You are on page 1of 10

Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuropsychologia
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

Deficient body structural description contributes to apraxic end-position T


errors in imitation
Hormos Salimi Dafsaria, Anna Doverna, Gereon R. Finka,b, Peter H. Weissa,b,∗
a
Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Center Jülich, Leo-Brandt-Str. 5, 52425, Jülich, Germany
b
Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine and University Hospital Cologne, > University of Cologne,, Kerpener Str. 62, 50937, Cologne, Germany

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Apraxia is a common cognitive deficit after left hemisphere (LH) stroke. It has been suggested that a disturbed
Body structural description (BSD) representation of the human body underlies apraxic imitation deficits. Thus, we here tested the hypothesis that a
Apraxia deficient body structural description (BSD), i.e., a deficient representation of a body part's position (relative to a
Imitation standard human body), contributes to apraxic end-position errors in imitation, while controlling for deficits in
Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping (VLSM)
the semantic representation of the human body (body image, BI) and naming deficits.
Parietal cortex
A quantitative pointing task to assess putative BSD deficits and an apraxia assessment, including imitation and
pantomime tasks, were applied to 27 patients with LH stroke and 19 healthy subjects. While LH stroke patients
without apraxia (n=15) did not differ from control subjects in their pointing performance, patients suffering
from imitation apraxia (n=10) showed a differential deficit when pointing to body parts of other humans
compared to object parts. Voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) revealed an association of these dif-
ferential pointing deficits (indicating a deficient BSD) with lesions in the angular gyrus of the left inferior
parietal cortex.
This first quantitative group study of BSD deficits in LH stroke patients supports the notion that apraxic end-
position errors in imitation are – at least in part – due to a deficient coding of the position of human body parts.

1. Introduction limited set of discrete body parts” (see also (Goldenberg, 2009)).
Therefore, body part coding is considered “an intermediate step be-
The impairments caused by apraxia, a cognitive motor deficit that is tween perception and reproduction of gestures” (Goldenberg, 1999;
often observed after left hemisphere (LH) stroke, cannot be fully ac- Goldenberg and Karnath, 2006). The finding that not only the imitation
counted for by primary sensory and motor deficits, disturbed commu- but also the matching of gestures is disturbed in LH stroke patients
nication, or lack of motivation (Dovern et al., 2012). Frequently ob- lends further support to the hypothesis that patients with imitation
served apraxic impairments pertain to the imitation of abstract and apraxia may suffer from a deficient conceptual knowledge about the
symbolic gestures, pantomiming the use of objects and tools, as well as human body (Goldenberg, 1999).
actual object use. The underlying pathophysiology of these apraxic However, studies investigating within the same patient population
deficits is still debated. apraxic deficits and deficits in processing human bodies/body parts are
Goldenberg suggested that apraxic patients are unable “to evoke sparse (Goldenberg, 1995). This is in part because different concepts
and represent conceptual knowledge about the human body, which is have been proposed to capture the neural representation of the human
necessary for performing the apparently simple task of imitating body (Schwoebel and Coslett, 2005): the body schema (BS), the body
[meaningless] gestures.” (Goldenberg, 1995). In the same vein, an in- structural description (BSD), and the body image (BI).
fluential account proposes an essential role of “body part coding” in Body schema (BS) is a „dynamic representation of the relative
apraxia (Goldenberg and Karnath, 2006). These authors stated that “the positions of body parts derived from multiple sensory and motor in-
body parts involved in gestures are categorized” “based on knowledge puts“ (Schwoebel and Coslett, 2005). Head and Holmes first described
about distinctive features and boundaries” of these body parts and that the BS as a body standard “against which all subsequent changes of
the “gestures are coded as simple spatial relationships between a posture are measured before they enter consciousness“ (Head and


Corresponding author. Cognitive Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3), Research Centre Jülich, Leo-Brandt-Str. 5, 52425, Jülich,
Germany.
E-mail address: P.H.Weiss@fz-juelich.de (P.H. Weiss).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107150
Received 16 June 2017; Received in revised form 24 June 2019; Accepted 26 July 2019
Available online 29 July 2019
0028-3932/ © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

Holmes, 1911). Note that in many publications “body schema” is used involving the BSD (judging the distance between two body parts
as an umbrella term for all three terms (body schema, body structural (Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2008) or relating the position of a rotated
description, and body image (Le Clec'H et al., 2000)) rather than in its arm to a standard body (Corradi-Dell'Acqua et al., 2009)).
specific meaning, i.e., a dynamic neural representation of the human Concerning the lesions underlying BSD deficits as investigated in
body that allows an online updating of the body part's position during studies with neurological patients, the current knowledge is largely
limb movement. In contrast, the body structural description (BSD) is derived from case studies (Buxbaum and Coslett, 2001; Felician et al.,
a „topological map of locations derived primarily from visual input that 2003). Deficient BSD has oftentimes been associated with left parietal
defines body part boundaries and proximity relationships“ (Schwoebel cortex lesions (Ogden, 1985; Semenza, 1988; Sirigu et al., 1991), a
and Coslett, 2005). Usually, the clinical assessment of the BSD involves lesion site that is also related to apraxia (Dovern et al., 2011; Hoeren
asking the patient to point to a body part on his/her own or another et al., 2014; Niessen et al., 2014). While there are some behavioural
human body (Semenza and Goodglass, 1985). Finally, the body image group studies in neurological patients (Semenza and Goodglass, 1985),
(BI, the alternative term used is “body semantics”) is involved in con- a quantitative lesion study examining the (differential) lesion patterns
sciously evaluating human bodies. BI is regarded to be „a lex- underlying apraxic end-position errors in imitation and deficits of the
ical–semantic representation of the body, including body part names, BSD is lacking. Therefore, we set out to develop a quantitative assess-
functions, and relations with artefacts“ (Schwoebel and Coslett, 2005). ment of BSD deficits in patients with LH stroke with and without
Again, in the psychiatric context, different use of the term “body image” apraxia. Then, these quantitative behavioural data were subjected to a
is quite common, e.g., body image distortion in anorexia nervosa statistical lesion analysis adopting voxel-based lesion symptom map-
(Wagner et al., 2003). ping (VLSM) to examine the hypothesis that a deficient BSD contributes
Concerning the body part coding hypothesis of imitation apraxia, to apraxic end-position errors in imitation.
the BSD is the relevant concept (Goldenberg, 1995). Patients with a
deficient BSD are impaired in localizing a body part (and its bound- 2. Methods
aries) on their body or another human's body. Note that many clinical
tests examining imitation deficits in stroke patients (e.g., the Cologne 2.1. Study participants
Apraxia Screening, KAS) concentrate on the end-position of the tested
body part (i.e., limb or face) rather than on the (movement) trajectory Nineteen healthy control subjects (mean age of 59.7 years, range
to this position. This also applies to the popular Goldenberg imitation 47–61 years) and 27 patients with first-ever LH stroke (mean age of
tests (imitating hand positions or finger configurations, (Goldenberg, 60.3 years, range 26–81 years) were included in the current in-
1995)), which were used in Goldenberg and Karnath's study on body vestigation.
part coding (Goldenberg and Karnath, 2006). However, in patients with Stroke localisation, including the territory of the medial cerebral
imitation deficits, dissociations concerning the trajectory and end-po- artery (MCA) of the LH, was confirmed with the help of clinical brain
sition can occur. Some patients show disturbed trajectories but achieve imaging by either CT or MRI scans. Imaging was performed within the
a proper end-position of the tested effector (e.g., hand), while other first days of hospitalisation. Few patients suffered from a concurrent
patients exhibit a smooth (movement) trajectory to a wrong end-posi- stroke in adjacent vascular territories of the LH (e.g., posterior (n=2) or
tion (Hermsdörfer et al., 1996). Thus, BSD deficits may contribute to anterior (n=1) cerebral artery strokes). The exclusion criteria were
apraxic end-position errors during imitation rather than to deficits in alcohol or drug abuse, uncorrected visual impairment, left-handedness,
(movement) trajectories (Reader et al., 2018). pre-existing strokes, and inability to give informed consent.
Nevertheless, BSD deficits could also contribute to object use defi- Additionally, patients with clinically relevant depression or dementia
cits in apraxia. Most likely, a deficient BSD affects both the panto- (diagnosed by the treating physicians) were excluded.
miming of object use and the actual use of objects in those cases where Patients and healthy subjects volunteered for the study. The healthy
the object is used with a given body part (e.g., brushing one's teeth, volunteers received a fixed reimbursement of €15 for the testing
combing one's hair). In contrast, object use that is not directed to the (lasting about an hour).
body but instead to another object (e.g., carving/sawing a piece of The ethics committee of the Medical Faculty, University Hospital
wood, cutting a slice of bread) is probably less affected by BSD deficits. Cologne, University of Cologne had approved the study.
Since the contribution of body part coding and thus BSD to imitation is
undisputed (in LH stroke patients), we here concentrate on the re- 2.2. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment
lationship between BSD deficits and apraxic end-position errors in
imitation. Consequently, we focused on those apraxic patients who Apraxia was assessed with the Cologne Apraxia Screening (KAS,
showed end-position errors in imitation in the current apraxia assess- (Weiss et al., 2013)). The KAS is a publicly available screening instru-
ment (i.e., the Cologne Apraxia Screening, KAS). Note that the scoring ment (published by Hogrefe: https://www.testzentrale.de/shop/
of the KAS imitation subtests is solely based on end-position errors and koelner-apraxie-screening.html). The KAS comprises four subtests that
not also on spatiotemporal errors as in other apraxia assessments (e.g., are organized in a factorial manner. One factor is task (pantomime
the TULIA (Vanbellingen et al., 2009) or the AST (Vanbellingen et al., versus imitation) and the other factor is effector (bucco-facial versus
2010)). limb gestures). This structure results in the following four subtests: 1.
Insights into the neural correlate of the BSD can be derived from pantomime of object use (i.e., transitive gestures) involving bucco-fa-
imaging studies in healthy populations and clinical studies in neuro- cial movements; 2. pantomime of object use (i.e., transitive gestures)
logical patients. involving limb movements; 3. imitation of (intransitive) bucco-facial
Functional imaging studies in healthy subjects implicated different gestures; 4. imitation of (intransitive) limb gestures. The stimulus ma-
parts of the (left) parietal cortex in the body schema (BS) and the BSD. terial consists of photos that either depict the object or a woman
In memory-guided reaching movements, a “dynamic representation of showing the to-be-imitated gesture, minimizing the influence of con-
the current postural configuration of the body” (i.e., the BS) was as- current aphasic deficits (P. H. Weiss et al., 2016, ). All subtests consist
sociated with activity in the superior parietal lobe (SPL, (Parkinson of five items each. Moreover, both imitation subtests contain two
et al., 2010)). In contrast, a task involving the BSD, i.e., pointing to meaningless items each (and three meaningful gestures).
human body parts (compared to pointing to body parts of dogs), acti- In the pantomime subtests, one or two points (depending on the
vated the inferior parietal cortex, in particular, the (left) angular gyrus complexity of the pantomime) are awarded for certain predefined fea-
(Felician et al., 2009). Other studies found activations in the left pos- tures of the pantomime. For example, for the item “pantomiming the
terior intraparietal sulcus (IPS), when healthy subjects performed tasks use of a toothbrush” the following movement features are scored with

2
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

Table 1
Demographic and neuropsychological data of the patient groups with left hemisphere (LH) stroke and the healthy controls.
Apraxic LH stroke patients LH stroke patients with imitation Non-apraxic LH stroke patients Healthy controls
(n=12) apraxia (n=10) (n=15) (n=19)

age (years) 60 (15.5) 59.8 (14.4) 60.6 (13.8) 59.7 (7.8)


gender (male/female) 7/5 6/4 10/5 9/10
time post-stroke (days) 8.1 (6.6) 9.1 (6.8) 8.6 (9.3)
stroke aetiology (ischaemic/haemorrhagic) 9/3 7/3 14/1
LQ +93.3 (9.9) +92.0 (10.3) +87.3 (16.8) +85.8 (20.4)
MRC scale (right hand) 3.63 3.45 4.43
(1.2) (1.2) (0.7)*
mRS 2.67 (1.4) 2.7 (1.3) 1.47 (1.2)*
KAS score 66.3 (21.1) 65.0 (23.0) 78.8 (1.2)*
ACL-K score 20.3 (13.5) 19.8 (14.4) 31.7 (7.82)*

Note that the ten LH stroke patients with imitation apraxia are a subgroup of the 12 apraxic LH stroke patients (see also Supplementary Tables).
Given are the means and standard deviations in brackets (SD). LH = left hemisphere.
ACL-K = Aphasia Check List-short version (Kalbe et al., 2005): maximum score 40 points; cut-off < 33 points; mild [26–32 points], moderate [15–25 points], severe
[0–14 points] language impairment.
KAS = Cologne Apraxia Screening (Weiss et al., 2013): maximum score 80 points; cut-off ≤ 76 points).
LQ: Laterality quotient as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971).
MRC = Medical Research Council rating scale for assessing paresis (O'Brien, 2000): Grade 0: No contraction; Grade 1: Flicker or trace of movement; Grade 2: Active
movement, with gravity eliminated; Grade 3: Active movement against gravity; Grade 4: Active movement against gravity and resistance; Grade 5: Normal power.
Grades 4-, 4 and 4+ may be used to indicate movement against slight, moderate, and strong resistance, respectively. Modified Rankin scale (mRS, (Rankin, 1957; van
Swieten et al., 1988)): grades: 0 = No symptoms at all; 1 = No significant disability despite symptoms; 2 = Slight disability; 3 = Moderate disability; 4 = Moder-
ately severe disability; 5 = Severe disability.

Significant difference between the LH stroke patients with imitation apraxia and those without apraxia (p < 0.05).

one point each: (i) the hand is almost closed to a fist, (ii) the hand is point-scale indicating the degree of paresis:
held laterally in front of the mouth, (iii) the mouth is slightly opened
and the teeth are shown, and (iv) circling/pushing movements of the ➢ Grade 0: No contraction
hand. When a given movement feature is absent, no point is given. For ➢ Grade 1: Flicker or trace of movement
the evaluation of the imitation subtests (i.e., imitation of bucco-facial ➢ Grade 2: Active movement, with gravity eliminated
and limb gestures), four points are given for the correct imitation on the ➢ Grade 3: Active movement against gravity
first trial. Note that an imitated gesture is considered correct if the ➢ Grade 4: Active movement against gravity and resistance
patient has adequately reproduced the final end-position of the gesture ➢ Grade 5: Normal power.
as depicted on the photos. If the first imitation trial fails, the stimulus
photo is shown for a second time. Two points are given for a successful Grades 4-, 4, and 4+ may be used to indicate movement against
second trial or no point for an erroneous second trial. For each of the slight, moderate, and strong resistance, respectively.
twenty KAS items, the patient can maximally achieve four points. Since the experimental procedures required patients to point with
Therefore, a maximum score of 20 points can be achieved in each of the their index finger, we asked them to use their left, ipsilesional hand to
four subtests. Thus, the KAS total score can be maximally 80 points. avoid putative confounding effects of the contralesional, right-sided
Based on the normative data of the KAS, a psychometric analysis re- paresis. Despite these instructions, two patients who were not severely
vealed that a patient with a score of 76 or less should be considered paretic (MRC paresis scale of 4+ and 5) decided to use their con-
apraxic. More information on the KAS can be found in Dovern et al. tralesional, right hand for pointing: one patient (rakl1001m) for both
(2012) and (Kusch et al., 2018). pointing tasks, the other patient (haed1109w) only for the object
As in previous studies (Binder et al., 2017; Dovern et al., 2017), pointing task. Patient haed1109w had a perfect score in the object
aphasic deficits were assessed by the short version of the aphasia pointing (relative score 1.0) and a nearly perfect score in the body
checklist (ACL-K). The ACL-K contains four subtests: a colour-figure pointing tasks (relative score 0.98).
test, a verbal fluency task, a reading task, and a rating of verbal com- The degree of handicap after stroke was assessed using the modified
munication (Kalbe et al., 2002). The scores of the ACL-K can amount up Rankin Scale (mRS; (Rankin, 1957; van Swieten et al., 1988)). The six
to 40 points. The lower the score, the more severe the aphasic deficits: grades of this common scale are: 0 = No symptoms at all; 1 = No sig-
14 points or less indicate severe aphasia, scores between 15 and 25 nificant disability despite symptoms; able to carry out all usual duties
points indicate moderate aphasia, scores between 26 and 32 indicate and activities; 2 = Slight disability; unable to carry out all previous
mild aphasia, and scores above 33 are within the normal range (i.e., no activities, but able to look after own affairs without assistance;
aphasia). 3 = Moderate disability; requiring some help, but able to walk without
(Pre-morbid) handedness was assessed with the help of the assistance; 4 = Moderately severe disability; unable to walk without
“Edinburgh Handedness Inventory” (EHI, (Oldfield, 1971)) for stroke assistance and unable to attend to own bodily needs without assistance;
patients and healthy subjects. The score of the EHI is a laterality quo- 5 = Severe disability; bedridden, incontinent, and requiring constant
tient (LQ), indicating the relative proportion of actions performed with nursing care and attention; 6 = Dead.
the right versus the left hand. Therefore, positive EHI-scores (i.e., po- Table 1 lists the demographic and neuropsychological data of the
sitive LQs) indicate right-handedness, while negative EHI-scores (i.e., healthy controls (n=19), LH stroke patients without apraxia (n=15),
negative LQs) indicate left-handedness. The higher the positive LQ, the LH stroke patients with apraxia (n=12), and the subgroup of apraxic
stronger the right-handedness. The lower the negative LQ, the stronger LH stroke patients with imitation apraxia (n=10). As a deficient BSD
the left-handedness. Note that patient and control groups were matched contributes mainly to apraxic end-position errors in imitation, we fo-
for handedness. cused our analyses on those ten apraxic patients with imitation apraxia
Contralesional limb paresis was evaluated using the Medical in the current apraxia assessment (i.e., Cologne Apraxia Screening,
Research Council (MRC) scale (O'Brien, 2000). The MRC-scale is a 5- KAS). Detailed information about the LH stroke patients with apraxia

3
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

Fig. 1. A. Schematic presentation of the body pointing task. B. Schematic presentation of the object pointing task.

can be found in the supplementary tables. task. A pre-test aimed at ruling out a body image deficit for the given
body part item. Such a body image deficit was operationalized as the
2.3. Experimental design inability to name that body part. Note that impaired body part naming
may result from either a deficient body image or from aphasic deficits.
Our primary research objective was to investigate specific BSD Therefore, participants were asked to name a body part item that was
deficits by assessing pointing to body versus object parts while con- presented on a computer screen, e.g., a thigh.
trolling for naming deficits and deficient body image (BI). We hy- If the subjects succeeded in naming that item, they then proceeded
pothesised that LH stroke patients with apraxic end-position errors in to point to the particular item on a picture of a human body (Fig. 1a).
imitation would obtain significantly lower scores in the body part However, if the participant failed to name the shown body part prop-
pointing tasks (compared to pointing to object parts). Such a beha- erly, he/she was told the body part's name. Then, the subject was
vioural pattern would suggest that a disturbed BSD is a relevant factor presented with three related body parts, one of which was the inquired
for apraxic end-position errors in imitation. body part, e.g., thigh, hips, and upper arms. This intermediate step in
After the neuropsychological assessment, subjects were comfortably the testing procedure ensured that patients with aphasic naming pro-
seated in front of a laptop computer (MacBook Pro), on which the visual blems, who nevertheless recognized the body part, could still partici-
stimuli were presented. pate in the body part pointing task. If the subject also failed this task,
the given body part item was excluded from the body part pointing
task. In other words, if a patient could not name a body part, she/he
2.3.1. Pointing to body parts
was told the name of this body part and was presented with pictures of
This task aimed to assess the subject's ability to point to parts of the
three related body parts. Then, the patient was asked to point to the
human body. First, we inquired the sections of the human body, i.e.,
particular body part – selecting it from the three presented body parts.
head, trunk, arms, and legs. Then, we inquired in more detail body
If the patient could accurately point to the previously named body part
parts within these sections (e.g., for the head: ear, eye, nose, mouth).
indicating that she/he could adequately associate the body part's name
Fig. 1a depicts a schematic representation of the body part pointing

4
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

Fig. 2. A. Photograph was taken during the body part pointing tasks (item “thigh”), the patient incorrectly points to the shank. B. Photograph was taken during the
object part pointing tasks (item “lampstand”), the patient correctly points to the stand of the lamp.

with the picture of that body part, that body part was included in the each item, we developed a scoring sheet: A yellow rectangle was closed
pointing to body part pointing task - although she/he could not name around the body/object part at which the subject should point to (e.g.,
the body part. In contrast, if the patient also failed to point to the thigh). A red rectangle was closed around the surrounding body parts
correct body part picture after being told the body part's name, that (e.g., hip and knee). If the subject correctly pointed to the inquired
body part was excluded from the body part pointing task (see below for body/object part within the yellow rectangle, the subject received full
the description of the resulting relative performance score). points. If the subject pointed to the body/object part nearest to the
Note that the inability of LH stroke patients to process a given item inquired part, i.e., within the red rectangle, but outside the yellow
that was excluded by the naming pre-test could either result from a rectangle, the subject received half of the points. If the subject had
deficient body image or aphasic deficits since both deficits could lead to pointed outside the red rectangle, the subject received no points. The
impaired body part naming. However, preserved body part naming subjects could score a total of two points for each of the 20 body part
excludes a relevant deficit of the body image. Thus, the pre-test ensured items (in total: 2 × 20=40 points) and a total of four points for each of
that a failure in pointing to human body parts could be considered to the ten object part items (in total: 4 × 10=40 points).
reflect a deficient BSD (and not a deficient body image), with lower Since some patients could not perform the tasks for all items due to
scores in the body pointing tasks reflecting a more severe BSD deficit. their naming deficits (see sections 2.3.1 and 2.3.2), a relative perfor-
For illustration purposes, Fig. 2a depicts a patient who was in- mance score was computed reflecting the individual performance in
structed to point to the thigh on a picture of a human body displayed on either task (see also Supplementary Table S3). This relative perfor-
the monitor. Note that the photo was taken after the patient's response. mance score was calculated by dividing the score obtained by a given
Therefore, the red and yellow rectangles used for scoring are already person by the attainable maximum score for this person in a given task.
present to illustrate the task and the scoring procedure (see below) Thus, if a subject could correctly name or select only 12 items in the
within the same figure. body part naming task, but performed correctly for these 12 items
(resulting in 2 points for each of these 12 items), this subject received a
2.3.2. Pointing to object parts relative performance score of 1 (individual obtained score: 2 × 12=24,
This task assessed the participants' ability to point to parts of non- individual attainable maximum score: 2 × 12=24, relative perfor-
human objects. The procedure was similar to that for the body part mance score: 24/24=1). However, if this subject would fail in 3 of the
pointing task. Fig. 1b depicts a schematic representation of the object- 12 items (resulting in 0 points for these 3 items), then her/his relative
part pointing task. In each trial, the participants were asked to name an performance score would be 0.75 (individual obtained score:
object that was presented to them on the computer monitor, e.g., a 0 × 3 + 2 × 9=18, individual attainable maximum score:
lamp. If the subjects failed to name the object correctly, they were told 2 × 12=24, relative performance score: 18/24=0.75).
the object's name. Then, they were presented with three related objects, The subjects were requested to touch the computer screen specifi-
one of which was the inquired object (e.g., lamp, mobile phone, and cally on the location of their answer (e.g., thigh) and keep their index
binoculars), and were asked to select the proper object. This inter- finger in that position until the evaluation was carried out by the ex-
mediate step in the testing procedure ensured that patients with aphasic aminer. The evaluation was performed with the help of the yellow and
naming problems, who nevertheless recognized the object, could still red rectangles, as described above. The rectangles appeared on the
participate in the object part pointing task. If subjects named or selected screen by a button press of the examiner, while the subjects were asked
the object correctly, they proceeded to name a part of the given object, not to move and to keep their index finger at the location of their an-
e.g., lampstand. If subjects failed in naming the object-part, they were swer.
told the name of the object-part and then instructed to select the given
object-part out of an array of three object parts, e.g., lamp stand, lamp 2.4. Data analyses
shade, and lamp holder. If subjects succeeded in naming or selecting the
object part, they proceeded to point to the object part on a picture of All statistical analyses were performed with the “Statistical Package
another object of the given type, e.g., lampstand of another lamp. for the Social Sciences” software (IBM SPSS Version 22.0 for Windows).
Fig. 2b demonstrates that when instructed to point to the lamp- A repeated-measures ANOVA was calculated to assess the (differential)
stand, the same patient who failed on the body part pointing task effects of the two pointing tasks on the patients' performance. Since the
(Fig. 2a) performed the object pointing task correctly for this test item. healthy control subjects performed the task “at ceiling” (i.e., without
The order of the two pointing tasks was pseudo-randomized, albeit any error in either task), the data violated the normal distribution re-
the corresponding naming tasks always preceded the pointing tasks for quirements for an ANOVA. Accordingly, the healthy control subjects’
either body or object items. data were not included in the ANOVA. As a deficient BSD contributes
mainly to apraxic end-position errors in imitation, we focused our
2.3.3. Scoring procedure analyses on those ten apraxic patients who showed end-position errors
A similar scoring system was adopted for both pointing tasks. For in imitation in the current apraxia assessment (i.e., Cologne Apraxia

5
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

Screening, KAS). Therefore, we computed a 2 × 2 repeated-measures


ANOVA for the relative performance scores with the within-subject
factor TASK (body versus object part pointing task) and the between-
subject factor GROUP (LH stroke patients with imitation apraxia
[n=10], LH stroke patients without apraxia [n=15]). Results are re-
ported at a significance level of p < 0.05.
Furthermore, correlation analyses between the apraxia test scores
(i.e., KAS total score and sub-scores) and the scores of the two pointing
tasks were performed for the same patient sample included in the
ANOVA (n=25, i.e., the LH stroke patients with imitation apraxia
[n=10] and the LH stroke patients without apraxia [n=15]).

2.5. Lesion mapping and analyses

Lesion mapping was based on clinical imaging by CT (n=6) or MRI


(n=20). For one of the 27 patients (haed1109w) no imaging suitable
for lesion mapping was available since the initial CT scan showed no
infarct demarcation despite persistent neurological/neuropsychological
deficits. Note, however, that the pattern of behavioural results was si-
milar when the patient haed1109w was excluded from the analyses.
Time from symptom onset to clinical imaging was on average 2.7 days
(standard deviation (SD) 5.6; range 0–26 days). Fig. 3. Mean relative performance scores of the three groups (healthy controls
Using the MRIcron software (http://people.cas.sc.edu/rorden/ [n=19]: white bars, patients without apraxia [n=15]: grey bars, patients with
imitation apraxia [n=10]: black bars) for the body (left side) and object (right
mricron/index.html), the investigator (HSD) manually copied the le-
side) pointing tasks. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean (SEM). Note
sions to a T1-weighted template brain (ch2.nii). The lesion mapping
that the SEM of the controls was 0.
was double-checked by two further investigators (AD & PHW); all in-
vestigators had to agree on lesion location and extent. At the time of
mapping, the two additional investigators (AD, PHW) were blind re- TASK and GROUP (F (1,23)=5.34, p < 0.031, ηp2=.188). This sig-
garding the test performance of a given patient (P.H. Weiss et al., nificant TASK by GROUP interaction resulted from the differentially
2016a). Neuroanatomic localisation of lesion sites was performed using reduced relative performance score of the LH stroke patients with
the anatomy toolbox in SPM (Eickhoff et al., 2005). imitation apraxia in the body pointing task (0.776, compared to their
Voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) was employed for relative performance score in the object pointing task: 0.889). In con-
statistical lesion analyses (Bates et al., 2003). VLSM was used to analyse trast, LH stroke patients without apraxia performed similarly in both
the relationship between lesion site and apraxia severity (oper- pointing tasks (relative performance scores: body part pointing task:
ationalized by the KAS total score) as well as between lesion site and 0.966, object part pointing task: 0.951).
BSD deficits (operationalized as the individual difference score of the Note that the LH stroke patients with imitation apraxia who suffered
body versus object pointing task). For this difference score, the relative from deficits in pointing to another person's body parts did also show
performance scores of the object pointing task were subtracted from the deficits in pointing to their own body parts when clinically assessed by
relative performance scores of the body pointing task in each patient. verbal command (Semenza and Goodglass, 1985). Due to the lack of a
Only voxels damaged in at least 3 patients (i.e., ≥10% of all patients) formal assessment of pointing to one's body parts, the current study
were tested in the VLSM analyses. The statistical threshold was set to could not differentiate BSD deficits related to one's body from those
p < 0.05 (corrected for False Discovery Rate, FDR) except when noted related to other human bodies and whether these contribute to (imi-
otherwise. tation) apraxia in a differential manner. In their seminal study,
Semenza and Goodglass (1985) used different conditions, one of which
3. Results (i.e., condition B: The subject points to body parts on a drawing of a
human figure on verbal command) resembled the current body part
3.1. Behavioural data pointing task. Notably, all examined patient groups of Semenza and
Goodglass performed similarly in condition B as in condition A (see
To investigate the hypothesised link between BSD disturbances and their Table 2 on page 166), in which the patients were asked to point to
apraxic end-position errors in imitation, we evaluated the relative their body parts on verbal command. Semenza and Goodglass collapsed
performance scores in the two pointing tasks between the LH stroke conditions A and B for the error analysis (see their Table 1 on page
patients with apraxic end-position errors in imitation (as assessed by 165). Consequently, these authors concluded, “that a common factor
the KAS, n=10) and the LH stroke patients without apraxia (i.e., those underlies success in identifying body parts under all conditions”.
patients with an unremarkable KAS score, n=15) using a 2 × 2 re- Therefore, it can be assumed that this “common factor” (here termed
peated-measures ANOVA. BSD) contributes to apraxic (imitation) deficits. Furthermore, the cur-
For the relative performance scores (see Fig. 3), the 2 × 2 repeated- rent study adds to the findings of Semenza and Goodglass by including a
measures ANOVA with the within-subject factor TASK (body versus task that assessed pointing to object parts allowing for a direct com-
object part pointing task) and the between-subject factor GROUP re- parison between body and object part tasks. Thereby, the current data
vealed a tendency for the main effect of TASK (F(1,23)=3.12, p=0.091, revealed a specific impairment in pointing to body parts (i.e., BSD
ηp2=.119), indicating a trend for higher relative performance scores in deficits) in the current LH stroke patients with apraxic end-position
the object pointing task (compared to the body part pointing task). errors in imitation.
Furthermore, there was a significant main effect of GROUP (F (1,23) Consistent with the more pronounced severity of aphasic deficits (as
=5.05, p < 0.035, ηp2=.180), with lower relative performance scores assessed by the ACL-K see Table 1) in the apraxic patients, more items
in the LH stroke patients with imitation apraxia (compared to the LH were excluded due to naming deficits, when examining LH stroke pa-
stroke patients without apraxia). tients with imitation apraxia compared to LH stroke patients without
Importantly, the ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between apraxia. Importantly, the amount of excluded items was similar for both

6
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

pointing tasks across groups. For the LH stroke patients without The significant interaction between the two pointing tasks and the
apraxia, the number of patients examined with the full set of items (i.e., factor GROUP (driven by the differentially reduced relative perfor-
20 items for the body part pointing task and 10 items for the object mance scores of the apraxic LH stroke patients in the body versus object
pointing task) versus a reduced set of items due to naming deficits was part pointing tasks) constitutes the main finding of our study at the
13 versus 2 for the body part pointing task and 14 versus 1 for the behavioural level. Therefore, a further VLSM analysis was performed
object pointing task. Considering all apraxic LH stroke patients (n=12, with the individual difference scores that were computed by subtracting
i.e., the 10 patients with apraxic end-position errors in imitation and the relative performance scores in the object pointing task from the
the two patients with selective pantomime apraxia), 7 apraxic patients relative performance scores in the body pointing task in each patient. In
were examined with the full set of items, while 5 apraxic patients were Fig. 4C, the lesion analysis (VLSM) with these individual difference
tested with a reduced item set for both pointing tasks (see scores is presented (p < 0.05, uncorrected). Lesions associated with a
Supplementary Table S3). worse performance in the body pointing task (compared to the object
The single subject data provided in the Supplementary Tables show pointing task) were mainly found in the angular gyrus of the left in-
that BSD deficits are associated with apraxia at the group level. How- ferior parietal lobe. Further smaller lesion sites that showed a similar
ever, at the single patient level, we also observed dissociations between association were located in the superior and middle temporal cortex.
the performances in the apraxia test and the pointing tasks (e.g., patient
amdi0503m is severely apraxic (and aphasic), but still achieves rea- 4. Discussion
sonable relative scores of about 0.6 in both pointing tasks). Concerning
aphasia severity, different result patterns were observed. While patients To examine the hypothesis that a deficient BSD contributes to
orro0324m and rakl1001m were severely aphasic (ACL-K-score of 0 and apraxic end-position errors in imitation, we developed a test for
9, respectively) and performed poorly in the body pointing task (re- quantitatively assessing deficits in pointing to body and object parts
lative performance score in the body pointing task: 0.32 and 0.4, re- that is clinically applicable to LH stroke patients and minimizes the
spectively), patient lado0805w was also severely aphasic (ACL-K score confounding effects of comorbid aphasia and a deficient body image
of 3), but performed well when pointing to body parts (relative per- (i.e., “a lexical–semantic representation of the body including body part
formance score in the body pointing task: 0.93). These result patterns names“ (Schwoebel and Coslett, 2005)). This test revealed that patients
suggest that aphasic deficits observed in our study cannot solely explain with LH stroke and imitation apraxia are specifically impaired in
the current BSD deficits. pointing to body (versus object) parts. Thus, they suffer from a deficient
Correlation analyses (performed for the same patient sample that body structural description (BSD). Within the lesion pattern causing
was included in the ANOVA (n=25), i.e., the LH stroke patients with apraxic deficits, the specific deficits in pointing to body parts (i.e., BSD
imitation apraxia [n=10] and those without apraxia [n=15]) between deficits) were associated with lesions of the angular gyrus of the left
the apraxia test scores (i.e., KAS scores) and the scores of the two inferior parietal lobe. Data support the notion that apraxic end-position
pointing tasks revealed significant correlations between the relative errors in imitation are – at least in part – due to a deficient coding of the
performance scores of the body part pointing task and the total KAS position of human body parts, i.e., a deficient BSD.
score (rho=0.463, p < 0.05) as well as the KAS imitation sub-score To our knowledge, quantitative and clinically applicable assess-
(rho=0.450, p < 0.05). Notably, the correlation between the relative ments of the body structural description (BSD, (Semenza, 1988;
performance scores of the body part pointing task and the KAS panto- Semenza and Goodglass, 1985)) or paradigms assessing the body image
mime sub-score revealed a non-significant trend only (rho=0.372, (BI, (Cash et al., 2004)) are scarce. The current test procedure ensured
p=0.07). In contrast, there were significant correlations between the that the observed pointing deficits are not solely due to aphasic deficits
relative performance scores of the object part pointing task and the or a deficient BI since a pre-test for each item ensured that the subject
total KAS score (rho=0.504, p < 0.05) as well as the KAS pantomime associated a given body part or object (part) with its name. This al-
sub-score (rho=0.552, p < 0.05), while the correlation between the lowed using a broad array of body and objects parts.
relative performance scores of the object part pointing task and the KAS Regarding the complex definitions of body schema, body image, and
imitation sub-score revealed a non-significant trend (rho=0.381, body structural description (BSD), we adopted for the current study the
p=0.06). Therefore, the correlation pattern rather supports the notion definitions proposed by Schwoebel and Coslett (Schwoebel and Coslett,
that BSD deficits (mainly) contribute to apraxic end-position errors in 2005). These authors consider body image to be „a lexical–semantic
imitation. However, it should be noted that the correlation results are representation of the body, including body part names, functions, and
relatively weak and thus should be interpreted with caution. relations with artefacts“. Accordingly, we operationalized deficits in the
body image as deficits in the lexical-semantic representation of body
3.2. Lesion analyses parts assessed by naming the body parts. Thus, the procedure of ex-
cluding body part items from testing (if a given patient was unable to
Since the imaging data were available for all but one patient of the name a particular body part item AND was unable to correctly point to
current study, Fig. 4A shows the lesion overlay of the 26 LH stroke this body part after the patient was told the body part's name) mini-
patients included in the lesion analyses. The highest lesion overlap was mized the impact of aphasic deficits on the current pointing tasks.
observed within the territory of the MCA, especially in the left middle Importantly, it also excluded a relevant influence of body image deficits
and superior temporal cortex and the inferior parietal cortex, i.e., an- (operationalized as deficits in the lexical–semantic representation of the
gular gyrus and supramarginal gyrus. In contrast, frontal and occipital body parts) on the performance in the body part pointing task.
regions were affected to a lesser degree. Furthermore, the procedures for assessing body and object part
In Fig. 4B, the lesion analysis using voxel-based lesion-symptom pointing were similar so that we could directly compare the respective
mapping (VLSM) for the total score of the Cologne Apraxia Screening relative performance scores for both sub-tests and identify the specific
(KAS) is presented. Lesions to the middle and superior temporal gyrus, body part pointing deficits of the LH stroke patients with apraxic end-
the operculum, insula, temporal cortex, supramarginal gyrus, angular position errors in imitation. Importantly, the current study, in which
gyrus, postcentral gyrus, and the centrally located white matter were aphasic deficits and deficits of the BI were controlled for, constitutes
significantly associated with reduced KAS scores and thus the severity one of the very few quantitative group studies on BSD deficits
of apraxic (pantomime and imitation) deficits (p < 0.05, FDR-cor- (Schwoebel and Coslett, 2005; Semenza and Goodglass, 1985). Most
rected). Notably, a very similar lesion pattern for the KAS was observed previous studies that carefully delineated BSD from body image (BI) or
in a recent study for a group of 44 patients with left hemisphere (LH) body schema (BS) were case reports (Buxbaum and Coslett, 2001; Sirigu
stroke (Binder et al., 2017). et al., 1991).

7
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

Fig. 4. A. Lesion overlay of all studied LH stroke patients for whom suitable imaging data was available (n=26). For one of the tested 27 patients no imaging data
suitable for lesion mapping was available. Colour shades represent the number of overlapping lesions. The highest overlap of stroke lesions was observed for the
territory of the middle cerebral artery (MCA). B. Lesion analysis adopting voxel-based lesion mapping (VLSM) of the KAS total scores (n=26). Lesions to the middle
and superior temporal gyrus, the operculum, insula, Heschl gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus, postcentral gyrus, and the centrally located white matter were
significantly associated with reduced KAS scores and thus the severity of apraxic (pantomime and imitation) deficits. Results are displayed at a threshold of
p < 0.05, corrected for False Discovery Rate (FDR). C. Lesion analysis (VLSM) for the differential scores of the body versus object pointing tasks (n=26, un-
corrected). Lesions associated with a worse performance in the body pointing task (compared to the object pointing task) were mainly found in the angular gyrus of
the left inferior parietal lobe. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.)

The relative performance scores in the body part pointing tasks al- BSD deficits (i.e., differential deficits in pointing to body parts
lowed us to draw conclusions regarding the severity of the BSD deficit. versus object parts) were predominantly associated with lesions of the
Our results showed clear and significant differences in the pointing task angular gyrus as part of the inferior parietal lobe. Therefore, within the
performances between the patient groups: patients with imitation broader lesions causing apraxia per se, lesions of the angular gyrus led
apraxia scored significantly worse in the body pointing tasks than non- to specific BSD deficits in the current apraxic LH stroke patients.
apraxic patients, while the performances of the two groups were similar However, it should be noted that the VLSM with the difference score
in the object pointing task. Thus, imitation apraxia was a relevant body part versus object part pointing did not survive thresholds cor-
predictor for the performance in the pointing task involving human rected for multiple comparisons, while the VLSM with the KAS-scores
body parts, but not in the pointing task with object parts. Hence, LH did survive correction for False Discovery Rate (FDR).
stroke patients with apraxic end-position errors in imitation seem to The angular gyrus has been associated with different deficits, such
have a body-selective pointing deficit. As the performance in the body as “speech comprehension deficits, finger agnosia, spatial disorienta-
part pointing task serves as a measure of the severity of a BSD deficit tion, acalculia, agraphia, and dementia” (Seghier, 2013). Note that
(controlled for general task demands by the object part pointing task), Goldenberg and Karnath (2006) found that their patients with selec-
the current data suggest that LH stroke patients with apraxic end-po- tively disturbed imitation of hand postures exhibited a lesion overlap in
sition errors in imitation have a specific and more severe BSD deficit the inferior parietal lobule (IPL). These authors argued that especially
than non-apraxic LH stroke patients. hand posture imitation draws on body part coding, a notion consistent
With respect to the lesion analyses with VLSM in the current sample with the results of the current quantitative lesion analysis. The current
of LH stroke patients (n=26), apraxic deficits in imitation and panto- test procedures controlled for the effect of aphasia, which is reflected in
mime (as assessed by the KAS) were mainly caused by lesions of the the lesion pattern. While BSD deficits in the current LH stroke patients
inferior parietal cortex (supramarginal gyrus, SMG; angular gyrus, AG), are associated with inferior parietal cortex lesions, lesions to the inferior
the temporal cortex, and the postcentral gyrus. This apraxic lesion frontal gyrus (Brodmann Area 44) led to combined apraxic and aphasic
pattern is consistent with previous findings (Dovern et al., 2011; deficits after LH stroke (P. H. Weiss et al., 2016). Consistent with the
Haaland et al., 2000; Hoeren et al., 2014; Niessen et al., 2014) and very results of our structural lesion analysis, a functional imaging study that
similar to the lesion pattern reported in a previous study that also used investigated the neural mechanisms underlying the BSD by adopting a
the KAS to test for apraxia (Binder et al., 2017). task of pointing to human body parts (compared to pointing to body

8
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

parts of dogs) also activated the inferior parietal cortex, in particular CRediT authorship contribution statement
the (left) angular gyrus (Felician et al., 2009).
Hormos Salimi Dafsari: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal
4.1. Limitations analysis, Writing - original draft. Anna Dovern: Data curation, Formal
analysis. Gereon R. Fink: Conceptualization, Writing - original draft.
While there has been considerable progress regarding lesion map- Peter H. Weiss: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Writing - original
ping methods (H.-O. Karnath et al., 2018; Rorden and Karnath, 2004), draft.
some limitations remain or are inherent to the method. One confound is
the time since stroke. We used clinical imaging by CT (n=6) or MRI Acknowledgements
(n=20) for lesion mapping. As a result, the interval between symptom
onset and the acquisition of the images used was, on average, 2.7 days The authors would like to thank their colleagues at Cognitive
(standard deviation (SD) 5.6; range 0–26 days). Thus, we used rather Neuroscience, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-3),
acute images for lesion mapping (H–O. Karnath and Rennig, 2017). Research Centre Jülich. We also thank Charlotte Schömig for testing
Notably, CT was only used for lesion mapping when there was a de- some of the stroke patients and Jan-Michael Werner and Daniel Bettin
marcated lesion. Nevertheless, the delineation of the exact lesion extent for photo-modelling. HSD was supported by the Köln Fortune Program
may have been more difficult than in cases in which MR images were of the Faculty of Medicine, University of Cologne (107/2012). GRF
available. However, the manual tracing technique is still considered to gratefully acknowledges financial support by the Marga and Walter Boll
be best suited for exact lesion delineation (Wilke et al., 2011). Foundation.
Given the variability in terms of lesion volume, lesion analyses can
be performed with co-varying out lesion volume. However, the dis- Appendix A. Supplementary data
advantage of this method is that it only has low statistical power. In
fact, “there were no significant voxels for the logistic regression ana- Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://
lyses for sub- and total pantomime and imitation scores when lesion doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107150.
size was added as a covariate” in a lesion mapping study of 96 LH stroke
patients (page 2803 of (Hoeren et al., 2014)). Consistent with this ob- References
servation, no voxel was found in the current VSLM analyses when we
included lesion volume as a covariate. Achilles, E.I.S., Fink, G.R., Fischer, M.H., Dovern, A., Held, A., Timpert, D.C., ... Weiss,
In addition to the above-mentioned aspects, there are also issues P.H., 2016. Effect of meaning on apraxic finger imitation deficits. Neuropsychologia
82, 74–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.022.
related to diaschisis (e.g., (Carrera and Tononi, 2014), (Grefkes and Bates, E., Wilson, S.M., Saygin, A.P., Dick, F., Sereno, M.I., Knight, R.T., Dronkers, N.F.,
Fink, 2014). Therefore, structural lesion mapping only in part captures 2003. Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Nat. Neurosci. 6 (5), 448–450. https://
the functional consequences of stroke lesions. doi.org/10.1038/nn1050.
Binder, E., Dovern, A., Hesse, M.D., Ebke, M., Karbe, H., Saliger, J., ... Weiss, P.H., 2017.
Lesion evidence for a human mirror neuron system. Cortex 90, 125–137.
5. Future directions Buxbaum, L.J., Coslett, H.B., 2001. Specialised structural descriptions for human body
parts: evidence from autotopagnosia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 18 (4), 17.
Carrera, E., Tononi, G., 2014. Diaschisis: past, present, future. Brain 137, 2408–2422.
The current study constitutes one of the very few group studies ex- Cash, T.F., Melnyk, S.E., Hrabosky, J.I., 2004. The assessment of body image investment:
amining BSD deficits in neurological patients and the first quantitative an extensive revision of the appearance schemas inventory. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 35,
lesion study on this topic. Note that our study could not comprehen- 305–316.
Corradi-Dell'Acqua, C., Hesse, M.D., Rumiati, R.I., Fink, G.R., 2008. Where is a nose with
sively address all interesting issues concerning the relationship between
respect to a foot? The left posterior parietal cortex processes spatial relationships
BSD and apraxic deficits. Future studies on this topic should explore in among body parts. Cerebr. Cortex 18 (12), 2879–2890. https://doi.org/10.1093/
more detail whether a deficient BSD rather contributes to apraxic def- cercor/bhn046.
icits in movements directed towards the body (e.g., combing one's hair) Corradi-Dell'Acqua, C., Tomasino, B., Fink, G.R., 2009. What is the position of an arm
relative to the body? Neural correlates of body schema and body structural de-
than to apraxic deficits in movements directed away from the body scription. J. Neurosci. 29 (13), 4162–4171. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
(e.g., cutting a slice of bread). It has been proposed that especially 4861-08.2009.
deficits in imitating meaningless (ML) gestures (compared to mean- Dovern, A., Fink, G.R., Saliger, J., Karbe, H., Koch, I., Weiss, P.H., 2011. Apraxia impairs
intentional retrieval of incidentally acquired motor knowledge. J. Neurosci. 31 (22),
ingful (MF) gestures) are related to deficient “body part coding” 8102–8108.
(Goldenberg and Karnath, 2006). Accordingly, it is worth to include the Dovern, A., Fink, G.R., Weiss, P.H., 2012. Diagnosis and treatment of upper limb apraxia.
meaning of a gesture as a further dimension in future studies on the J. Neurol. 259, 1269–1283.
Dovern, A., Niessen, E., Ant, J.M., Saliger, J., Karbe, H., Fink, G.R., ... Weiss, P.H., 2017.
relationship between BSD and apraxia to elucidate whether the Timing independent spatial motor sequence learning is preserved in left hemisphere
meaning of a gesture modulates the effect of a deficient BSD on apraxic stroke. Neuropsychologia 106, 322–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
imitation deficits (Achilles et al., 2016; Goldenberg and Hagmann, neuropsychologia.2017.09.030.
Eickhoff, S.B., Stephan, K.E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G.R., Amunts, K., Zilles, K.,
1997).
2005. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and
functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25, 1325–1335.
6. Conclusion Felician, O., Anton, J.L., Nazarian, B., Roth, M., Roll, J.P., Romaiguere, P., 2009. Where is
your shoulder? Neural correlates of localizing others' body parts. Neuropsychologia
47 (8–9), 1909–1916. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.001.
We have successfully developed a short and clinically applicable test Felician, O., Ceccaldi, M., Didic, M., Thinus-Blanc, C., Poncet, M., 2003. Pointing to body
for quantitatively assessing deficits caused by a disturbed BSD in pa- parts: a double dissociation study. Neuropsychologia 41 (10), 1307–1316.
tients suffering from LH stroke. LH stroke patients with apraxic end- Goldenberg, G., 1995. Imitating gestures and manipulating a mannikin - the re-
presentation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia. Neuropsychologia 33 (1),
position errors in imitation showed a differential deficit in pointing to 63–72.
body (versus object) parts. The specific body pointing deficits were Goldenberg, G., 1999. Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients
mainly associated with angular gyrus lesions. Thus, our data support with damage in the left or right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 37, 559–566.
Goldenberg, G., 2009. Apraxia and the parietal lobes. Neuropsychologia 47, 1449–1459.
the notion that apraxic end-position errors in imitation are – at least in Goldenberg, G., Hagmann, S., 1997. The meaning of meaningless gestures: a study of
part – due to a deficient coding of the position of human body parts. visuo-imitative apraxia. Neuropsychologia 35 (3), 333–341.
Goldenberg, G., Karnath, H.-O., 2006. The neural basis of imitation is body part specific.
J. Neurosci. 26 (23), 6282–6287.
Disclosures Grefkes, C., Fink, G.R., 2014. Connectivity-based approaches in stroke and recovery of
function. Lancet Neurol. 13, 206–216.
The authors report no disclosures. Haaland, K.Y., Harrington, D.L., Knight, R.T., 2000. Neural representation of skilled

9
H.S. Dafsari, et al. Neuropsychologia 133 (2019) 107150

movement. Brain 123, 2306–2313. Reader, A.T., Royce, B.P., Marsh, J.E., Chivers, K.J., Holmes, N.P., 2018. Repetitive
Head, H., Holmes, G.L., 1911. Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34, transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals a role for the left inferior parietal lobule in
102–254. matching observed kinematics during imitation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 47 (8), 918–928.
Hermsdörfer, J., Mai, N., Spatt, J., Marquardt, C., Veltkamp, R., Goldenberg, G., 1996. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13886.
Kinematic analysis of movement imitation in apraxia. Brain 119, 1575–1586. Rorden, C., Karnath, H.-O., 2004. Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from
Hoeren, M., Kummerer, D., Bormann, T., Beume, L., Ludwig, V.M., Vry, M.S., ... Weiller, a past era in the fMRI age? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 813–819.
C., 2014. Neural bases of imitation and pantomime in acute stroke patients: distinct Schwoebel, J., Coslett, H.B., 2005. Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the
streams for praxis. Brain 137 (Pt 10), 2796–2810. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/ human body. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17 (4), 543–553.
awu203. Seghier, M.L., 2013. The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. The
Kalbe, E., Reinhold, N., Brand, M., Kessler, J., 2002. The short aphasia-check-list: an Neuroscientist 19 (1), 43–61. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858412440596.
economical screening for detecting aphasia. Eur. J. Neurol. 9 (2), 209–210. Semenza, C., 1988. Impairment in localization of body parts following brain damage.
Kalbe, E., Reinhold, N., Brand, M., Markowitsch, H.J., Kessler, J., 2005. A new test battery Cortex 24 (3), 443–449.
to assess aphasic disturbances and associated cognitive dysfunctions - German nor- Semenza, C., Goodglass, H., 1985. Localization of body parts in brain injured subjects.
mative data on the aphasia check list. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 27, 779–794. Neuropsychologia 23 (2), 161–175.
Karnath, H.-O., Rennig, J., 2017. Investigating structure and function in the healthy Sirigu, A., Grafman, J., Bressler, K., Sunderland, T., 1991. Multiple representations con-
human brain: validity of acute versus chronic lesion-symptom mapping. Brain Struct. tribute to body knowledge processing. Brain 114, 629–642.
Funct. 222, 2059–2070. van Swieten, J.C., Koudstaal, P.J., Visser, M.C., Schouten, H.J., van Gijn, J., 1988.
Karnath, H.-O., Sperber, C., Rorden, C., 2018. Mapping human brain lesions and their Interobserver agreement for the assessment of handicap in stroke patients. Stroke 19
functional consequences. Neuroimage 165, 180–189. (5), 604–607.
Kusch, M., Schmidt, C.C., Goden, L., Tscherpel, C., Stahl, J., Saliger, J., ... Weiss, P.H., Vanbellingen, T., Kersten, B., Van de Winckel, A., Bellion, M., Baronti, F., Müri, R.,
2018. Recovery from apraxic deficits and its neural correlate. Restor. Neurol. Bohlhalter, S., 2010. A new bedside test of gestures in stroke: the apraxia screen of
Neurosci. 36 (6), 669–678. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-180815. TULIA (AST). J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry (in press).
Le Clec'H, G., Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Poline, J.-B., ... Le Bihan, D., Vanbellingen, T., Kersten, B., Van Hemelrijk, B., Van de Winckel, A., Bertschi, M., Müri,
2000. Distinct cortical areas for names of numbers and body parts independent of R., ... Bohlhalter, S., 2009. Comprehensive assessment of gesture production: a new
language and input modality. Neuroimage 12 (4), 381–391. test of upper limb apraxia (TULIA). Eur. J. Neurol. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-
Niessen, E., Fink, G.R., Weiss, P.H., 2014. Apraxia, pantomime and the parietal cortex. 1331.2009.02741.x.
Neuroimage Clinical 5, 42–52. Wagner, A.D., Ruf, M., Braus, D.F., Schmidt, M.H., 2003. Neuronal activity changes and
O'Brien, M.D., for the Guarantors of Brain, 2000. Aids to the Examination of the body image distortion in anorexia nervosa. Neuroreport 14 (17), 2193–2197.
Peripheral Nervous System. W. B. Saunders, London. Weiss, P.H., Kalbe, E., Scherer, A., Binder, E., Kessler, J., Fink, G.R., 2013. Das Kölner
Ogden, J.A., 1985. Autotopagnosia. Brain 108, 1009–1022. Apraxie-Screening (KAS). Hogrefe, Göttingen.
Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Weiss, P.H., Ubben, S.D., Kaesberg, S., Kalbe, E., Kessler, J., Liebig, T., Fink, G.R., 2016.
Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. Where language meets meaningful action: a combined behavior and lesion analysis of
Parkinson, A., Condon, L., Jackson, S.R., 2010. Parietal cortex coding of limb posture: in aphasia and apraxia. Brain Struct. Funct. 221 (1), 563–576. https://doi.org/10.1007/
search of the body-schema. Neuropsychologia 48 (11), 3228–3234. https://doi.org/ s00429-014-0925-3.
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.039. Wilke, M., De Haan, B., Juenger, H., Karnath, H.-O., 2011. Manual, semi-automated, and
Rankin, J., 1957. Cerebral vascular accidents in patients over the age of 60. II. Prognosis. automated delineation of chronic brain lesions: a comparison of methods.
Scott. Med. J. 2, 200–215. Neurolmage 56, 2038–2046.

10

You might also like