You are on page 1of 10

Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior

Violence exposure and the development of school-related functioning:


Mental health, neurocognition, and learning
Suzanne Perkins a,⁎, Sandra Graham-Bermann b
a
University of Michigan, Department of Psychiatry, United States
b
University of Michigan, Department of Psychology, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The relation between history of violence exposure and the development of academic and mental health prob-
Received 5 May 2011 lems is explored. Violence exposed children have an increased risk of developing school-related problems in-
Received in revised form 19 September 2011 cluding: mental health problems, learning disabilities, language impairments, and other neurocognitive
Accepted 21 October 2011
problems. These problems interact to create a complex web of deficits and disabilities where intervention ac-
Available online 28 October 2011
cess points are difficult to assess. Often mental health problems and academic problems develop in parallel.
Keywords:
Timing of violence exposure and the developmental stage of the child during exposure complicate our under-
Child maltreatment standing of the underlying mechanism. A model is presented that explores pathways linking violence expo-
Language impairment sure to aspects of school-related functioning, both academically and behaviorally. Early life stress, in the form
Disability of violence exposure, is related to neurocognitive deficits, including executive functioning and problems in
Externalizing behavior self-regulation. Deficits in self-regulation at the level of behavior, and cognitive control and executive func-
Internalizing behavior tioning, at the level of brain processing, are related to both academic and mental health problems, suggesting
Self-regulation a possible psychological mechanism. Biological mechanisms are also included in the model to illustrate the
contribution of the stress response, neuroendocrine system response, and neuroanatomical structural and
functional impairments associated with violence exposure.
© 2011 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
1.1. Violence exposure and functioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
1.1.1. Mental health . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
1.1.2. Academic achievement . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
1.1.3. Potential pathways . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
2. Biological mechanisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
2.1. Neuroendocrine disruption . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92
2.2. Brain development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
3. Language development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
3.1. Early childhood communication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
3.2. Language skills . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
3.3. Reading skills . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95
4. Self-regulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95
4.1. Mental health and academic problems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95
4.1.1. Executive function . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95
4.1.2. Violence exposure and neurocognition . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95
5. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Abbreviations: 5-HT, serotonin; ADHD, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; AFDC, Aid for Families with Dependent Children; CD, Conduct Disorder; CPS, Child Protective
Service; DA, dopamine; LD, Learning Disability; LI, Language Impairment; MAOA, monoamine oxidase A; NE, norephinephrine; ODD, Oppositional Defiant Disorder; PTSD, Posttrau-
matic Stress Disorder.
⁎ Corresponding author at: Psychiatry Affective Neuroimaging Program, University of Michigan Department of Psychiatry, 2709 Rachel Upjohn Building, 4250 Plymouth Rd., Ann
Arbor, MI 48109-5765, United States. Tel.: + 1 734 232 0315; fax: + 1 734 936 7868.
E-mail address: sperkinz@umich.edu (S. Perkins).

1359-1789/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.10.001
90 S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98

6. Clinical and research implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96


Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97

1. Introduction maltreatment, but we use the term more broadly to refer to any sub-
set of child abuse and neglect. The rationale for the use of the broad
Exposure to violence is related to problems in school functioning, term, violence exposure, is twofold. First, multiple studies have
including mental health, cognitive processing and language develop- shown exposure to one form of violence increases the likelihood of
ment. These problems interact to create a complex web of disabilities exposure to other forms of violence and also outcomes of violence
where intervention access points are difficult to assess. Children with exposure vary based on severity, developmental stage of the child,
histories of violence exposure are often referred for mental health and the individual child's developmental trajectory making violence
services at an early age and frequently develop emotional, and behav- type specific mechanisms unlikely (Andersen et al., 2008; Margolin,
ioral disorders. The cost to both individuals and to society is large. Vickerman, Oliver, & Gordis, 2010). In other words, children who
One in eight children experiences some form of maltreatment experience one form of violence can have a variety of social and emo-
(Finkelhor, Ormrod, Turner, & Hamby, 2005), as either abuse or the tional behavioral outcomes and children who experience another
neglect of proper care. Abuse has particular repercussions for the de- form of violence may have the same diversity of outcomes. This
velopment of school skills such as language learning, cognitive pro- necessitates the discussion of violence exposure more broadly.
cessing and self-regulation. Interpersonal violence and community It is also necessary to say a word about mental health problems as
violence experienced in childhood are related to myriad psychosocial they relate to school function. A number of different disorders that
problems including, attachment problems, speech, language and so- fall under the broader umbrella of externalizing disorders (ADHD,
cial interactions, delays in emotion processing, and intellectual and Conduct Disorder (CD), Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD), and
behavior problems (Azar & Wolfe, 2006). A better understanding of Autism) and internalizing disorders (anxiety, depression, and post-
the developmental trajectory of the relation between school-related traumatic stress disorder (PTSD)) impact school function. Many
function and violence exposure, at both the level of behavior and children exhibit both internalizing and externalizing problems
the level of brain development, will help to guide research and inter- simultaneously (Graham-Bermann, Gruber, Girz, & Howell, 2009;
vention toward sensitive periods in development. Hudziak, Achenbach, Althoff, & Pine, 2007). In addition, limited cog-
There are critical turning points in the parallel development of nitive function, problems with executive functioning, diagnosed lan-
emotion regulation and academic skills in children exposed to vio- guage impairments (LI), delayed language development, diagnosed
lence that deserve further study. Violence exposure during childhood learning, reading or math disorders, and school failure are within
happens in concert with critical periods in language and social devel- the Diagnostic and Statistical Manuel category of disorders most
opment, from infancy through adolescence. Violence exposure, par- often diagnosed in childhood.
ticularly family violence, can wreak havoc on the development of For purposes of this paper, we consider school related academic
neural circuits underlying basic mechanisms of affective and cogni- issues as separate from mental health in order to elucidate the trans-
tive development (Andersen et al., 2008; Choi, Jeong, Rohan, Polcari, actional relation between school functioning and mental health.
& Teicher, 2009; Miskovic, Schmidt, Georgiades, Boyle, & Macmillan, Theoretically, a specific learning disorder could be the cause of a spe-
2010; Seckfort et al., 2008, Sheu, Polcari, Anderson, & Teicher, cific behavioral disorder or vice versa. More likely, there is multifin-
2010). There is no longer any doubt that the assault of violence, ality in the way that behavioral disorders transact within a particular
both through physical or psychological trauma, can and does disrupt child and family context. The purpose of this review is to examine
the process of normal child development. this complex relation, to examine the role of violence in mental
Children with a history of violence exposure follow certain devel- health and academic development and to propose a model of the
opmental trajectories, often withdrawing socially or behaviorally role of violence in the development of both academic and mental
regressing, which can cause problems with peer relationships, espe- health problems (see Graphical abstract).
cially in demanding social settings, such as in school. At the level of
the brain, children exposed to violence may exhibit neurological 1.1. Violence exposure and functioning
changes that lead to problems of cognition in memory, executive
functioning (the ability to organize and synthesize information), 1.1.1. Mental health
self-regulation, language causing learning delays or disabilities (De Violence exposure in various forms has been related to problems
Bellis, Hooper, Spratt, & Woolley, 2009; De Bellis, Hooper, Woolley, & in the development of psychosocial health in some children and ado-
Shenk, 2009; DePrince, Weinzierl, & Combs, 2009; El-Hage, Gaillard, lescents. Although many people show great resilience after violence
Isingrini, & Belzung, 2006;Seckfort et al., 2008; Watts-English, Fortson, exposure, exposure to violence in the home and community is a pre-
Gibler, Hooper, & DeBellis, 2006). Integrating research from a number cursor to the development of externalizing and internalizing mental
of fields, we review the confluence between violence exposure, mental health problems in some, with the degree of exposure determining
health problems, language learning, neurocognitive development and the extent of the problem (Buckner, Beardslee, & Bassuk, 2004;
disabilities. Finzi-Dottan, Dekel, Lavi, & Su'ali, 2006; Lewis et al., 2010; Ozer &
Violence exposure, as used here, refers to violence in the commu- McDonald, 2006; Ruchkin, Henrich, Jones, Vermeiren, & Schwab-
nity and family. Generally, exposure to interpersonal violence, Stone, 2007). National lifetime rates of witnessing community vio-
whether witnessing violence between caregivers or direct child mal- lence as a child are around 30% (Finkelhor et al., 2005), but rates differ
treatment, has a more negative impact on children than does expo- based on community. Children who experience one form of violence
sure to community violence and is the focus of many of the studies are much more likely to experience community violence with rates
reviewed here. The term maltreatment is understood to include close to 100% if children report other violence exposure (Turner,
both child abuse and neglect. Child abuse encompasses physical, sex- Finkelhor, & Ormrod, 2010). In a population of homeless children, re-
ual and emotional abuse of a child. Often individual studies reviewed searchers found the rate of exposure to community violence to be
here focus more discreetly on one of the specific aspects of over 60% (Buckner et al., 2004). In urban populations rates of
S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98 91

community violence exposure have been found to be as high as 70% the rate was 14%, but was 24% for the maltreated group. Developmental
(Thompson & Massat, 2005). risk factors as well as demographic factors, such as being male and
Although total community violence exposure predicts both inter- African American, were associated with entry into special education.
nalizing and externalizing mental health problems, in the Buckner Developmental risk, when combined with maltreatment, increased
study, this was particularly salient for internalizing mental health the relation to later special education. In terms of abuse, physical
problems where greater exposure was salient for both genders but abuse increased the rate for later special education by 50%. The re-
was particularly salient for girls in the manifestation of internalizing. searchers found that children with physical abuse had a heightened
Of note, the authors also found self-regulatory ability to mediate this risk of serious emotional disturbance (SED), neglect was related to men-
relation. Self-regulation has been found in a number of studies to be tal retardation, sexual to learning disability and mixed abuse to child
a salient mediator and at the brain level, in the form of executive delay and other health problems. Full-scale IQ deficits have also been
function or cognitive control, is a possible mechanism for the devel- found in children with community violence exposure (Ratner et al.,
opment of both mental health and academic problems following 2006), domestic violence exposure (Ybarra et al., 2007), neglect (Noble,
exposure to violence. Tottenham, & Casey, 2005), and child abuse (De Bellis, Hooper,
Exposure to violence in the home, either witnessing violence be- Spratt, & Woolley, 2009; Perkins, Smith-Darden, & Graham-Bermann,
tween parents or experiencing direct violence, is also a precursor of 2011).
the development of mental health problems (English, Marshall, & Community violence and child abuse are both related to lower
Stewart, 2003; English et al., 2005; Hanson et al., 2008; Kim & reading ability (De Bellis, Hooper, Spratt, & Woolley, 2009; Ratner
Cicchetti, 2009; Perkins, Cortina, Smith-Darden, & Graham-Bermann, et al., 2006). In a large urban sample, Ratner and colleagues found
2012; Ybarra, Wilkens, & Lieberman, 2007). The rate at which violence that violence victimization was related to lower overall ability and
exposure is predictive differs based on gender and violence type comprehension at a trend level. Interestingly, they also found that a
(Hanson et al., 2008). In general, violence more than doubles the feeling of protection at home and school was related to higher read-
risk of development of PTSD and depression. Sexual abuse is most ing ability and comprehension (Ratner et al., 2006). In addition,
commonly experienced by girls and is the highest predictor of child neglect has been related to deficits in vocabulary, language pro-
PTSD, at triple the rate, and equal to physical violence in predicting cessing speed, memory and measures of attention and executive pro-
depression (Hanson et al., 2008). Among boys, sexual abuse is partic- cessing (De Bellis, Hooper, Spratt, et al., 2009; De Bellis, Hooper,
ularly damaging. Although boys experience sexual abuse less com- Woolley, et al., 2009). For maltreated children, memory may be par-
monly, risk of the development of PTSD in boys is more than five ticularly salient in the connection between violence exposure and de-
times (Hanson et al., 2008). velopment of PTSD (De Bellis, Hooper, Spratt, et al., 2009; De Bellis,
Age of first exposure, frequency of exposure and exposure to mul- Hooper, Woolley, et al., 2009).
tiple types of violence are important variables to consider in the de-
velopment of mental health problems. In a study of children referred 1.1.3. Potential pathways
to child protective service for child abuse and neglect, multiple types Violence exposures can cause neurocognitive and language pro-
of abuse and chronicity of abuse predicted internalizing mental cessing problems through two pathways: neurological changes
health problems (English et al., 2005). In the same study, externaliz- directly resulting from the violence exposure, and differences in ex-
ing was most predicted by the earlier age of first exposure. Consider- posure to interpersonal communication and language over the course
ing that in early childhood one of the core developmental tasks is the of development, including number of words and type of speech (see
development of social interactions with others, it would make sense Fig. 1). In the first instance, violence exposure can be a direct cause
that early exposure to violence would disrupt that process and make of academic problems and disabilities through brain injury. In fact,
children more vulnerable to developing externalizing behaviors. rates of brain injury from shaken baby syndrome are over 90%
The authors examined later social competence in this population (Sobsey & Nehring, 2005). Violence can also be an indirect cause of
and found that chronicity of violence, physical abuse, age, and multi- academic problems through brain changes related to learning. In the
ple types of abuse predicted deficits in social competence (English second instance, violence is related to exposure to language, which
et al., 2005). Although they did not measure self-regulatory capacity in turn is related to differences in child language acquisition (Taylor,
directly, self-regulation of emotional response is central to social Donovan, Miles, & Leavitt, 2009). Parents who communicate with
competence. There is evidence that in addition to a direct relation their children using high amounts of negative control (use of
between abuse and mental health problems, peer-rejection and anger, criticisms, threats, punishments, slapping and spanking),
self-regulation may mediate this relation (Kim & Cicchetti, 2009; fewer questions, and more commands and prohibitions, overall,
Perkins et al., 2012). use fewer words and different types of words. Children in these
In another study, total CPS referrals also predicted child health prob- dyads also use fewer words and different types of words but they
lems, including physical disabilities, emotional disorders and learning also use words with fewer morphemes (the smallest part of a
disorders, underlying the complex relations between psychopathology word that carries meaning), fewer grammatical word types, and lan-
and academic problems (English et al., 2003). Finally, school-related guage with fewer different functions (Taylor et al., 2009). These dif-
violence, which is an often over-looked area of violence scholarship, is ferences in cognitive and language processing, if not remediated
important in the lives of children and can be related mental health early, are direct precursors of learning and cognitive disabilities
problems (Flannery, Wester, & Singer, 2004). (Taylor et al., 2009).
One core psychological construct that is linked to both biological
1.1.2. Academic achievement changes resulting from violence exposure and language development
In terms of language and academic achievement, community and is executive function deficits (or behavioral self-regulation), which
interpersonal violence exposure have been related to more negative may be central to the connection between academic achievement
outcomes. Children with exposure to child abuse and neglect have and mental health problems after violence exposure (see Fig. 1).
twice the rate of referral for special education (Jonson-Reid, Drake, Next, we examine the evidence for these pathways in turn.
Kim, Porterfield, & Han, 2004). In this prospective study of children
who received Aid for Families with Dependent Children (AFDC), 2. Biological mechanisms
matched based on later substantiated maltreatment, 19% of the total
sample entered special education later at a mean age of eight years The study of neurobiological processes is one logical avenue of re-
old. Among the children from AFCD backgrounds without a CPS referral search for further understanding the underlying mechanisms that
92 S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98

Fig. 1. Graphical abstract depicting an overview of relations between violence exposure, academic function and mental health.

may produce the co-occurrence of mental health and academic func- 2.1. Neuroendocrine disruption
tioning deficits after violence exposure. Childhood is a time of neural
plasticity and environmental influences, including abuse and neglect, Autonomic stress response can be measured in humans by systolic
can impact the developing brain function of children (Weiss & and diastolic blood pressure, heart rate variability (or respiratory sinus
Wagner, 1998). There are three major ways in which, theoretically, arrhythmia, a measure of the variability of heart rate over the respiration
alteration of normal brain development can influence learning and cycle), galvanic skins response and differences in neuroendocrine ex-
behavior (see Fig. 2). First, alteration of the neuroendocrine system, pression, in particular the cortisol response. The experience of violence
neuroanatomical structure (in the form of white matter and gray exposure disrupts the autonomic system including blood pressure
matter), and brain function may inhibit an individual's ability to de- (Murali & Chen, 2005), heart rate variability (Gordis, Feres, Olezeski,
velop optimal functioning following exposure to violence. Second, Rabkin, & Trickett, 2010; Heim et al., 2000; Murali & Chen, 2005), galvan-
these systems interact and associate with each other to produce cog- ic skin response (Gordis et al., 2010) and cortisol (Heim et al., 2000;
nitive functions (such as executive functioning or cognitive control) Murali & Chen, 2005; Murray-Close, Han, Cicchetti, Crick, & Rogosch,
necessary for the development of self-regulation and in turn emo- 2008; Obradović, Bush, Stamperdahl, Adler, & Boyce, 2010). Blood
tional or behavioral functioning. Finally, exposure to violence may pressure, heart rate variability, and galvanic skin response are physical
influence the developing brain at a critical period making completion manifestations of an internal body–brain regulation of hormones, neu-
of certain core developmental processes difficult, delayed, or impossi- rotransmitters and metabolic responses. These systems interact under
ble. We review the evidence for neuroendocrine disruption, and neu- stress in complex ways through the regulation of neurotransmitters,
roanatomical functional and structural changes below. which in turn regulate the release of stress hormones.

Fig. 2. Biological mechanisms related to both mental health and school function.
S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98 93

Within the neuroendocrine system, cortisol is particularly im- Self-regulation is particularly important in that it is tied to both
portant as it is involved in the fight or flight response to stress mental health and school function through deficits in attention and
(Meewisse, Reitsma, De Vries, Gersons, & Olff, 2007). Cortisol is a ADHD. The three primary systems that have been implicated in the de-
glucocorticoid produced by the adrenal gland. Corticotrophin- velopment of ADHD are the noradrenergic (NA), dopaminergic (DA)
releasing hormone, a neurotransmitter released by the hypothala- and serotonergic (5-HT) systems (Halperin, Newcorn, & Sharma,
mus, is activated by the stress response and begins a cascade by 1996). The NA system is the site of function for most ADHD medica-
activating adrenocorticotropic hormone in the pituitary gland, that tions, but none of these systems is singularly implicated in ADHD.
in turn activates the release of the stress hormone cortisol from There is a distinction between children with comorbid aggression
the adrenal gland. Basal cortisol, cortical diurnal rhythms and corti- and ADHD and those with a more “pure” ADHD (Campbell, Sameroff,
cal reactivity to stress have all been implicated in exposure to stress, Lewis, & Miller, 2000; Halperin et al., 1996; Lahey, McBurnett, &
but a thorough evaluation of the literature is beyond the scope of Loeber, 2000). Reading Disordered (RD) and non-RD children with
this paper. Exposure to violence is related to higher basal cortical ADHD differ in their NA response, whereas children who are aggres-
levels, greater flattening of the cortisol diurnal rhythm (Cicchetti, sive and have ADHD differ from pure ADHD in 5-HT response suggest-
Rogosch, Gunnar, & Toth, 2010; Murali & Chen, 2005), and a slower ing that the 5-HT system is more central to aggression whereas the NA
growth in cortisol over the course of development (Trickett, Noll, system may be more central to RD and ADHD (Halperin et al., 1996).
Susman, Shenk, & Putnam, 2010).
Timing of the child's exposure to violence is particularly critical in
changes in cortical response to later stress (Cicchetti et al., 2010; 2.2. Brain development
Trickett et al., 2010). Early abuse may be particularly damaging
(Cicchetti et al., 2010), but timing of abuse in relation to the develop- Research has shown that violence exposure impacts normal brain
ment of a normative cortisol response in children is particularly difficult development. Violence exposure appears to be related to dysfunc-
to study, as children often experience abuse for a number of years tional development in brain volumes, white matter development
(Trickett et al., 2010). Cortisol reactivity is also implicated in reduced and regional brain function. Violence exposed children have smaller
school engagement and academic competence (Obradović et al., 2010) brain volumes in areas related to cognition and emotion processing
and the development of psychopathology (Cicchetti et al., 2010; (Andersen et al., 2008), less white matter cohesion (Choi et al.,
Obradović et al., 2010). The developmental stage of the child and the 2009; Seckfort et al., 2008), dysfunction in dopamine rich brain re-
demands of both schooling and social development are likely to influ- gions (Sheu et al., 2010), and altered lateralization of brain function
ence the behavioral outcomes of abuse at various developmental stages. (Miskovic et al., 2010). Complicating the science of understanding
Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) is an enzyme, controlled by the this link, the timing, duration, and length of exposure determine at
MAOA gene, implicated in the regulation the neurotransmitters what point within normative development, violence exposure influ-
serotonin (5-HT), dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) (Mead, ences the brain and what structures and functions are most impacted
Beauchaine, & Shannon, 2010). Maltreated children with a func- (Andersen et al., 2008). Smaller brain volumes are related to the age
tional polymorphism in the MAOA gene have increased rates of at which violence exposure occurs and are a sign of disrupted devel-
mental health disorders including attention and emotional problems opment. Deficit in white matter cohesion is considered a sign of dys-
(Kim-Cohen et al., 2006). Neuroendocrine dysregulation in 5-HT, DA, functional brain networking. Dopamine rich areas are necessary for
and NE and related systems has been found in studies of children the development of self-regulatory function. Finally, altered laterali-
with violence exposure histories (De Bellis et al., 1999). In this zation is interpreted as a dysfunction in the development of regional
seminal study, maltreated children with PTSD (n = 18) were found functional differences. Normal developmental changes, such as in
to have higher urinary epinephrine, norepinephrine, dopamine and cortical thickness, are also influenced by individual factors, such as
free cortisol. Duration of abuse was positively correlated with all intelligence, which in turn may be influenced by violence exposure
four measures while NE and DA were primarily correlated with (Graham-Bermann, Howell, Miller, Kwek, & Lilly, 2010; Shaw et al.,
PTSD symptomatology. 2006).
The MAOA gene is also related to a number of other mental health Taken together, these findings suggest that violence exposure
and behavioral outcomes including aggression (Kim-Cohen et al., negatively impacts the neuroendocrine system and brain develop-
2006), depression (Bremner, 2003), anti-social (Kim-Cohen et al., ment during critical periods of the normal developmental trajectory.
2006) and self-regulatory behavioral problems (Kim-Cohen et al., This disruption leads to vulnerabilities in systems necessary for cog-
2006; Oades et al., 2008). Using data from the Environmental Longi- nitive and emotion processing. In particular, neuroendocrine disrup-
tudinal Risk study a birth cohort of 975 boys with genotyped data, tion of stress related function and the combination of vulnerabilities
Kim-Cohen and colleagues examined 62 boys with physical abuse in cognitive control and emotion brain areas, can lead to a deficit in
histories compared to matched controls. They examined polymor- self-regulatory processing. Adequate self-regulatory processing is a
phisms of the MAOA gene associated with high and low activity of necessary component of classroom functioning and often is the first
MAOA. Exposure to physical abuse was related to higher mental school-related psychopathology to raise the attention of schools and
health problems in both groups. In addition, they found an interac- parents.
tion whereby the Low MAOA polymorphisms had lower mental
health problems in the unexposed group but the group exposed to
physical abuse with Low MAOA, showed a steeper rise in mental 3. Language development
health problems. This same interaction was found for attention, anti-
social and emotion problems, if low activity MAOA and no exposure The development of the relationship between language disorders
to physical abuse, problems were less than in the high activity MAOA and behavior problems can be seen as going through a series of stages
group but surpassed the high activity MAOA if exposed to physical that are reviewed in turn below. The first stage is a delay in language
abuse (Kim-Cohen et al., 2006). This suggests that the low activity development in preschool with its association with attentional
MAOA polymorphism is a phenotypic risk factor for mental health problems. In middle childhood, children with reading disabilities and
problems when exposed to the violence. In addition, this work sug- those with a history of language delay show higher rates of internaliz-
gests that violence exposure and genetic risk interact to create a ing problems. In adolescence, which is associated with a general trend
vulnerability to different mental health problems, including both in increased risk behavior, there is a link between unremediated read-
aggression and self-regulation. ing disabilities and externalizing behavior problems (see Fig. 3).
94 S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98

Fig. 3. Developmental course of language development as a factor in the development of mental health problems in children.

3.1. Early childhood communication likely to hear disjointed language and receive irrelevant replies
(Dale, 1996). Parents with higher expressive language skills use
One mechanism that has been posited for language disorders lead- more physical comforting with their children and more explanations
ing to problem behaviors is through communication failure. Children for requests during child conflict situations, rather than demands
with language impairments are more likely to have difficulty with (Stansbury & Zimmermann, 1999; Taylor et al., 2009). Expressive
pragmatic language, such as turn taking, which would interfere with language is related to cognitive development, whereas the use of di-
their social development and day-to-day communication with peers rective language is related to decreased development of a number of
and teachers. Stevenson (1996) argues that this “communication fail- elements of language including total word usage, receptive, sematic
ure may lead to frustration on the part of language impaired children and pragmatic language (Murray & Hornbaker, 1997; Taylor et al.,
that may lead to both externalizing and internalizing” (see Fig. 3). So- 2009). Family environment contributes to the language skills of the
cial cognitive deficits have also been linked to language impairment. child, and then, subsequently, children's language skills promote
Children with language impairments may experience a decrease in behaviors in primary caregivers that either inhibit or disinhibit emo-
self-esteem (McGee, Share, Moffitt, Williams, & Silva, 1988) putting tional regulation, which in turn can lead to externalizing behavior
them at-risk of developing negative social behaviors. Children with development.
language impairment have been shown to have a more limited under- Approximately half of all children with language delays have been
standing of emotion in social situations. reported to exhibit comorbid behavior problems, and by eight years of
The development of self-regulation of emotion and the develop- age language delay is related to increased behavior problems
ment of early language are bidirectional. Toddlers express emotion (Benasich, Curtiss, & Tallal, 1993). Children with expressive language
through facial expression and body language but are unable to delays are more likely to show hyperactive and under-controlled
express emotion through the use of language (Dale, 1996). Toddlers behavior, which is not the case for children with receptive language de-
use language in order to communicate in unaroused states. Children lays (Stevenson, 1996). The reverse relation has also been found.
progress through stages of language development that allow for in- Among psychiatrically referred children, in one study, over a quarter
creased use of internal state language, or language used to express had previously been diagnosed with Language Impairments (LI)
and describe emotions. Internal state language is developed with (Cohen, Davine, Horodezky, Lipsett, & Isaacson, 1993). Children with di-
five major influences: the maternal language used toward the child, agnosed LI differed from those with undiagnosed LI in having more se-
the mother-infant attachment bond, the cognitive development of vere expressive language delays but previously undiagnosed children
the child, sibling and peer influences and the child's style of language were rated more aggressive and delinquent by their teachers and par-
learning. There are differences in the ways that boys and girls learn ents. Approximately 50% of children with ADHD also have speech/lan-
internal state language (Dale, 1996). Both mothers and older siblings guage impairments (Tannock & Schachar, 1996).
have been found to use more internal state language with girls, with Vocabulary development is important in the understanding of
a corresponding positive impact on the use of internal state language both aggressive behavior and ADHD. Vocabulary development is
by girls at 24 months. Children with behavioral problems experience related to aggression in 19-month-old twins (Dionne, Tremblay,
more parental criticism and less parental warmth that may lead to Boivin, Laplante, & Perusse, 2003). In this study, aggression and
delays in language development (Dale, 1996). vocabulary growth each have both a genetic and an environmental
component. However, the percent of variance explained differed
3.2. Language skills between these two phenotypes, suggesting that they differ in terms
of the extent to which genetics and environment influence their de-
Another model posits that family environment and later hyperac- velopment. For aggression, 58% of the variance was explained by
tive behaviors may be indirectly linked through language and non- genetic factors but for expressive vocabulary, 51% of the variance
verbal skills. Language skills at 18 months predict hyperactive be- was accounted for by shared environment, suggesting that family
havior at 5 years (Girouard et al., 1998). Less educated parents are rearing practices play an important role in vocabulary growth. Hart
likely to use fewer words, less complicated syntax and fewer refer- and Risley (1995) found that total number of words spoken in the
ences to events not in the present when communicating with their home vary greatly and are the single strongest determinant of child
children (Hart & Risley, 1995). Children in stressed homes are more vocabulary growth.
S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98 95

3.3. Reading skills One interesting study examined adolescent boys exposed to terror
attacks (e.g., suicide bombings and shootings) in Israel (Finzi-Dottan
Children with language impairments develop reading disabilities at et al., 2006). This study compared learning disability (LD) and history
higher rates than children without language impairments. Reading dis- of prior exposure as vulnerabilities for the development of mental
orders, which by definition cannot be diagnosed until children are read- health problems (in the form of PTSD). Learning disability predicted
ing, have been implicated in the relationship with both internalizing both avoidance and hypervigilant symptoms in youth with LD sug-
and externalizing problems. Willcutt and Pennington (2000) found gesting, perhaps, that some core psychological process associated
both reading disordered girls and boys showed higher rates of ADHD, with LD leads to a failure at top-down control over emotion. Illustrat-
ODD and CD and had higher rates of anxiety and depression. They also ing how complicated these interactions are in populations exposed to
scored higher on Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) internalizing and violence, past history of threatening life events, such as car accident,
externalizing subscales. Results were particularly pronounced with hospitalization and violence exposure, and parental attachment
boys in rates of ADHD, ODD and CD and with girls in rates of depression. also predicted PTSD symptoms. Studies of youth with violence expo-
It is clear there is a transactional relationship between child psychopa- sure and/or behavioral disorders may miss these developmentally
thology and learning disorders, although the specific mechanisms at complex relations by studying middle childhood or adolescence,
work are likely to be diverse and complex. Much less clear is the role where academic problems may have been present but undetected
of the context in moderating these causal relationships. For children for much of the time prior to study.
with a history of maltreatment, the context of their maltreatment is in-
tegral to their development. 4.1.1. Executive function
Externalizing, internalizing, and comorbid children have lower IQs Executive dysfunction has been posited as a core neurocognitive
(Diamond, Muller, Rondeau, & Rich, 2001; Kusche, Cook, & Greenberg, function linking psychopathology and academic problems. Executive
1993) and reading math and spelling achievement (Kusche et al., function is the ability to plan, organize, and delay pre-potent re-
1993). ADHD is comorbid with CD, Major Depressive Disorder, Anxiety sponses in order to reach more complex goals. Failures in executive
Disorder, and learning disabilities (LD) or special needs (Faraone, functioning have been posited as the underlying cause of attention
Biederman, & Keily, 1996). Externalizing and comorbid groups are signif- deficit disorder. This higher order skill is seen as necessary in the de-
icantly more likely to be represented in special education (Kusche et al., velopment of self-regulation, a deficit seen in children with ADHD. In
1993). Children with internalizing disorders have equal rates of oral addition, self-regulation is skill that is integral to the development of
language problems as those with externalizing problems (Donahue, language skills and other skills central to school success.
Hartas, & Cole, 1999). PTSD symptomology predicts lower cognitive Executive dysfunction has been implicated in the relationship be-
functioning (Diamond et al., 2001). These studies of the correlation tween learning problems and aggression as well. In an early study,
between psychopathology and learning problems typically do not Nigg, Quamma, Greenberg, and Kusche (1999) found that children
address the directionality of effect. It may be that learning problems with lower scores on the Stroop test had later externalizing prob-
either pre-date psychopathology or co-occur in early childhood. lems. Seguin, Boulerice, Harden, Tremblay, and Pihl (1999) found
that children who showed stable aggression over a six year period
4. Self-regulation performed less well than both unstable aggressive children (those
who showed aggression at some assessment periods but not others)
Violence exposure is also to linked to increased internalizing and and nonaggressive children on tasks of general memory IQ, and fron-
externalizing behavior through self-regulation. Violence exposure tal lobe tests.
causes decreases in self-regulatory behavior (Kim & Cicchetti, 2009)
tied to problems in executive functioning, the ability to plan, orga- 4.1.2. Violence exposure and neurocognition
nize, and synthesize information (DePrince et al., 2009; Seckfort et Violence exposed children exhibit neurological changes that
al., 2008). In particular, the relation between self-regulation, psycho- might contribute to trouble with learning. A burgeoning literature
pathology and academic learning may mediate the development of is showing that family trauma exposure and early life stress are relat-
academic problems after violence exposure. ed to poorer executive function (DePrince et al., 2009; Seckfort et al.,
2008). Several studies by De Bellis have shown that children with
4.1. Mental health and academic problems abuse and neglect histories have deficits in executive functioning
(Beers & De Bellis, 2002; De Bellis, Hooper, Spratt, & Woolley,
One of the most stable findings in developmental psychology is 2009). De Bellis postulates that neuroendocrine changes in response
that behavior disorders and other forms of psychopathology are co- to stress from abuse and neglect negatively impact the frontal lobe
morbid with cognitive impairment, school failure and learning dis- of the brain, causing inattention and other problems associated
abilities such as reading and language disorders (Benasich et al., with executive functioning (De Bellis, 2001; De Bellis, Hooper, Sapia,
1993; Cohen et al., 1993; Dale, 1996; Donahue et al., 1999). More re- Vasterling, & Brewin, 2005; Watts-English et al., 2006). Violence expo-
cent research has shown that there is a bidirectional relation between sure also has deleterious effects on memory (De Bellis, Hooper, Spratt,
symptoms that undermine school function and failures in functioning & Woolley, 2009; De Bellis, Hooper, Woolley, & Shenk, 2009; El-Hage
contributing to symptoms (Kim & Cicchetti, 2009; Masten et al., et al., 2006) and on performance speed (El-Hage et al., 2006), both skills
2005). In one study, the combination of language impairment and pa- necessary for successful school functioning.
rental distress predicted separately to both aggression and delin- Childhood executive functioning deficits have been posited as the
quency (Brownlie et al., 2004). In another, ADHD and youth with basis for problems with self-regulation. Students who have difficulty
language disorders had high rates of aggression, depression and anx- with self-regulation are often removed from normative learning situ-
iety symptoms (McGillivray & Baker, 2008). Although these rates ations, through removal from the regular classrooms, putting them at
were subclinical, high rates of mental health symptoms, which do greater risk of the development of cognitive and language problems.
not rise to the level of true mental health disorders, may impact At home, children who lack self-regulation hear more directive lan-
peer and teacher relationships. Both peer and teacher relationships guage, a correlate with delayed language development (Taylor et al.,
are central to success at school, which means that failures in these re- 2009). At school, children are removed from more inclusive environ-
lationships are likely to contribute to the circularity of the relation be- ments, also a risk factor for the development of academic skills. In
tween school academic function and psychopathology (Kim & turn, cognitive delays and language processing problems likely play
Cicchetti, 2009). a role in the development of executive function and self-regulation
96 S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98

skills. This circular relationship between executive functioning and There is evidence from cognitive neuroscience that violence ex-
self-regulation, and language and cognitive processing is difficult to posure causes neurobiological changes that have implications for
disentangle. However, each type of delay has been connected with learning, executive functioning and self-regulation. Findings from
internalizing mental health problems. Executive function deficits, neurobiology show that the experience of violence alters the neuro-
self-regulation problems, language processing and cognitive devel- chemistry of an individual in ways that may impact learning or may
opment delays each hamper the child's ability to understand and mimic or create learning disabilities. Cascading stress hormones also
respond to social cues, which results in withdrawal or acting out in influence self-regulatory capacity. Children who have experienced
social situations. violence exhibit abnormalities in a variety of brain regions in cortical
thickness, white matter structure and function. It is reasonable to hy-
pothesize that these neurological differences could result in difficul-
5. Conclusions ties in school. The frontal region is utilized in planning, setting goals,
organizing, and prioritizing information. The temporal lobe is the
The research literature has repeatedly documented a connection site of the Wernicke's area, an important site for language compre-
between violence exposure and later psychopathology. Previous hension, and the auditory cortex where auditory stimuli are pro-
studies have documented that different types of violence exposure cessed. The amygdala is part of the limbic system and has a role in
have different outcomes for children. Sexual abuse is particularly the processing of emotion. Finally, the corpus callosum is necessary
damaging in terms of mental health, most specifically in relation to for the transfers and integration of information from both halves of
the development of PTSD (Hanson et al., 2008). In terms of educa- the brain. These areas are at-risk for change due to violence exposure.
tional outcomes, physical abuse is most likely to contribute to spe- It is unlikely that different experiences with violence exposure,
cial education diagnosis, primarily in the diagnosis of serious which are likely to differentially influence the neurobiology of indi-
emotional disturbance. Sexual abuse was most likely to be related viduals, would have singular or simple patterns of influence on the
to LD (Jonson-Reid et al., 2004). This might suggest that PTSD in learning styles of students. However, it is clear that brain abnormali-
school is less recognized and has a less marked impact on school- ties are likely to impact sensory processing and integration in a num-
related behavior. It is also possible that the path for sexual abuse ber of distinct ways depending on the individual. In it also clear that
might be from abuse to up-regulation of emotion to failure to con- repeated maltreatment and traumatic events have a compounding
centrate, whereas for physical abuse the path might be from abuse effect on the brain (van der Kolk, 1997, 2003; Van Voorhees &
to failure to down-regulate to emotional dysregulation and concur- Scarpa, 2004).
rent academic problems. We have argued here that cognitive processing and learning prob-
Differential pathways based on type of violence have yet to be lems may be at the center between the known relation between vio-
established. However, the emergence of both externalizing and in- lence exposure and psychopathology. Children with Conduct Disorder
ternalizing adjustment problems can be a critical turning point in can be discriminated from controls by measuring language skills,
the development of violence-exposed children. Most vulnerable to punishment by parents, ADHD symptoms and oppositional symptoms
these deleterious effects are those functions that are concurrently (Toupin, Dery, Pauze, Mercier, & Fortin, 2000). Children with ADHD
developing at the time of violence exposure (Andersen et al., show deficits in pragmatic speech and with interpretations of social
2008). During early childhood, self-regulatory processing and lan- situations. Pragmatic speech seems to be positively influenced by
guage development can be most impaired in children with mental the use of stimulant medication prescribed for ADHD. The influence
health problems related to violence exposure. Academic and social of executive function is bidirectional. Executive function influences
functioning can be affected by later exposure. At each stage of devel- the development of pragmatic language and social communication
opment, academic function and social function can work together to and these factors in turn influence the development of executive
impact mental health either positively or negatively. function (Tannock & Schachar, 1996).
Social functioning difficulties have implications for school related
functioning. Both internalizing and externalizing behavior problems
remove students from social learning situations that promote lan- 6. Clinical and research implications
guage and cognitive growth. Removal from positive school and
family educational settings limits the youth's ability to learn more Targeted interventions directed at executive function and reme-
complex language patterns, organizational skills and self monitoring diating disability would be essential for assisting children with a
behaviors as the demands of social and school environment change. history of violence exposure regardless of whether or not their func-
The combination of social withdrawal and behavioral problems that tioning on any of those dimensions rises to the level of a diagnosable
lead to smaller, socially removed classroom environments, allow for disability. Development of cognitive and learning skills is central to
fewer opportunities to develop self-regulatory skills and social skills the known relation between maltreatment and internalizing and
necessary for healthy peer-to-peer and adult-to-child relationships. externalizing problems. This would suggest that screening for cogni-
The reverse is also true. Cognitive processing problems, language tive deficits and learning problems should be provided to children
delays, and disabilities are often associated with internalizing and with histories of violence exposure before they present with prob-
externalizing problems. Children with weak language skills may de- lems that reach a clinical cut off in special education or in the realm
velop, or continue to develop, externalizing problems, internalizing of mental health.
problems, or both through a number of mechanisms. Language im- Future research should focus on the interrelations between exec-
paired children have more difficulty understanding emotions of utive function, self-regulation, disability and other cognitive proces-
others and social situations. This may lead to communication failure sing deficits. Within the cognitive literature, the directions are
with other children or adults, which in turn leads to frustration. unclear. Executive function has been posited as a mediator between
These children may develop low self-esteem and resort to aggression language and cognitive processing and behavior. This line of research
or withdrawal from others as a result. In early childhood, children would help to determine cognitive interventions that could moder-
with language impairment may appear to be hyperactive or inatten- ate the role of cognitive impairments in the development of violence.
tive. In later childhood, language impairment can lead to reading dis- Another important future direction for research is in determining the
ability, which is associated with both internalizing and externalizing extent of neurobiological changes due to timing, duration and extent
behavior in childhood. In adolescence, children with continued read- of exposure to violence and the implications of these changes on the
ing disabilities are more likely to show externalizing problems. development of language and cognitive processing.
S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98 97

Finally, research might focus on intervention programs that can De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Sapia, J. L., Vasterling, J. J., & Brewin, C. R. (2005). Early
trauma exposure and the brain. Neuropsychology of PTSD: Biological, cognitive, and
target specific language and cognitive deficits that have been found clinical perspectives (pp. 153–177). Guilford Press.
to result from the maltreatment of children. Early intervention De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Spratt, E. G., & Woolley, D. P. (2009). Neuropsychological
with at-risk families and their children that focuses on screening findings in childhood neglect and their relationships to pediatric PTSD. Journal of
the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(06), 868.
for language and cognitive development problems should be incor- De Bellis, M., Hooper, S., Woolley, D., & Shenk, C. (2009, Dec 11). Demographic, maltreat-
porated into programs for children at risk of and exposed to violence ment, and neurobiological correlates of PTSD symptoms in children and adolescents.
and those at risk of developing psychopathology. Children who are Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 1–8.
DePrince, A., Weinzierl, K., & Combs, M. (2009). Executive function performance and
showing early language and cognitive delays would benefit from trauma exposure in a community sample of children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33
programs that provide targeted instruction on infant-caregiver in- (2009), 353–361.
teraction in terms of language and language-rich environments for Diamond, T., Muller, R. T., Rondeau, L. A., & Rich, J. G. (2001). The relationships among
PTSD symtomatology and cognitive functioning in adult survivors of child maltreat-
children.
ment. Advances in Psychology Research, Vol. V. (pp. 253–279)Huntington, NY, US:
Nova Science Publishers, Inc..
Dionne, G., Tremblay, R. E., Boivin, M., Laplante, D., & Perusse, D. (2003). Physical
Acknowledgments aggression and expressive vocabulary in 19-month-old twins. Developmental
Psychology, 39(2), 261–272.
Donahue, M., Hartas, D., & Cole, D. (1999). Research on interactions among oral language
The authors would like to thank the following people for assis- and emotional/behavioral disorders. In D. L. Rogers-Adkinson, & P. L. Griffith (Eds.),
tance in the preparation of this manuscript. Arnold Sameroff and Communication disorders and children with psychiatric and behavioral disorders
(pp. 466). San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.
the Development of Psychopathology and Mental Health certificate El-Hage, W., Gaillard, P., Isingrini, M., & Belzung, C. (2006). Trauma-related deficits in
program at the University of Michigan. Dr. Sameroff read versions of working memory. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11(1), 33–46.
this manuscript for his class on the development of psychopathology English, D. J., Upadhyaya, M. P., Litrownik, A. J., Marshall, J. M., Runyan, D. K., Graham, J. C.,
et al. (2005). Maltreatment's wake: the relationship of maltreatment dimensions to
and the program as a whole trained Dr. Perkins in thinking develop- child outcomes. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29(5), 597–619.
mentally about psychopathology and the role of both contextual English, D. J., Marshall, D. B., & Stewart, A. J. (2003). Effects of family violence on
and environmental influences. In particular, Toni Antonucci and child behavior and health during early childhood. Journal of Family Violence, 18
(1), 43–57.
Catherine Lord were instrumental in that instruction. We would like Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Keily, K. (1996). Cognitive functioning, learning disability,
to thank Dr. Gregory Dalack and the Department of Psychiatry for and school failure in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A family study perspec-
providing support and protected time to complete this work. Finally tive. In J. H. Beitchman, N. J. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas, & R. Tannock (Eds.),
Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical
Dr. Perkins would like to thank her former students who inspired
perspectives. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press (pp. xv, 582).
her academic career. Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., Turner, H., & Hamby, S. L. (2005). The victimization of
children and youth: A comprehensive, national survey. Child Maltreatment, 10(1),
5–25.
Finzi-Dottan, R., Dekel, R., Lavi, T., & Su'ali, T. (2006). Posttraumatic stress disorder
References reactions among children with learning disabilities exposed to terror attacks.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47(2), 144–151.
Andersen, S., Tomada, A., Vincow, E., Valente, E., Polcari, A., & Teicher, M. (2008). Pre- Flannery, D., Wester, K., & Singer, M. (2004). Impact of exposure to violence in school
liminary evidence for sensitive periods in the effect of childhood sexual abuse on on child and adolescent mental health and behavior. Journal of Community Psychology,
regional brain development. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 32(5), 559–573.
20(3), 292–301. Girouard, P. C., Baillargeon, R. H., Tremblay, R. E., Glorieux, J., Lefebvre, F., & Robaey, P.
Azar, S. T., & Wolfe, D. A. (2006). Child physical abuse and neglect. In E. J. Mash, & R. A. (1998). Developmental pathways leading to externalizing behaviors in 5 year olds
Barkley (Eds.), Treatment of childhood disorders (pp. 595–646). (3rd ed.). New born before 29 weeks of gestation. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
York: The Guilford Press. 19(4), 244–253.
Beers, S. R., & De Bellis, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological function in children with Gordis, E. B., Feres, N., Olezeski, C. L., Rabkin, A. N., & Trickett, P. K. (2010). Skin conduc-
maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, tance reactivity and respiratory sinus arrhythmia among maltreated and comparison
159(3), 483–486. youth: Relations with aggressive behavior. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 35(5),
Benasich, A. A., Curtiss, S., & Tallal, P. (1993). Language, learning, and behavioral distur- 547–558.
bances in childhood: A longitudinal perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Graham-Bermann, S. A., Gruber, G., Girz, L., & Howell, K. H. (2009). Factors discriminat-
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), 585–594. ing among profiles of resilient coping and psychopathology in children exposed to
Bremner, J. D. (2003). Long-term effects of childhood abuse on brain and neurobiology. domestic violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33(9), 648–660.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12(2), 271–292. Graham-Bermann, S. A., Howell, K. H., Miller, L. E., Kwek, J., & Lilly, M. (2010). Traumatic
Brownlie, E., Beitchman, J., Escobar, M., Young, A., Atkinson, L., Johnson, C., et al. (2004). events and maternal education as predictors of verbal ability for preschool children
Early language impairment and young adult delinquent and aggressive behavior. exposed to intimate partner violence (IPV). Journal of Family Violence, 25(4), 383–392.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(4), 453–467. Halperin, J. M., Newcorn, J. H., & Sharma, V. (1996). Neurochemical correlates of aca-
Buckner, J., Beardslee, W., & Bassuk, E. (2004). Exposure to violence and low-income demic achievement deficits and aggressive behavior in children with ADHD. In J.
children's mental health: Direct, moderated, and mediated relations. The American H. Beitchman, N. J. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas, & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language,
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 74(4), 413–423. learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives.
Campbell, S. M., Sameroff, A. J., Lewis, M., & Miller, S. M. (2000). Attention-deficit/ New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press (pp. xv, 582).
hyperactivity disorder: A developmental view. Handbook of developmental psycho- Hanson, R., Borntrager, C., Self-Brown, S., Kilpatrick, D., Saunders, B., Resnick, H., et al.
pathology (pp. 383–401). (2nd ed.). Kluwer Academic Publishers. (2008). Relations among gender, violence exposure, and mental health: The national
Choi, J., Jeong, B., Rohan, M., Polcari, A., & Teicher, M. (2009). Preliminary evidence for survey of adolescents. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 78(3), 313–321.
white matter tract abnormalities in young adults exposed to parental verbal abuse. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young
Biological Psychiatry, 65(3), 227–234. American children. Baltimore, MD, US: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co..
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F., Gunnar, M., & Toth, S. (2010). The differential impacts of early Heim, C., Newport, D., Heit, S., Graham, Y., Wilcox, M., Bonsall, R., Miller, A., et al.
physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol rhythm (2000). Pituitary-adrenal and autonomic responses to stress in women after sexual
in school-aged children. Child Development, 81(1), 252–269. and physical abuse in childhood. JAMA : the journal of the American Medical Associ-
Cohen, N. J., Davine, M., Horodezky, N., Lipsett, L., & Isaacson, L. (1993). Unsuspected lan- ation, 284(5), 592–597.
guage impairment in psychiatrically disturbed children: Prevalence and language and Hudziak, J., Achenbach, T., Althoff, R., & Pine, D. (2007). A dimensional approach to
behavioral characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent developmental psychopathology. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric
Psychiatry, 32(3), 595–603 doi:10.1097/00004583-199305000-00016 Research, 16(Supplement 1), S16–S23.
Dale, P. (1996). Language and emotion: A developmental perspective. In J. H. Beitchman, Jonson-Reid, M., Drake, B., Kim, J., Porterfield, S., & Han, L. (2004). A prospective analysis of
N. J. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas, & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, learning, and behavior the relationship between reported child maltreatment and special education eligibil-
disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives. New York, NY, US: ity among poor children. Child Maltreatment, 9(4), 382–394.
Cambridge University Press (pp. xv, 582). Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2009). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment,
De Bellis, M. D. (2001). Developmental traumatology: The psychobiological development emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology
of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. and Psychiatry, 51(6), 706–716.
Development and Psychopathology, 13(3), 539–564. Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Craig, I. W., et al. (2006).
de Bellis, M. D., Baum, A. S., Birmaher, B., Keshavan, M. S., Eccard, C. H., Boring, A. M., MAOA, maltreatment, and gene–environment interaction predicting children's
Jenkins, F. J., et al. (1999). A.E. Bennett Research Award. Developmental traumatol- mental health: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11(10),
ogy Part I: Biological stress systems. Biological Psychiatry, 45(10), 1259–1270. 903–913.
98 S. Perkins, S. Graham-Bermann / Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 (2012) 89–98

Kusche, C. A., Cook, E. T., & Greenberg, M. T. (1993). Neuropsychological and cognitive Ratner, H., Chiodo, L., Covington, C., Sokol, R., Ager, J., & Delaney-Black, V. (2006).
functioning in children with anxiety, externalizing, and comorbid psychopathology. Violence exposure, IQ, academic performance, and children's perception of safety.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology: Special Issue: The Neuropsychological Basis of Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52, 264–287.
Disorders Affecting Children and Adolescents, 22(2), 172–195. Seckfort, D., Paul, R., Grieve, S., Vandenberg, B., Bryant, R., Williams, L., et al. (2008).
Lahey, B. B., McBurnett, K., & Loeber, R. (2000). Are attention-deficit/hyperactivity Early life stress on brain structure and function across the lifespan: A preliminary
disorder and oppositional defiant disorder developmental precursors to conduct study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 2(1), 49–58.
disorder? Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology (pp. 431–446). (2nd ed.). Seguin, J. R., Boulerice, B., Harden, P. W., Tremblay, R. E., & Pihl, R. O. (1999). Executive
New York, NY, US: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. functions and physical aggression after controlling for attention deficit hyperactiv-
Lewis, T., Kotch, J., Thompson, R., Litrownik, A. J., English, D. J., Proctor, L. J., Runyan, D. K., ity disorder, general memory and IQ. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and
et al. (2010). Witnessed Violence and Youth Behavior Problems: A Multi-Informant Allied Disciplines, 40(8), 1197–1208.
Study. The American journal of orthopsychiatry, 80(4), 443–450 doi:10.1111/j. Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Clasen, L., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., et al. (2006).
1939-0025.2010.01047.x Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature,
Margolin, G., Vickerman, K. A., Oliver, P. H., & Gordis, E. B. (2010). Violence exposure in 440(7084), 676–679.
multiple interpersonal domains: Cumulative and differential effects. Journal of Sheu, Y. -S., Polcari, A., Anderson, C., & Teicher, M. (2010, Jul 14). Harsh corporal
Adolescent Health, 47(2), 198–205. punishment is associated with increased T2 relaxation time in dopamine-rich
Masten, A., Roisman, G., Long, J., Burt, K., Obradovic, J., Riley, J., et al. (2005). Develop- regions. NeuroImage, 1–8.
mental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing Sobsey, R., & Nehring, W. M. (2005). Violence. Health promotion for persons with
symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology, 41(5), 733–746. intellectual and developmental disabilities (pp. 205–234). American Association on
McGee, R., Share, D., Moffitt, T. E., Williams, S., & Silva, P. A. (1988). Reading disability, Mental Retardation.
behaviour problems and juvenile delinquency. Individual Differences in Children Stansbury, K., & Zimmermann, L. K. (1999). Relations among child language skills,
andAdolescents (pp. 158–172). New Brunswick, NJ, US: Transaction Publishers. maternal socializations of emotion regulation, and child behavior problems. Child
McGillivray, J., & Baker, K. (2008). Effects of comorbid ADHD with learning disabilities Psychiatry and Human Development, 30(2), 121–142.
on anxiety, depression, and aggression in adults. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12 Stevenson, J. (1996). Developmental changes in the mechanisms linking language
(6), 525–531. disabilities and behavior disorders. In J. H. Beitchman, N. J. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas,
Mead, H. K., Beauchaine, T. P., & Shannon, K. E. (2010). Neurobiological adaptations to & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, Learning, and Behavior Disorders: Developmental,
violence across development. Development and Psychopathology, 22(01), 1–22. Biological, and Clinical Perspectives. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press
Meewisse, M., Reitsma, J. B., De Vries, G., Gersons, B. P., & Olff, M. (2007). Cortisol and (pp. xv, 582).
post-traumatic stress disorder in adults: Systematic review and meta-analysis. The Tannock, R., & Schachar, R. (1996). Executive dysfunction as an underlying mechanism
British Journal of Psychiatry, 191(5), 387–392. of behavior and language problems in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In J.
Miskovic, V., Schmidt, L., Georgiades, K., Boyle, M., & Macmillan, H. (2010). Adolescent H. Beitchman, N. J. Cohen, M. M. Konstantareas, & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language,
females exposed to child maltreatment exhibit atypical EEG coherence and psychi- learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives.
atric impairment: Linking early adversity, the brain, and psychopathology. Devel- New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press (pp. xv, 582).
opment and Psychopathology, 22(02), 419–432. Taylor, N., Donovan, W., Miles, S., & Leavitt, L. (2009). Maternal control strategies,
Murali, R., & Chen, E. (2005). Exposure to violence and cardiovascular and neuroendo- maternal language usage and children's language usage at two years. Journal of
crine measures in adolescents. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 30(2), 155–163. Child Language, 36(02), 381–404.
Murray, A. D., & Hornbaker, A. V. (1997). Maternal directive and facilitative interaction Thompson, T., & Massat, C. R. (2005). Experiences of violence, post-traumatic stress,
styles: Associations with language and cognitive development of low risk and high academic achievement and behavior problems of urban African-American
risk toddlers. Development and Psychopathology, 9(3), 507–516. children. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 22(5–6), 367–393.
Murray-Close, D., Han, G., Cicchetti, D., Crick, N. R., & Rogosch, F. A. (2008). Neuroendo- Toupin, J., Dery, M., Pauze, R., Mercier, H., & Fortin, L. (2000). Cognitive and familial
crine regulation and physical and relational aggression: The moderating roles of contributions to conduct disorder in children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry
child maltreatment and gender. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1160–1176. & Allied Disciplines, 41(3). (pp. 2333–2344): US Cambridge University Press.
Nigg, J. T., Quamma, J. P., Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche, C. A. (1999). A two-year longitudinal Trickett, P. K., Noll, J. G., Susman, E. J., Shenk, C. E., & Putnam, F. W. (2010). Attenuation
study of neuropsychological and cognitive performance in relation to behavioral of cortisol across development for victims of sexual abuse. Development and
problems and competencies in elementary school children. Journal of Abnormal Psychopathology, 22(01), 165.
Child Psychology, 27(1), 51–63. Turner, H. A., Finkelhor, D., & Ormrod, R. (2010). Poly-victimization in a national sample of
Noble, K., Tottenham, N., & Casey, B. (2005). Neuroscience perspectives on disparities children and youth. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 38(3), 323–330.
in school readiness and cognitive achievement. The Future of children/Center for van der Kolk, B. A. (1997). The psychobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. The
the Future of Children, the David and Lucile Packard Foundation, 15(1), 71–89. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 58(Supplement 9), 16–24.
Oades, R., Lasky-Su, J., Christiansen, H., Faraone, S., Sonuga-Barke, E., Banaschewski, T., van der Kolk, B. A. (2003). The neurobiology of childhood trauma and abuse. Child and
et al. (2008). The influence of serotonin- and other genes on impulsive behavioral Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North America, 12(2), 293–317 ix.
aggression and cognitive impulsivity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity Van Voorhees, E., & Scarpa, A. (2004). The effects of child maltreatment on the
disorder (ADHD): Findings from a family-based association test (FBAT) analysis. hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 5(4), 333–352.
Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4(48), 48. Watts-English, T., Fortson, B. L., Gibler, N., Hooper, S. R., & DeBellis, M. D. (2006). The
Obradović, J., Bush, N., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N., & Boyce, W. (2010). Biological sensitivity psychobiology of maltreatment in childhood. Journal of Social Issues, 62(4),
to context: the interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on 717–736.
socioemotional behavior and school readiness. Child Development, 81(1), 270–289. Weiss, M. J. S., & Wagner, S. H. (1998). What explains the negative consequences of
Ozer, E., & McDonald, K. (2006). Exposure to violence and mental health among adverse childhood experiences on adult health? Insights from cognitive and neuro-
Chinese American urban adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39(1), 73–79. science research. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 356–360.
Perkins, S. C., Cortina, K., Smith-Darden, J., & Graham-Bermann, S. (2012). The mediat- Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Psychiatric comorbidity in children and
ing role of disability and self-regulation between violence exposure and mental adolescents with reading disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and
health functioning in incarcerated adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, Allied Disciplines, 41(8), 1039–1048.
27(7). Ybarra, G., Wilkens, S., & Lieberman, A. (2007). The influence of domestic violence on
Perkins, S. C., Smith-Darden, J., & Graham-Bermann, S. (2011). The relation of violence preschooler behavior and functioning. Journal of Family Violence, 22(1), 33–42.
exposure and ethnicity to intelligence and verbal-performance discrepancies in
incarcerated male adolescents. Violence and Victims, 26(4), 496–512.
Ruchkin, V., Henrich, C., Jones, S., Vermeiren, R., & Schwab-Stone, M. (2007). Violence
exposure and psychopathology in urban youth: The mediating role of posttraumatic
stress. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(4), 578–593.

You might also like