You are on page 1of 11

Ecotoxicology

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10646-018-02012-y

Mating under the influence: male Siamese fighting fish prefer EE2-
exposed females
Rebecca A. Cram1 Jaslynn M. Lawrence1 Teresa L. Dzieweczynski
● ●
1

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
Countless pharmaceuticals and endocrine disrupting chemicals (EDCs) exist on the market with more added each day. Many
of these compounds are not removed during the wastewater treatment process and enter bodies of water in their active form.
EDCs are known to have physiological and behavioral effects in exposed organisms. Exposure to the synthetic estrogen 17α-
ethinylestradiol (EE2), a common EDC found in birth control pills, has been found to lead to population collapse after only a
few generations in some fish species. Mechanisms identified as potential driving forces for collapse include feminization of
males and altered fecundity in both sexes. However, an additional way in which EE2 could lead to population collapse is by
1234567890();,:
1234567890();,:

altering courtship behavior, which could then change mating preferences and decrease mating opportunities. The current
study had the following objectives: determine if exposing female Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens, to EE2 changes
mate choice in males; assess if the dose and duration of female exposure matters; and examine if exposing males to EE2
influences their mating preferences. Both unexposed and exposed males were presented with pairs of females that differed in
EE2 dose and exposure duration. The results indicate that males were more responsive to EE2-exposed females than
unexposed females, with males being most responsive to females exposed to the low versus high dose. Furthermore, exposed
males responded less overall than unexposed males. If EE2 concentration increases in the environment, the likelihood of
successful mating could decrease and, therefore, potentially lead to adverse population impacts.
Keywords Endocrine disrupting chemicals Mate choice 17α-ethinylestradiol Courtship
● ● ●

Introduction treatment facilities lack the ability to properly detect, dilute,


or degrade PPCPs before they are released in effluent into
Humans use hundreds, if not thousands, of pharmaceuticals surrounding bodies of water (Baronti et al. 2000; Arnold
and personal care products (PPCPs) each year worldwide. et al. 2013). This means that PPCPs often enter the envir-
As populations grow and age, the use of PPCPs is only onment in active form where they adversely affect exposed
likely to increase (La Farre et al. 2008; Blair et al. 2013; wildlife. Perhaps, the most well-studied and prevalent class
Gaw et al. 2014). While PPCPs are used by humans, non- of PPCPs are endocrine disrupting chemicals (EDCs),
target organisms face effects of exposure as many of these which act by mimicking or inhibiting the normal
PPCPs are designed to have an effect at small doses and functioning of steroid hormones (Gleason and Nacci 2001;
remain in their active state even after they are excreted (Frye Sumpter 2005). Through this interference, EDCs can impact
et al. 2012; Arnold et al. 2013). Estradiol is excreted in a any organism within an exposed environment, although the
conjugated, inactive form, but is then returned to a biolo- severity and specific mechanism of the effects require
gically active form during the sewage treatment process greater study.
(Ternes et al. 1999). Additionally, most wastewater Exposure to EDCs has produced morphological, phy-
siological, behavioral, and even genetic changes in aquatic
organisms (Kime 1998; Tyler et al. 1998). Because many of
the EDCs found in the environment have estrogen
* Teresa L. Dzieweczynski mimicking properties (Kime 1998), the bulk of the studies
tdzieweczynski@une.edu
done on the effects of exposure to these compounds have
1
Department of psychology, University of New England, 11 Hills focused on reproductive changes. Negative consequences of
Beach Road, Biddeford, ME 04005, USA exposure include year-round production of vitellogenin
R. A. Cram et al.

(Purdom et al. 1994), reduced fertilization and hatching important as it would provide a behavioral cause for
success (Länge et al. 2001), and occurrence of intersex in population collapse even if exposed individuals appear to be
male fish and reptiles (Guillette et al. 1995; Jobling et al. unchanged morphologically.
1998) as well as decreases in courtship and aggression in Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens, is a model species
exposed males (Bell 2001). Changes observed in the that is widely used in laboratory settings in part due to the
behavior of males, such as altered social hierarchies (Coe ritualized aggressive and mating displays used to commu-
et al. 2008), impaired ability to acquire mates (Saaristo et al. nicate with members of both sexes (Simpson 1968). Recent
2009), delayed nest building (Saaristo et al. 2010), and work in this species has focused on these displays (e.g.,
reduced social status (Colman et al. 2009), are likely to Jaroensutasinee and Jaroensutasinee 2001; Clotfelter et al.
produce population-level consequences. Indeed, exposure 2006; Verbeek et al. 2008), how they might vary within-
to one of the most prevalent EDCs, the estrogen mimic 17α- and between-individuals (e.g., Dzieweczynski et al. 2010;
ethinylestradiol (EE2), has led to population collapse in Dzieweczynski and Hebert 2013), and if they are impacted
multiple fish species (Nash et al. 2004; Kidd et al. 2007; by social network (e.g., Doutrelant and McGregor 2000;
Palace et al. 2009). Given that EE2 reduces courtship Matos and Schlupp 2005). Additionally, over the past
behaviors in males, changes in mate preference that result decade, exposure to various EDCs has been found to
from these courtship reductions could be one potential influence this species behaviorally, reproductively, and
mechanism contributing to these collapses. neurologically (Clotfelter et al. 2010; Stevenson et al. 2011;
EE2 is a synthetic estrogen that is the active component Dzieweczynski et al. 2014). Much work has been done on
in oral contraceptives and is commonly used in many types how EE2 alters social behavior in this species. For example,
of hormone replacement therapies. Concentrations of EE2 males suppress female-directed behaviors following EE2
in aquatic ecosystems are usually between 1 and 10 ng/L exposure on either an acute or chronic scale (Dzieweczynski
but concentrations as high as 50–60 ng/L have been found and Hebert 2013; Dzieweczynski et al. 2014), females avoid
in highly populated areas of the world (Larsson et al. 1999; males exposed to EE2 (Dzieweczynski and Kane 2017), and
Kolpin et al. 2002; Muller et al. 2008; Ciocan et al. 2010). males are less attentive to males that have been exposed to
EE2 binds with higher affinity to estrogen receptors and EE2, especially when they themselves have been exposed
degrades at slower rates than natural steroids, making (Dzieweczynski and LaMonica 2016). These studies pro-
exposed organisms highly susceptible to its effects (Shore vide further support for the utility of this species as a model
et al. 1993). Because of these properties, EE2 may bioac- for effects of EDCs on reproductive behaviors that may
cumulate and has been found in exposed fish at levels up to have individual or population-level consequences in the
300 times greater than those found in the environment wild.
(Länge et al. 2001; Lai et al. 2002). Therefore, given EE2’s In order to examine if changes in mate choice may be a
slow degradation, high binding affinity, and potential to factor in population collapse following EE2 exposure, we
bioaccumulate, it poses a serious threat to organisms living allowed both unexposed and exposed male Siamese fighting
in aquatic ecosystems where EE2 is present. fish to view females that differed in their level of EE2
It is well established that females across taxa choose exposure (0, 5, and 10 ng/L). We presented males with
males based on traits that are related to their fitness females that had been exposed for 1 week and 2 weeks to
(Andersson 1994; Stapley 2008; Hunt et al. 2009). There- determine if duration of exposure influenced male choice.
fore, if EE2 exposure influences courtship and aggression in Both of the doses utilized in this study fall within the range
males, it may alter female mating decisions and social of EE2 levels found in the wild. Based on previous studies
dynamics. While females tend to be the choosier sex, males that have found a decrease in male courtship behavior,
may also prefer females with some trait variants over those including gill flaring, fin spreading, and tail beating, fol-
with different variants. Degree of male preference may lowing exposure to EE2 (Dzieweczynski et al. 2014), we
relate to degree of male investment such as if males are predicted that exposed males would perform less female-
building nests or are involved in caring for offspring. Male directed courtship behavior overall than unexposed males.
preferences may change if physical attributes of either sex We also predicted that these exposed males would express a
are affected by EE2 exposure. This, in turn, could reduce weaker preference than unexposed males due to a lack of
the number of matings within a population or alter mate courtship motivation as exposure to EE2 should reduce
choice. For example, male threespine stickleback court testosterone levels (Adeel et al. 2017), thereby decreasing
females with distended abdomens more vigorously than male-typical behaviors. EE2 exposure has led to reductions
females with less distended abdomens (Rowland 1982) and in a wide range of male-typical behaviors including
male desert gobies adjust their courtship efforts based on aggression in fathead minnows (Salierno and Kane 2009),
female size (Lehtonen et al. 2011). Studying how mate nest building in sand gobies (Saaristo et al. 2009) and
choice changes following EE2 exposure could be especially threespine stickleback (Brian et al. 2006), and courtship and
Mating under the influence: male Siamese fighting fish prefer EE2-exposed females

reproductive success in guppies and sand gobies (Kris- changes but no EE2, only ethanol, added to their home
tensen et al. 2005; Saaristo et al. 2010). In terms of male containers. This exposure was done daily over a 2-week
preference towards females experiencing the three different testing period. Females were assigned to one of the three
EE2 exposure levels, we hypothesized that males would exposure groups: control (0 ng/L), low dose (5 ng/L), or
prefer exposed over unexposed females. This was hypo- high dose (10 ng/L), and exposure for the females was done
thesized because exposed female rats have heightened in an identical manner to that for the male subjects
receptivity following an influx of estrogen (Asarian and throughout the 2-week experimental period. Both low- and
Geary 2002). Additionally, increased estrogen may lead to high-dose exposures were used to determine whether
the production of more eggs, a trait males prefer and assess varying degrees of exposure had an effect on male pre-
via female size (Doyle et al. 2013). There was no specific ference. Both concentrations used fall within the spectrum
prediction as to male response to females exposed to a of levels in the wild. The low dose is well within the range
higher or lower dose of EE2 or if duration of exposure found in the aquatic environment (e.g., Baronti et al. 2000)
would have an effect as these aspects have not been studied and the high dose, while higher than the amount found in
previously. most areas, is a dose commonly used in ecotoxicology
studies (Frye et al. 2012). Additionally, the high dose is a
pharmacologically relevant dose and could be seen more
Methods frequently in bodies of freshwater in the future due to
increased use of EE2 and population growth. Concentra-
Subjects tions of EE2 in the storage containers were verified by
taking water samples collected right before the daily water
Male and female Siamese fighting fish (males: 66.43 ± changes on day 1, day 6, and day 13 from a subset of
7.97 mm, 1.84 ± 0.56 g; females: 52.67 ± 5.27 mm, 1.44 ± randomly selected control, low-dose females, high-dose
0.27 g), B. splendens, that were sexually mature but had females, and exposed males (n = 5 each group). EE2 was
never mated, were purchased from a commercial distributor extracted from the water samples via C18 solid-phase car-
and then allowed to acclimate to laboratory conditions tridges (Sep-pack Plus C18 cartridges) and then GC-MS
(26.7 °C; 16:8 h light:dark cycle) for a minimum of two was used to analyze the samples to determine EE2 con-
weeks before testing. Fish were purchased at two different centrations (control: below detectable limits; low dose:
time points (January 2016: 60 males; 100 females; Sep- 4.8 ± 0.6 ng/L; high dose: 10.2 ± 1.1 ng/L; exposed males:
tember 2016: 20 males; 60 females). A total of 80 male 4.7 ± 0.8 ng/L).
subjects (40 control; 40 EE2-exposed) and 160 females (50
control; 110 EE2-exposed) were used. Males were housed Experimental setup and procedure
in opaque 475 mL containers to prevent them from obser-
ving conspecifics that could elicit aggressive behavior. The experimental setup consisted of one 37.9-liter tank
However, because female Siamese fighting fish typically (51 × 25 × 30 cm) centered between two 20.5-liter tanks
form shoals, they were housed in 475 mL transparent con- (43 × 23 × 28 cm) on either side of it (Fig. 1). The male was
tainers so that they could see other females. Males and allowed to swim freely about the experimental tank while
females were housed on separate shelving units that pre- the females were confined in containers within their tanks
vented them from seeing one another outside of the so that these females were always at a set distance from the
experimental testing. All fish were fed Hikari Bio-Pure© focal tank. This design allowed the male to easily observe
freeze-dried bloodworms ad libitum daily and received and interact with the females while preventing the females
100% water changes daily during testing. from viewing one another and keeping both females in
standardized locations due to the distance between the tanks
17α-Ethinylestradiol exposure and the placement of the female containers. The experi-
mental tank was visually divided into different areas: a
Male subjects were randomly assigned to one of the two neutral zone (middle third of the tank) and two preference
groups: those that received exposure to EE2 during testing zones at the end of the tank (two fish lengths from the
and those that remained unexposed. To administer EE2, a compartment containing the female in the adjacent tank).
stock solution of EE2 was first created by dissolving EE2 To start a trial, opaque partitions were first placed
(98% purity; Cayman Chemical Company; CAS 57-63-6) in between the experimental tank and the two adjacent female
95% ethanol (Bell 2001 for methodology). Exposed males tanks. Then a male and two females were placed into the
then received 5 µL of this solution in their home containers appropriate tanks. After a 5-min acclimation period and
after the daily water change to reach a total concentration of after the male had swam into the neutral zone of the
5 ng/L. Males in the unexposed group had daily water experimental tank, the opaque partitions separating the
R. A. Cram et al.

Female Tank Preference Female Tank


Preference Neutral Zone
Zone Zone

Fig. 1 Diagram of experimental tank setup. The test tank in the middle contained the male subject, while the two tanks on the sides were where the
females were presented. Opaque dividers between the tanks were removed at the start of a trial

tanks were removed and the 5-min trial started. All trials and number of tail beats to a female. Since there were two
were recorded with a Sony HandyCam digital camcorder females within each viewing combination, these five
positioned in front of the experimental setup so that all three behaviors were coded for each female, for a total of 10
tanks were in view. When the trial was completed, the measures in each trial. Time in a preference zone was
opaque partitions were replaced and the male started the defined by a male being within two body lengths of a
next acclimation period. female. Time spent orienting to a female was defined as a
Five different viewing combinations of females (HL = male being within a preference zone facing the female.
high + low exposure; HH = high + high exposure; LC = Time spent tracking (following the female while orienting
low + control; CC = control + control; and HC = high + to and within a body length of that female), time spent gill
control) were used to examine if male behavior changed as flaring (extension of the opercular flaps), and number of tail
a byproduct of female exposure to EE2. To reduce risk of beats (deliberate slaps of the tail directed at a conspecific)
habituation due to repeated testing, the video exposures were only counted when a male was in a preference zone.
were divided so that a male had three combinations of These behaviors have been found to be suitable proxy
females 1 day and the other two combinations on the second measures of mate choice in this species (Jaroensutasinee
testing day. Each week, a group of 10 males was selected and Jaroensutasinee 2001; Clotfelter et al. 2006; Dzie-
for testing. Measures were taken to control for side bias and weczynski et al. 2013). To minimize observer bias, blinded
order effects, such as randomizing combination order and methods were used when all behavioral data were scored by
the side on which a female of a given exposure type was RAC and JML.
presented. Additionally, males never saw the same female
more than once. Testing lasted 2 weeks with males seeing Statistical analyses
females that had been exposed to EE2 for 1 week during the
first week of testing and then females exposed for two First, a series of three-way repeated-measures analyses of
weeks the second week. This was done to determine if the variance (RMANOVAs) on ranks with treatment (stimulus
length of time females were exposed affected male presentation types), male exposure (control and exposed),
preferences. and duration of female exposure (week 1 and week 2) as
factors was run. This was done to examine if side bias
Behaviors measured affected total amount of response or difference in response
to one female versus the other (e.g., time by female on
Behaviors were scored using the event recording program, left–time by female on right). Next, we examined if the
Event-PC. Five behaviors were measured toward a given strength of preference to one female versus the other varied
female: time spent within a preference zone, time orienting across the five treatments. In other words, whether male
to a female, time tracking, amount of time spent gill flaring, response to one female differ based on the type of female he
Mating under the influence: male Siamese fighting fish prefer EE2-exposed females

was presented with. To do this, two different series of


Table 1 Results of the three-way RMANOVAs run on the difference scores of the five different behaviors measured towards each of the two females within a presentation type (time by the

exposure. When females of the same level of exposure were presented simultaneously, the behavior towards the female on the right was subtracted from the amount of that behavior towards the
female on the left. Male exposure (Exposure), female presentation type (Treatment), and duration of female exposure (Duration) were factors in the RMANOVAs, and all possible interaction
Difference scores were obtained by subtracting the behavior towards the female with the least amount of EE2 exposure in a treatment from that behavior towards the female with the greater
three-way ANOVAs on ranks with treatment, male expo-
sure, and duration of female exposure as factors and all

F4,799 = 0.33, p = 0.86

F4,799 = 0.31, p = 0.87

F4,799 = 0.14, p = 0.97

F4,799 = 0.61, p = 0.66

F4,799 = 0.51, p = 0.73


Treatment × duration ×
possible interaction effects were run. One series was run on
the total amount of behavior within a trial and the other was
run on the difference scores. The total amount of behavior,
exposure

regardless of which female the behavior was directed at


(e.g., time orienting to female A + time orienting to female
female, time spent orienting to the female, time spent tracking the female, time spent gill flaring towards the female, and number of tail beats directed at a female)

B), was calculated for each individual in each treatment and


then ANOVAs on ranks with post hoc Wilcoxon tests and
Duration × exposure

F1,799 = 0.93, p =

F1,799 = 1.79, p =

F1,799 = 2.23, p =

F1,799 = 0.06, p =

F1,799 = 0.22, p =
Bonferonni corrections were conducted on these scores.
Additionally, difference scores were calculated for each
male in each treatment (e.g., time by high-exposure
female–time by low-exposure female) and the same sta-
0.34

0.18

0.14

0.81

0.64

tistical tests were run on these scores. For these difference


scores, female A was always the female with the greatest
F4,799 = 2.60, p = 0.03

F1,799 = 0.16, p = 0.69 F4,799 = 0.35, p = 0.85 F4,799 = 2.35, p = 0.06

F1,799 = 3.67, p = 0.06 F1,799 = 1.08, p = 0.30 F4,799 = 0.57, p = 0.68 F4,799 = 0.16, p = 0.14

F1,799 = 0.09, p = 0.77 F1,799 = 6.74, p = 0.01 F4,799 = 1.26, p = 0.28 F4,799 = 1.18, p = 0.32
Treatment × exposure

exposure concentration (e.g., high exposure–control). In the


F1,799 = 0.98, p = 0.32 F1,799 = 2.79, p = 0.10 F4,799 = 0.20, p = 0.94 F4,799 = 12.29, p <

treatments where both females had the same level of


exposure (e.g., high exposure and high exposure), female A
was always the female on the left of the testing tank and
0.0001

female B was on the right side of the tank.


Treatment × duration

Results
F4,799 = 0.67, p =

Side bias
0.612

Side bias did not significantly influence any of the depen-


dent measures (treatment: F4,799 ≤ 1.18, p ≥ 0.32; duration:
F1,799 ≤ 3.22, p ≥ 0.07; male exposure: F1,789 ≤ 3.89, p ≥
F1,799 = 0.0004, p =

0.06; treatment × duration: F4,799 ≤ 2.47, p ≥ 0.12; treat-


ment × male exposure: F4,799 ≤ 1.95, p ≥ 0.10; treatment ×
duration × male exposure: F4,799 ≤ 1.92, p ≥ 0.11). This
Exposure

means that whether a particular female was presented on


0.98

the left or right side of the tank did not affect any of the
measured behaviors.
F1,799 = 0.0001, p =

F1,799 = 0.0002, p =

Difference scores
effects were included. Significant effects are bolded

A positive difference score indicates that more behavior


Duration

was directed to the higher exposure female, a negative


0.99

0.99

difference score shows that males are more responsive to


females of lower exposure, and a difference score close to
zero demonstrates that there is little difference in behavior
F4,799 = 121.65, p <

Orienting F4,799 = 108.26, p <

towards the two females in a viewing condition. Treatment


F4,799 = 54.75, p <

F4,799 = 56.13, p <

Tail beats F4,799 = 30.97, p <

had a significant effect on the difference scores (Beha-


viorFemaleA − BehaviorFemaleB) for time spent within the
Treatment

preference zone (F4,799 = 121.65, p < 0.0001); however,


0.0001

0.0001

0.0001

0.0001

0.0001

because a treatment × male exposure interaction effect was


found (F4,799 = 2.60, p = 0.03), this factor cannot be ana-
lyzed separately (Table 1). Difference scores for unexposed
Tracking
Time by

males in the high-exposure + low-exposure treatment were


flaring
Gill

more negative than all other treatments by female exposure


R. A. Cram et al.

+ control treatments, meaning that males preferred the


a
a a a exposed female to the control female (Z ≥ 6.08, p < 0.0001).
For the difference between the amount of time males
spent gill flaring to one female versus the other female, both
treatment and an interaction between treatment type and
b
b male exposure (treatment: F4,799 ≥ 56.13, p < 0.0001; treat-
b b ment × exposure: F4,799 ≥ 12.29, p < 0.0001; Table 1) had an
influence. Difference scores were the most negative for
unexposed males in the high-exposure female + low-expo-
c sure female treatment (Z ≥ 4.22, p < 0.0001; Fig. 3a). The
second most negative difference score was found for
d exposed males in this same treatment (Z ≥ 2.00, p ≤ 0.046).
Difference scores for gill flaring were the most positive for
unexposed males in the low-exposure + control and high-
exposure + control treatments meaning both exposed and
unexposed males preferred low-exposure females over
those from the high exposure (Z ≥ 3.63, p ≤ 0.003; Fig. 3a)
and the next highest difference scores occurred when
exposed males were in these same two treatments showing
Fig. 2 Difference scores (BehaviorFemaleA − BehaviorFemale B) for time
spent by the females in the five different female pairings males
gill flaring was increased when females were exposed to
received: control + control (CC), high exposure + high exposure either EE2 treatment (Z ≥ 3.35, p < 0.001). Again, difference
(HH), low exposure + control (LC), high exposure + control (HC), scores were close to zero for both control and exposed
and high exposure + low exposure (HL) for both control and exposed males in the high-exposure + high-exposure and control +
males. Significant differences are denoted by differing letters. Error
bars represent ± 1 SEM and α = 0.05
control treatments.
Difference scores for tail beats were influenced by male
exposure, treatment, and a male exposure × treatment
dose combinations for time spent in the preference zone interaction (exposure: F1,799 ≥ 6.74, p = 0.01; treatment:
(Z ≥ 2.61, p ≤ 0.001; Fig. 2). This means that control males F4,799 ≥ 30.97, p < 0.0001; exposure × treatment: F4,799 ≥
spent more time near the female exposed to the lower 2.78, p = 0.03; Table 1). Control males in the high-expo-
concentration of EE2 than to the higher concentration when sure + control and low-exposure + control treatments had
encountering both simultaneously. Exposed males had more the highest difference scores in the number of tail beats they
negative difference scores in this same treatment than all delivered to the two females (Z ≥ 2.52, p < 0.01; Fig. 3b).
other treatments by exposure combinations with the Exposed males in these same two treatments had the next
exception of unexposed males in the high-exposure + low- highest difference scores indicating EE2 treated females
exposure treatment (Z ≥ 5.14, p ≤ 0.0001). Difference scores were preferred by both exposed and unexposed males (Z ≥
for time spent in the preference zone were the highest in the 2.04, p ≤ 0.04). Finally, the most negative difference scores
low-exposure + control and high-exposure + control treat- were found in the high-exposure + low-exposure treatment
ments, regardless of whether or not the males were exposed meaning low-exposure females elicited more tail beats than
to EE2, meaning that males preferred exposed females (Z ≥ high-exposure females (Z ≥ 2.76, p ≤ 0.006; Fig. 3b).
5.01, p ≤ 0.0001; Fig. 2). Difference scores in treatments in
which males received females of the same exposure level Total overall responses
(e.g., control + control) were close to zero, indicating that
males directed similar amounts of behavior to both females. Male exposure type (control vs. EE2-exposed) and female
Treatment affected the difference scores for time spent viewing combination influenced the total amount of time
orienting as well as the amount of time spent tracking males spent in the preference zones (exposure: F1,799 =
(F4,799 ≥ 54.75, p < 0.0001; Table 1). The difference scores 99.80, p < 0.0001; treatment: F4,799 = 9.23, p < 0.0001;
within the high-exposure + low-exposure treatment were Table 2). Exposed males spent less time overall in the
more negative than those found in the other four treatments preference zones than unexposed males (Z = 9.99, p <
for both of these behaviors suggesting a low-exposure 0.0001). Males spent less time in the preference zones when
female was preferred over a high-exposure one (Z ≥ 6.04, p females of the same level of exposure were present (i.e.,
< 0.0001). Additionally, the difference scores for time spent control + control) compared to when females differing in
orienting and time spent tracking were the most highly EE2 exposure levels were present (Z ≥ 2.52, p ≤ 0.01). Male
positive in the low-exposure + control and high-exposure exposure, treatment, and an interaction effect between these
Mating under the influence: male Siamese fighting fish prefer EE2-exposed females

a) b)
a a
e
e

d d b b
c
c c c
c c c c
b

d d
a

Fig. 3 Difference scores for a the amount of time males spent gill subtracted from the response to the female with the higher level of
flaring to and b number of tail beats directed they directed at the exposure. When the pair experienced equivalent amounts, the behavior
females in the five treatments: control + control (CC), high exposure to the female on the right was subtracted from the female on the left.
+ high exposure (HH), low exposure + control (LC), high exposure + Significant differences are denoted by differing letters. Error bars
control (HC), and high exposure + low exposure (HL). A male’s represent ± 1 SEM and α = 0.05
response to the female with the lower level of exposure was always

two factors, affected the total amount of time males spent number of tail beats was affected by male exposure (F1,799
orienting to the females (exposure: F1,799 = 133.36, p < = 50.45, p < 0.0001; Table 2). As with the majority of the
0.0001; treatment: F4,799 = 9.47, p < 0.0001; exposure × measured behaviors, exposed males performed fewer tail
treatment: F4,799 = 3.22, p = 0.01; Table 2). Overall, beats than unexposed males (Z = 7.10, p < 0.0001).
exposed males spent less time orienting than unexposed
males, regardless of treatment type (Z ≥ 5.27, p ≤ 0.0001;
Fig. 4). Additionally, exposed males spent less time Discussion
orienting to the females when the females had the same
level of EE2 exposure versus differing levels (e.g., control The presence of the estrogen mimic EE2 in aquatic eco-
+ control versus high exposure + control; Z ≥ 3.02, p ≤ systems has known physiological and behavioral effects on
0.003). organisms living in these waters (Frye et al. 2012). Most of
The total amount of time males spent tracking was the research in this area has focused on changes in males
influenced by duration of female exposure to EE2 and with females receiving little attention. In this study, we
treatment type (Table 2). Males spent less time tracking examined whether exposure to EE2 affected females in a
when females had been exposed to EE2 for 2 weeks com- manner that might influence male preference. To address
pared to 1 week (Z = 3.77, p = 0.0002). This suggests that this, changes in male behavior towards females were
females may behave differently the longer they are exposed examined as proxy measures of altered female behavior
to EE2. Males also spent more time tracking when females and/or reproductive physiology. It has recently been found
of different levels of EE2 exposure were present than when that females avoid males that have been exposed to EE2
females of the same amount of EE2 exposure were present (Dzieweczynski and Kane 2017). Therefore, by examining
(Z ≥ 2.10, p ≤ 0.04). The time males spent gill flaring was how exposure to EE2 produces changes in male and female
affected by duration of female exposure and subject expo- reproductive behavior, we can gain a more complete picture
sure but no other factors (duration: F1,799 = 5.14, p = 0.02; of how this compound impacts mating systems. The results
exposure: F1,799 = 153.62, p < 0.0001; Table 2). Males suggest that male Siamese fighting fish actually prefer
spent less time gill flaring to females that were exposed to females that have been exposed to EE2, perhaps because the
EE2 for 2 weeks than those exposed for 1 week (Z = 2.27, addition of estrogen increases female receptivity. Increased
p = 0.02). Exposed males also spent less time gill flaring male preference could be beneficial for females if it
than control males (Z = 12.39, p < 0.0001). Finally, the total decreases aggression during courtship (Persaud and Galef
R. A. Cram et al.

a a a

Male exposure (Exposure), female presentation type (Treatment), and duration of female exposure (Duration) were factors in the RMANOVAs, and all possible interaction effects were included.
a
a

b b

F4,799 = 0.34, p = 0.86

F4,799 = 0.31, p = 0.87

F4,799 = 1.22, p = 0.30

F4,799 = 0.75, p = 0.56

F4,799 = 1.26, p = 0.28


Treatment × duration ×

c
c
exposure
Treatment × duration Treatment × exposure Duration × exposure

F1,799 = 1.66, p =

F1,799 = 1.99, p =

F1,799 = 2.40, p =

F1,799 = 0.01, p =

F1,799 = 0.03, p =
0.20

0.16

0.12

0.94

0.86
F4,799 = 3.22, p =
F4,799 = 2.36, p =

F4,799 = 2.04, p =

F4,799 = 0.39, p =

F4,799 = 0.60, p =

Fig. 4 The total amount of time males spent orienting to the females
(i.e., BehaviorFemaleA − BehaviorFemale B) was influenced by an inter-
action effect between the combination of females they viewed: (con-
trol + control (CC), high exposure + high exposure (HH), low
0.06

0.01

0.09

0.82

0.66
Table 2 Results of the three-way RMANOVAs run on the total behavior directed to both females within a treatment

exposure + control (LC), high exposure + control (HC), and high


exposure + low exposure (HL)) and their own exposure level. Sig-
nificant differences are denoted by differing letters. Error bars repre-
F1,799 = 99.80, p < 0.0001 F4,799 = 1.07, p =

F4,799 = 0.85, p =

F4,799 = 1.33, p =

F4,799 = 0.69, p =

F4,799 = 0.96, p =

sent ± 1 SEM and α = 0.05

2005; Dzieweczynski et al. 2013) or if it provides oppor-


tunities for females to gain access to higher quality mates
0.37

0.50

0.26

0.60

0.43

(Edward and Chapman 2011). Males preferred females


exposed to a lower dose of EE2 over those exposed to a
F1,799 = 50.45, p = 0.01
F1,799 = 0.54, p = 0.46

higher concentration, indicating that, is all females in a


F1,799 = 133.36, p <

F1,799 = 153.62, p <

population are exposed, there may be a point where


exposed females are no longer more attractive. Levels of
EE2 in aquatic ecosystems may reach as high as 50 ng/L
Exposure

(Ciocan et al. 2010), a concentration at which it is unlikely


0.0001

0.0001

males would exhibit a preference for females as they would


be completely feminized. Additionally, EE2 exposure has
negative reproductive consequences on males including
F1,799 = 5.14, p = 0.02
F1,799 = 0.07, p = 0.79

F1,799 = 0.08, p = 0.78

F1,799 = 2.71, p = 0.10

reduced courtship rates and lower sperm viability (e.g.,


F4,799 = 4.38, p = 0.002 F1,799 = 14.21, p =

Kristensen et al. 2005; Dzieweczynski and Kane 2017).


Since both males and females would simultaneously
Duration

experience an EE2 exposure in the wild, the overall effects


0.0002

of exposure on reproduction are likely to be negative such


as decreased courtship attempts from males and altered
receptivity in females.
F4,799 = 1.45, p = 0.22

Tail beats F4,799 = 0.26, p = 0.90

Difference scores and total responsiveness both


F4,799 = 9.23, p <

F4,799 = 9.47, p <

Significant effects are bolded

demonstrate that males alter their behavior towards females


based on whether or not females had experienced EE2
Treatment

exposure. Males performed more courtship behaviors to the


0.0001

0.0001

exposed than the unexposed female in a pair, reflected by


greater positive difference scores. Additionally, males
Orienting

preferred females exposed to a lower dose of EE2 more


Tracking
Time by

flaring

than females exposed to a higher dose when the two were


Gill

presented simultaneously. Male preference when a low-


Mating under the influence: male Siamese fighting fish prefer EE2-exposed females

exposure female and a high-exposure female were present responses and difference scores. However, there is a limit to
was greater than when an unexposed female and an exposed this preference as, when high- and low-dose females were
female were presented together. This indicates that males both present, males directed more behavior to the female
have stronger preferences when they can simultaneously with the lower exposure. These results suggest that altera-
assess females exposed to different concentrations of EE2. tions in mate choice may be a potential mechanism by
When females of identical levels of exposure were present, which EE2 exposure could lead to changes in population
males responded similarly towards both females, indicating structure and/or population collapse. Chronic exposure to
that the observed preferences were due to the difference in a low dose of EE2 quickly led to population
EE2 concentration rather than some inherent differences collapse in fathead minnows (Kidd et al. 2007) and zebra-
between females in the study. fish (Nash et al. 2004) by altering oogenesis in females and
Whether or not males experienced EE2 exposure them- producing intersex in males. Both males and females
selves also had an influence on reproductive behavior. influence reproduction and likely experience exposure in
While both unexposed and exposed males exhibited similar the wild simultaneously, although they may differ in how
preferences, these preferences were weaker in exposed much exposure they encounter. As human population sizes
males. Exposed males may have a more difficult time grow, levels of EDCs worldwide will continue to persist
assessing differences between females because EE2 expo- and likely increase (Adeel et al. 2017; Gaw et al. 2014).
sure has been found to affect recognition abilities and Therefore, even if exposure to EE2 is not directly detri-
attention in other exposed species (Zala and Penn 2004; mental to exposed females, it could still have negative
Mills and Chichester 2005; Peterson et al. 2017). Most implications because EE2 decreases male-typical behaviors
importantly, and not surprisingly, exposed males responded necessary for successful reproduction (Peterson et al. 2017).
less than males that were not exposed to EE2, further In other words, even if unexposed males prefer exposed
supporting the existing body of literature on reductions in females in our system, any benefit of this is expected to be
male-typical behaviors following exposure. is minimal for two reasons; first, EE2 decreases male-
It is possible that EE2 exposure in females increased typical behaviors (Peterson et al. 2017) and, second,
female receptivity, causing behavioral or morphological because females have been shown to avoid males exposed
changes that were apparent to males but not detected by the to EE2 (Dzieweczynski and Kane 2017). The current results
human observers. Female Siamese fighting fish display further demonstrate the negative effects EE2 has on males
receptive bars along their sides when they are preparing for because exposed males, while exhibiting similar preferences
copulation (Simpson 1968) and increased estrogen may to unexposed males, had weaker preferences and were less
have caused subtle changes in this barring pattern to appear. responsive overall to females. Thus, EE2 could contribute
There was no difference in the number of females that to population collapse even if exposed individuals are still
displayed these bars with the three exposure levels, but the physiologically capable of mating. Future studies should
intensity of the bar coloration or duration of expression may examine mating behavior and examine offspring viability
have differed in a manner that was noticeable to the male following exposure. In the current study design, males and
subjects but not noticeable to the observers. Female body females were separated from one another and, therefore,
size and mass are often positively correlated with estrogen could only interact through visual means. The role of
levels as well as male preference (Doyle et al. 2013), pheromonal information in mating interactions within this
however, these factors were controlled for in this study so species should be examined as it might be influenced by
they cannot be responsible for differences in male pre- EE2 exposure as well. By understanding how reproductive
ference across treatments. Disappointingly, few studies behaviors such as mate choice are impacted by exposure to
have assessed the effects of EE2 on female receptivity (but EDCs, we can obtain a more accurate picture of the long-
see Dzieweczynski and Kane 2017). EE2 at low doses sti- term consequences of exposure.
mulates egg production but higher amounts decrease ferti-
lity in female zebrafish (Silva et al. 2012; Söffker et al. Data availability
2012). Early exposure to EE2 increases bottom-dwelling
behavior but has no effect on social or reproductive beha- All data analyzed during this study are available via the
viors in adulthood in female guppies (Volkova et al. 2012). Open Science Framework (osf.io/e3qj9).
Thus, determining the precise morphological and/or beha-
vioral changes that occur in females following exposure to Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank Haley LaMonica
for her assistance in running behavioral trials. They also thank Jessica
EDCs that then, in turn, alter male choices is an important
Kane, Kelley Portrais, Megan Stevens, and two anonymous reviewers
area for further study. for their feedback on a previous version of this manuscript. Amy
In closing, this study demonstrates that males prefer Keirstead and Molly Wright were instrumental in creating the
females exposed to EE2, as reflected by both overall EE2 solution and analyzing EE2 in water samples.
R. A. Cram et al.

Compliance with ethical standards Doutrelant C, McGregor PK (2000) Eavesdropping and mate choice in
female fighting fish. Behaviour 137:1655–1669
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of Doyle TJ, Bowman JL, Windell VL, McLean DJ, Kim KH (2013)
interest. Transgenerational effects of di-(2-ethylhexyl)phthalate on testi-
cular germ cell associations and spermatogonial stem cells in
mice. Biol Reprod 88:112
Ethical approval All applicable international, national, and/or institu-
Dzieweczynski TL, Gill CE, Walsh MM (2010) The nest matters: the
tional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.
influence of reproductive status on decision-making to conflicting
Animal care and experimental protocols were approved by the UNE
stimuli in male Siamese fighting fish. Behaviour 147:805–823
IACUC and covered under protocol UNE-20130910DZIET.
Dzieweczynski TL, Hebert OL (2013) The effects of short-term
exposure to an endocrine disrupter on behavioral consistency in
Informed consent All authors have approved this version of the work. juvenile and adult male Siamese fighting fish. Arch Environ
Contam Toxicol 64:316–326
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to Dzieweczynski TL, Hentz KB, Logan B, Hebert OL (2014) Chronic
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. exposure to 17α-ethinylestradiol reduces behavioral consistency
in male Siamese fighting fish. Behaviour 151:633–651
Dzieweczynski TL, Kane JL (2017) The bachelorette: female Siamese
fighting fish avoid males exposed to an estrogen mimic. Behav
References Proc 140:169–173
Dzieweczynski TL, LaMonica HJ (2016) Court like you mean it: male
Adeel M, Song X, Wang X, Francis D, Yang Y (2017) Environmental Siamese fighting fish are less attentive to courting males that have
impact of estrogens on human, animal and plant life: a critical been exposed to an estrogen mimic. Ethology 122:991–998
review. Environ Intern 99:107–119 Dzieweczynski TL, Russell AM, Forrette LM, Mannion KL (2013)
Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, NJ Male behavioral type affects female preference in Siamese
Arnold KE, Boxall ABA, Brown AR, Cuthbert RJ, Gaw S, Hutchinson fighting fish. Behav Ecol 25:136–141
TH et al. (2013) Assessing the exposure risk and impacts of Edward DA, Chapman T (2011) The evolution and significance of
pharmaceuticals in the environment on individuals and ecosys- male mate choice. Trends Ecol Evol 26:647–654
tems. Biol Lett 9:20130492 Frye CA, Bo E, Calamandrei G et al. (2012) Endocrine disruptors: a
Asarian L, Geary N (2002) Cyclic estradiol treatment normalizes body review of some sources, effects, and mechanisms of actions on
weight and restores physiological patterns of spontaneous feeding behaviour and neuroendocrine systems. J Neuroendocrinol
and sexual receptivity in ovariectomized rats. Horm Behav 24:144–159
42:461–471 Gaw S, Thomas KV, Hutchinson TH (2014) Sources, impacts and
Baronti C, Curini R, D’Ascenzo G, Di Corcia A, Gentili A, Samperi R trends in pharmaceuticals in the marine and coastal environment.
(2000) Monitoring natural and synthetic estrogens at activated Philos Trans R Soc B 369:20130572
sludge sewage treatment plants and in a receiving river water. Gleason TR, Nacci NE (2001) Risks of endocrine-disrupting com-
Environ Sci Technol 34:5059–5066 pounds to wildlife: extrapolating from effects on individuals to
Bell AM (2001) Effects of an endocrine disrupter on courtship and population response. Hum Ecol Risk Assess 7:1027–1042
aggressive behavior of male three-spined stickleback, Gaster- Guillette LJ, Gross TS, Gross DA, Rooney AA, Percival HF (1995)
osteus aculeatus. Anim Behav 62:775–780 Gonadal steroidogenesis in vitro from juvenile alligators obtained
Blair BD, Crago JP, Hedman CJ, Klaper R (2013) Pharmaceutical and from contaminated or control lakes. Environ Health Pers
personal care products found in the Great Lakes above con- 103:3136
centrations of environmental concern. Chemosphere Hunt J, Breuker CJ, Sadowski JA, Moore AJ (2009) Male-male
93:2116–2123 competition, female mate choice and their interaction: determin-
Brian JV, Augley JJ, Braithwaite VA (2006) Endocrine disrupting ing total sexual selection. J Evol Biol 22:14
effects on the nesting behavior of male three-spined stickleback Jaroensutasinee M, Jaroensutasinee K (2001) Sexual size dimorphism
Gasterosteus aculeatus. J Fish Biol 68:1883–1890 and male contest in wild Siamese fighting fish. J Fish Biol
Ciocan CM, Cubero-Leon E, Puinean AM, Hill EM, Minier C, Osada 59:1614–1621
M et al. (2010) Effects of estrogen exposure in mussels, Mytilus Jobling S, Nolan M, Tyler CR, Brighty G, Sumpter JP (1998) Wide-
edulis, at different stages of gametogenesis. Environ Pollut spread sexual disruption in wild fish. Environ Sci Technol
158:2977–84 32:2498–2506
Clotfelter ED, Curren LJ, Murphy CE (2006) Mate choice and Kidd KA, Blanchfield PJ, Mills KH, Palace VP, Evans RE, Lazorchak
spawning success in the fighting fish Betta splendens: the JM, Flick RW (2007) Collapse of a fish population after exposure
importance of body size, display behavior and nest size. Ethology to a synthetic estrogen. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:8897–8901
112:1170–1178 Kime DE (1998) Endocrine disruption in fish. Kluwer, Boston
Clotfelter ED, McNitt MM, Carpenter RE, Summers CH (2010) Kolpin DW, Furlong ET, Meyer MT, Thurman EM, Zaugg SD, Barber
Modulation of monoamine neurotransmitters in fighting fish Betta LB, Buxton HT (2002) Pharmaceuticals, hormones, and other
splendens exposed to waterborne phytoestrogens. Fish Physiol organic wastewater contaminants in U.S. streams, 1999-2000: a
Biochem 36:933–943 national reconnaissance. Environ Sci Technol 36:1202–1211
Coe TS, Hamilton PB, Hodgson D, Paull GC, Stevens JR, Sumner K, Kristensen T, Baatrup E, Bayley M (2005) 17α-ethinylestradiol
Tyler CR (2008) An environmental estrogen alters reproductive reduces the competitive reproductive fitness of the male guppy
hierarchies, disrupting sexual selection in group-spawning fish. (Poecilia reticulata). Biol Reprod 72:150–156
Environ Sci Technol 42:5050–5025 La Farre M, Perez S, Kantiani L, Barcelo D (2008) Fate and toxicity of
Colman JR, Baldwin D, Johnson LL, Scholz NL (2009) Effects of the emerging pollutants, their metabolites and transformation pro-
synthetic estrogen, 17ɑ-ethinylestradiol, on aggression and ducts in the aquatic environment. Trends Anal Chem
courtship behavior in male zebrafish (Danio rerio). Aquat Tox- 27:991–1007
icol 91:346–354
Mating under the influence: male Siamese fighting fish prefer EE2-exposed females

Lai KM, Scrimshaw MD, Lester JN (2002) Prediction of the bioac- behaviour of male sand gobies (Pomatoschistus minutus). Che-
cumulation factors and body burden of natural and synthetic mosphere 79:541–546
estrogens in aquatic organisms in the river systems. Sci Total Saaristo M, Craft JM, Lehtonen KK, Lindström K (2009) Sand goby
Environ 289:159–168 (Pomatoschistus minutus) males exposed to an endocrine dis-
Länge R, Hutchinson TH, Croudace CP, Siegmund F, Schweinfurth H, rupting chemical fail in nest and mate competition. Horm Behav
Hampe P, Panther GH, Sumpter JP (2001) Effects of the synthetic 56:315–321
estrogen 17α-ethinylestradiol on the life-cycle of the fathead min- Salierno JD, Kane AS (2009) 17α-ethinyloestradiol alters reproductive
now (Pimephales promelas). Environ Toxicol Chem 20:1216–1227 behaviours, circulating hormones, and sexual morphology in
Larsson DGJ, Adolfsson-Erici M, Parkkonen J, Pettersson M, Berg male fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas). Environ Toxicol
AH, Olsson P-E, Förlin L (1999) Ethinylestradiol: an undesired Chem 28:953–961
fish contraceptive? Aquat Toxicol 45:91–97 Shore LS, Gurevitz M, Shemesh M (1993) Estrogen as an
Lehtonen TK, Svensson PA, Wong BBM (2011) Both male and environmental pollutant. Bull Environ Contam Toxicol
female identity influence variation in male signaling effort. BMC 51:361–366
Evol Biol 11:233 Silva P, Rocha MJ, Cruzeiro C, Malhão F, Reis B, Urbatzka R,
Matos RJ, Schlupp I (2005) Performing in front of an audience: sig- Monteiro RAF, Rocha E (2012) Testing the effects of ethiny-
nalers and the social environment. In: McGregor PK (ed) Animal lestradiol and of an environmentally relevant mixture of
communication networks. Cambridge University Press, Cam- xenoestrogens as found in the Douro River (Portugal) on the
bridge, pp 63–83 maturation of fish gonads—a stereological study using the zeb-
Mills LJ, Chichester C (2005) Review of evidence: are endocrine rafish (Danio rerio) as model. Aquat Toxicol 124:1–10
disrupting chemicals in the aquatic environment impacting fish Simpson MJA (1968) The display of the Siamese fighting fish Betta
populations? Sci Total Environ 343:1–34 splendens. Anim Behav Monogr 1:1–73
Muller M, Rabenoelina F, Balaguer P, Patureau D, Lemenach K, Söffker M, Stevens JR, Tyler CR (2012) Comparative breeding and
Budzinski H, Barcelo D, De Alda ML, Kuster M, Delgenes JP, behavioral responses to ethinylestradiol exposure in wild and
Hernandez-Raquet G (2008) Chemical and biological analysis of laboratory maintained zebrafish (Danio rerio) populations.
endocrine-disrupting hormones and estrogenic activity in an Environ Sci Technol 46:11377–11383
advanced sewage treatment plant. Environ Toxicol Chem Stapley J (2008) Female mountain log skinks are more likely to mate
27:1649–1658 with males that court more, not males that are dominant. Anim
Nash JP, Kime DE, van der Ven LT, Wester PW, Brion F, Maack G, Behav 75:529–538
Stahlschmidt-Allner P, Tyler CR (2004) Long-term exposure to Stevenson LM, Brown AC, Montgomery TM, Clotfelter ED (2011)
environmental concentrations of the pharmaceutical ethynyles- Reproductive consequences of exposure to waterborne phytoes-
tradiol causes reproductive failure in fish. Environ Health Per- trogens in male fighting fish, Betta splendens. Arch Environ
spect 112:1725–1733 Contam Toxicol 60:501–510
Palace VP, Evans RE, Wautier KG, Mills KH, Blanchfield PJ, Park BJ Sumpter JP (2005) Endocrine disruptors in the aquatic environment:
et al. (2009) Interspecies differences in biochemical, histopatho- an overview. Acta Hydroch Hydrob 33:9–16
logical, and population responses in four wild fish species Ternes TA, Kreckel P, Mueller J (1999) Behavior and occurrence of
exposed to ethinylestradiol added to a whole lake. Can J Fish estrogens in municipal sewage treatment plants--II. Aerobic batch
Aquat Sci 66:1920–1935 experiments with activated sludge. Sci Total Environ 12:91–99
Persaud KN, Galef Jr. BG (2005) Female Japanese quail (Coturnix Tyler CR, Jobling S, Sumpter JP (1998) Endocrine disruption in
japonica) mated with males that harassed them are unlikely to lay wildlife: a critical review of the evidence. Crit Rev Toxicol
fertilized eggs. J Comp Psychol 119:440–446 28:319–361
Peterson EK, Buckwalter DB, Kerby JL, LeFauve MK, Varian-Ramos Verbeek P, Iwamoto T, Murakami N (2008) Differences in aggression
CW, Swaddle JP (2017) Integrative behavioral ecotoxicology: between wild-type and domesticated fighting fish are context
bringing together fields to establish new insight to behavioral dependent. Anim Behav 73:75–83
ecology, toxicology, and conservation. Curr Zool. https://doi.org/ Volkova K, Reyhanian N, Kot-Wasik A, Olsén H, Porsch-Hällström I,
10.1093/cz/zox010 Hallgren S (2012) Brain circuit imprints of developmental 17α-
Purdom CE, Hardiman PA, Bye VJ, Eno NC, Tyler CR, Sumpter JP ethinylestradiol exposure in guppies (Poecilia reticulata): per-
(1994) Estrogenic effects of effluents from sewage treatment sistent effects on anxiety but not on reproductive behavior. Gen
works. Chem Ecol 8:275–285 Comp Endocrinol 178:282–290
Rowland WJ (1982) Mate choice by male sticklebacks, Gasterosteus Zala SM, Penn DJ (2004) Abnormal behaviors induced by chemical
aculeatus. Anim Behav 30:1093–1098 pollution: a review of the evidence and new challenges. Anim
Saaristo M, Craft JA, Lehtonen KK, Lindstrom K (2010) Exposure to Behav 68:649–664
17α-ethinyl oestradiol impairs courtship and aggressive

You might also like