are
statistically significant, was not particularly strong (r ⫽ .46), thus characterized by low CPI and creative-role identity (Tier-
showing the relative independence of these two constructs. People ney & Farmer, 2002; Karwowski, 2012), probably will not care
may not only distinguish between different levels of creativity too much whether creativity is malleable or fixed. On the other
when describing themselves but also hold qualitatively distinct hand, the understanding of creativity among people whose CPI
“theories” of different levels of creativity—that is, little-c creativ- is well developed may be more complex. People who are
ity correlated strongly with self-esteem and perceiving oneself as professionally engaged in creative activity hold implicit theo-
a successful person and Big-C creativity correlated with fixed ries of creativity similar to those of laypeople (Glück et al.,
traits such as being gifted and talented (Karwowski, 2009). 2002), but their descriptions of creativity are usually more
The fixed-versus-growth (malleable) creativity mindsets and complex and elaborate. Both Sternberg (1985) and Glück et al.
their consequences have been studied by Makel (2008) and (2002) have demonstrated the complexity of expert definitions.
O’Connor et al. (2013). Makel (2008), following the procedure Whereas laypeople usually define creativity using a person-
employed by Dweck et al. (1995), did not find differences in centered approach (Pavlović, Maksić, & Bodroza, 2013), ex-
creative problem solving among entity and incremental (people perts highlight the role of person, process, product, and press
holding the fixed mindset and people holding the growth mindset, approaches (Glück et al., 2002), as well as the role of prepara-
respectively) theorists of creativity. O’Connor et al. (2013) mea- tion and effort (Amabile, 2001). Elsbach and Kramer (2003),
sured the fixed/growth mindset using a five-item Likert scale, with
have demonstrated that studio executives and producers in
one end of the continuum constituting the fixed, and the other the
Hollywood assess the potential of screenwriters during pitches
growth, mindset. The growth-creative mindset correlated substan-
by comparing them with one of the seven creative prototypes.
tially with a malleable theory of intelligence (r ⫽ .50, which
These prototypes (artist, storyteller, show runner, neophyte,
translates into r ⫽ .59 after correction for attenuation). Endorse-
journeyman, dealmaker, and nonwriter) differ in terms of their
ment of an entity theory translated into a lower interest in creative
creative potential and persuasiveness during the pitch. Although
thinking, lower self-reported creativity, and divergent thinking
not analyzed in terms of mindset, the complexity of these types
production. Another study demonstrated negative associations be-
suggests that experts may hold both a fixed mindset (i.e.,
tween entity theory and creative achievements. When incremental
perceiving the creative person as an artist type) and a growth
creativity was primed, participants behaved significantly more
mindset.
creatively than when entity theory was primed. These findings
shed light on the potentially important role played by the fixed-
versus-malleable creative mindsets and their role in motivating or The Present Studies
demotivating people to perform creative actions. When people
perceive creativity as a fixed trait, they may encounter problems Three studies were conducted to develop a method of measuring
such as rationalizations about why they should engage in activities creative mindsets and to establish (a) its factor structure (Study 1),
requiring creative thinking. An incremental mindset increases the (b) its associations with creative self-concept variables (Study 2),
probability of engagement in creative tasks and, consequently, and (c) its efficiency in solving insight problems (Study 3). In
creative solutions during problem-solving activities. addition, aggregated data from Studies 2 and 3 were used to
The one limitation of both studies into the creative mindset is examine the generalizability of the factor structure obtained during
related to the measurement instruments used. Both O’Connor et al. Study 1, while aggregated data from Studies 1 through 3 were used
(2013) and Makel (2008) used short scales, inspired by the work of to examine the Creative Mindset Scale (CMS, which was designed
Dweck and her colleagues (Dweck et al., 1995; Dweck & Leggett, for this study) using item-response theory. The three studies pre-
1988). These scales usually consist of items like: “You have a sented below were independent, but all were conducted (with
certain amount of intelligence (creativity) and you really can’t do 50,000 Poles altogether, a representative nationwide sample of
much to change it” or “You are stuck with whatever amount of Internet users) by an online panel from the Millward Brown
intelligence (creativity) you are born with.” Given the complex Poland (Warsaw, Poland) research company, whose members take
nature of creativity, however, especially its multifaceted character part in various research programs once or twice per year.
64 KARWOWSKI

Study 1: CMS: Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) with the results of an item-response-theory model estimated to
assess both the test-information functions of the scales and their
The aim of the first study was to develop a short yet psycho-
standard measurement errors after aggregation of the data from
metrically sound instrument to measure creative mindsets. An
Studies 1 to 3.
initial pool of 25 items covering both malleable and entity theories
Descriptive statistics of all items as well as the results of the
of creativity was generated and discussed by a group of five
EFA (with the maximum likelihood method and oblimin rotation)
experts in creativity. The redundant items were deleted and, after
are presented in Table 1.
several changes, 10 items were selected for further analysis.
Almost all items were normally distributed (except Item 5, in
which agree answers predominated). EFA suggested that the
Method fixed- and the growth-creative mindsets constituted two separate
Participants. In total, 492 Poles participated in Study 1; the factors rather than two ends of one continuum. Five items loaded
sample included slightly more women (n ⫽ 291; 59%) than men onto a growth-mindset factor and another five loaded onto a
(n ⫽ 201, 41%). Participant ages ranged from 16 to 60 (M ⫽ fixed-creative mindset. These two factors together explained 50%
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

32.71, SD ⫽ 11.08). Almost half of all participants held a college of the total variance.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

or university degree (48%), 40% had completed higher education, When items from EFA were averaged to form two scales, the
8% held secondary degrees, and 4% held vocational degrees. two mindsets were negatively correlated, and participants held
Measure: The creative mindset. A 10-item scale was used to stronger growth than fixed mindsets (see Table 2).
measure participant perception of the nature of creativity. Answers
were measured on a 5-point Likert scale (1 ⫽ definitely not, 5 ⫽
definitely yes). All items with descriptive statistics are presented in Discussion
Table 1. Although people usually are characterized as being either entity
Procedure. Participants were treated in accordance with the
or incremental theorists (Dweck, 2006), findings from the first
ethical guidelines set out by the American Psychological Associ-
study suggest that, in the specific case of creativity, it is possible
ation (2009). They were not rewarded for participating and were
to hold both a fixed and a growth mindset. These two implicit
informed that they could withdraw at any time.
theories (incremental vs. entity) were negatively correlated; how-
ever, the strength of this association was weak, suggesting their
Results relative independence rather than providing evidence that they
The data were analyzed in two steps. First, the descriptive were two ends of one continuum. Further studies should analyze
statistics of all items were computed for an initial analysis of their the implicit theories behind both mindsets, as it can be speculated
distribution. Second, EFA was conducted, as it was not clear that people may hold different implicit theories of Big-C and
whether a one- or two-factor solution should be expected. The little-c creativity. When they perceive Big-C creativity in a fixed
structure obtained was then tested by confirmatory factor analysis manner, but also define little-c creativity as a malleable trait, it is
(CFA) on a larger sample composed of participants from Studies 2 possible that having a fixed Big-C mindset would not harm their
and 3. CFA results are presented in the last part of the article, along efficiency or self-perception. Thus, in the second study, I consid-

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Factor Loadings of Items From the Creative Mindsets Scale (Study 1)

Growth-mindset Fixed-mindset
Items M SD Skewness Kurtosis factor (30%) factor (20%)

(1) Everyone can create something great at some point if he or she is


given appropriate conditions 3.70 .98 ⫺.71 ⫺.07 .55 ⫺.22
(2) You either are creative or you are not—even trying very hard you
cannot change much 2.84 1.06 .10 ⫺.88 ⫺.22 .72
(3) Anyone can develop his or her creative abilities up to a certain level 3.64 .92 ⫺.81 .31 .38 .13
(4) You have to be born a creator—without innate talent you can only
be a scribbler 2.90 1.05 .06 ⫺.81 ⫺.16 .68
(5) Practice makes perfect—perseverance and trying hard are the best
ways to develop and expand one’s capabilities 4.18 .75 ⫺1.05 2.16 .57 ⫺.06
(6) Creativity can be developed, but one either is or is not a truly
creative person 3.58 .94 ⫺.59 ⫺.12 .05 .53
(7) Rome wasn’t built in a day—each creativity requires effort and
work, and these two are more important than talent 3.79 .93 ⫺.65 ⫺.08 .55 ⫺.12
(8) Some people are creative, others aren’t—and no practice can change
it 2.87 1.07 .12 ⫺.90 ⫺.19 .70
(9) It doesn’t matter what creativity level one reveals—you can always
increase it 3.60 .88 ⫺.66 .22 .60 ⫺.29
(10) A truly creative talent is innate and constant throughout one’s entire
life 3.01 1.11 ⫺.02 ⫺.98 ⫺.07 .50
Note. Structure matrix is presented in case of exploratory factor analysis results.
CREATIVE MINDSETS 65

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics and Differences Between Growth and Fixed Mindsets

Mindset M SD Skewness Kurtosis Cronbach’s ␣ Paired sample t test d (95% CI)


ⴱⴱⴱ
Growth mindset 3.78 0.58 ⫺0.57 1.16 .65 t(491) ⫽ ⫺15.92 0.72 (0.62–0.82)
Fixed mindset 3.04 0.75 ⫺0.05 ⫺0.11 .76
Note. Growth-mindset scale was calculated by averaging items 1, 3, 5, 7, and 9 from Table 1, and fixed-mindset scale was calculated by averaging items
2, 4, 6, 7, and 10 from Table 1.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001.

ered whether and how the two mindsets might be associated with important to me; (10) Creativity is an important part of me; (11)
the creative self-concept constructs, CSE and CPI. Ingenuity is a characteristic which is important to me. Each of the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

statements on both scales were measured on a 5-point Likert scale


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Study 2: Creative Mindset and Creative Self-Concept (1 ⫽ definitely not, 5 ⫽ definitely yes). In this study, the internal
consistency of both scales was high: CSE ␣ ⫽ .87, CPI ␣ ⫽ .85.
Creative self-concept characteristics are of increasing interest In previous studies, both scales were characterized by high reli-
among creativity scholars. CSE is associated with creative abilities ability and validity (Karwowski et al., in press).
and self-description (Karwowski, 2011), optimism (Li & Wu, Procedure. The procedure was the same as was employed in
2011), teacher behavior (Beghetto, 2006), and curiosity (Kar- Study 1. All study materials were completed online. The partici-
wowski, 2012). CSE often is studied together with CPI, which is pants were not rewarded and were informed that they could with-
the belief that creativity is an important element of self-description draw at any time.
(Farmer, Tierney, & Kung-McIntyre, 2003; Jaussi, Randel, &
Dionne, 2007). Plucker and Makel (2010) argue that CPI is also an
Results
appreciation of creativity and a positive attitude toward it. As
self-concept variables are much more dynamic and malleable than The results were analyzed in two steps. First, I calculated
“hard core” personality traits (Asendorpf & van Aken, 2003; descriptive statistics and correlations between both mindsets and
Marsh, Trautwein, Ludtke, Koller, & Baumert, 2006), I hypothe- creative self-concept variables (see Table 3). Then, I fit a structural
sized that a creative self-concept (a higher level factor of CSE and equation model with the two creative mindsets explaining a
CPI) is positively associated with a growth-creative mindset; how- higher-order creative self-concept latent factor (composed of CSE
ever, I had no expectations concerning a relationship between a and CPI).
fixed-creative mindset and a creative self-concept. As hypothesized, the creative-growth mindset was substantially
related to both CSE and CPI, whereas the fixed mindset was not
Method associated with creative self-concept variables.
A structural equation model, controlling for construct unreli-
Participants. In total, 296 Poles participated in Study 2; the ability and allowing the modeling of relationships between higher
sample included 145 women (49%) and 151 men (51%). Partici- order creative self-concept and creative mindsets, is presented in
pant ages ranged from 17 to 60 with M ⫽ 34.89 and SD ⫽ 12.10. Figure 1.
Almost half of all participants held a college or university degree The model fit the data well, ␹2(df ⫽ 182) ⫽ 338.60, p ⬍ .001,
(46%), 41% had completed higher education, 7% held vocational ␹2/df ⫽ 1.86, CFI ⫽ .94, SRMR ⫽ .06, RMSEA ⫽ .054, 90% CI
degrees, and 6% held secondary degrees. [.045, .063]. Both mindsets were associated with creative self-
Measures. concept variables, explaining 27% of their variance. A growth
The creative mindset. The same 10-item CMS employed in mindset was substantially related to creative self-concept (␤ ⫽ .55,
Study 1 was used. The internal consistency of the fixed-mindset p ⬍ .001), but a fixed mindset also was associated with it (␤ ⫽ .25,
scale was satisfactory (␣ ⫽ .79); the growth-mindset scale was p ⫽ .001).
moderately reliable (␣ ⫽ .65).
The creative self-concept. The Short Scale of Creative Self
(SSCS) (Karwowski, 2012; Karwowski, Lebuda, & Wisniewska, Table 3
in press) was used to measure the CSE and the CPI. The SSCS is Correlations Between Two Mindsets and Creative Self-Efficacy
composed of 11 items—six measuring CSE and five measuring and Creative Personal Identity (Study 2)
CPI. CSE is described by the following statements on the SSCS:
(3) I know I can efficiently solve even complicated problems; (4) Variables M SD 1 2 3 4
I trust my creative abilities; (5) Compared with my friends, I am ⴱ ⴱⴱ
1. Growth mindset 3.72 0.63 (.65) ⫺.20 .33 .29ⴱⴱ
distinguished by my imagination and ingenuity; (6) I have proved 2. Fixed mindset 3.16 0.82 (.79) .08 .07
many times that I can cope with difficult situations; (8) I am sure 3. CSE 3.65 0.67 (.87) .78ⴱⴱ
I can deal with problems requiring creative thinking; (9) I am good 4. CPI 3.66 0.67 (.85)
at proposing original solutions to problems. The statements de- Note. CSE ⫽ creative self-efficacy; CPI ⫽ creative personal identity;
scribing CPI were: (1) I think I am a creative person; (2) My N ⫽ 296; Cronbach’s alpha is on the diagonal in parentheses.

creativity is important for who I am; (7) Being a creative person is p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
66 KARWOWSKI
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. Types of mindset predicting creative self-concept.

Discussion pant ages ranged from 16 to 60, with M ⫽ 35.64 and SD ⫽ 11.84.
The majority of participants held high school (41%) or college
Consistent with the hypothesis, perceiving creativity as a mal- degrees (43%), 8% held vocational degrees, and 8% held second-
leable characteristic translated into higher levels of CSE and CPI. ary degrees.
This result replicates earlier findings (O’Connor et al., 2013) that Measures.
incremental creativity theorists rate themselves as more creative The creative mindset. The same 10-item CMS employed in
than entity theorists do; similar hypotheses also have been formu- Study 1 and Study 2 was used in this study. The internal consis-
lated in the past (Karwowski, 2009). This strong relationship with tency of the fixed mindset scale was satisfactory (␣ ⫽ .78), as was
a growth mindset sheds light on possible conditions for the devel- the growth-mindset scale (␣ ⫽ .73).
opment of CSE and CPI, and may have important educational The insight problems. Eight insight problems were adminis-
consequences. People who believe that creativity is conditioned by tered in counterbalanced order (see Appendix). The reliability of
effort rather than an inborn quality tend to perceive themselves as this scale was moderate (␣ ⫽ .68), but due to the scoring of 0 to
more creative. The open question remains whether their beliefs 1, it was probably underestimated. Confirmatory factor analysis
may engender higher creative achievement or efficiency in prob- showed the almost perfect fit of a one-factor model, ␹2(df ⫽ 14) ⫽
lem solving (O’Connor et al., 2013). I tested this hypothesis in the 9.01, p ⫽ .83, ␹2/df ⫽ .64, GFI ⫽ .99, AGFI ⫽ .98, CFI ⫽ 1.00,
third study. SRMR ⫽ .005; RMSEA ⫽ .000, 90% CI [.000, .034] and latent
factor reliability (Raykov & Hancock, 2005) was good (H ⫽ .72).
Study 3: Creative Mindsets and Insight-Problem
Procedure. The procedure was the same as was employed in
Solving the two previous studies. The same Internet panel was used, but no
Study 3 was conducted to test relationships between creative participants in Study 1 or 2 participated in Study 3. Participants
mindsets and effectiveness in solving insight problems. Specifi- completed the CMS and then solved insight problems.
cally, I examined (a) whether creative mindsets predict insight-
problem solving and (b) the role of the coincidence of both Results
mindsets in predicting the effectiveness of problem solving.
After initial analysis of the descriptive statistics and correlations
O’Connor et al. (2013) showed a positive relationship between
between study variables (see Table 4), I created a regression model
incremental mindset and problem solving, and so I hypothesized
predicting insight-problem solving by growth and fixed mindsets,
positive associations between a growth mindset and problem solv-
as well as by their interaction (see Table 5).
ing and negative associations between a fixed mindset and prob-
Consistent with the hypothesis, a fixed mindset correlated neg-
lem solving. The possible interactive effects of both mindsets were
atively (albeit weakly) with effectiveness of insight-problem solv-
treated as exploratory.
ing, whereas a growth mindset was associated with insight-
problem solving positively, albeit also weakly.
Method
After controlling for associations between mindsets in the re-
Participants. In total, 403 Poles participated in Study 3; the gression model, only the fixed mindset continued its negative
sample included 211 women (52%) and 192 men (48%). Partici- association with insight-problem solving; the growth-mindset co-
CREATIVE MINDSETS 67

Table 4
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Between Growth and Fixed Mindsets and Effectiveness in
Solving Insight Problems (Study 3)

Variables Min Max M SD 1 2 3

1. Growth mindset 1 5 3.71 0.66 (.73) ⫺.12ⴱ .10ⴱ


2. Fixed mindset 1 5 3.14 0.78 (.78) ⫺.12ⴱ
3. Insight problems 0 1 .42 .28 (.72)
Note. N ⫽ 403. Cronbach’s alpha is on the diagonal in parentheses.

p ⬍ .05.

efficient was no longer statistically significant. The cross-product ␹2(df ⫽ 34) ⫽ 161.88, p ⬍ .001, ␹2/df ⫽ 4.76, GFI ⫽ .96, AGFI ⫽
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

term introduced in the second step demonstrated an increase in the .93, CFI ⫽ .92, SRMR ⫽ .06, RMSEA ⫽ .073, 90% CI [.062, .085];
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

explained variance (⌬R2 ⫽ .036, p ⬍ .001) and a highly significant see Figure 3. Although the chi-square test value was statistically
interaction (see Figure 2). significant, it is well-known that this test is overly powerful when
The growth mindset was positively related to effectiveness in applied to large samples (Bentler, 1990). A one-factor CFA model fit
solving insight problems among participants with a low fixed mind- the data poorly, ␹2(df ⫽ 35) ⫽ 649.50, p ⬍ .001, ␹2/df ⫽ 18.56,
set—the simple slope: b ⫽ .08, SE ⫽ .02, p ⬍ .001—whereas, among GFI ⫽ .79, AGFI ⫽ .68, CFI ⫽ .62, SRMR ⫽ .13, RMSEA ⫽ .16
people with a high fixed mindset, there was a tendency toward 90% CI [.148, .169]; its fit was significantly worse than the two-factor
negative associations between a growth mindset and problem solv- model (⌬␹2 ⫽ 487.62; ⌬df ⫽ 1, p ⬍ .001). The latent factors’
ing—the simple slope: b ⫽ ⫺.04, SE ⫽ .03, p ⫽ .15). reliabilities (Raykov & Hancock, 2005; Silvia, 2011) were good for
growth mindset (H ⫽ .73) and very good for fixed mindset (H ⫽ .81).
Discussion In the last step, using the IRT methodology, and specifically, the
Rash Rating Scale Model (RRSM; Wright & Masters, 1982), I ex-
This study demonstrated the inhibiting effects of a fixed mindset on amined the distribution of test-information functions and standard
creative problem solving. In addition, a fixed mindset moderated the errors of measurement for both scales (the fixed and the growth
relationship between a growth mindset and creative problem solving. mindsets) to assess which scale more efficiently measured the fixed
Only among people with a low fixed mindset was a growth mindset and the growth mindset at the different levels of latent traits. The
positively related to this efficiency. This study also provides an information function and the standard errors of measurement are
argument in favor of looking for simultaneous consideration of both shown in Figure 4.
mindsets while analyzing creativity. Perhaps people with both a low The fixed-mindset scale measured the latent trait (␪) more
growth and a low fixed mindset do not really care what creativity is efficiently at an average level, whereas the growth-mindset
and thus are not motivated to solve creative problems. On the other scale measured it more efficiently at lower levels. The standard
hand, it is still possible that people can hold both mindsets, believing error distribution is characterized by a U-shaped curve, which is
that little-c creativity may be developed and requires effort, whereas typical of IRT—the reliability of the measurement is highest at
Big-C creativity is connected with a more fixed-creative mindset. medium levels of ␪ and the measurement errors increase at very
low and very high levels of the latent traits.
Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Item Response
Theory (IRT) Properties of CMS
To corroborate the obtained CMS structure, I conducted a confir-
matory factor analysis on an aggregated sample of participants from
Studies 2 and 3 (N ⫽ 699). According to the commonly applied
indexes (Hu & Bentler, 1999), the two-factor model fit the data well,

Table 5
Predicting Effectiveness in Solving Insight Problems by Growth
and Fixed Mindsets and Their Interaction (Study 3)

Predictors B (SE) ␤ R2 ⌬R2

Step 1 .02ⴱⴱ
Growth mindset 0.036 (.021) 0.085†
Fixed mindset ⫺0.039 (.018) ⫺0.11ⴱ
Step 2 .058ⴱⴱⴱ .036ⴱⴱⴱ
Growth mindset 0.017 (.021) 0.04
Fixed mindset ⫺0.041 (.018) ⫺0.12ⴱ
Growth ⫻ Fixed ⫺0.074 (.019) ⫺0.20ⴱⴱⴱ
Figure 2. Growth and fixed mindset predicting effectiveness in solving
ⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ

p ⬍ .10. p ⬍ .05. p ⬍ .01. p ⬍ .001. insight problems.
68 KARWOWSKI

from knowledge about creativity, implicit theories of creativity, or


both.
This study also demonstrated that the two mindsets have predicted
creative self-concepts and efficiency in solving insight problems dif-
ferently, thus providing an additional rationale for a separate analysis
of growth and fixed mindsets. As hypothesized, a growth mindset was
strongly related to the creative self-concept latent trait (composed of
CSE and CPI), but a fixed mindset was also positively (yet weakly)
associated with this factor. Given that the active and dynamic char-
acter of the creative self-concept is comprised of and treated as
malleable characteristics (Asendorpf & van Aken, 2003), the positive
associations with creative self-concept were expected. CSE is a dy-
namic, malleable belief (Karwowski, 2011), more strongly connected
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Figure 3. A two-factor structure of the Creative Mindset Scale—results with curiosity (Karwowski, 2012) than with more fixed personality
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of confirmatory factor analysis. traits (Karwowski, Lebuda, Wisniewska, & Gralewski, 2013).
Equally interesting, but also potentially problematic is the observed
association between a fixed mindset and a creative self-concept.
Although weaker, this relationship also was statistically significant,
General Discussion
calling for further exploration and theoretical explanation. It may be
Decades of research into sociocognitive theory (Bandura, 1997) speculated that creative self-concept, associated with an appreciation
have demonstrated that people’s beliefs about the nature of their for creativity, translates into higher awareness and knowledge about
psychological characteristics play an important regulatory role, influ- the complex nature of creativity, hence showing that it may be both
encing both their motivations and their behaviors (Dweck, 1999). This malleable and stable. On the other hand, this finding—and generally
area of research is underdeveloped in the creativity literature, however the role of creative self-concept for understanding creative mindsets—
(Makel, 2008; O’Connor et al., 2013). Although there are several requires further study.
works on implicit theories of creativity (Andiliou & Murphy, 2010), The third study confirmed the hypothesized negative associations
the naïve perception of creativity as a malleable (state-like) or entity between a fixed-creative mindset and effectiveness in solving insight
(trait-like) characteristic has rarely been studied. These exploratory problems (see also O’Connor et al., 2013), but also demonstrated a
studies represent an initial attempt to address this gap. Hence, they can more complex relationship between a growth mindset and insight-
contribute to our understanding of both general mindsets and specific problem solving. Although a growth mindset was weakly and posi-
mindsets and their relationship to creativity in several ways. tively correlated with problem solving, it lost its statistical signifi-
Because creativity is a complex phenomenon, it is possible to cance in regression analysis when correlations between mindsets were
analyze it from different perspectives (Rhodes, 1961; Glăveanu, controlled for. The interaction of both mindsets was significant, show-
2013), taking into consideration different levels of creativity (Kauf- ing different patterns of association between the growth mindset and
man & Beghetto, 2009). Although one of the most common creativity problem solving among people with low and high fixed-creative
myths assumes that creativity is a fixed trait and cannot be changed mindsets. When the fixed mindset was low, the growth mindset
(Plucker, Beghetto, & Dow, 2004; Sawyer, 2006), in fact, this belief clearly translated into higher problem-solving effectiveness. Addi-
represents only one of the mindsets about the nature of creativity. In tional studies should focus on the motivational correlations of both
previous research, scholars have treated the creativity mindset as a mindsets, and especially their interaction, looking for different moti-
singular term, with fixed and growth orientations forming two ends of vational orientations and problem-solving styles and strategies.
a continuum (O’Connor et al., 2013). This study provides empirical
arguments for treating growth and fixed creativity mindsets as nega-
tively associated, yet relatively independent, constructs. Although
people exhibited a clear preference for the growth mindset, the size of
the difference between growth and fixed mindsets was moderate.
It was speculated that holding both growth and fixed mindsets may
stem from the complex nature of the creativity construct. There is
widespread belief that creativity treated as potential (Runco, 2003), or
little-c creativity (Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009), is distributed normally
(Karwowski, 2009; Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009) and may be stimu-
lated and developed (i.e., Karwowski & Soszynski, 2008). On the
other hand, people commonly believe that great creative (Big-C)
accomplishments require special talents and gifts (Glück et al., 2002).
Thus, if people are able to differentiate between levels of creativity,
they will probably attribute different mindsets to the different levels
and types of creativity. This expectation needs examination in future
studies, especially studies of implicit theories of creativity among
people with different creative mindsets. Future studies should also Figure 4. Test information functions and standard measurement errors of
explore the extent to which the acceptance of both mindsets stems growth and fixed mindset scales—Rash IRT analysis.
CREATIVE MINDSETS 69

Limitations and Future Studies Beghetto, R. A. (2006). Creative self-efficacy: Correlates in middle and
secondary students. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 447– 457. doi:
Several limitations should be considered when the findings of this 10.1207/s15326934crj1804_4
study are interpreted. First, the three studies were conducted online, Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psy-
which may reduce the generalizability of the findings and requires chological Bulletin, 107, 238 –246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
corroboration in further studies. Second, the results may be limited by Chan, D. W., & Chan, L. (1999). Implicit theories of creativity: Teachers’
the measurement instruments used. Specifically, further studies perception of students characteristics in Hong Kong. Creativity Research
should explore the nature of creative mindsets, and the relationship Journal, 12, 185–195. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1203_3
between them, using interviews to obtain better insight into their Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality
separation or association. Future studies also should explore the and development. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor and Francis/Psychology
properties of the CSM proposed in this article. Although its psycho- Press.
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset. New York, NY: Random House.
metric qualities seem satisfactory, Rash IRT (Wright & Masters,
Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role
1982) analysis showed that the growth mindset is best measured at
in judgements and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psycho-
relatively low levels of latent trait (␪). This suggests the risk of a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

logical Inquiry, 6, 267–285. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0604_1


“ceiling effect” and in general, a tendency to accept items connected
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to


with a growth mindset. It is likely that this is connected with a social motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256 –273. doi:
desirability effect. Hence, longer scales with more difficult growth- 10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256
mindset items probably would allow for better measurement. Last but Dziedziewicz, D., Oledzka, D., & Karwowski, M. (2013). Developing 4 to
not least, the character of items included in the CMS may be treated 6-year old children’s figural creativity using a doodle-book program.
as a limitation. All items were formulated in a general form, not Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2012.09.004
dealing with specific types or levels of creativity (i.e., mini-c, little-c, Elsbach, K. D., & Kramer, R. M. (2003). Assessing creativity in Holly-
pro-c or Big-C). Thus, taking into consideration that a possible reason wood pitch meetings: Evidence for a dual-process model of creativity
for the two-dimensional solution obtained during EFA/CFA may stem judgments. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 283–301. doi:
from different perceptions of creativity (especially the distinction 10.2307/30040623
between malleable little-c creativity and fixed Big-C creativity), it Farmer, S. M., Tierney, P., & Kung-McIntyre, K. (2003). Employee
would be interesting to create an instrument comprised of items creativity in Taiwan: An application of role identity theory. Academy of
Management Journal, 46, 618 – 630. doi:10.2307/30040653
measuring both growth and fixed mindsets related to both little-c and
Glăveanu, V. P. (2013). Rewriting the language of creativity: The five A’s
Big-C creativity.
framework. Review of General Psychology, 17, 69 – 81. doi:10.1037/
Probably the two most important research problems that need to be
a0029528
tackled in the near future are (a) research into implicit theories of Glück, J., Ernst, R., & Unger, F. (2002). How creative define creativity:
creativity among people with different creativity mindsets and (b) Definitions reflects different types of creativity. Creativity Research
research examining the relationship between creative mindsets and Journal, 14, 55– 67. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1401_5
creative production (i.e., divergent thinking tasks), especially under Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York,
conditions of success and failure. Previous work on mindsets has NY: Wiley. doi:10.1037/10628-000
demonstrated the detrimental effects of a fixed mindset when an Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in
individual fails (Mueller & Dweck, 1998). It is important to examine covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alterna-
whether this finding also is observed in the case of creativity. tives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/
Despite their limitations, the presented findings offer potentially 10705519909540118
interesting information about creativity mindsets: their structure, cor- Jaussi, K. B., Randel, A. E., & Dionne, S. D. (2007). I am, I think, and I
relates, and consequences. These results also are potentially impor- do: The role of personal identity, self-efficacy, and cross-applications of
tant, especially for educators, counselors, and managers, because of experiences in creativity at work. Creativity Research Journal, 19,
247–258. doi:10.1080/10400410701397339
the consequences for understanding and cultivating favorable creativ-
Karwowski, M. (2009). I’m creative, but am I Creative? Similarities and
ity mindsets.
differences between self-evaluated small and Big C creativity in Poland.
The International Journal of Creativity and Problem Solving, 19, 7–26.
References Karwowski, M. (2011). It doesn’t hurt to ask . . . but sometimes it hurts to
believe: Polish students’ creative self-efficacy and its predictors. Psy-
Amabile, T. M. (2001). Beyond talent: John Irving and the passionate craft
of creativity. American Psychologist, 56, 333–336. doi:10.1037/0003- chology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 154 –164. doi:10.1037/
066X.56.4.333 a0021427
American Psychological Association. (2009). Publication manual of the Karwowski, M. (2012). Did curiosity kill the cat? Relationship between
American Psychology Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. trait curiosity, creative self-efficacy and creative role identity. Europe’s
Andiliou, A., & Murphy, P. K. (2010). Examining variations among Journal of Psychology, 8, 547–558. doi:10.5964/ejop.v8i4.513
researchers’ and teachers’ conceptualizations of creativity: A review and Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., & Wisniewska, E. (in press). Measurement of
synthesis of contemporary research. Educational Research Review, 5, creative self-efficacy and creative role-identity. High Ability Studies.
201–219. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2010.07.003 Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., Wisniewska, E., & Gralewski, J. (2013). Big
Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Personality-relationship Five personality factors as the predictors of creative self-efficacy and
transaction in adolescents: Core versus surface personality characteris- creative personal identity: Does gender matter? The Journal of Creative
tics. Journal of Personality, 71, 629 – 666. doi:10.1111/1467-6494 Behavior, 47, 215–232. doi:10.1002/jocb.32
.7104005 Karwowski, M., & Soszynski, M. (2008). How to develop creative imag-
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: ination? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3, 163–171. doi:10.1016/j.tsc
Freeman. .2008.07.001
70 KARWOWSKI

Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The four Raykov, T., & Hancock, G. R. (2005). Examining change in maximal
c model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 1–12. doi: reliability for multiple-component measuring instruments. British Jour-
10.1037/a0013688 nal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 58, 65– 82. doi:10.1348/
Kelly, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York, 000711005X38753
NY: Morton. Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappan, 42,
Li, C. H., & Wu, J. J. (2011). The structural relationships between opti- 305–310.
mism and innovative behavior: Understanding potential antecedents and Runco, M. A. (2003). Education for creative potential. Scandinavian Jour-
mediating effects. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 119 –128. doi: nal of Educational Research, 47, 317–324. doi:10.1080/
10.1080/10400419.2011.571184 00313830308598
Makel, M. (2008). The malleability of implicit beliefs of creativity and Runco, M. A., & Johnson, D. J. (2002). Parents’ and teachers’ implicit
creative production. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Indiana Univer- theories of children’s creativity: A cross-cultural perspective. Creativity
sity, Bloomington, IN. Research Journal, 14, 427– 438. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1434_12
Maksić, S., & Pavlović, J. (2011). Educational researchers’ personal ex- Sawyer, R. K. (2006). Explaining creativity: The science of human inno-
plicit theories of creativity and its development: A qualitative study. vation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

High Ability Studies, 22, 219 –231. doi:10.1080/13598139.2011.628850 Seng, Q. K., Keung, H. K., & Cheng, S. K. (2008). Implicit theories of
Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., Ludtke, O., Koller, O., & Baumert, J. (2006). creativity: A comparison of student-teachers in Hong Kong and Singa-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Integration of multidimensional self-concept and core personality con- pore. Compare: A Journal of Comparative Education, 38, 71– 86. doi:
structs: Construct validation and relations to well-being and achieve- 10.1080/03057920701419959
ment. Journal of Personality, 74, 403– 456. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494 Sen, R. S., & Sharma, R. (2011). Through multiple lenses: Implicit theories
.2005.00380.x of creativity among Indian children and adults. The Journal of Creative
Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Praise for intelligence can Behavior, 45, 273–302. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2011.tb01431.x
undermine children’s motivation and performance. Journal of Person- Silvia, P. J. (2011). Subjective scoring of divergent thinking: Examin-
ality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.33 ing the reliability of unusual uses, instances, and consequences tasks.
O’Connor, A. J., Nemeth, C. J., & Akutsu, S. (2013). Consequences of Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6, 24 –30. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2010.06
beliefs about the malleability of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, .001
25, 155–162. doi:10.1080/10400419.2013.783739 Spiel, C., & von Korff, C. (1998). Implicit theories of creativity: The
Pavlović, J., Maksic, S., & Bodroza, B. (2013). Implicit individualism in conceptions of politicians, scientists, artists and school teachers. High
teachers’ theories of creativity: Through the “four P’s” looking glass. Ability Studies, 9, 43–58. doi:10.1080/1359813980090104
The International Journal of Creativity & Problem Solving, 23, 39 –57. Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and
Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 607– 627.
more important to educational psychologist? Potentials, pitfalls, and doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.607
future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist, 39, Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Potential
83–96. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3902_1 antecedents and relationship to creative performance. Academy of Man-
Plucker, J. A., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Assessment of creativity. In J. C. agement Journal, 45, 1137–1148. doi:10.2307/3069429
Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cre- Wright, B. D., & Masters, G. N. (1982). Rating scale analysis: Rasch
ativity (pp. 48 –74). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. measurement. Chicago, IL: MESA Press.

Appendix
Insight Problems Used in Study 3

1. A number of lilies in a pond doubles each day. At the How many full 24-hr days does it need to climb all the
beginning of summer there is only one lily. After 30 way up the pole?
days the whole pond is full of lilies. On which day is the
pond half-full with lilies? 5. Earth weighs 6 sextillion tons. How much would it weigh
if one were to build a wall weighing 1 sextillion tons?
2. What is it: an antique invention still used in most
countries of the world which makes it possible to look 6. A woman has two sons who were born at the same time, on
through walls? the same day, month, and year. They are not twins— how
is it possible?
3. There are black and brown socks in the drawer, mixed
in the proportion of 4:5. How many socks does one need 7. A car driver has a brother, but the brother of the driver does
to take out of the drawer to be certain to complete a pair not have a brother. Who is the driver for the brother?
of the same color?
Received November 5, 2012
4. A snail climbs up a pole 10 m in length. It climbs 2 m Revision received July 15, 2013
per day, but at night it falls asleep and falls down 1 m. Accepted August 5, 2013 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like