You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Gerontology: PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCES Copyright 2002 by The Gerontological Society of America

2002, Vol. 57B, No. 4, P358–P366

Fear of Death in Older Adults: Predictions From


Terror Management Theory
Victor G. Cicirelli

Purdue University, Lafayette, Indiana.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


Terror management theory asserts that death fear (fear of annihilation) is buffered by self-esteem and beliefs in
literal and symbolic immortality achieved through participation in the cultural system. The aims of this study
were to determine how variables suggested by the theory were related to fear of death measures. Participants
were 123 Black and 265 White elders aged 60 to 100 years; they were assessed on the Multidimensional Fear of
Death Scale (MFODS), self-esteem, religiosity, locus of control, socioeconomic status, social support, and health.
Regression analysis findings ( p , .05) offered partial support to the theory, with greater Fear of the Unknown
(fear of annihilation) related to weaker religiosity, less social support, and greater externality; the effect of self-
esteem was mediated by externality. Other predictors were related to an overall fear score based on the remaining
7 MFODS subscales. Findings are interpreted in terms of changing sources of self-esteem in old age.

F EAR of death, or death anxiety, is a common phenome-


non in all societies and is often regarded as the prime
motivation for human behavior (Becker, 1973). Although
The purpose of the present study was twofold: (a) to ex-
tend the application of the theory to older adults and to fear
of death measured at the level of conscious awareness, and
some earlier authors made a distinction between death anxi- (b) to test predictions from the theory which would explain
ety and fear of death, most now use the terms interchange- the lower fear of death among older adults.
ably. Additionally, fear of death is now generally regarded
as a multidimensional concept (Lester, 1994; Neimeyer & Terror Management Theory (TMT)
Moore, 1994; Wong, Reker, & Gesser, 1994), with most The basic assumption of TMT (Becker, 1973; Greenberg
measuring instruments now assessing several aspects of fear. et al., 1997; Solomon et al., 1991a, 1991b) is that all hu-
Fear of death among older adults is not well understood. mans are instinctively driven toward survival and continued
Although existing research is somewhat inconsistent, most existence, while at the same time have knowledge of their
evidence leads to the conclusion that fear of death tends to inevitable mortality. As a result, there is a potential for them
be greater among younger age groups and declines with in- to consciously experience a terror of death. The core of
creasing age (Bengtson, Cuellar, & Ragan, 1977; Gesser, death fear is fear of annihilation, which refers to the extinc-
Wong, & Reker, 1987–1988; Neimeyer & Van Brunt, 1995; tion of mind, spirit, and soul, as well as the destruction of
Thorson & Powell, 1991, 2000). A recent meta-analysis of the body—that is, total nonexistence. According to these
49 studies of fear of death in older adults (Fortner, Nei- theorists, the experience of such death terror would be para-
meyer, & Rybarczyk, 2000) found that fear of death was es- lyzing without some means of suppressing it from aware-
sentially stable over the age range 61–87. The few available ness. The basic task of the theory is to identify the factors
studies of the oldest old (Johnson & Barer, 1997; Tobin, helping to maintain this suppression.
1991) used interviews rather than quantitative measures and Terror management theorists (Greenberg, Pyszczynski,
reported that the oldest old had no fear of death itself al- Solomon, Simon, & Breus, 1994; Greenberg et al., 1997;
though they did fear the dying process. Solomon et al., 1991a, 1991b) assert that, over time, indi-
Overall, the best interpretation of existing findings is that viduals in various societies developed cultural worldviews
fear of death declines over the years of middle adulthood that characterized the universe or society as having rational-
but does not continue declining in old age. This seems para- ity, predictability, and permanence. Socialization into any of
doxical in that one might expect elders to have a greater fear these cultural worldviews provides protection against fear
of death than younger people in view of the fact that in- of annihilation, because it allows people to create standards
creasing age and frailty render them ever more vulnerable to of value for a meaningful life as well as ways of transcend-
death. To understand why this should be the case, one can ing death. If individuals become socialized and meet the
turn to theory in the hope of finding an explanation. At standards valued by the culture, they attain a greater self-
present, terror management theory (Greenberg, Solomon, & esteem and a promise of immortality.
Pyszczynski, 1997; Solomon, Greenberg, & Pyszczynski, Self-esteem is regarded as the primary psychological
1991a, 1991b) seems to be the most comprehensive of the mechanism whereby culture acts as a buffer to facilitate the
existing approaches dealing with the cause and control of individual’s suppression of death fear (Greenberg et al.,
fear of death (Tomer, 1994) and is supported by extensive 1994; Greenberg et al., 1997; Solomon et al., 1991a,
empirical research. 1991b). Developing early in life as children interact with

P358
TERROR MANAGEMENT AND FEAR OF DEATH P359

and meet the standards of their parents, the process is car- the ideas of TMT. Variables related to the suppression of
ried forward into adulthood when individuals maintain or fear by adhering to the standards and values of the culture
enhance their self-esteem by learning the teachings of the and through the promise of literal and symbolic immortality
culture, participating in cultural rituals, attaining goals val- (self-esteem, religious belief, locus of control, social sup-
ued by the culture, fulfilling cultural roles, experiencing so- port, and status within society) may be hypothesized to also
cial validation in personal relationships, and using defensive mitigate fears that come to awareness from time to time.
responses when self-esteem is threatened. Within this context, one might examine existing research on
The culture includes beliefs, values, and norms that allow fear of death in older adults to see if there is any empirical
the individual to create the illusion of being protected from evidence that TMT variables are related to fear of death

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


death or eventually transcending it (Greenberg et al., 1994; assessed at the level of awareness.
Greenberg et al., 1997; Solomon et al., 1991a, 1991b). By Unfortunately, only a few studies relate to major TMT
believing in the cultural systems, individuals can achieve variables. (Most studies of fear of death in old age have fo-
both literal and symbolic immortality (Lifton, 1983) and cused on the relationship of fear to age and various demo-
thus reduce the fear of death. Literal immortality refers to graphic and health variables.) With regard to self-esteem, no
the belief that a noncorporeal aspect of the individual will studies of older adults were found, but there was evidence
live on indefinitely in some way. It is attained through reli- that among college students, those with higher self-esteem
gious beliefs concerned with a soul and afterlife. Symbolic had less fear of death (Davis, Martin, Wilee, & Voorhees,
immortality refers to the belief that individuals are repre- 1978); this may also be true for older adults. Existing evi-
sented by something or someone other than themselves that dence regarding locus of control is mixed, with some
will continue to exist after they are dead. Individuals may studies linking externality to greater death anxiety and inter-
continue to live symbolically by viewing children as exten- nality to less death anxiety and other studies reporting no re-
sions of themselves that continue through time, by feeling that lationship (Hayslip & Stewart-Bussey, 1986–1987; Hunt,
they are valued parts of a culture that endures over time and Lester, & Ashton, 1988; Schulz, 1978). The effects of reli-
beyond individual death, by making a permanent mark in the gion on fear of death are complex, depending on whether re-
world through things that they produce or achieve, and so on. ligious behaviors or intrinsic religiosity (faith in God and an
TMT suggests two distinct modes of responding to death afterlife) are measured. Studies of fear of death in older
concerns (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999). First, adults (Clement, 1998; Fortner et al., 2000) support the rela-
individuals may use such defense mechanisms as distrac- tionship between intrinsic religiosity and reduced fear of
tion, distancing themselves, and denying their vulnerability death. No empirical studies were found linking socioeco-
to help remove death threats from immediate focal aware- nomic status (SES) and fear of death. The only study deal-
ness. Stimuli initially in awareness lead to suppression of ing with social support involved AIDS patients; those with
concerns with death, but such concerns apparently continue more family support had less fear of death (Catania, Turner,
outside of awareness, somehow signaling and motivating Choi, & Coates, 1992).
the defenses of self-esteem and faith in the worldview Beyond the five major TMT variables discussed above, it
(Greenberg et al., 1997). It seems that this signal effect fur- can be argued that women and ethnic minority group mem-
ther suggests that self-esteem and faith in the worldview bers have less accessibility to the rewards and acceptance of
might be related to fear of death at the level of conscious the culture, implying less belief in the values of the culture,
awareness as well as outside of awareness. and ultimately a greater fear of death. Existing studies offer
However, processes at different levels of awareness ap- only slight support to this contention. In general, although
pear to run parallel to one another, dealing with death fears many fear of death studies found no gender differences,
of different duration and intensity. Terror management theo- whenever differences were found, women reported greater
rists seem to admit that there is some kind of connection be- fear than men (Neimeyer & Van Brunt, 1995). With regard
tween these parallel processes (Greenberg et al., 1994). to gender differences in old age, the meta-analysis of Fort-
Thus, death concerns at the level of immediate awareness ner and colleagues (2000) failed to find a significant gender
both activate conscious defense mechanisms and signal the effect. Studies of ethnic differences between Whites and
need for mechanisms to deal with fear of annihilation out- Blacks have had mixed findings (Cicirelli, 2000; Neimeyer
side the level of awareness. In such a two-step theory, fear & Van Brunt, 1995). Overall, no clear findings exist regard-
of annihilation experienced at the level of awareness in re- ing fear of death in relation to either gender or ethnicity.
sponse to death stimuli may be incompletely suppressed by It can be argued that two other variables, poor health and
transitory defense mechanisms or maintained in conscious- loss of spouse, imply a sense of increasing vulnerability to
ness for a longer duration before being reduced to a man- death, with the increased salience of death associated with
ageable level by mechanisms that were signaled and elicited greater fear. Existing studies offer some evidence for a
to deal with fear outside the level of awareness. greater fear of death among those in poor health. Mullins
and Lopez (1982), in a study of nursing home patients,
Extension of TMT to Fear at the Level found that fear of death was greater among an older, sicker
of Conscious Awareness group than among those who were younger and healthier.
Assuming that death is made salient and fears are aroused Although some mixed findings regarding health exist, the
at the level of conscious awareness by the administration of Fortner and colleagues (2000) meta-analysis found that
a fear of death instrument, then the study of fear of death at greater physical and mental health problems were found to
the level of awareness would be justified as an extension of predict higher levels of death anxiety.
P360 CICIRELLI

Study Hypotheses programs serving retirement housing complexes. I visited


Even though not all of them are supported by the limited each center during a regularly scheduled group meeting,
existing literature, major hypotheses based on TMT are as explained the study, and asked for participants. All who
follows: consented were scheduled to be interviewed at the center
at a convenient time. Participation rates at the 20 centers
1. Individuals with more positive self-esteem will have less ranged from 40% to 85%, with an overall participation rate
fear of death. of 66%.
2. Individuals with an internal locus of control are expected Of 460 people who consented to participate in the study,
to experience less fear of death, and, conversely, individ- 447 were interviewed (13 began the interview process, but
uals with an external locus of control orientation are pre-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


declined to continue). The sample used for the analyses re-
dicted to have greater fear of death. ported here consisted of 388 older people for whom all data
3. Individuals with a strong support group of others with
were complete, ranging in age from 60 to 100 (M 5 72.65,
similar cultural beliefs will have less fear of death.
SD 5 7.73). There were 285 women (73%) and 103 men
4. Individuals of higher SES levels within the society will
(27%), 265 Whites (68%) and 123 Blacks (32%), and 293
have less fear of death.
from Indianapolis and 95 from Lafayette. All Blacks in the
5. Individuals with stronger religious beliefs will have less
sample were from Indianapolis. (Preliminary analyses by
fear of death.
Cicirelli, 1997, indicated only minor differences between
In addition, several predictions are made regarding the Lafayette White and Indianapolis White samples, so
health and demographic variables. Increasing age and poor samples from the two sites were combined for further analy-
health would each seem to imply increasing nearness to sis.) With regard to marital status, about 35% of participants
death, with a heightened sense of vulnerability, and, conse- were married with a living spouse, with the remainder either
quently, greater fear of death. Similarly, loss of spouse widowed, divorced, separated, or never married. The mean
through widowhood or divorce may leave the survivor with educational level (M 5 4.42, SD 5 1.45) was between that
an increased sense of vulnerability and greater fear of death. of a high school graduate and a high school graduate with
Finally, women and minority group members are expected some additional training. The mean occupational level (M 5
to have greater fear of death. 3.72, SD 5 1.55) was between that of a skilled manual oc-
The present study examined the relationship of the hy- cupation and a clerical, technical, or salesclerk occupation.
pothesized TMT variables to fear of death. By so doing, the The mean health rating of participants (M 5 4.35, SD 5
study not only increases the understanding of factors influ- 0.90) was between “good” and “very good.” The means and
encing fear of death at the level of conscious awareness in standard deviations of study participants on background and
old age, but also advances existing knowledge of fear of psychosocial variables are summarized in Table 1.
death by investigating its relationship to personality and When I compared the sample of 388 elders who com-
background variables that either have not been investigated pleted all measures with the total sample of 447 study
in older adults or have not been studied in a sample contain- participants on key demographic variables, there was no dif-
ing large numbers of elders beyond age 75 years. ference in gender composition, ethnic composition, or self-
rated health. However, those persons completing all mea-
METHODS sures were slightly younger (72.6 compared to 73.2 years),
of higher educational level (4.4 compared to 4.2), and of
Sample higher occupational level (3.7 compared to 3.6) than the
I obtained the sample of 388 study participants from two total group. Overall, there was little difference between
sites: a medium-sized Midwestern city (Greater Lafayette, the two samples.
IN, with approximately 100,000 residents) and a large urban
area (Indianapolis, IN, with a population of approximately
1,250,000). Study participants were at least 60 years of age,
Table 1. Means and SDs of Total Group (N 5 388)
were living in private homes or apartments in the commu- on Study Variables
nity, and were alert, oriented, and of sufficient cognitive
ability to respond to the interview-questionnaire (as judged Variable Mean SD Item Meana Item SD
by the interviewer or the senior center director). Age 72.65 7.73
I sampled elders through seniors’ organizations, first se- Educational Level 4.42 1.45
curing a representation of such organizations in each site Occupational Level 3.72 1.55
and then interviewing elders within organizations. I col- SES 43.71 15.55
lected data at 20 different centers, 16 in Indianapolis and 4 Health 4.35 0.90
Religiosity 14.14 2.33
in Lafayette, selected to represent different geographic areas Externality 55.92 17.54
of the two cities and thus a wide range of socioeconomic Self-Esteem 32.35 4.66
status levels. The Indianapolis site included 12 senior pro- Social Support 1.14 0.36
grams in community centers and 4 multidenominational Total Fear of Death 102.35 23.33 2.77 0.63
church-operated centers; 11 of the 16 centers served both Fear of the Unknown 10.30 4.54 2.06 0.91
Black and White seniors. The Lafayette site included one Note: SES 5 socioeconomic status.
community-operated senior center serving the entire city, aEach item mean equals the corresponding scale mean divided by the number

one interdenominational church-operated center, and two of items in the scale.


TERROR MANAGEMENT AND FEAR OF DEATH P361

Measures that it reflects fear and uncertainty that transcendence of the


noncorporeal self will take place following death. Thus, one
Fear of death.—To assess fear of death in the present might expect the set of predictor variables derived above
study, I used the Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale from TMT to be more strongly related to Fear of the Un-
(MFODS; Hoelter, 1979; Neimeyer & Moore, 1994). The known than to the Fear of the Known factor comprising the
instrument consists of 42 5-point items yielding eight sub- other MFODS subscales.
scales. The subscales include Fear of the Dying Process
(including painful or violent deaths), Fear of the Dead (in- Religiosity.—To assess subjective (or intrinsic) aspects
cluding avoidance of human or animal bodies), Fear of Be- of religiosity, three items drawn from the work of Markides

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


ing Destroyed (including cremation or dissection of the (1983) and Krause (1993) were used: importance of religion
body for autopsy or organ transplants), Fear for Significant (a 5-point item), God (a 6-point item), and private prayer in
Others (including apprehension about the impact of the re- the respondent’s life (a 5-point item). The score is the sum
spondent’s death on others), Fear of the Unknown (includ- of the item scores and could range from 3 to 16 for subjec-
ing fear of nonexistence and lack of knowledge about after- tive religiosity, with a high score indicating greater religios-
life), Fear of Conscious Death (including concerns about ity. Cronbach’s a for the participants in this study was .79.
falsely being declared dead), Fear for the Body After Death
(including concern about decay and isolation of the body), Self-esteem.—The measure of self-esteem used was the
and Fear of Premature Death (concerns about being unable Rosenberg (1965) Self-Esteem Scale. The scale consists of
to accomplish desired goals or experiences). Appropriate 10 items, each with a 4-point response scale; the total score
items are summed to yield subscores, with items coded so is the sum of the item scores, with a high score indicating
that a high score indicates greater fear of death. Neimeyer greater self-esteem. Estimates of internal consistency reli-
and Moore (1994) reported internal consistency reliabilities ability for the instrument range from .77 to .88, with test–
(Cronbach’s a) for the subscales ranging from .65 to .82 and retest reliability ranging from .82 to .85. Cronbach’s a for
test–retest reliabilities over a 3-week period ranging from the participants of this study was .78. Considerable evidence
.61 to .81. In addition, they reported factor analytic evidence for the convergent and discriminant validity of the scale ex-
for the subscales as well as evidence for construct validity. ists (Robinson, Shaver, & Wrightsman, 1991).
Cronbach’s a for the participants in the present study
ranged from .65 to .81 for the subscales, values considered Locus of control.—Levenson’s (1981) Multidimensional
adequate for studies involving group comparisons. Locus of Control measure was used to assess respondents’
In an effort to reduce the eight dependent variables of the locus of control beliefs. The measure consists of 24 items,
study (the MFODS subscores) to a smaller number, I carried each with a 7-point response scale indicating degree of
out a principal components analysis with varimax rotation agreement. Scores for Internality, Powerful Others, and
(see Table 2). One strong factor labeled Fear of the Known Chance, obtained by summing the appropriate eight items in
(on which seven of the eight subscores had loadings above each case, indicate belief in internal control of events, con-
0.3), and one weak factor (on which Fear of the Unknown trol by powerful others, and control by chance. Scores on
had the only loading above 0.3), were identified. Accord- each subscale could range from 8 to 56, with a high score
ingly, I used two MFODS scores as dependent variables in indicating belief that the locus of control was of the given
the analysis: Fear of the Unknown and Fear of the Known type. Levenson reported Cronbach’s a for the three sub-
(the sum of the remaining seven MFODS subscores). scales ranging from .64 to .73, as well as evidence for the
Fear of the Unknown most closely approximates fear of scales’ validity. In addition to the above subscores, the Pow-
annihilation, the core fear of TMT. The remaining subscales erful Others and Chance scores can be summed to yield a
are concerned with fears of various known aspects of death, total Externality score. Cronbach’s a of the three subscores
dying, and the fate of the body, but do not deal with the fear for the participants of this study ranged from .69 to .75;
of annihilation of the self. Fear of the Unknown differs in Cronbach’s a for Externality was .86. Because Internality
had negligible relationships with the Fear of Death sub-
Table 2. Factor Loadings of Eight MFODS Subscores scores in preliminary analysis whereas Externality had
stronger correlations, only the Externality score was used in
Factor
this study.
MFODS Subscore I II
Fear of Dying Process .79 .15
Perceived social support.—One indicator of social sup-
Fear of the Dead .69 2.14 port is the number and closeness of people perceived by an
Fear of Being Destroyed .47 2.29 individual to be in his or her support network. To assess this,
Fear for Significant Others .62 2.08 the circles technique from Antonucci and Akiyama’s (1987)
Fear of Conscious Death .68 .16 Social Networks in Adult Life Survey was used. The investi-
Fear for the Body After Death .73 .15 gator asks respondents to identify persons who are “close
Fear of Premature Death .65 .21
Fear of the Unknown .21 .79
and important” to them at each of three levels of closeness
depicted by concentric circles, with the inner circle includ-
Eigenvalue 3.23 1.04 ing those persons who are very close, the middle circle in-
% of variance explained 40.34 13.04
cluding those who are close, and the outer circle including
Note: MFODS 5 Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale. those who are not so close. Scores consist of the number of
P362 CICIRELLI

persons named at each level (inner, middle, and outer). Evi- 18 participants who had visual impairments or who suffered
dence for validity includes the expected correlations with from arthritis which made writing difficult.
indicators of psychological health, and a correlation be-
tween respondents’ and their significant others’ reports of RESULTS
social support. For the present study, I used the sum of scores
for the inner and middle levels as an indicator of the size of Differences in Fear of Death Scores
the respondent’s close support network. Because the distri- The means and standard deviations of the two fear of death
bution of scores on this measure was skewed, I applied a log factors are presented in Table 1. The means for Fear of the
transformation before data analysis. Known and Fear of the Unknown are not directly compara-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


ble because they contain differing numbers of items; there-
Health.—A single-item self-rating of health was used, fore, each mean score was divided by its respective number
with ratings on a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (very poor) to of items to get a corresponding item mean for comparison
6 (excellent). Although participants also completed the In- purposes. When the item means are compared, it can be
strumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) measure seen that participants’ Fear of the Known is stronger (M 5
(Lawton, 1972), the mean IADL score was very low, indi- 2.77) than Fear of the Unknown (M 5 2.06).
cating little or no functional impairment for most study par-
ticipants. For this reason, I did not use the IADL score in Correlations of Fear of Death Scores With
data analysis. Background and Psychosocial Variables
As a first step in examining the relationship of the back-
Demographic background.—I assessed several demo- ground variables (ethnicity, gender, age, health, and marital
graphic variables: ethnicity (0 5 White; 1 5 Black), gender status) and the psychosocial variables (religiosity, external-
(0 5 male; 1 5 female), chronological age in years, marital ity, self-esteem, social support, and SES) with two fear of
status (0 5 married; 1 5 widowed, divorced, or unmarried), death scores, I computed intercorrelations of the study vari-
educational level, and occupational level (using occupation ables. These are presented in Table 3.
before retirement). Educational level and occupational level Looking at the relationship with Fear of the Unknown
were coded using the 7-point Hollingshead (1957) scales, first, all 5 TMT variables were significantly correlated ( p ,
with 7 representing the highest levels. I computed Hollings- .05), although the correlations with self-esteem and SES
head’s SES index as the sum of 4 3 the educational level 1 were very weak. The correlation of religiosity with Fear of
7 3 the occupational level. the Unknown (r 5 2.45) was the strongest of all correla-
tions obtained. Those elders with greater religiosity, more
Procedure positive self-esteem, more social support, higher SES, and a
Interviewers administered interview-questionnaires to less external locus of control had less Fear of the Unknown.
study participants in a separate activity room at the senior None of the 5 background variables (ethnicity, gender, age,
center which they attended. Most elders completed the health, and marital status) were significantly related.
interview-questionnaire independently in a small group set- In the case of Fear of the Known, 4 of the 5 TMT vari-
ting (from 3 to 10 persons), with two interviewers present to ables were significantly correlated, although the correlations
answer any questions or to clarify misunderstandings. De- with religiosity, self-esteem, and SES were very weak.
pending on the number of elders who wished to partici- Those elders with a less external locus of control, greater re-
pate at a given site, interviewers arranged to administer the ligiosity, more positive self-esteem, and higher SES had less
interview-questionnaire at several convenient times. On av- Fear of the Known. Of the background variables, only gen-
erage, the interview-questionnaire took about an hour and a der and health were significantly related. Men and those in
half to complete. Individual interviews were conducted for better health had less Fear of the Known.

Table 3. Intercorrelations of MFODS Scores, Background Variables, and Psychosocial Variables (N 5 388)
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. Fear of the Known —
2. Fear of the Unknown 35** —
3. Ethnicity 205 01 —
4. Gender 17** 208 209 —
5. Age 203 201 225** 11* —
6. Health 212* 203 213* 04 06 —
7. Marital Status 05 06 201 34** 17** 203 —
8. Religiosity 210* 245** 19** 20** 03 202 06 —
9. Externality 30** 31** 202 201 12* 206 08 207 —
10. Self-Esteem 211* 211* 11* 04 210 21** 03 03 221** —
11. Social Support 07 224** 201 14** 207 03 210 23** 210* 06 —
12. SES 213* 210* 226** 208 202 24** 211* 206 229** 16** 11* —

Note: MFODS 5 Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale; SES 5 socioeconomic status.


*p , .05; **p , .01.
TERROR MANAGEMENT AND FEAR OF DEATH P363

Table 4. Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses for Variables variable, while at the same time the relationship of self-
Predicting Fear of the Known and Fear of the Unknown (N 5 388) esteem to the dependent variable was reduced or eliminated
Variable B SE B b R Adj. R2
in the presence of the mediating variable. Neither religiosity
nor social support met the first criterion, because the corre-
Fear of the Known lation of self-esteem with religiosity was only .03 and the
Marital status 20.70 2.61 2.01
Ethnicity 22.12 2.66 2.04
correlation of self-esteem with social support was only .06.
Health 22.91 1.31 2.11* However, self-esteem was significantly related to externality
Age 20.24 0.16 2.08 (r 5 2.21, p , .01), and externality was in turn signifi-
Gender 10.08 2.88 .19** cantly related to Fear of the Unknown (r 5 2.30). (A sim-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


Externality 0.38 0.07 .29** ple path analysis including only externality and self-esteem
Social support 5.60 3.59 .08 as independent variables was carried out to further verify
Self-esteem 20.17 0.26 2.03
Religiosity 21.22 0.51 2.12*
the indirect effect of self-esteem on Fear of the Unknown.
SES 20.06 0.08 2.04 The direct path coefficients were .30 for externality and
.40** .14 2.07 for self-esteem. Thus, the direct relationship between
self-esteem and Fear of the Unknown was reduced from its
Fear of the Unknown
Marital status 0.44 0.46 .05
correlation of 2.11 between these variables in the absence
Ethnicity 0.78 0.47 .08 of externality to 2.07 when externality was present. The in-
Health 0.04 0.23 .01 direct effect of self-esteem on Fear of the Unknown when
Age 20.02 0.03 2.04 mediated by externality was 2.06. The size of the indirect
Gender 0.13 0.50 .01 effect was the product of the coefficients for the effect of
Externality 0.07 0.01 .28** self-esteem on externality and the effect of externality on
Social support 21.22 0.60 2.10*
Self-esteem 20.04 0.04 2.04
Fear of the Unknown, or 2.21 3 .30. This small but signifi-
Religiosity 20.83 0.09 2.43** cant effect still has relevance from a theoretical perspec-
SES 20.01 0.01 2.01 tive.) Therefore, one can conclude that self-esteem had an
.56** .29 effect on Fear of the Unknown which was mediated by ex-
Note: SES 5 socioeconomic status.
ternality. That is, higher self-esteem was associated with a
*p , .05; **p , .01. less external locus of control, which was in turn associated
with a lower Fear of the Unknown.

Fear of the Known.—Of the 5 TMT variables, only ex-


Regression of Fear of Death Scores on Background ternality (b 5 .29) and religiosity (b 5 2.12) were signifi-
and Psychosocial Variables cant (p , .05) predictors of Fear of the Known. Self-
Multiple regression analysis was used to determine the esteem, social support, and SES were not significantly re-
relationship between the background and psychosocial vari- lated. A more external locus of control and lower religiosity
ables and the two dependent variables: Fear of the Unknown were related to greater Fear of the Known. Of the five back-
and Fear of the Known. These analyses are summarized in ground variables, only gender (b 5 .19) and health (b 5
Table 4. 2.11) were significant predictors. In this case, women and
those in poorer health had greater Fear of the Known. Over-
Fear of the Unknown.—Of the 5 TMT variables, only 3 all, the multiple regression coefficient R was .40, with the
were significant predictors (p , .05) of Fear of the Un- adjusted R2 of .14 indicating that the 2 TMT variables and 2
known in the multiple regression analysis: religiosity (b 5 background variables accounted for 14% of the variance of
2.43), externality (b 5 .28), and social support (b 5 Fear of the Known.
2.10). Self-esteem and SES were not significantly related. Using a similar procedure to that used for Fear of the Un-
Greater Fear of the Unknown was predicted by lower religi- known, externality was also found to act as a mediating vari-
osity, a more external locus of control, and weaker social able for self-esteem in its effect on Fear of the Known. Higher
support. None of the background variables were significant self-esteem was associated with a less external locus of con-
predictors. Overall, the multiple regression coefficient R trol, which was associated in turn with less Fear of the Known.
was .56, with the adjusted R2 of .29 indicating that the 3
TMT variables accounted for 29% of the variance of Fear of DISCUSSION
the Unknown. Findings of this study provide partial support for the hy-
It was puzzling that self-esteem, a core variable in TMT, pothesis that cultural worldview variables suggested by
was not a significant predictor of Fear of the Unknown in TMT are related to fear of death assessed at the level of im-
view of the fact that it had a significant, although weak, mediate awareness. Predictions regarding the relationships
univariate correlation with the fear variable. One possibility of religiosity, externality, and social support to fear of anni-
was that the effect of self-esteem might have been mediated hilation were supported. However, one important element of
by one of the other TMT variables. Therefore, I examined the hypothesis, that higher self-esteem would be associated
each of the three significant predictors in turn for this possi- with less fear of annihilation (as assessed by Fear of the Un-
bility. To qualify as a mediating variable for self-esteem, known on the MFODS), was only partially supported. Al-
self-esteem needed to be significantly related to the mediat- though a very weak correlation (r 5 2.11) between self-
ing variable, which was in turn related to the dependent esteem and fear of annihilation was found, self-esteem was
P364 CICIRELLI

not a significant predictor of Fear of the Unknown in the re- scious level, a level of fantasy or imagery, and a noncon-
gression analysis. However, it did have an indirect effect scious level. The way of thinking about TMT in relation to
mediated by externality. fear of death at the level of immediate awareness as dis-
One possible explanation for the weak effect of self- cussed in this article fits in with Feifel’s thinking; it is an ex-
esteem is that, on the one hand, its effect in suppressing fear tension of the theory to the level of immediate awareness
of annihilation takes place only outside of conscious aware- without denying its basic tenets regarding fear of death out-
ness, and that different variables are important in dealing side of awareness. Certainly more research into the relation-
with fear at the level of immediate awareness. ship between these levels of awareness and the role of self-
On the other hand, self-esteem may not be an important esteem in dealing with fear of death at each level in an older

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


variable in relation to fear of death in a population of older population is needed.
adults except in terms of its indirect effect. Further research Although generativity can aid in maintaining self-esteem,
is needed here. Several aspects of the roles of self-esteem it is also a means of achieving symbolic immortality
and fear of death in the lives of elderly persons seem puz- through transmission of one’s achievements in the culture to
zling. In a recent chapter, McCoy, Pyszczynski, Solomon, future generations (Solomon et al., 1991a). TMT also holds
and Greenberg (2000) pointed out that although one would that religious faith leads to the promise of literal immortal-
expect aging adults to have increased fear of death accom- ity, the continuation of life in some form after bodily death.
panying their greater vulnerability in view of increasing age, Expectations of both symbolic and literal immortality
illness, and general frailty, fear of death studies have consis- achieved through participation in the cultural worldview are
tently shown them to have a reduced fear of death compared hypothesized to reduce fear of death. This expectation was
to younger adults. At the same time, one would expect de- supported by the findings regarding religiosity. Not only did
fenses against fear of annihilation to weaken because many the older adults of this study manifest high intrinsic religios-
sources of self-esteem are no longer available to aging indi- ity, but it was substantially related to lower fear of annihila-
viduals. They are no longer able to meet the standards of the tion. Further research should attempt to more carefully dis-
culture due to declines in certain physical and cognitive tinguish sources of literal and symbolic immortality (Lifton,
abilities, obsolescence of previous roles in life that are no 1983) and to include adequate measures of each in order to
longer valued in a time of technological advances, loss of determine the contribution of each type of belief on fear of
control over events, and the erosion of contemporary social annihilation.
supports making it more difficult to achieve social valida- Feifel (1990) is concerned with the multidimensionality
tion. According to McCoy and colleagues (2000), “The el- of fear of death, as are most contemporary researchers,
derly face a chronic state of mortality salience coupled with whereas TMT is concerned only with the basic fear of anni-
a possible lessened capacity to use the cultural worldview or hilation. Thus, in applying the theory when using multi-
resulting self-esteem to maximally protect the self” (p. 45). dimensional measures, one would expect the set of predic-
Yet, existing research (Bengtson, Reedy, & Gordon, 1985; tors to be more strongly related to the fear of annihilation
Dietz, 1996) indicates that self-esteem remains high for (Fear of the Unknown on the MFODS) than to other fears.
most older adults. Study findings confirmed this expectation, providing further
McCoy and cohorts (2000) go on to suggest that older evidence in support of TMT.
adults use alternative routes for self-esteem maintenance Although the 5 background variables (ethnicity, gender,
other than through the consensual worldview favored by age, marital status, and health) were unrelated to Fear of the
younger adults. These include socioemotional selectivity Unknown (annihilation), gender and health were related to
(Carstensen, 1992) to minimize contact with those threaten- the Fear of the Known (a composite of other subscales of
ing the self-concept, selective optimization with compensa- the MFODS), supporting the universality of TMT across
tion (Baltes, 1997), downward adjustment of standards to various levels of these background variables.
minimize discrepancy between real and ideal self (Ryff, Predictors suggested by TMT accounted for a much
1991), cognitive reframing of events with optimistic reinter- greater portion of the variance in Fear of the Unknown
pretation, and reduced reliance on social consensus. Addi- (29%) than in Fear of the Known (14%), thereby supporting
tionally, elders are able to maintain self-esteem as well as the proposition that identification with the culture serves to
gain a sense of death transcendence through generativity, in buffer the individual against fear arising from awareness of
which they seek to help future generations. However, the vulnerability to total annihilation.
question remains: If self-esteem is maintained throughout The role of locus of control in reducing fear of death was
old age, why isn’t it directly related to fear of death at the supported by the findings regarding externality. That is, fear
level of immediate awareness? of death was greater among those elders who felt that envi-
Another puzzling aspect of the relation of self-esteem to ronmental events came about by chance or were controlled
fear of death is elders’ apparent willingness to acknowledge by others who had power over them. Although this finding is
the inevitability of death and deal with it at the level of im- not unexpected in view of previous literature (e.g., Hayslip
mediate awareness. In this regard, Galt and Hayslip (1998) & Stewart-Bussey, 1986–1987; Hunt et al., 1988; Schulz,
found that, compared to younger adults, older adults evi- 1978), the lack of a relationship between fear of death and in-
denced higher levels of overt death fear but lower levels of ternality in preliminary correlations is puzzling. However,
covert fear. If so, they may deal with fear by more direct high internal control might imply invulnerability to death (and
means and gradually come to an acceptance of death. Feifel low fear) for some, whereas for others death might be viewed
(1990) also viewed fear of death at different levels: a con- as uncontrollable (with higher fear). Also, some studies have
TERROR MANAGEMENT AND FEAR OF DEATH P365

found that people become more external in locus of control Acknowledgments


as they grow older (Schulz, 1986; Siegler & Gatz, 1985). This article is based in part on a study supported by the AARP Andrus
If one is serious about extending TMT to fear of death at Foundation.
the level of immediate awareness (while maintaining the ba- Address correspondence to Victor G. Cicirelli, Department of Psycho-
sic theory of processes taking place outside of awareness), logical Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907-1364. E-mail:
further theoretical formulation is needed as to how the dif- victor@psych.purdue.edu
ferent levels of awareness may be coordinated and how self-
esteem and religious faith may be related to fear of death at References
each level of awareness. These topics need to be investi- Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (1987). Social networks in adult life and a

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


gated further from an empirical point of view, as well. Care- preliminary examination of the convoy model. Journal of Gerontology,
fully planned studies manipulating mortality salience and 42, 519–527.
Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny:
measuring self-reported fear of death might tease out differ- Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of develop-
ences in death thoughts at different levels of awareness and mental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380.
accessibility (see Harmon-Jones et al., 1997). Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York: Free Press.
More research is also needed to identify aspects of the Bengtson, V. L., Cuellar, J. B., & Ragan, P. K. (1977). Stratum contrasts
culture that are related to self-esteem and faith in the world- and similarities in attitudes toward death. Journal of Gerontology, 32,
76–88.
view, as recommended by Greenberg and colleagues (1997). Bengtson, V. L., Reedy, M. N., & Gordon, C. (1985). Aging and self-
Finally, TMT needs elaboration in relation to the multivari- conceptions: Personality processes and social contexts. In J. E. Birren
ate conceptions of fear of death. Presumably the fear of an- & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (2nd ed.,
nihilation is basic and all other fears are derived from it. Is pp. 544–594). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Motivation for social contact across the lifespan:
it possible that, under different conditions and with differ- A theory of socioemotional selectivity. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska
ent age groups, the derivative fears also may have im- Symposium on Motivation, 1992. Developmental perspectives on moti-
portance for the theory? The relative strength of Fear of the vation (pp. 209–254). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Known as compared to Fear of the Unknown indicates Catania, J. A., Turner, H. A., Choi, K., & Coates, T. J. (1992). Coping with
the importance of other aspects of fear of death for older death anxiety: Help seeking and social support among gay men with
various HIV diagnoses. AIDS, 6, 999–1005.
people, as does the relationship of certain MFODS sub- Cicirelli, V. G. (1997). Relationship of psychosocial and background vari-
scales to elders’ end-of-life decisions (Cicirelli, 1997). The ables to older adults’ end-of-life decisions. Psychology and Aging, 12,
need to deal both theoretically and empirically with the var- 72–83.
ious dimensions of fear of death (even if derivative) is un- Cicirelli, V. G. (2000). Older adults’ ethnicity, fear of death, and end-of-life
decisions. In A. Tomer (Ed.), Death attitudes and the older adult: The-
derscored by recent research (Tomer, Eliason, & Smith, ories, concepts, and applications (pp. 175–191). Philadelphia: Taylor
1997) that not only found that the relationship of predictor & Francis.
variables to death anxiety depended on the dimension con- Clement, R. (1998). Intrinsic religious motivation and attitudes toward
sidered, but that the dimensionality of death anxiety itself death among the elderly. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learn-
varied with age. ing, Personality, Social, 17, 237–248.
Davis, S. F., Martin, D. A., Wilee, C. T., & Voorhees, J. W. (1978). Rela-
The limitations of this article are acknowledged. It is rec- tionship of fear of death and level of self-esteem in college students.
ognized that the older people participating in the study rep- Psychological Reports, 42, 419–422.
resent a specialized population of elders who take part in ac- Dietz, B. E. (1996). The relationship of aging to self-esteem: The relative
tivities at community senior centers, and this may influence effects of maturation and role accumulation. International Journal of
findings. These elders might be better integrated into the Aging and Human Development, 43, 249–266.
Feifel, H. (1990). Psychology and death: Meaningful rediscovery. Ameri-
larger culture than elders who are loners with few ties to the can Psychologist, 45, 537–543.
community; whether they are as well integrated into the cul- Fortner, B. V., Neimeyer, R. A., & Rybarczyk, B. (2000). Correlates of
ture as elders who participate in other community organiza- death anxiety in older adults: A comprehensive review. In A. Tomer
tions and have many social ties is open to question. Also, (Ed.), Death attitudes and the older adult: Theories, concepts, and ap-
plications (pp. 95–108). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.
better measures of some of the variables hypothesized to be Galt, C. P., & Hayslip, B., Jr. (1998). Age differences in the level of overt
related to fear of death might reveal stronger relationships. and covert death anxiety. Omega, 37, 187–202.
Another limitation is that self-response measures of fear Gesser, G., Wong, P. T. P., & Reker, G. T. (1987–1988). Death attitudes
of death may suffer from social desirability effects, with re- across the life span: The development and validation of the death atti-
spondents reporting less fear than they actually feel. Cer- tude profile (DAP). Omega, 18, 113–128.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., & Breus, M.
tainly, if fear of annihilation is regarded as the core fear of (1994). Role of consciousness and accessibility of death-related
death in TMT, then further testing of the theory requires a thoughts in mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social
more fully developed measure of this aspect of death fear Psychology, 67, 627–637.
than the 5-item Fear of the Unknown subscale of the MFODS. Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management
theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments
It was assumed in this study that Fear of the Unknown was and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experi-
an adequate indicator of fear of annihilation, but this needs mental Social Psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61–139). San Diego: Academic
to be demonstrated using a better measure of the concept. Press.
In general, if a valid extension of TMT is to be attempted Harmon-Jones, E., Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S.,
integrating processes occurring both within and outside of & McGregor, H. (1997). Self-esteem, mortality salience, and defense of
the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
awareness, then one can only view this study as exploratory, 72, 24–36.
and far more refined thinking regarding the mechanisms in- Hayslip, B., & Stewart-Bussey, D. (1986–1987). Locus of control—Levels
volved and their testing needs to be done. of death anxiety relationships. Omega, 17, 41–50.
P366 CICIRELLI

Hoelter, J. W. (1979). Multidimensional treatment of fear of death. Journal Ryff, C. D. (1991). Possible selves in adulthood and old age: A tale of shift-
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 996–999. ing horizons. Psychology and Aging, 6, 286–295.
Hollingshead, A. B. (1957). Two-factor index of social position. New Ha- Schulz, R. (1978). The psychology of death, dying, and bereavement. Read-
ven, CT: Author. ing, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Hunt, D. M., Lester, D., & Ashton, N. (1988). Fear of death, locus of con- Schulz, R. (1986). Successful aging: Balancing primary and secondary
trol, and occupation. Psychological Reports, 53, 1022. control. Adult Development and Aging News, 13(3), 3–4.
Johnson, C. L., & Barer, B. M. (1997). Life beyond 85 years: The aura of Siegler, I. E., & Gatz, M. (1985). Age patterns in locus of control. In E. Pal-
survivorship. New York: Springer. more, E. W. Busse, G. L. Maddox, J. B. Nowlin, & I. E. Siegler (Eds.),
Krause, N. (1993). Measuring religiosity in later life. Research on Aging, 2, Normal aging III (pp. 138–143). Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
170–197. Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991a). A terror manage-
Lawton, M. P. (1972). Assessing the competence of older people. In D. ment theory of social behavior: The psychological functions of self-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article/57/4/P358/593449 by guest on 22 December 2021


Kent, R. Kastenbaum, & R. Sherwood (Eds.), Research planning and esteem and cultural worldviews. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in ex-
action for the elderly (pp. 122–143). New York: Behavioral Publications. perimental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 93–159). San Diego: Aca-
Lester, D. (1994). The Collett-Lester Fear of Death Scale. In R. A. Nei- demic Press.
meyer (Ed.), The death anxiety handbook: Research, instrumentation, Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991b). Terror manage-
and application (pp. 45–60). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis. ment theory of self-esteem. In C. R. Snyder & R. D. Forsyth (Eds.),
Levenson, H. (1981). Differentiating among internality, powerful others, Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The health perspective
and chance. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control (pp. 21–40). New York: Pergamon Press.
construct (Vol. 1, pp. 15–63). New York: Academic Press. Thorson, J. A., & Powell, F. C. (1991). Construction of death among those
Lifton, R. J. (1983). The broken connection: On death and continuity of high in intrinsic religious motivation: A factor analytic study. Death
life. New York: Basic Books. Studies, 15, 131–138.
Markides, K. S. (1983). Aging, religiosity, and adjustment: A longitudinal Thorson, J. A., & Powell, F. C. (2000). Death anxiety in younger and older
analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 621–625. adults. In A. Tomer (Ed.), Death attitudes and the older adult: Theo-
McCoy, S. K., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2000). Tran- ries, concepts, and applications (pp. 123–136). Philadelphia: Taylor &
scending the self: A terror management perspective on successful ag- Francis.
ing. In A. Tomer (Ed.), Death attitudes and the older adult: Theories, Tobin, S. S. (1991). Personhood in advanced old age: Implications for
concepts, and applications (pp. 37–63). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis. practice. New York: Springer.
Mullins, L. C., & Lopez, M. A. (1982). Death anxiety among nursing home Tomer, A. (1994). Death anxiety in adult life—Theoretical perspectives. In
residents: A comparison of the young-old and the old-old. Death Edu- R. A. Neimeyer (Ed.), Death anxiety handbook: Research, instrumenta-
cation, 6, 75–86. tion, and application (pp. 3–28). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.
Neimeyer, R. A., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Validity and reliability of the Tomer, A., Eliason, G., & Smith, J. (1997, November). The structure of
Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale. In R. A. Neimeyer (Ed.), Death death anxiety in young and old adults—A simultaneous structural
anxiety handbook: Research, instrumentation, and application (pp. equation models analysis. Paper presented at the 50th annual meeting
103–119). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis. of The Gerontological Society of America, Cincinnati, OH.
Neimeyer, R. A., & Van Brunt, D. (1995). Death anxiety. In H. Wass & R. A. Wong, P. T. P., Reker, G. T., & Gesser, G. (1994). Death Attitude Profile–
Neimeyer (Eds.), Dying: Facing the facts (3rd ed., pp. 49–88). Wash- Revised: A multidimensional measure of attitudes toward death. In
ington, DC: Taylor & Francis. R. A. Neimeyer (Ed.), Death anxiety handbook: Research, instrumen-
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-process tation, and application (pp. 121–148). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.
model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related
thoughts: An extension of terror management theory. Psychological Re-
view, 106, 835–845.
Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. E., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Measures of per-
sonality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego: Academic Press. Received January 31, 2001
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, Accepted September 10, 2001
NJ: Princeton University Press. Decision Editor: Margie E. Lachman, PhD

You might also like