You are on page 1of 30

Possessions and the Extended Self

RUSSELL W. BELK*

Our possessions are a major contributor to and reflection of our Identiti?s. A variety
of evidence is presented supporting this simple and compelling premise. Related
streams of research are identified and drawn upon in developing this concept and
implications are derived for consumer behavior. Because the construct of extended
self involves consumer behavior rather than buyer behavior, It appears to be a
much richer construct than previous formulations positing a relationship between
self-concept and consumer brand choice.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


pq as pessoas
colecionam
Hollow hands clasp ludicrous possessions because they of importance to understanding consumer behavior,
are links in the chain of life. If it breaks, they are truly but also on the premise that understanding the ex-
lost.-Dichter 1964 tended self will help us learn how consumer behavior
contributes to our broader existence as human beings

W e cannot hope to understand consumer behav- (Belk 1987a). The first section considers various evi-
ior without first gaining some understanding dences that possessions are an important component
of the meanings that consumers attach to possessions. of sense of self. The most direct form of evidence is
A key to understanding what possessi.ons m~an is .rec- found in the nature of self-perceptions. Additional,
ognizing that, knowingly or unknowmgly, mt~ntIOn­ especially striking evidence is found in the dimin-
ally or unintentionally, we regard our posseSSIOns as ished sense of self when possessions are unintention-
parts of ourselves. As Tuan argues, "Our fragile sense ally lost or stolen. More evidence ofthe role ofpos~es­
of self needs support, and this we get by having and sions in sense of self comes from anthropologIcal
possessing things because, to a large degree, we are studies of the way possessions are treated ritually and
what we have and possess" (1980, p. 472). That we after death. Because extended self is such a broad
are what we have (e.g., Van Esterick 1986; Feirstein topic, several unreviewed areas of evidence on the ex-
1986; Rosenbaum 1972) is perhaps. the most basic tent and nature of the relationship between posses-
and powerful fact of consumer behavIOr. sions and sense of self also are identified. In so doing,
The premise that we regard our possessions as parts the scope of the present treatment is also defined.
of ourselves is not new. William James (1890, pp. The question of what functions the extended self
291-292), who laid the foundations for modern con- serves is addressed in the second section, which be-
ceptions of self, held that: gins with a brief review of the basic states of our exis-
tence: having, doing, and being. These states are rele-
a man's Self is the sum total of all that he CAN call vant to the question of how we define who we are.
his not only his body and his psychic powers, but his Next the functions of possessions in human develop-
clothes and his house, his wife and children, his ances- ment are considered. Four stages are identified: (1)
tors and friends his reputation and works, his lands, the infant distinguishes self from environment, (2)
and yacht and b~nk-account. All these things give him
the same emotions. If they wax and prosper, he feels the infant distinguishes self from others, (3) posses-
triumphant; if they dwindle and die away, he feels cast sions help adolescents and adults manage their identi-
down,-not necessarily in the same degree for each ties, and (4) possessions help the old achie~e a sense
thing, but in much the same way for all. I of continuity and preparation for death. Fmally, the
role of possessions in creating or maintaining a sense
If we define possessions as things we call ours, James of past is considered.
was saying that we are the sum of our possessions. The third section examines several processes in-
The purpose of this article is to examine the rel~­ volved in self-extension. One process is the initial in-
tionship between possessions and sense of self. It IS corporation of objects into our extended selves. A
based not only on the premise that this relationship is number of incorporation processes are discussed, not
all of which involve possession in the sense ofindivid-
*Russell W. Belk is the N. Eldon Tanner Professor of Business ual ownership. A particular process of self-extension
Administration, Graduate School of Business, University ofUta~,
Salt Lake City, UT 84060. The author wishes to thank ~elame
Wallendorf, floyd Rudmin, and Grant McCracken for their com- 'James called his text an encyclopedia of psychology and quotes
ments on an earlier version of this article. Herr Horwicz's Psychologische Analysen (no date or publisher
given) as a source of many of his ideas on self.
139
© JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH. Vol. 15. September 1988
140 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

that is considered in some detail is contamination. In ours, or are they us? Certainly men have been ready to
contamination, both good and bad aspects of objects disown their very bodies and to regard them as mere
are seen to attach to us through physical contact or vestures, or even as prisons of clay from which they
proximity. A final process theorized is the mainte- should some day be glad to escape.
nance of multiple levels of the self, such as viewing Although prior theories and research on consumer
our family, city, and nation to be a part of who we are. self-concept (see Sirgy 1982 for a review) are moder-
The fourth section of this article focuses on a num- ately supportive of the contention that possessions
ber of special categories of possessions that are com- are incorporated into self-concept, this research prob-
monly incorporated into the sense of self. These cate- ably considerably underestimates the extent to which
gories are collections, money, pets, other people, and this is true. One reason is that prior research methods
body parts. In each case, research is reviewed support- generally attempt to find a correspondence between
ing the contention that this category of objects is a perceived characteristics of these objects and per-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


part of the extended self and is therefore treated ceived characteristics of the self. But, one can hold an
differently from objects not considered to be a part of object like the Statue of Liberty to be a part of one's
self. identity without having to hold a self-concept com-
The final section discusses implications of the ex- posed of characteristics attributed to this statue. Sec-
tended self formulation for consumer research. The ond, as argued by Belk (l984b), the focus of these
areas of implications outlined include gift-giving, vi- studies on brand images prior to acquisition is too
carious consumption (generally through other family limited. Both nonbrand images (e.g., cigarette
members), care of possessions, organ donation, prod- smoker, wine connoisseur) and post-acquisition ob-
uct disposition, and the contribution of extended self ject bonding (e.g., with one's pet) may contribute
to defining meaning in life. The latter topic elevates strongly to the sense of self. Third, as argued by Belk
the focus of consumer behavior research to a level of (1984b) and Solomon and Assael (1988), rather than
greater significance than satisfaction with product a single product or brand representing all of one's self-
performance. Following the final section, the formu- concept, only a complete ensemble of consumption
lation of the extended self is reviewed briefly and con- objects may be able to represent the diverse and possi-
clusions are offered. bly incongruous aspects of the total self. For all of
these reasons, the present focus on extended self is
EVIDENCES substantially different than prior consumer self-con-
cept research. For research applications within the
Possessions in Self-Perception Research perspective advocated here, see Belk (1987b, 1988)
and Belk and Austin (1986).
The term extended self has not been applied pre- This more expansive view of the extended self can
viously to the conception of self-plus-possessions, but be examined in light of several prior conceptualiza-
Rochberg-Halton (1984, p. 335) comes close: tions and studies focusing on distal elements of the
Valued material possessions. . . act as signs of the self self. McClelland (1951) suggested that external ob-
that are essential in their own right for its continued jects become viewed as part of self when we are able to
cultivation, and hence the world of meaning that we exercise power or control over them, just as we might
create for oursleves, and that creates our selves, ex- control an arm or a leg. In the case of tools, instru-
tends literally into the objective surroundings. ments, and weapons, envisioning the basis for the ex-
One difference in the present view is that the extended tended self metaphor is easy. The greater the control
self is seen not to be limited to external objects and we exercise, the more closely allied with selfthe object
personal possessions, but also includes persons, should become. This principle led McClelland to hy-
places, and group possessions as well as such posses- pothesize the following hierarchy of most to least
sions as body parts and vital organs. The notion of closely self-allied object categories: (1) me, my "free
extended self is a superficially masculine and Western will," (2) my body, my conscience, (3) my belongings,
metaphor comprising not only that which is seen as (4) my friends, and (5) strangers, physical universe.
"me" (the self), but also that which is seen as "mine." The predicted closer alignment of self to belongings
As James (1890, p. 291) notes, the two concepts are than to friends recognizes the "free will" of people
interwoven in the way we think of our selves: (friends) that is lacking in most belongings.
Pre linger (1959) tested James's premise that posses-
The Empirical Self of each of us is all that he is tempted sions are viewed as parts of self and McClelland's hy-
to call by the name of me. But it is clear that between pothesis that control dictates the strength of this link-
what a man calls me and what he simply calls mine the age. He had subjects sort 160 items onto a four-posi-
line is difficult to draw. We feel and act about certain
things that are ours very much as we feel and act about tion (zero to three) continuum of not-self to self. The
ourselves. Our fame, our children, the work of our items were selected so that each of eight conceptual
hands, may be as dear to us as our bodies are, and categories was represented by 20 items. These catego-
arouse the same feelings and the same acts of reprisal ries and the mean "self' scores for the items within
if attacked. And our bodies themselves, are they simply them were in descending order:
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 141

1. Body parts (e.g., the skin, the genital organs), that the object is "me." McCarthy (1984) concludes
2.98; that such objects act as reminders and confirmers of
our identities, and that our identities may reside in
2. Psychological or intraorganismic processes objects more than they do in individuals.
(e.g., the conscience, an itching on the sole of Allport (1937) hypothesized that the process of
the foot), 2.46; gaining an identity, and in so doing gaining self-es-
3. Personal identifying characteristics and attri- teem, progresses from infancy by extending self via a
butes (e.g., age, occupation), 2.22; continuously expanding set of things regarded as
one's own. This hypothesis was tested by Dixon and
4. Possessions and productions (e.g., watch, per- Street (1975) who conducted an approximate replica-
spiration, toilet articles), 1.57; tion of Prelingers' study among 6- to 16-year-olds.
They found essentially the same rank ordering of item

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


5. Abstract ideas (e.g., the morals of society, the categories regarded as "self," but found only two cat-
law), 1.36; egories for which this tendency changed significantly
6. Other people (e.g., the people in your home- with age: other people and possessions. In both cases,
town, father), 1.10; older children were more likely than younger chil-
dren to categorize such objects as being part of self
7. Objects within the close physical environment ("you"). In a three-generational study offavo rite pos-
(e.g., dirt on the hands, furniture in this sessions, Rochberg-Halton (1984, 1986; Csikszent-
room), 0.64; mihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981) found that as we
8. Distant physical environment (e.g., the ad- age the possessions that people cite as "special" tend
joining room, the moon), 0.19. increasingly to be those that symbolize other people
(e.g., gifts from people, photographs of people). Their
Although it is unfortunate that Pre linger grouped interpretation of these findings suggests an age-re-
some autonomic bodily productions with posses- lated widening of the boundaries of self (Rochberg-
sions, these findings still support James's contention Halton 1984, p. 352). These findings also may suggest
that possessions are seen as part of self. They also sug- that possessions are regarded not only as a part of self,
gest an ordering of the "selfness" of these object cate- but also as instrumental to the development of self.
gories that is parallel to the hierarchy suggested by Other work on the role that special possessions play
McClelland. To test McClelland's control hypothesis in easing life transitions also suggests that possessions
more directly, Prelinger had five judges separate the can be instrumental to maintenance of self-concept
160 items into three groups: those that are predomi- (e.g., McCracken 1987a).
nantly under the control of people, those that primar- A study by Belk (1987b, 1988; Belk and Austin
ily control or affect people, and those that are pre- 1986) examines the self-defining role of places, public
dominantly neutral in both regards. The first two cat- monuments, experiences, time periods, television
egories both received high mean "self' scores (over programs, motion pictures, and public figures, in ad-
1.8) from subjects, while the neutral items clearly re- dition to the sort of objects, persons, and traits stud-
ceived "non-self' scores (mean less than 0.2). These ied in prior research. Of the additional extended self
findings suggest that besides control over objects, categories considered, places and experiences tend to
control by objects may also contribute to an item be- be seen as most clearly a part of extended self. Added
ing viewed as part of self. That is, we may impose our to the previously noted findings then, we may sum-
identities on possessions and possessions may impose marize the major categories of extended self as body,
their identities on us. internal processes, ideas, and experiences, and those
Interestingly, control also has been suggested to be persons, places, and things to which one feels at-
the critical determinant of feelings of possession tached.
(Furby 1978; Tuan 1984). Ifboth hypotheses are cor- Of these categories, the last three appear to be the
rect, the more we believe we possess or are possessed most clearly extended. However, given the difficulties
by an object, the more a part of self it becomes. It is in separating mind and body in philosophies and psy-
telling that the categories of extended self just noted chologies ofthe self(e.g., Campbell 1984; Englehardt
correspond quite closely to the areas in which Ellis 1973; Tuner 1984), objects in all of these categories
(1985, pp. 115-117) found evidence of human pos- will be treated as potential parts of the extended self.
sessiveness (no hierarchical ordering was reported): In conversations in English (although less true in
(1) one's body, (2) personal space, (3) ingestibles, (4) some other languages such as Japanese), it is clear that
territory, (5) domicile, (6) copulatory partners, (7) some objects in the former categories are treated as
offspring, (8) friends, (9) tools, and (10) objects of aes- both a part of extended self and a part of essential un-
thetic appeal, play and amusement, pets and memen- extended self. For instance, saying I have a dark tan
tos. Nuttin (1987) finds that even the letters in our or my body is tan (possessive and extended uses) is
names are viewed possessively. Apparently, in claim- more usual than saying I am a tan body (a nonposses-
ing that something is "mine," we also come to believe sive and an unextended usage). However, saying I am
142 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

tired (unextended) is more common than saying my seen to be bestowed by the organization. The present
body is tired (extended). Even greater complications focus would suggest that those who have less of their
in making distinctions between extended and unex- extended selves stripped from them may adjust more
tended selves are found with asomatognostics who readily to such situations.
cannot apprehend the existence of parts of their bod- Another instance in which non voluntary loss of
ies (Litwinski 1956; Sacks 1985), amputees who de- possessions may bring about a diminished sense of
velop phantom limbs (Plugge 1970), and recent treat- self is when possessions are lost to theft or casualty. In
ments of beliefs as possessions (Abelson 1986; Abel- the case of burglary victims, Rosenblatt, Walsh, and
son and Prentice forthcoming). From the present Jackson (1976) suggest that a process of grief and
perspective, the issue is an empirically resolvable one mourning may follow the discovery of theft, just as
that depends upon perceptions. For instance, Belk one might grieve and mourn the death of a loved one
and Austin (1986) found the following mean scores who had been a part of one's life. What is lost in both

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


for various body parts on a four-point scale of "self- cases may be a part of self. As the college student vic-
ness," where four is the highest possible score: eyes tim of a bicycle theft accuses the unknown thief, she
3.5, hair 3.2, heart 3.1, legs 3.1, hands 3.1, fingers 3.0, reveals the identity invested in the bike (Donner
genitals 3.0, skin 3.0, nose 2.7, knees 2.7, chin 2.6, 1985, p. 31):
kidneys 2.6, liver 2.6, and throat 2.5. For this sample, It hurts to think that someone else is selling something
it seems best to conclude that none of these body parts that for me is more precious than money. . . Every-
is necessarily an inherent part of un extended self, but one who owns a bike has their own story that makes
that eyes, hair, and heart are mote likely to be treated their bike more than just machinery to them. And you
in this way than are kidneys, liver, and throat. The ripped it off. You stole a piece of my life. You didn't
study also found some evidence of sex and age differ- just steal a chunk of metal to sell. . . You walked off
ences in the incorporation of body parts into sense with my memories.
of self. Furthermore, it is likely that those who have The present author conducted a small-scale test of
undergone such elective body alterations as plastic this hypothesis using data from interviews with a non-
surgery and tattooing are likely to view the affected representative sample of 20 burglary victims who
body parts as being more a part of self (e.g., Sanders were asked in open-ended questions to recall their ini-
1988). tial thoughts and feelings upon discovering the loss.
Following anger and rage, the most commonly re-
Loss of Possessions ported reactions were feelings of invasion and viola-
tion. In fact, eight of the 11 females in the sample
If possessions are viewed as part of self, it follows spontaneously suggested that it was as though they
that an unintentional loss of possessions should be re- had been violated, polluted, or raped. There are sim-
garded as a loss or lessening of self. Goffman (1961) ilar reports in Maguire's (1980) study of British bur-
provides a thorough review of the evidence of deliber- glary victims, although only 12 percent of the females
ate lessening of self brought about in such institutions in his study suggested such a feeling. Additional con-
as mental hospitals, homes for the aged, prisons, con- firmation of this feeling of personal violation is found
centration camps, military training camps, boarding in studies by Korosec-Serfaty (1985) and Paap
schools, and monasteries. One of the first steps in re- (1981).
ceiving new members into these institutions is to sys- There are also reports of feelings of loss of a part of
tematically deprive them of all personal possessions self among victims of natural disasters. McLeod
including clothing, money, and even names. Their (1984) found that those who lost possessions to a
bodies may be standardized to some degree, as with mudslide went through a process of grief similar to
military haircuts, and their behaviors and conversa- that in losing a loved one-moving from denial to an-
tions may be severely restricted. They are reissued ger, to depression, and finally to acceptance (often af-
standard wardrobes and minimal possessions to aid ter many months). The author joined several other
in rebuilding a new standardized identity. The result researchers in conducting depth interviews with flood
of this systematic substitution of standardized "iden- victims during the summer of 1986, and found that
tity kits" for former possessions is an elimination of after six weeks most victims were still in the early
uniqueness (Snyder and Fromkin 1981) and a corre- stages of grief and often could not talk about the di-
sponding and often traumatic lessening of the indi- saster or cried while attempting to do so. Fieldnotes
vidual's sense of self. Although the new, more stan- from one such interview include this account:
dardized possessions that are substituted may eventu-
The losses that concerned (the flood victim) most were
ally restore some sense of self, the new self should those of his record collection,. . . a first edition book
necessarily be less unique and involve more of a collection, ... the tools that his father-the cabinet
shared group identity. Furthermore, the individual maker-had used, ... the ceiling and paneling of the
typically becomes a user of these new objects rather basements that he had installed with the help and ad-
than an owner of them. Because control is restricted vice of his father, and (upstairs), the hutch, lowboy,
and the organization remains the owner, identity is and stereo cabinet that his father had made.
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 143

Clearly what is mourned here is a loss of self. Similar ing possessions to make it through the winter reflects
findings were obtained in the Buffalo Creek flood (Cottle 1981, p. 18):
(Erikson 1976). As Georg Simmel observes, "material
property is, so to speak, an extension of the ego, and I stand in those lines with my suitcase full of things to
any interference with our property is, for this reason, practically give away; I stand in that hock shop, and I
tell myself that my entire life is being sold ... Don't
felt to be a violation of the person" (1950, p. 322). make me hock my life away, I beg you.
The flood victim also illustrates how the labor of the
individual (in this case the victim's recently deceased Of course, there is a more utilitarian explanation of
father) adheres in the objects produced. In this sense, the feelings of resentment at the loss of possessions.
the loss of possessions was also a further loss of his In this more utilitarian view, we merely regret the loss
father's extended self that remained in his father's of valued possessions because of the benefits they pro-
creations.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


vide rather than from any feelings of self deri ved from
Besides the more direct loss of self when personal or mingled with these objects. James (1890, p. 293)
possessions are lost to theft or casualty, the vulnera- challenges the sufficiency ofthis view:
bility revealed in such losses may damage the sense of
self derived from the attachments to home and neigh- although it is true that a part of our depression at the
borhood. Home (e.g., Cooper 1974; Duncan 1976; loss of possessions is due to our feeling that we must
Duncan and Duncan 1976) and neighborhood (e.g., now go without certain goods that we expected the pos-
Bakker and Bakker-Rabdau 1973; Gerson, Stueve, sessions to bring in their train, yet in every case there
remains, over and above this, a sense of the shrinkage
and Fischer 1977) have been suggested to be strong of our personality, a partial conversion of ourselves to
sources of personal identity. As with more personal nothingness, which is a psychological phenomenon by
possessions, home and neighborhood have been hy- itself.
pothesized to contribute to sense of self to the degree
that a person feels control over them (Bakker and Extreme examples of this partial annihilation of self
Bakker-Rabdau 1973; Edney 1975). This may ex- are cited by Beaglehole (1932) and Rigby and Rigby
plain why Brown (1982) found that burglary victims (1949) in accounts of art collectors who have gone to
report less sense of community, less feeling of pri- such great lengths as suicide to avoid facing the forced
vacy, and less pride in their house's appearance than breakup of their collections. Less extreme examples
do their nonburglarized neighbors. The same phe- are found in the simple nostalgic regret at the disposal
nomenon has been observed in those displaced by of worn out clothing and similar items that have been
slum clearance, even when they were relocated to associated with pleasant memories of one's past (e.g.,
"better" housing (Fried 1963). In the words of Peter Lurie 1981, p. 33; Rooney 1984, pp. 3-4).
Marris, "They identify with the neighborhood: it is If involuntary loss of possessions causes a loss of
part of them, and to hear it condemned as a slum is a self, one of the primary reactions following such loss
condemnation ofthemselves too" (1986, p. 55). should be an attempt at self-restoration. This phe-
Besides loss of possessions to theft or casualty, oth- nomenon has been observed in psychoanalysis and
ers have maintained that whenever the functions and has led to the hypothesis that, along with body loss,
property of individuals are taken over by institutions, object loss is the fountainhead of creativity (Nieder-
such as government and schools, there is a regrettable land 1967; Niederland and Sholevar 1981). Body loss
loss of self (Dietze 1963; Wiggins 1974). Although the refers to some real or imagined physical deformity or
intent of these institutions is presumably not to lessen bodily imperfection that detracts from sense of self.
others' sense of selves, Wiggins (1974) suggests that Object loss normally refers to the death of a close fam-
there are instances in which a person's possessions are ily member, but is also used by Niederland to refer to
damaged with the intent of diminishing the owner. the traumatic loss of possessions. In body and object
He gives as one example a child who destroys the loss, the creation of art, craft, concept, or writing is
property of a larger child or of an inviolable sibling in seen as an attempt to extend the self in new ways that
an effort to more effectively direct aggression at this make up for the loss and restore the self to wholeness.
person. Vandalism may be motivated similarly with That is, periods of creativity may follow the loss of
the targets being society, those who seem to be more one's possessions.
fortunate, or public institutions (e.g., Chester 1976; Niederland and Sholevar (1981) also suggest that
Fisher and Baron 1982). for many young American males, the automobile is a
The trauma that may attend involuntary loss of part of their extended selves and their ego ideals. This
possessions normally is not present in voluntary dis- view is supported by consumer self-concept research
position of possessions. Indeed, La Branche (1973) (e.g., Bloch 1982; Grubb and Hupp 1968; Jacobson
observes that when possessions are recognized as in- and Kossoff 1963). The processes of creating and nur-
consistent with our images of self, we gladly neglect turing extended self through an automobile may be
or dispose of them. But, when the disposition is seen in customizing (personalizing) the car and in lav-
forced, as by economic circumstances, the parting ishing great care on its maintenance. When such a car
likely brings sorrow. As one elderly respondent pawn- is damaged, the owners react as if their own bodies
144 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

have been injured. Consider the sense of personal in- properties of the Amanita muscaria mushroom that
jury described by Bellow (1975, p. 36) after a treas- these priests ritually consume (Wasson 1972). Each
ured car was assaulted: of these practices suggests the desire to tap into the
life force of nature or other people by symbolically
Someone had done to my car as rats, I had heard, did merging with these forces.
when they raced through warehouses by the thousands
and tore open sacks of flour for the hell of it. I felt a In addition, the association of people and posses-
similar rip at my heart . . . I had allowed the car to sions is shown in the practice of burying the dead with
become an extension of my own self. . . ,so that an their possessions. This practice began at least 60,000
attack on it was an attack on myself. It was a moment years ago (Maringer 1960) and perhaps more than
terribly fertile in reactions. 100,000 years ago (Leaky 1981). Alekshin ~1983)
compared the grave goods of men and women In Eu-
Furthermore, the possessors of such damage? trea- rope and found evidence that suggests women only

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


sures are anxious to either restore the auto to Its for- began to experience inferior status in the third millen-
mer perfection or replace it with a more perfect sub- nium B.c. (Le., the number and quality of their grave
stitute. These reactions reflect the desire to restore the goods did not differ until then). Rathje and McGuire
damaged sense of (extended) self caused by the injury (1982) have performed similar analyses of grave
to the automobile. goods of the Maya. That anthropologists assume that
possessions tell us about their possessors is itself evi-
Investing Self in Objects dence of the tendency to see possessions as symbols of
self. The inference process is not unlike that of police
The idea that we make things a part of selfby creat- detectives who attempt to construct an identity for
ing or altering them appears to be a universal human unknown corpses by using the corpses' possessions
belief. Anthropologists generally agree that the maker (Pogrebin, Poole, and Regoli 1986).
of an object, the user of land, and the cultivator of a In more recent traditional societies, using the cloth-
plant are regarded as being entitled to the product of ing or possessions of the dead is often a taboo. Until
their labor (e.g., Herskovits 1952; Lewinski 1913). outlawed 100 years ago in India, the wife, as "prop-
Locke (1690) made this the foundation for his views erty" of a deceased husband, was expected to join him
on property and government, explaining the "natural in death (Bordewich 1986). Such notions of posses-
basis" for private property in three steps: (1) we own sion surviving even death suggest a strong association
ourselves (see Wikse 1977), (2) therefore we own our between self and possessions. To the extent that other
labor (what we direct our bodies to do), and (3) there- people can be viewed as possessions (this point will be
fore we own 'what we produce from our labor out of pursued in a subsequent section), mourning for dead
the unowned materials of nature. Csikszentmihalyi loved ones also may be interpreted as grieving for a
and Rochberg-Halton (1981) provide a more psycho- loss of self. The prior possessions of the deceased can
logical explanation in suggestln~ that we inves~ "psy- be powerful remains of the dead person's extended
chic energy" in an object to WhICh we have dIrected self. These remains are often the focus of normal and
our efforts, time, apd attention. This energy and its pathological mourning (Volkan 1974). The same as-
products are regarded as a part of self because t~ey sociation is shown in sympathetic magic in which ma-
have grown or emerged from the self. The same pnn- levolence is directed at a person through their cloth-
ciple has been suggested to apply to objects that are ing, hair or nail clippings, or other belon~ings (Clodd
forcefully appropriated from others (Veblen 1898). 1920). Evidence of the power of posseSSIOns to cap-
After the development of money payment for labor, ture the extended self is also shown in the angry de-
purchasing objects offers anot.her m~ans for in~esting struction of objects left behind by the Shah of Iran
self(in this case more symbolIcally) In possessIOn~. and Ferdinand Marcos in the Philippines after they
Beaglehole (1932) reviews other anthropologIcal were deposed (Goldstein 1987).
evidence of the link between possessions and self. The Contemporary consumption also shows that the
almost literal incorporation of objects into self and feeling of identity invested in material objects can be
self into objects is shown in various practices oftradi- extraordinarily high. For instance, Ames (1984, pp.
tional peoples. These practices include licking new
30-31) records feelings attached to a 19th century
possessions, burying the umbilical cord on tribal
purchase of a parlor organ:
land inserting removed foreskin beneath the bark of
a pe~sonal tree, eatipg or taking the l1ame of c.on- Buying a prominent object like a parlor organ might
quered enemies, burying ancestors on sacred ~flbal initiate a new chapter in a set of lives, not only by pro-
land, and claiming ownership of new land or 3rrtlfacts viding a new way to use time but also a new tool to
by touching them, naming them for a part of the per- measure time. In later years the object would serve to
son's body, leaving a lock of hair on them, or shed- remind its owners of the day it first entered their home
ding blood on them. Another example, perhaps re- and of the time that had passed since then. It would
pugnant to Western observers, is the drinking of t~e not' only structure their present but also their percep-
urine of Vedic priests to partake of the psychogemc tions of their own past.
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 145

They knew from experience that purchasing a major cal emphasis or with the focus on consumer behavior.
object could be a significant and momentous occasion Future research seeking a broader perspective would
in itself, a time of heightened positive emotions and benefit from consulting the additional literatures in
feelings of well-being and importance ... a major Marxism and neoMarxism, critical theory, folklore,
purchase would transform them in their own eyes and political philosophy, environmental psychology, ma-
in the eyes of others. They would become worth more
. . . and acquire greater status. By so doing they would cromarketing, semiotics, impression management,
receive more respect and deference from others which and collective memory. The literature on property,
would, in turn, make them feel better about them- ownership, and possession also provides a wealth of
selves. Buying a parlor organ would make them some- relevant material (see Rudmin, Belk, and Furby
thing they were not before. 1987).
The scope ofthis artical also is delimited by its pre-
One of the modern equivalents of the parlor organ dominant focus on societies that hold an individualis-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


in terms of impact on extended selfis the automobile, tic concept of self. As Belk (1984c) suggests, there are
especially for males (e.g., Myers 1985; Weiland times and places in world history during which the
1955). The owner of an expensive Porsche describes operative notion of self is more collective than indi-
his attachment in this way (Stein 1985, p. 30): vidual. For a series of excellent discussions of the
Sometimes I test myself. We have an ancient, battered emergence of the individual self, see Campbell
Peugeot, and I drive it for a week. It rarely breaks, and (1987), Carrithers, Collins, and Lukes (1985), and
it gets great mileage. But when I pull up next to a beau- Macfarlane (1978). The present discussion addresses
tiful woman, I am still the geek with the glasses. collective selves in a section dealing with levels of the
Then I get back into the Porsche. It roars and tugs to self, but the primary focus is on the individual. Most
get moving. It accelerates even going uphill at 80. It of the present formulation also applies in instances of
leadeth trashy women . . . to make pouting looks at collective conceptualizations of the self, but collec-
me at stoplights. It makes me feel like a tomcat on the tive self involves additional concepts not addressed
prowl. ... here-for instance, group rituals for fusing a new ob-
Nothing else in my life compares-except driving ject into collective identity. Thus, an adequate theo-
along Sunset at night in the 928, with the sodium-va- retical formulation of collective extended self must
por lamps reflecting off the wine-red finish, with the air await further work. In the following section on the
inside reeking of tan glove-leather upholstery and the functions of extended self, social functions of this
. . . Blaupunkt playing the Shirelles so loud it makes construct largely are ignored.
my hair vibrate. And with the girls I will never see
again pulling up next to me, giving the car a once-over,
and looking at me as ifI were a cool guy, not a worried, FUNCTIONS OF EXTENDED SELF
overextended 40-year-old schnook writer.
Having, Doing, and Being
As these examples suggest, the degree to which self
may become extended into possessions can be great. Objects in our possession literally can extend self,
In extreme cases, we again may note McCarthy's as when a tool or weapon allows us to do things of
(1984) contention that identity sometimes may lie which we would otherwise be incapable. Possessions
more in extended self than in un extended self. can also symbolically extend self, as when a uniform
or trophy allows us to convince ourselves (and per-
Relevant Perspectives and Domain haps others) that we can be a different person than we
would be without them. Tanay (1976) suggests that
The preceding discussion has presented eclectic ev- handguns represent a symbolic penis for their owners.
idence supporting the proposition that we regard our However, Kates and Varzos (1987) challenge this in-
possessions as parts of our selves. As this article devel- terpretation and instead emphasize the real rather
ops a deeper theoretical understanding of this phe- than symbolic power given by guns. This sense of en-
nomenon, it will continue to draw upon a broad base hancement of personal power is what made the six-
of literature from psychology, consumer research, gun the "equalizer" in American Western lore. Ta-
psychoanalytic theory, material and popular culture nay's symbolic interpretation focuses on the sense of
studies, feminist studies, history, medicine, anthro- being presumably provided by such a weapon,
pology, and sociology. These areas and particular whereas this alternative interpretation maintains that
studies within them deal with constructs that are use- it is what one can do with a gun that contributes to
ful in advancing the arguments and explanations of sense of self. Thus, having possessions can contribute
the following sections. A number of other areas of in- to our capabilities for doing and being. The relation-
quiry as well as omitted subfields from these areasjust ships among having, doing, and being are strong and
noted are potentially relevant to the study of ex- have been most fully explored by existential psychol-
tended self, but have been excluded either because of ogist and philosopher Jean-Paul Sartre.
space considerations or because ofthe areas' perspec- In his major work, Being and Nothingness, Sartre
tives being less compatible with the present theoreti- (1943) suggests that doing is merely a transitional
146 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

state or a manifestation of the more fundamental de- mode of existence that Fromm proposes, this orienta-
sires to have or to be. Further, Sartre maintains that tion to have is rejected in favor of an opposing orien-
the only reason we want to have something is to en- tation to share, to give, and to sacrifice. The outcome
large our sense of self and that the only way we can of practicing this being mode of existence, according
know who we are is by observing what we have. In to Fromm, is to realize one's identity without the
other words, having and being are distinct but insepa- threat oflosing it, a threat that is inherent in the hav-
rable. When an object becomes a possession, what ing mode-for which he asks "If! am what I have and
were once self and not-self are synthesized and having if what I have is lost, who then am I?" (1976, p. 76).
and being merge. Thus, according to Sartre, posses- The views of Sartre, Marx, and Fromm on having,
sions are all-important to knowing who we are. Peo- doing, and being present significant questions that are
ple seek, express, confirm, and ascertain a sense of be- not necessary or possible to resolve here. All acknowl-
ing through what they have. edge, however, that having possessions functions to

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


Other people also affect relationships among hav- create and to maintain a sense of self-definition and
ing, doing, and being, according to Sartre. Besides that having, doing, and being are integrally related.
others sometimes serving in an object capacity as pos-
sessions, others are an important mirror through
which we see ourselves. These others first come to as-
Mastery of Possessions and Human
sociate possessions and possessor and then, depend- Development
ing upon which is known best, either come to infer Self Versus Environment. The functions that pos-
the traits of the person from the nature of the posses- sessions fulfill in our lives are not constant over our
sions or the nature of the possessions from the traits of life spans. According to Freudian and other psycho-
the person (Belk 1978). Belk, Bahn, and Mayer (1982) analytic theories (e.g., Erikson 1959), the infant be-
and Holman (1981) review abundant buyer behavior gins life being unable to distinguish selffrom the envi-
literature supporting this veiw. However, as Douglas ronment, including mother. As Ausubel, Sullivan,
and Isherwood (1979, p. 72) remind us, to think that and Ives (1980) point out, this may be seen as a per-
a single item can successfully inform others about us ceptual problem in distinguishing figure from
is equivalent to thinking that a single word from a ground. Others suggest that the distinction soon
poem can convey the meaning it creates in the con- emerges as a result of the contingency and kinesthetic
text ofthe poem. feedback produced by the infant's actions (Lewis and
Sartre's view that having and being are the central Brooks 1978; Seligman 1975). That is, as the infant's
modes of existence contrasts with Karl Marx's view motor skills develop, those objects that can be con-
that doing, and particularly working, is central to ex- trolled come to be seen as self and those objects that
istence and self-worth. The problem with having, in cannot be controlled come to be seen as environ-
Marx's view, is that it produces a false path to happi- ment. According to Isaacs (1933, p. 226), the moth-
ness through "commodity fetishism" (Marx 1978). In er's caregiving also produces the first sentiments of
commodity fetishism, consumers worship goods and ownership:
believe that goods have magical powers to bring hap-
piness, provoking a pervasive and ongoing expecta- In the case of the infant at the breast, to have is literally
tion that happiness lies in the next purchase or "I and simply to take into oneself, into one's mouth. The
would be happy if I could just have. . . ." Marx sug- nipple is only here at all when it is in my mouth, when
gests instead that real happiness is achieved through it is (in feeling) a part of me. And to bite and swallow
a thing is for long the only sure way of retaining it.. . .
doing meaningful and properly rewarded work (Marx This is the ultimate form of ownership, from which all
1967). Accordingly, the perspective advocated by others are derived.
Marxists is that we should live to work rather than
work to live (Dyke 1981). This is also the major basis Even though the infant's mother provides care,
for the Marxist objection to capitalism. When the nourishment, and security, her lack of perfect respon-
capitalist owns the products of a worker's labor, the siveness to the infant's desires makes it likely that she
worker has been alienated from that which s/he has is the first object that the infant regards as not self.
created. The worker has been robbed of a part of self. The separation from mother also has led others to
The capitalist, in Marx's view, is seen not only as an suggest that the "security blanket" serves as a transi-
exploiter of labor, but also as a thief of the worker's tional object helping the child to feel the security of
very self (Marx 1964). the mother through an object that symbolizes her
Fromm (1976) instead advocates being as the pre- (e.g., Furby and Wilke 1982; Weisberg and Russell
eminent form of existence. Like Marx, Fromm at- 1971; Winicott 1953). Bowlby (1969) suggests that
tacks "radical hedonism," or concentration on hav- such material objects often aid in identity formation
ing, as being unrewarding. He suggests that this view when children recognize their independence and sep-
promotes a having mode of existence that views arateness from their mothers.
things, experience, time, and life itself as possessions If the early changes in person-object relationships
to be acquired and retained. In the alternate being may be described as moving from being one with the
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 147

environment to having objects that aid the transition terial resources for love resources is difficult accord-
to a world where self is distinct from the environment, ing to research by Foa and Foa 1974 and perceptual
then the next changes may be characterized as mov- findings by Brinberg-Brinberg and Castell 1982;
ing from having transition objects to doing things Brinberg and Wood 1983), from the parents' points of
with or to the environment. This motivation is la- view, control of their children's material possessions
beled "competence" or "mastery" motivation (White offers a means of bringing about desired behaviors.
1959). Furby (1980) expanded this concept by sug- Whiting (1960) provides a succinct model of this sort
gesting that we develop a stronger sense of self by of resource mediated socialization:
learning to actively control objects in our environ-
ment rather than feeling controlled by them. Furby 1. Parents can use resources to reinforce behav-
and Wilke (1982) presented evidence showing that ior in three ways-
until six months of age the child may be most inter- a. Giving (e.g., a "treat" for being "good"),

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


ested in simply controlling an object, whereas by b. Withholding (e.g., no dessert until vegeta-
twelve months the child is more interested in practic- bles are eaten),
ing emerging skills (e.g., with blocks). In both cases, c. Depriving (e.g., no more television view-
producing some intended effect by doing something ing-something already "possessed" -un-
with an object is the goal. til the child "behaves");

Self Versus Others. Data from Kline and France 2. Resources involved must be-
(1899, pp. 446-447) and Isaacs (1935) suggest that a. Scarce (i.e., not freely available to the
the relationship between a person and an object is child),
never as simple as a person-thing bond, because other b. Valued (at the time) by the child,
people often seek to control these objects: c. Controlled by the parent;
a great part ofthe value of those things which little chil- 3. Anticipations of resource availability in the
dren want to own is far from intrinsic. It arises directly future can also be modified to mediate behav-
from the fact that others have or want the object. And ior through-
thus we enter the open field of rivalry. Not to have what a. Threats to withhold or deprive resources,
others have, or to have less than they, is to feel shut out b. Promises to give resources.
from the love and regard of the person giving. It is to
be treated as not loveworthy (Isaacs 1935, p. 74). The way parents use such resource mediated behav-
ioral modification not only affects behaviors-those
In this sense, relationships with objects are never two- concerning possessions as well as other ones-but
way (person-thing), but always three-way (person- also creates new attitudes toward the possessions used
thing-person). This brings forth a meum et tuum con- as reinforcements. For example, if sweets are with-
cern with object ownership (Beaglehole 1932). held or deprived or ifthreats to do so are made, these
The rivalry aspects of possessions seem clear actions may enhance the value of sweets, encourage
among young children. Piaget (1932) reported that 8- the delay of gratifications until unpleasant tasks are
to 12-month-old children often display violent rage completed, or instill an attitude that good perfor-
when a toy is taken from them and given to another mance should be followed by indulgence. The poten-
child. Mueller (1978) and Mueller and Brenner tial effects of such socialization on adult material life-
(1977) found that between 80 and 90 percent of social styles are envisioned easily.
interactions of children up to two years of age are fo-
cused on physical objects; the authors did not report Adolescence and Adulthood. Erikson (1959) sug-
what proportions of these interactions involved con- gested that adolescents predictably undergo an "iden-
flicts. Furby's (1982) examination of this issue re- tity crisis." One hypothesis is that adolescents at this
vealed that for 18- to 21-month-olds, more than 85 stage increasingly seek identity through acquiring
percent of their object-oriented interactions with and accumulating selected consumption objects.
peers involved conflict about retaining possession in- Montemayor and Eisen's (1977) study, which asked
stead of sharing or giving. Horney (1964) suggested teenagers to describe who they are, found that this is
that such competitiveness, along with other evidence true in early teenage years when respondents cited
of lack of affection from parents or peers, leads the possessions, name, and location as part of who they
child to compensate as an adult through neurotic are. However, in later teenage years, they found that
strivings for power, prestige, and possessions. Al- respondents were more likely to cite skills (e.g., ath-
though this may not be a complete explanation of letic, artistic) and traits (e.g., expressions of moral
these adult traits, it seems a more plausible basis for character, self-sufficiency). A study of 8- to 30-year-
adult orientations toward possessions than are expla- old Chicagoans (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-
nations via Freudian oral and anal fixations (Belk Halton 1981) found that this generation is more likely
1982a). than its parents and grandparents to cite as favorite
Although receiving material objects may convey a possessions those that reflect skills (e.g., athletic
sense oflove and worth to the child (substituting ma- equipment) or objects which they can manipulate or
148 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

control (e.g., musical instruments, stereo, pets). Ma- who possess such remembrances are happier than
terial possessions such as clothing and automobiles those who do not. McCracken (1987a) suggests that
are seen as an important source of prestige during homes for the aged would do well to consider the
high school (Snyder 1972), but there is probably some identity deprivation that occurs when these people
tendency to ascribe such prestige to one's family are made to discard possessions. Places that are espe-
rather than to one's self as an individual. These find- cially relevant to one's past have also been found to
ings suggest that only certain types of possessions are be particularly valued by the old because of the mem-
valued as extensions of self during adolescence and ories that places can stir (Howell 1983; Lowenthal
that self-definition through doing things may be pre- 1975). In contrast, the young tend to value places ac-
ferred to self-definition through having things. . cording to the activities these places facilitate (Csiks-
During preretirement adulthood, Csikszentmihalyi - zentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton 1981; Hart 1979).
and Rochberg-Halton (1981) found that emphasis During old age, the sense of one's own mortality

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


shifts from defining oneself by what one does to de- also becomes more and more undeniable. With de-
fining self through what one has. Furby (1978) found creasing future years, declining skills and abilities,
that 40- to 50-year-olds are the most likely of all age and a shrinking network of old friends, sense of self
groups to cite social power and status as reasons to possibly contracts as well. However, this is not neces-
own personal possessions. Csikszentmihalyi (1982, sarily the case. Many people seek to assure that their
pp. 5-6) explains: selves will extend beyond their deaths. Lifton (1973)
suggests five ways through which this extension may
A person who owns a nice home, a new car, good furni- be attempted: (1) through one's children, (2) through
ture, the latest appliances, is recognized by others as
having passed the test of personhood in our society belief in a life after death, (3) through one's works
. . . the objects we possess and consume are . . . (e.g., artistic, literary, scholarly), (4) through identi-
wanted because . . . they tell us things about ourselves fication with nature (which will continue), and (5)
that we need to hear in order to keep our selves from through experiential transcendence (e.g., absorption
falling apart. This information includes the social rec- in music may allow one to transcend the world of here
ognition that follows upon the display of status sym- and now and symbolically be reborn).
bols, but it includes also the much more private feed- A sixth way, which is not mentioned, is to have
back provided by special household objects that objec- one's possessions (especially those in collections one
tify a person's past, present, and future, as well as his has created) "live on" through heirs or museums
or her close relationships. (Rigby and Rigby 1949). Based on interviews with
Olson (1981, 1985) found that young couples cite persons ages 62 to 85, interviews with their friends,
as favorite objects in the home those that reflect their relatives, and acquaintances, and an analysis of let-
future plans and goals, but older couples cite objects ters, mementos, and conversations of the dying and
that relate to their experiences together. Cameron their survivors, Unruh (1983) found evidence of the
(1977) conducted a series of experiments suggesting widespread use of this strategy. He detected first a so-
that having children is a key life event that causes the lidification of identity through creating letters, jour-
parents to become less self-focused and more focused nals, memos, and poems that were meant to be left
on their children. Feibleman (1975) notes the emer- behind. Second, artifacts including photographs,
gence of a tendency of parents by late middle age to scrapbooks, souvenirs, and jewelry were accumu-
live vicariously through their children. At this point, lated. And third, these artifacts were distributed to
children represent an extension of self, but not to the persons who were believed to be willing to care for
exclusion of material possessions. In fact, Belk (1985) them, and in so doing honor and remember the do-
found parents to be more materialistic and possessive nor. This distribution was accomplished through pre-
than their children and their own parents. Because of death gifts and wills and testaments. Western society
accumulated possessions, well-developed skills, pos- seldom elevates reverence for ancestors to the level
session of both a past and a future, and parenthood, of Far Eastern cultures such as Japan and China, but
the middle years of life also are likely to involve the Western society does revere its heroes' and villains'
most extended concept of self. possessions, as illustrated by pilgrimages to Elvis Pre-
sley's Graceland mansion and William Randolf Hear-
Old Age. If the young are future-oriented, the old st's castle (Maines 1978).
are past-oriented. Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-
Halton (1981) found that for their Chicago sample, Possessions and the Sense of Past
such possessions as photographs, athletic trophies, Integral to a sense of who we are is a sense of our
and mementos are most treasured by grandparents. past. Possessions are a convenient means of storing
The reason most often cited for possessions being the memories and feelings that attach our sense of
treasured by this group is that possessions have the past. A souvenir may make tangible some otherwise
ability to symbolize others, often because they are intangible travel experience. An heirloom may record
gifts from these important others. Sherman and New- and recall family heritage just as a historic monument
man (1977) found that postretirement-age persons may help to create a sense of a nation's past.
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 149

Overall, Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-Halton likely to chronicle our cultures' successes than their
(1981) found that the three types of possessions that failures.
the 315 Chicago families most frequently cite as treas- The desire to know one's individual past can ex-
ured are furniture, visual art (including that created plain the retention of personal memorabilia, just as
by family and friends), and photographs. In each case, the desire to remember family heritage can explain
the most frequently given explanation for valuing retention of family heirlooms and the desire to appre-
these objects is the memories they call forth of other ciate national history can explain museum patronage
people, occasions, and relationships. These reasons and visits to historic sites. However, what can explain
overshadow functional explanations for attachments the desire to acquire and collect antiques and antiqui-
to furniture and aesthetic reasons for valuing art ob- ties from another time, place, and family? Clearly, it
jects and photographs. As one oftheir informants ex- is not a claimable sense of past that is achieved at any
plains (Rochberg-Halton 1984, p. 171): except the broadest level of identity.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


This [painting] is my great, great grandfather. I've had Part of the answer lies in the desire to identify with
it since childhood. It's more than just a portrait-it's an era, place, or person to which we believe a desir-
a person! I'd grab it right away in a fire. [Without it] able set of traits or values adheres. At a national level,
my life would be lessened. I'd go on living, but it would neoclassical architecture seems to have this objective.
deplete my secure "lump." It would mean that I At a more personal level, owning artifacts that once
wouldn't be able to hand it down to my children. The belonged to a famous historical figure seems to share
kids already say, "I'm gonna inherit this and that."
... It's part of the continuity of who I am, where I this objective (Rigby and Rigby 1949; Wallendorfand
came from, where I'm going. Belk 1987). In each case, there seems to be a desire to
bask in the glory of the past in the hope that some of
Older respondents are especially likely to link such it will magically rub off-a form of positive contami-
objects to past experiences. One explanation is that nation (Levi-Strauss 1963). This nostalgic desire to
our attachment to memory-evoking possessions gain the glory of the superstar or of a mythical golden
grows as we accumulate experiences from our past age of the past shares something in common with the
and reduce the stock of pleasurable experiences likely tendency McCracken (1988) describes as depositing
to occur in our futures. Also, as Kastenbaum (1977) and retrieving cultural meaning in places where it is
observes, "the older person scans the past for evi- unlikely to be disturbed by contradictions present in
dence that he once was competent, once was loved, reality (e.g., Davis 1979).
once commanded respect." Gifts received from oth-
ers are one such evidence oflove from significant oth- Another reason for the accumulation of antiquities
ers (Belk 1982c; Wallendorf and Arnold 1988). that are found or acquired rather than inherited or
Thus, cherished possessions are not likely to be a claimed on the basis of a more direct linkage to the
random assortment of items that recall our pasts. Just extended self is that antiques are rare and therefore
as we pose family photographs to capture the "good" , potentially serve as symbols of status or "status mark-
(happy) moments of our lives and then selectively ers" (Douglas and Isherwood 1979). Other motives
edit the best of these into albums (Chalfen 1987; Mil- might be found in the amusement of collecting curi-
gram 1976; Sontag 1973), we are also likely to trea- osities, aesthetic preference for antiques over cur-
sure most those possessions associated with pleasant rently produced artifacts, and a preference for hand-
memories. These possessions are likely to include ob- crafted works over current mass-produceq works.
jects such as newspaper clippings and trophies repre- However, each ofthese additional explanations relies
senting past accomplishments, mementos of past ro- on something of the extended self of the previous
mances, and souvenirs of enjoyable travel experi- owner, artist, or craftsperson adhering to the work.
ences, and to exclude others such as belongings of Just as we seek to extend our selves by incorporating
estranged former spouses, poor report cards, and gifts or owning certain objects, we may still seek the sym-
from suitors whp later rejected us. pathetic magic (contagion) of possessions that retain
Note that social institutions such as museums fol- a part of the extended self of valued others. This is
Iowa similar process in selectively retaining aesthetic, also true with gifts received from loved ones. Note
scientific, and historical cultural artifacts. Mukerji that we also abhor the art forgery or reproduction pre-
(1978) makes a distinction between goods that are ini- cisely because it lacks the personal mana of its creator
tially produced as art works and are acquired and re- that is present in the original (e.g., Battin 1979; Belk
tained based on presumably aesthetic judgments, and 1986a; Lessing 1965). Furthermore, we may prefer
goods that are initially produced for more utilitarian the handcrafted item to the mass-produced item
purposes but are later regarded as worthy of preserva- largely because it took longer to create-i.e., more of
tion. Although the retention criteria are somewhat others' selves were invested in it (Stewart 1984).
different for the two classes of objects, in both cases Fieldwork with owners of antiques and handmade
the decisioq.s to retain the object rather than reject it furniture supports this motivation (Wallendorf and
determine the picture of our cultural past that is avail- Belk 1987). Thus, we are symbolically larger and
able to future generations. Obviously, we are more more powerful if we possess such a laboriously crafted
150 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

antique. In so doing, we appropriate part of the self of Sartre also sees giving possessions to others as a
the object's creator, even if this creator is anonymous. means of extending self-a special form of control. A
Finally, fascination with things past also involves gift continues to be associated with the giver so that
nostalgia. Stewart (1984) describes nostalgia as a sad- the giver's identity is extended to include the recipi-
ness without an object, and Kant describes it as a ent. At the same time, giving (as well as destroying)
longing for one's childhood (Kant 1798; Starobinski objects is an affirmation of self in Sartre's view, be-
1966, p. 94). Davis (1979, p. 31) notes the relevance cause this act of doing clearly shows the control one
of such longing to the self: has of these possessions. However, gift recipients
may, despite increasing the objects in their control,
nostalgia (like long-term memory, like reminiscence, lose some sense of self through their lack of control in
like daydreaming) is deeply implicated in our sense of
who we are, what we are about, and (though possibly choosing the gift. They are then encumbered by this
partial imposition ofthe giver's identity and must ac-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


with much less inner clarity) whither we go. In short,
nostalgia is. . . a readily accessible psychological lens knowledge the giver's mastery by accepting the gift
. . . for the never ending work of constructing, main- (e.g., Codere 1950; Dillon 1968). In contrast, the pos-
taining, and reconstructing our identities. sessive gift recipient would like to receive rare gifts
that are a part of the giver's extended self and thereby
McCracken (1986) described how individuals and symbolize the recipient's hold on the giver's self (Katz
cultures, through idealized and nostalgic visions of 1976). Because most gifts are received from loved
the "golden age" of a misty past, use the past to main- ones, gift receipt should generally be found to be re-
tain values that never existed. With such an unassail- garded as a positive extension of self. This would
able image of the past, antiques from that era become seem to explain the high frequency with which gifts
powerful symbols by which we may listen to the past are cited as favorite possessions in the United States
and hear it confer its imagined virtues upon us. and other countries (Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg-
Thus, the functions that Poss«ssions play in the ex- Halton 1981; Wallendorf and Arnould 1988).
tended self involve the creation, enhancement, and A second way of having an object and incorporat-
preservation of a sense of identity. Possessions help ing it into self is by creating it; this view echoes an-
us at all ages to know who we are. However, this does thropological findings and Locke's (1690) political
not imply that we are always active in selecting the philosophy. Whether the thing created is a material
possessions that we see as a part of our selves. As the object or an abstract thought, the creator retains an
next section discusses, passive receipt of objects into identity in the object for as long as it retains a mark
the extended self also occurs. or some other association with the person who
brought it into existence. This identity is codified
PROCESSES OF SELF-EXTENSION through copyrights, patents, and scientific citations
that preserve associations between people and their
Ways ofIncorporating Possessions into the mental creations.
Extended Self Sartre feels that buying an object is merely another
form of creating the object, and that even the latent
Sartre (1943) suggests three primary ways through buying power of money contributes to sense of self.
which we learn to regard an object as a part of self. "Stop before a showcase with money in your pocket;
One way is through appropriating or controlling an the objects displayed are already more than half
object for our own personal use; this view is similar yours" (Sartre 1943, p.753). "That which exists for
to McClelland's (1951) hypotheses about power and me through the medium of money, that which I can
control. Sartre also holds that we can appropriate in- pay for, i.e., which money can buy, that am I, the pos-
tangible or nonownable objects by overcoming, con- sessor of money" (Marx 1975, p. 377). In such a
quering, or mastering them. For instance, a mountain sense, we may suppose that money enlarges the sense
climber in reaching a peak has asserted control ofthe of self because it enlarges imaginable possibilities of
mountain and the panorama it affords. Similarly, it is all that we might have and do. Money also gives us
only through learning to ride a first bicycle, manipu- the power to selectively acquire or reject purchasable
lating a new computer system, driving a first car, or objects, thereby more selectively shaping our ex-
successfully negotiating rapids in a new kayak that tended selves.
these objects really become parts of the extended self. The third way in which objects become a part of self
This is an important point, for it provides an explana- is by knowing them. Whether the object known is a
tion of how nondurable products or services and pub- person, place, or thing, Sartre maintains that the rela-
lic property or events may become viewed as posses- tionship in knowing the object is inspired by a carnal
sions and thereby potentially contribute to sense of and sexual desire to have the object. It is no accident,
self. For instance, as we master getting around in a in Sartre's view, that sexual relations have often been
formerly unfamiliar subway system, our mobility lit- described as knowing or having another person, as it
erally increases and our self figuratively extends to in- is our intimate knowledge ofthe other person that al-
clude the subway system. lows us to consider the person ours and a part of self.
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 151

Likewise, as Beaglehole (1932) observed, our inti- 2. Touching and bodily contact;
mate knowledge of a community, store, or book
makes them not only "ours" but also part of self. But, 3. Glancing, looking, and staring;
like sexual knowledge, such knowing cannot be pas- 4. Noise pollution;
sionless and distanced if the object is to become a part
of the extended self. As Dixon explains, "To know 5. Talking to/addressing one; and
without desire is an offense against the known, to treat 6. Bodily excreta-
the known as object and victim" (1973, p. 4). Only a. Corporeal excreta (spittle, snot, perspira-
when the object is known passionately does it become tion, food particles, blood, semen, vomit,
subject rather than object. urine, and fecal matter-and stains of
All three means outlined by Sartre to make objects these);
a part of extended self(controljmastery, creation, and

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


b. Odor (e.g., flatus, tainted breath, body
knowledge) are active and intentional ways of self-ex- smells);
tension. Clothing (Solomon 1986a), housing (Jager c. Body heat (e.g., on toilet seats);
1983), and automobiles are all acquired as a "second d. Markings left by the body (e.g., plate leav-
skin" in which others may see us. Objects such as land ings-leftover food).
to the farmer, handcrafted pieces to the craftsperson,
and artworks to the artist may become a part of ex- An important omission in this list of modes of in-
tended self, because we have intentionally worked terpersonal contamination is the acquisition of pos-
upon or created these things, investing both energy sessions of another person that have been intimately
and self in them. And, objects like a parlor organ and associated with that person. Burying the dead with
household furnishings may become a part of us their possessions (grave goods) is one example of
through the knowing that comes with habituation- avoiding such contamination. O'Reilly et al. (1984)
they have become a part of our familiar interior land- found that secondhand clothing worn close to its for-
scapes, have been the setting for numerous special as mer owner (e.g., underwear) does not sell and appar-
well as ordinary occurrences in our lives, and often ently enjoys a similar taboo against reuse to avoid
have received the same amount of care and attention contamination. Lurie (1981, p. 24) suggests that
that we lavish upon ourselves and immediate family when adolescent girls exchange clothing they share
members. During their tenure with us, a great many not only friendship, but also identities-they become
memories are likely to have accreted in these objects. soulmates. This is an instance of positive contamina-
All of these forms of self-extension are largely active tion rather than the more commonly recognized neg-
and intentional. But, an additional means of self-ex- ative contaminations.
tension exists that mayor may not be active and in- Because food so obviously is incorporated into self,
tentional: contamination. sharing food is a symbolic way of sharing group iden-
tity. The neighborly cup of coffee, holiday meals, the
dinner party, and the more traditional feast, are all
Contamination examples of bonding through food (e.g., Farb and
Cannibalism is the most extreme instance in which Armelagos 1980; Caplow et al. 1982). The Christian
consumers attempt to incorporate the traits of an- sacrament of communion (symbolically partaking of
other through contamination. That this is not an en- the body and blood of Christ) is a similar way of sym-
tirely extinct practice is shown, at least metaphori- bolically sharing an identity. However, even within
cally, in the recent kidnapping, slaughter, and ritual such rituals, there is a social prohibition against eat-
feasting ofthe University of Texas mascot bull, Bevo, ing the plate leavings of others (although Goffman
by rival athletes and students at Texas A & M Univer- 1971, p. 55, notes that this prohibition may be lifted
sity. Apart from cannibalism, rape is the most ex- for others with whom we are most intimate, including
treme instance in which one person may be said to spouse, parents, and children-those who are seen as
contaminate another. The concern here is not with a part of extended self). This prohibition is strongest
the medical sense of germ contamination and spread when the Ie aver has made a personal imprint on the
of disease, but with the symbolic contamination in- food or on a utensil-teeth marks or lipstick, for in-
volved in involuntarily incorporating another into stance. Utensils qualify for the prohibition because,
one's extended self(see Rozin, Millman, and Nemer- like toothbrushes, they are incorporated into another
off 1986). As Douglas (1966) argues, germ theory may through the mouth. Chewed food is disgusting for the
be merely a rationalization of our disgust at unwanted same reason, and also potentially has been contami-
symbolic contamination by others. Goffman (1971, nated by the spittle of the chewer, just as a comb is
pp. 44-47) suggests six modes of interpersonal con- disgusting because it potentially has been contami-
tamination: nated by the hair and body oils of its owner.
Douglas (1966, p. 160) notes that an important cri-
1. Violation of one's personal space (e.g., Som- terion for disgust at contamination by others' inti-
mer 1971; Lyman and Scott 1967); mate possessions is the possessions' abilities to con-
152 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

vey their owners' original identities. Rubbish is not Regardless of the number of levels, a primary dis-
disgusting unless it is disturbed enough to reveal the tinction in the levels of self construct is between an
hair, food, or wrappings that compose it. Similarly, individual versus collective conception of self. As
the bones of the dead are not disgusting if they are Boorstin (1973) suggests, one of the key ways of ex-
unrecognizable dust or ashes. For this reason, crema- pressing and defining group membership is through
toria are careful to screen the ashes of the dead before shared consumption symbols. Such symbols help
giving them to relatives who presumably would be identify group membership and define the group self.
disgusted by any recognizable remains. Sympathetic Although we may be more individualistic and have
magic depends upon the nails, hair, sweat, blood, or more separable and independent group memberships
other parts of the body remaining recognizable. As than was true before societal specialization, division
with cannibalism and other taboo-breaking rituals, of labor, and movement of production from the
sympathetic magic may depend upon the violation of household to the office or factory (Belk 1984c), we

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


inherent norms of purity for its power. Perhaps the clearly still define ourselves through group identity at
same is true in sexual intimacy; because it violates various levels.
norms of how we treat most others in the world, such Just as an individual may use personal possessions
intimacy may gain the power of a strong bonding such as jewelry, automobile, make-up, and clothing
ritual. to help define an individual sense of self (e.g., Solo-
If disgust at others' possessions depends on their mon 1986a), a family is most apt to use distinct fam-
recognizability as parts ofthese others' selves, disgust ily possessions to define a family self for its members.
at one's own possessions and productions may de- The key consumption object in this case is the
pend upon their unrecognizability as a part of one's home-both the dwelling and its furnishings. Jager
own self. As Allport (1955, p. 43) explains: (1983, p. 56) asks,
Think first of swallowing the saliva in your mouth, or How is it that a kitchen table we once admired in a
do so. Then imagine expectorating it into a tumbler shop window can later become the stable, silent foun-
dation offamily meals and conversations with friends?
and drinking it! What seemed natural and "mine" sud-
denly becomes disgusting and alien. Or picture your- How can a house lose its status as a confronted object
self sucking blood from a prick in your finger; then to become a virtual foundation of our life? All these
questions lead us back to the body.
imagine sucking blood from a bandage around your
finger! What I perceive as separate from my body be- Two points are important here. The first is that the
comes, in the twinkling of an eye, cold and foregin. house is a symbolic body for the family. Just as cloth-
ing alters the individual's body, furnishings and deco-
The same principle may apply to visiting one's former rations alter the family's body. The second important
residence. If it retains most of its former character, point is that the expressive imagery of the house that
including the changes the visitor once made to it, it is definitional of the family is only fully acquired dur-
may be a source of delight. But ifit has been substan- ing consumption. At the point of acquisition, only a
tially altered by subsequent residents, it may seem portion of the ultimate meaning of these objects is
cold, foreign, or even disgusting. Neighborhoods and present (Kron 1981; Saile 1985).
cities in which one formerly resided may seem warm Just as individuals with different un extended core
or cold partly for these reasons. selves are likely to incorporate different objects into
their extended selves, families with different core
Maintaining Multiple Levels of Self selves are likely to embrace different objects in their
extended selves. Research has found support for the
As previously noted, some possessions are more common sense expectation that families with differ-
central to self than are others. The possessions central ent lifestyles and from different social classes tend to
to self may be visualized in concentric layers around live in different types of homes decorated in different
the core self, and will differ over individuals, over fashions. For instance, Weisner and Weibel (1981)
time, and over cultures that cr~ate shared symbolic found significant differences in decor, including
meanings for different goods. However, there is an- different apparent degrees of materialism, evident in
other sense in which the individual has a hierarchical the homes of families classified into four lifestyle
arrangement of levels of self, 'because we exist not groups. Research by Duncan and Duncan (1976) sug-
only as individuals, but also as collectivities. We often gests that a self-expressive house is more important to
define family, group, subculture, nation, and human lower social classes and to those who are more mobile.
selves through various consumption objects. The par- McCracken (1987b) found that "homeyness' is the
ticular number of such levels of self is an open ques- expressive attribute that the lower social classes seek
tion-Rapoport (1981) suggests that there are four most in a home, but the antithetical attribute of "sta-
levels of self, Atkin (1981) seven, and Feldman (1979) tus" is what the more 'socially mobile higher classes
11. For purposes of the present discussion, only four seek most.
levels of self-individual, family, community, and Other researchers (e.g., Cooper 1972, 1974; Tuan
group-need be identified. 1978) have suggested that the interior decor of the
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 153

house represents, for the family, something akin to bicycles, motorcycles, or automobiles are also means
true self, while the exterior appearance of the house of group identification, as are musical knowledge and
represents something akin to social self (as seen by preference, bar, club, and entertainment attendance,
others). McCracken ( 198 7b) also found differences in support of specific cultural arts, and knowledge and
the perceived expressiveness of various rooms of the preference for sports teams (e.g., Cialdini et al. 1976;
home and detected a tendency to use room decor to Lynes 1980). The relative variability of such con-
"embrace" oneself with successive layers of furnish- sumption tastes within groups should tell us some-
ings. These views of interior and exterior also can cor- thing about the degree to which group members rely
respond to the analogy of the body. upon the group for an identity.
Perhaps the degree of internalization of owned land One test of these ideas was performed by Wicklund
is especially intense for family farmers. As Steinbeck and Gollwitzer (1982). They predicted that MBA stu-
(1939, p.50) wrote ofthe U.S. dustbowl farmers: dents who are more insecure about their job prospects

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


would tend to adopt more of the consumption pat-
If a man owns a little property that property is him, it's terns of a stereotypical businessperson-high status
part of him and it's like him. Ifhe owns property only watch, "business shoes," short hair, lack of facial
so he can walk on it and handle it and be sad when it hair, and other high status accessories such as attache
isn't doing well, and feel fine when the rain falls on it,
that property is him, and in some way he's bigger be- cases and expensive pens. Among students who are
cause he owns it. committed to a business career, the study found a
stronger adoption of these stereotypical businessper-
A part of what the ownership of farm land means to son symbols by students with poorer job prospects.
such families is self-sufficiency and possibly the link- Also, those who are more committed to a business ca-
age to prior generations of the family who worked the reer and who are in the poor prospects group are more
land (the sense of past aspect of extended self). There likely to own such symbols than are those students
also may be a strong symbolism of family nurturance less committed to a business career. Similar findings
expressed in the plant and animal husbandry of the regarding business suit ownership by business stu-
farm (Berg 1975). Perhaps all these factors combine dents have been obtained by Solomon and Anand
to make it especially traumatic for farm families to (1985). The authors note the correspondence of such
become dispossessed (Farmer 1986). possessions to magic amulets and totemic emblems in
The community level of self is also a part of the ex- more traditional societies.
planation of the feelings of displaced farm families. However, symbols of group identity need not be in-
Rural communities are associated with a strong sense dividually owned products. They can also be such
of Gemeinschaft in which community identity domi- things as landmarks (natural or man-made), places,
nates the Gesellschaft individual identity thought to leaders, media "stars," inventions, institutions,
be more typical of city life (Tonnies 1957). However, sports teams, scientists, and public monuments (e.g.,
even urbanites can feel a strong sense of community Geist 1978). In the United States, one has only to re-
within neighborhoods. Edney (1972) found a rela- call the sense ofloss experienced when the space shut-
tionship between suburban territorial markers such tle Challenger exploded to realize how deeply related
as fences, hedges, and flower borders and willingness such symbols can be on an aggregate level sense of
to defend one's neighborhood. Brown and Werner self. It is also perhaps the sense of extended self that
(1985) found that such markers as well as holiday dec- causes pride rather than anger or envy at the extrava-
orations on homes tend to predict attachment to gant consumption of political and media stars (Leach
community as well as deter property crimes for 1986). That is, because these stars are a part of group
homes displaying such symbols of community. extended self (with the group in this case being the
Greenbaum and Greenbaum (1965) found more resi- nation or generation), we are proud of their consump-
dence personalization in the neighborhood homes of tion and find it fitting rather than shameful.
those Slavic-Americans who had a stronger sense of Proshansky (1978) suggests that the degree to
subcultural identity. And, Ley and Cybriwsky (1974) which one identifies with city landmarks and shared
found that graffiti is a means of establishing and ex- consumption objects within a given city depends
pressing ethnic, neighborhood, and gang identity in upon the condition of the city and one's period of
inner-city Philadelphia. likely residence there. The first hypothesis finds some
Just as clothing, accent, grooming, and jewelry can support in the tendency detected by Cialdini et al.
distinguish an individual from others and express an (1976) to identify with and wear or display the colors
individual sense of being, they can also indicate group of winning (but not losing) sports teams. Proshan-
identity and express belonging to a group. Formal sky's latter hypothesis suggests stronger identification
uniforms are an obvious example, but informal "uni- with local shared consumption objects by older resi-
forms" also exist for social groups (either small scale dents and those with lesser geographic mobility. This
or symbolic-e.g., yuppies, preppies, Sloane Rangers, expectation is supported in a study by Belk (1988).
Bon Chic, Bon Genre-Belk 1986b). Tattooing, ear Similarly, Hansen and Altman (1976) found that col-
piercing, hair style, and ownership of various styles of lege students who decorate their dormitory rooms to
154 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

a greater degree and with more personally symbolic ing rather than by the less tangible means of doing or
items are less likely to drop out of college than are being. Collecting has become a significant activity in
those who do not personalize their rooms. our consumer society as it has become more widely
Recognition that a part of one's extended self can affordable through the discretionary time and money
be shared, or at least perceived to be shared, with oth- available to the general population rather than just to
ers helps to explain acts of civic responsibility, patrio- the wealthy elite (Mason 1981). Marchand's (1985)
tism, and charity. This explanation suggests that such analysis of advertisements suggests that the merchan-
acts of apparent altruism are based on aggrandizing a dising of ensembles of cosmetics, clothing accesso-
broader level of self than that confined to the individ- ries, and furniture in the 1920s helped stimulate the
ual's body and mind. Such nonreciprocal altruism passion for collecting. However, contemporary col-
can be seen as acts that benefit the broader communi- lections more often are specialized to allow the collec-
ties incorporated within extended self. Even acts of tor an ability to gain control and uniqueness within

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


self-sacrifice for a group with which one strongly self-prescribed boundaries (Treas and Brannen
identifies can be seen as helping this broader self1ive 1976). Thus, one might be a collector of knickknacks
indefinitely, giving the individual a sort of immortal- in the form of a favored ("totemic") animal, of salt
ity. Although such acts are unusual, their occurrence and pepper shakers, or of golden oak furniture. As
gives some credence to John Donne's (1623, p. 795) with more widespread collections of family photo-
words: graphs, record albums, and clothing items such as
shoes or hats, both the items included and the order
No man is an island, entire of itself; every man is a imposed on them are expressive of one's identity. We
piece of the continent, a part of the main. If a clod be
washed away by the sea, Europe is the less, as well as if may not be able to control much of the world about
a promontory were, as well as if a manor of thy friend's us, but the collection, whether of dolls, "depression
or thine own were. Any man's death diminishes me be- glass," or automobiles, allows us total control of a
cause I am involved in mankind, and therefore never "little world." Furthermore, collecting legitimizes ac-
send to know for whom the bell tolls; it tolls for thee. quisitiveness. As Clifford (1985, p. 238) notes, "An
excessive, sometimes even rapacious, need to have is
SPECIAL CASES OF EXTENDED SELF transformed into rule-governed meaningful desire."
Goldberg and Lewis (1978, p. 64) go further in sug-
The basic ways in which objects can become a part gesting that "Many collectors who are inhibited and
of extended self have already been discussed. In this uncomfortable in social interaction, surround them-
section, several unique areas of consumer behavior selves with favored objects upon which they project
that are affected by such self-extension are consid- humanlike qualities. They practically talk to these
ered: collections, money, pets, other people, and body objects; they find comfort in being with them and re-
parts. gard them as friends." In this sense, collections may
be seen as transition objects or security blankets for
adults.
Collections Because of the purposefulness and the commit-
As Belk (l982b) notes, humans and animals once ment oftime and energy spent in developing a collec-
primarily assembled collections of necessities for fu- tion, it is natural that a collection may be seen as more
ture security, but today humans more often assemble a part of one's self than are isolated consumption
collections of nonnecessities for distinction and self- items. The desire of collectors for closure in complet-
definition. Collections ofthis sort may be initiated by ing or filling' gaps in a collection (Saarinen 1958;
gifts or other unintended acquisitions (this was often Wiseman 1974) may be seen as a form of symbolic
found to be the case in the study by Belk et al. 1988), self-enhancement. The stamp collection that lacks a
but the cultivation of a collection is a purposeful self- few entries is often seen as having a cavernous gap
defining act. Rigby and Rigby (1949, p. 35) note: that cries out to be filled. What is likely felt is a lack
of self-completion, which is perceived to be gained
From the small boy to the connoisseur, the joy of through completion of the collection. (Although,
standing before one's accumulated pile and being able ironically, completion of a collection is also feared
to say 'this belongs to me' is the culmination of that because the quest then is through, unless, as often
feeling that begins with ownership of the first item ...
they become us. happens, one redefines the collecting focus as comple-
tion nears.) The symbolic self-completion thesis of
Stewart (1984, p. 159) similarly concludes that the Wicklund and Gollwitzer (1982) suggests that when
way to most effectively disparage a collection is not to one experiences low self-esteem, the addition of ob-
charge that it is inauthentic, but rather to say "it is jects to one's collection may be viewed as compensa-
not you." tory in restoring a more complete sense of self. Devo-
Stewart (1984) also observes that creating one's ex- tion to the collection can also provide a sense of pur-
tended selfthrough devoted development of a collec- pose and worth (see Benjamin 1955). It is also
tion is the ultimate in self-definition by means ofhav- reasonable to think of many collections as compul-
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 155

sions and to think of active collectors as addicts. For although increases in income that do not raise one
instance, a collector of Mickey Mouse toys and dolls above comparison to others appear to have little effect
described how he would scrimp and save to acquire (Duncan 1975). Some evidence also exists that we
new items each week (Wallendorfand Belk 1987). He tend to judge those with higher incomes as being bet-
called these acquisitions his "Mickey fix" and re- ter adjusted, happier, and healthier (Luft 1957).
called having frequently spent gas and rent money to Nevertheless, there are clearly pathologies associ-
make these purchases. Thus, there can be unhealthy ated with extreme uses of money in the service of en-
aspects to some obsessive collecting, even though it is hancing extended self. The most commonly analyzed
done in the intended service of self-enhancement. is miserliness in which a person attempts to substitute
money for love and happiness (e.g., Bergler 1959;
Money and Extension of Self Jones 1948; Krueger 1986). Goldberg and Lewis
(1978) treat miserliness as a vain attempt to collect

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


For some people, money is too abstract, invisible, security, while more Freudian psychologists see it as
or "commiditized" (Kopytoff 1986) to become a part anal retentiveness (e.g., Borneman 1976; Ferenczi
of extended self. If the desire is to extend self through 1914). Prodigal spending of money to enhance self is
having, then using the money to buy more tangible, instead linked to oral aggression (Bergler 1959; Klein
visible would-be extensions of self is more likely for 1957). Bergler (1959) also sees compulsive bargain
such people. Money is seen as profane and must be hunting as an attempt to restore a sense of personal
transformed into "sacred" material goods (Belk and adequacy among oral personality types. Compulsive
Wallendorf 1988; Graves 1965, pp. 21-22). But, for gambling is seen as another pathological use of
others, money itself is regarded as an extension of self money to seek an illusive happier self (Bergler 1959;
and, in this capacity, becomes an end rather than a Furnham and Lewis 1986; Goldberg and Lewis 1978).
means to doing or having other things.
Wiseman (1974) notes that the Cinderella story Pets as Extended Self
conveys a popular fantasy of transforming oneself Secord (1968) noted that pets are often seen as a
through the power of money. In the fantasy, "this part of the pet owner's self to the extent that the atti-
other 'rich me' would not merely have more money, tude is "love me, love my dog." Veevers (1985) re-
but would be changed fundamentally, would be ports evidence of the opposite inference: "hate me,
stronger, less fearful, more charming, wiser, less vul- hate my dog." That is, others' treatment of pets is
nerable, and so on. Money is endowed with magical seen to reflect their regard for the owners, just as oth-
powers" (Wiseman 1974, p. 10). Money is thought to ers' treatment of young children is seen to reflect their
bring love, fame, and respect. Money is commonly regard for the parents. Pets also are regarded com-
seen as a symbol of success and power (Rubenstein monly as representative of self and studies show that
1981). we attempt to infer characteristics of people from
Like other parts of extended self, when consumers their pets (Foote 1956; Heiman 1967). Some relation-
believe strongly enough in money as part of extended ship between personality and choice of pets does, in
self, their well-being is linked to the well-being of fact, exist (Kidd and Kidd 1980). Others have ob-
their money. Psychoanalyst Fingert (1952) reports a served that, like people, pets are regarded as family
male patient who was reluctant to pay analysis fees members (e.g., Cain 1985; Friedmann and Thomas
because the patient feared he would be losing a part 1985; Hickrod and Schmitt 1982; Rochberg-Halton
of his own body. Knight (1968) notes the comparable 1985; Wallendorfand Belk 1987). In this regard, it is
sentiment expressed when people say they feel naked significant that we name our pets, feed and care for
without their pocketbooks. And one study found a them, photograph them, spend money on them,
positive correlation between the sexual potency of groom them, talk to them, protect them, sleep and
businessmen and the level of the Dow Jones Indus- play with them, and mourn their death (Hickrod and
trial Average (New Yorker 1975). This appears to be Schmitt 1982; Meer 1984). Carmack (1985), Cowles
another case of viewing money as an unconscious (1985), and Keddie (1977) found that in some cases of
symbol of masculinity and power (Lindgren 1980). pet death the mourning is similar to that which occurs
Goldberg and Lewis (1978) note that discussion of due to the loss of a home or the loss of a limb. Just as
money and income is a strong taboo in modern West- cannibalism is taboo, eating a pet, or even an animal
ern society. Wiseman (1974) and Yamauchi and that is likely to be thought of as a pet in a particular
Templer (1982) observe that we know more about culture, is taboo. Thus, in the West we eat pigs, but
contemporary sexual behavior of people than we do not dogs, while in ancient Polynesia-where pigs
about their money dealings. Knight (1968, p. 83) re- were pets-just the opposite was true (Titcomb
flects that "It is as if they (psychiatric patients in this 1969). Researchers such as Levinson (1972) and
case) equated money with their in-most being." Robin and Bensel (1985) found that pets are so instru-
Consistent evidence shows that those with higher mental to self-identity that they are often useful as
incomes report higher levels of self-esteem, subjective transition objects (surrogate parents) for children and
happiness, and satisfaction in life (e.g., Diener 1984), as surrogate children for adults.
156 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

These observations and popular treatments suggest Clearly, our laws allow us to regard our children,
that pets can be therapeutic in expanding the self of biological or adopted, as possessions (Derdeyn 1979).
children, hospital patients, and the elderly. Although The embryo also legally is treated as property in cases
this is the contention of many (e.g., Fogle 1981), re- of in vitro fertilization (Albury 1984). And as Lifton
cent results suggest that pet ownership does not al- (1973) notes, children and grandchildren may be as
ways have healthy effects. Tuan (1984) contends that close as the average person gets to immortality. Such
much pet ownership, as well as pet-like relationships a living legacy is often a strongly desired extension
with plants and people, represents a cruel desire to of self. Smith (1983) notes that we take this sort of
impose control over them and command them. Evi- possessive attitude toward children when we make
dence from interviews by the author suggests that boastful claims about them and when we "give them
U.S. males may prefer dogs to cats because of dogs' away" in marriage. The same extended self notion en-
greater responsiveness to commands. Although Horn ters arguments about abortion (Paul and Paul 1979).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


and Meer (1984) found that pet owners report feeling Children also are treated as possessions in divorce
better than do nonowners of pets, Martinez and Kidd proceedings (Hobart 1975).
(1980) found that male nonowners had greater feel- The incorporation of others into extended self can
ings of well-being than male owners of pets. Cameron involve a demeaning objectification of these other
and Matson (1972) found that pet owners have lower persons. Tournier (1957) describes the inability to re-
ego strength than nonowners. Cameron and col- late to people that causes some of us to treat others as
leagues (Cameron et al. 1966; Cameron and Matson things rather than as human beings. Similarly, Dwor-
1972) also found that pet owners like people less than kin (1981) documents the laws that allowed men to
do non owners, and report liking their pets more than treat women as chattel property until the 19th cen-
they like people. Such findings suggest that although tury. She argues that men still treat women as objects
pets, like other objects that become part of extended in pornography, prostitution, and rape. Although
self, may be beneficial, they can also become harmful these latter objectifications of women by men are un-
fetishes if too much of one's self and one's world is likely to add to extended sense of self, and certainly
invested in them. However, it is unclear whether pet not for women, more normal relationships between
ownership brings about such self-image problems or people may extend the sense of self of both partici-
results from them. pants.
If other people are a part of our extended selves, it
follows that there should be a sense of self-loss during
Other People divorce and at the death of a spouse, child, or close
friend. This is consistently observed to be the case,
This section does not refer to slavery, although that although after the individual has regained a new sense
is surely one historical instance of the tendency to of self following a divorce (Le., if sufficient time has
treat people as possessions and extensions of self, al- elapsed), grieving for a former spouse who dies is mild
most in the manner that a tool extends self. Rather, in comparison to grieving for a current spouse who
the focus here is on the symbolic extension of self that dies (Doka 1986). In most all other instances, how-
James (1890, see quotation on p. 1) saw in "his wife ever, the loss of a child or spouse is felt as a loss of self.
and children, his ancestors and friends." There is evi- As a widow relates, "It's as ifmy inside had been torn
dence that some people tend to choose potential out and left a horrible wound there. . . as if half of
mates as they might choose pets, seeking someone myself was missing" (Parkes 1972, p. 97).
who will reflect favorably on them (Snyder, Ber- Jealousy at the emotional or sexual infidelity of a
scheid, and Glick 1985). Some homosexual males re- spouse or lover also reflects a great ego wounding and
fer to "wearing" an attractive companion to a party a fear of total loss of this part of self (Clanton and
or public event. The general tendency to claim casual Smith 1977). Most recent treatments see jealousy as
acquaintances as close friends and drop prominent possessiveness applied to people and emphasize that
names in conversations (and thereby enhance percep- it is an egoistic and unhealthy emotion (e.g., Ber-
tions of one's popularity and status) has been dubbed scheid and Fei 1977; Davis 1949; Jones 1948). Several
"pronoia" (Goldner 1982). As Bateson (1982, p. 3) studies also have linked jealousy to a social and cul-
observes: tural pattern that emphasizes competition and pri-
vate ownership of property (Mazur 1977; Whitehurst
People these days are fond of pointing out that you are 1977).
what you eat. That proposition is true enough, but Another evidence of the incorporation of other per-
there is another which I think is a good deal more pro- sons into one's extended self is the sense of personal
found, namely, that you are the company you keep.
Your identity, your self, depends upon the people and
injury when a close friend or relative is injured. This
things that compose your associations. And perhaps is certainly the case when such a person is raped, for
even more important, your knowledge of yourself and example (Burgess and Holmstrom 1976). Brown-
your development as a person are both predicated on miller (1975) and Kutash, Kutash, and Schlesinger
those same associations. (1978) note that rape is an assault (with intent to in-
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 157

jure another person) and a robbery-like property possessions, which has been almost totally neglected,
crime (with intent to get another's property-in this (4) organ donation research, (5) product disposition
case by "having" her). Thus, the rapist sees the victim and disuse, and (6) the role of extended self in gener-
as "both a hated person and desired property" ating meaning in life, arguably the most significant
(Brownmiller 1975, p. 201). From the rape victim's implication ofthe extended self.
perspective, there is also a violation of self due to the
unwanted incorporation of the rapist into self. Thus, Vicarious Consumption
although the rapist may perceive an extended sense of
self, the rape victim and those who include this victim Veblen (1899) saw wives and children playing a
within their senses of self feel traumatic losses of self. decorative and expressive role for turn of the century
nouveau riche. In this view, clothing and bejeweling
one's wife is not unlike decorating one's house-it is

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


Body Parts an advertisement for self. Similarly, Veblen noted
As noted in the self-perception research (e.g., All- that one can vicariously consume through one's de-
port 1937; McClelland 1951; Pre linger 1959), body pendents, so that consumption that enhances their
parts are among the most central parts of the ex- extended selves enhances one's own extended self, of
tended self. In psychoanalytic terms, such self-exten- which they are a part. Although today's families are
sion is called cathexis. Cathexis involves the charging less patriarchal than those of Veblen's day, the ten-
of an object, activity, or idea with emotional energy dency to vicariously consume through those who are
by the individual. The concept most commonly has a part of extended self perhaps is not dissimilar. We
been applied to body parts and it is known, for in- gain in self-esteem from the ego enhancing consump-
stance, that women generally tend to cathect body tion of these people. If our friend lives in an extrava-
parts to a greater degree than men and that such ca- gant house or drives an extravagant car, we feel just a
thexis reflects self-acceptance (Rook 1985; Secourd bit more extravagant ourselves. Such vicarious con-
and Jourard 1953). Rook (1985) found that when a sumption differes from Solomon's (1986b) "surro-
body part is more highly cathected, there is greater gate consumers" who are really surrogate informa-
use of grooming products to care for this part of the tion gatherers and buyers rather than consumers.
body. Csikszentimihalyi and Rochberg-Halton If one's spouse is seel1 as an extension of self, it
(1981) proposed the seemingly identical concept of would seem to follow that the success of a spouse
psychic energy investment to describe the process of should raise one's self-esteem in much the same way
identification with possessions of any type. Because as personal success. Weathers (1978) found that col-
we are permanently attached to our body parts, these lege senior women, especially those who are married,
body parts are expected to be more strongly cathected tend to perceive a shared sense of prestige when hus-
than material possessions that can be more easily ac- bands succeed. However, this is much less true for
quired and discarded. those who plan to go to graduate school and is not
Because body parts are normally central to concep- true at all for those who are committed to their own
tions of self, the loss of body parts is tantamount to professional careers. In addition, Macke, Bohrnstedt,
losing one's identity and one's very being. Indeed, the and Bernstein (1979) found that for housewives, the
loss of a limb often is viewed by those from whom it husband's success is associated with higher personal
has been severed in just this way (e.g., Parker 1982; self-esteem (apparently largely due to increased
Schilder 1950). One is literally and symbolically household income), but for professional women (and
afraid of being less of a person following an amputa- for those husbands' successes that do not impact in-
tion. come), the husband's success can actually be associ-
ated with lower self-esteem. These findings suggest a
SOME IMPLICATIONS OF EXTENDED complex situation in which a spouse can be both an
SELF FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH extension of one's self and a rival. The rivalry aspects
of a relationship can cause one's spouse's success rela-
In addition to the general ~mplication that the do- tive to the other to have a negative effect on sense of
main of consumer behavior is enriched and enlarged self. Given the declining prevalence ofthe traditional
by the extended self formulation (e.g., research is workhusband/housewife household, we might antici-
needed into each of the preceding "special cases" and pate that extension of self via spouse may be a declin-
into the processes of self-extension), consumer re- ing phenomenon and the social comparison rivalry
search into a number of important neglected areas of aspects may be increasingly common (see Bremer and
consumer behavior may be opened by this formula- Vog11984; Davis and Robinson 1988).
tion. Six neglected areas that can benefit from the ex-
tended self construct are: (1) James's vision that we Gift-Giving
vicariously consume through other family members,
(2) gift-giving, which has received only limited atten- The incorporation of one's children into extended
tion from consumer researchers, (3) care of durable self allows an explanation for apparently altruistic
158 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

acts of generosity and kindness toward these children. washing, waxing, and changing the oil in one's car is
Existing economic and anthropological theories have significantly correlated with centrality of the vehicle
a difficult time dealing with such altruism, and tend to one's extended self. As with houses, cars that are
to explain it as a long-term exchange process such as newer, more recently acquired, and in better condi-
that envisioned in Trivers' (1971) notion of recipro- tion also are cathected more highly. Similar findings
cal altruism. In this view and related exchange theo- have been obtained by Richins and Bloch (1986) who
retical views, the only reason we help or act gener- report lesser involvement with automobiles after
ously toward another human being is because this their newness "wears off."
helps ensure our own welfare. If we help another, we Young females in the same study reported greater
can feel more confident that this other will help us if bathing or showering frequency when they had higher
and when we are in need. In the case of children, they body part cathexis scores (findings were nonsignifi-
continue to be seen as sources of financial and emo- cant for males, as expected, since females cathect

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


tional support in the event of catastrophe or diffi- body parts to a greater degree than do males; Secord
culty. However, this pessimistic view of human be- and 10urard 1953). Generally then, the more an ob-
havior is not needed when the notion of extended self ject is cathected into one's extended self, the more
including other people is recognized. The explana- care and attention it tends to receive. Inasmuch as
tion is simply that we give to our children and certain consumer research has done little to investigate con-
others because making them happy makes that part sumption (as opposed to purchase or information ac-
of us that includes them happy. The smile on our quisition), notions of extended self may be especially
child's face puts a heart-felt smile on our own face. useful in suggesting promising directions for such re-
This explanation may still involve egoism rather than search.
purely other-focused altruism, but it is less cynical
and more compatible with traditional notions of hu- Organ Donation
manitarianism based on empathy.
Similarly, it is expected that gift-giving is more self- The donation of blood and kidneys and promises
gratifying to the extent that the recipient is a part of to allow one's other organs to be "harvested" upon
the giver's extended self. When this is not the case, death are voluntary decisions that seem likely to be
gift-giving is expected to be done only grudgingly or affected by cathexis of the particular body parts in-
only in accordance with rules of reciprocity. For giv- volved. Pessemier, Bemmaor, and Hanssens (1977)
ing to others incorporated within one's extended self, found that those who rate their general body image as
however, reciprocal gifts from a highly cathected re- less important are more willing to donate body or-
cipient should not be required for continued giving. gans. For specific body organs sought in cadaver do-
Because some degree of rivalry was noted to occur nations, Fulton, Fulton, and Simmons (1977) re-
with a spouse or partner and because reciprocal giving ported that those organs generally seen as more cen-
is a ritual that helps bring friends within one's ex- tral to identity are less likely to be approved for
tended self, such nonreciprocal giving is most likely removal by surviving relatives. Among live potential
for gifts to relatives such as parents to whom self-ex- donors, Wilms et al. (1987) found that people are less
tension is fixed by more permanent means (see Cheal willing to donate organs seen as more sacred, emo-
1986, 1987). Research on such issues may help to ex- tional, mysterious, and not well understood. In the
tricate consumer research from the narrow perspec- United States, where their research was conducted,
tive that consumer behavior involves exchange as a the more sacrosanct organs are the eyes, brain, and
sole means of product and service acquisition. heart, which are most often vetoed when the family
approves other organs for transplantation. In con-
Care of Possessions tntst, Thukral and Cummins (1987) suggest that Bud-
dhists may find it a high honor to donate their eyes,
A relationship should exist between incorporation because they believe that this allows them to live on
of an object into one's extended self and the care and after death.
maintenance of the object. In one study (Belk 1987b, Other evidence shows that transplantation of im-
1988), the more strongly homeowners cathected their portant body organs can be psychologically traumatic
dwellings, the more frequently or recently they re- for the donor and the recipient. For donors, the fear
ported mowing the grass, remodeling the house, is that a significant part of one's self will be lost (Sim-
painting the interior, and dusting. Homeowners ca- mons, Klein, and Simmons 1977). Barnett et al.
thected their dwellings more strongly when the dwell- (1987) cite such fears as a reason why they found that
ing was built and acquired more recently and was in organ donation appeals stressing benefits to self(e.g.,
better condition. people will think of you as a good and caring person)
A similar pattern was obtained in the same study are more successful than altruistic appeals to help
for car care by 19- to 28-year-old male car owners others (the traditionally cited reason for organ dona-
(findings for females were not significant here, as was tions-'-e.g., Cleveland 1975; Fellner and Marshall
expected-e.g., Bloch 1982). Reported frequency of 1981; McIntyre et al. 1987). For transplant recipients,
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 159

the fear is twofold. First, recipients fear a loss of self come in handy some day," and in fact eventually lose
because their defective organs must be removed and track of the accumulated possessions in their attics.
discarded. Research using the Draw-a-Person test La Branche (1973) suggested that such retention of
finds that transplant recipients draw more distorted possessions is due to "the fear of annihilation of our
and smaller figures than others do (often with body current histories." That is, material possessions form-
parts missing), reflecting a loss of self-esteem, inse- ing parts of our extended selves seem to form an an-
curity, anxiety, and poor body image (Chaturvedi and chor for our identities that reduces our fear that these
Pant 1984). Second, there is a fear of contaminating identities will somehow be washed away. We may
self with the organ of another person. Transplanted speculate that the stronger the individual's unex-
organs from an opposite sex sibling often lead to fears tended or core self, the less the need to acquire, save,
of becoming a homosexual (Basch 1973; Castel- and care for a number of possessions forming a part
nuovo-Tedesco 1973). Similarly, blacks express fears ofthe extended self.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


of contamination by receiving organs from whites Conversely, one of the points in our lives when we
(Callender et al. 1982). In some cases, the organ recip- should be inclined to discard possessions that form a
ient expects to acquire the donor's traits and skills part of extended self is when the unextended self has
(such as artistic and language skills or altruism). Gen- grown in strength and extent so that the buffer of ex-
erally, however, the trauma of acquiring body organs tended self becomes less necessary. Such times are
from another person brings about a depression that is likely when key life stages and rites of passage have
attributable at least partly to difficulty in psychologi- occurred, such as school graduations, new jobs, mar-
cally accepting the organ that drugs have helped the riage, moving residences, and retirement. Another in-
body to physiologically accept (e.g., Biorck and Mag- stance when consumers should shed or neglect pos-
nusson 1968; Castelnuovo-Tedesco 1971; Klein and sessions is when possessions no longer fit consumers'
Simmons 1977). ideal self-images. This can occur either because the
From the point of view of understanding willing- ideal self-image has changed or because the images of
ness to donate body organs, a further consideration the objects formerly incorporated in extended self
derives from the fact that persons and groups also have changed. As La Branche (1973) noted, we are
may be seen as a part of one's extended self (refer to our own historians. So, another factor in the retention
sections on levels of self and people as possessions). or discarding of possessions that no longer fit our view
For example, one should be more willing to donate of ourselves is their fit with our perceptions of our en-
body organs to those seen as more central to one's ex- tire personal history. Possessions may show where we
tended self. This expectation is supported by investi- have come from and thereby remain valuable as a
gations of various family members approached to do- point of contrast to present extended self. As with the
nate a kidney, in that those with closer relationships other areas of consumer implications noted in this
to the patient in need of the kidney are more willing to section, much work remains to be done regarding dis-
submit to testing and ultimately to donate their own position, retention, and object neglect.
kidneys (Simmons, Bush, and Klein 1977). Those
who cathect community to a greater degree are ex- Extended Self and Meaning in Life
pected to be more willing to donate body organs to
others within their community. This expectation is The preceding subsection should not be taken to
borne out for blood donations (Titmus 1970) as well suggest that possessions are merely a crutch to shore
as for donations of tissue organs (Belk and Allstin up weak or sagging personalities. The possessions in-
1986; Prottas 1983). Thus, although cathexis of body corporated in extended self serve valuable functions
organs and other persons are not necessarily antithet- to healthy personalities. One such function is acting
ical to one another, they are thought to create oppo- as an objective manifestation of the self. As Douglas
site barriers and incentives to organ donation. Belk and Isherwood (1979) noted, such possessions are
and Austin (1986) also found that those who are more "good for thinking." Possessions help us manipulate
materialistic (i.e., who attach more importance to our possibilities and present the self in a way that gar-
possessions-Belk 1984a, p. 291) see body organs as ners feedback from others who are reluctant to re-
more central to their identities and are less willing to spond so openly to the unextended self.
part with them. The possessions in our extended self also give us a
personal archive or museum that allows us to reflect
Product Disposition and Disuse on our histories and how we have changed. Through
heirlooms, the family is able to build a similar archive
Consumer behavior has done little to investigate and allow individual family members to gain a sense
the disposition and disuse of durable possessions (see of permanence and place in the world that extends
Belk, Sherry, and Wallendorf 1988 for an exception beyond their own lives and accomplishments. Com-
and a review of extant literature). Korosec-Serfaty munities, nations, and other group levels of self are
(1984) found that in France people tend to "irratio- similarly constituted via monuments, buildings,
nally" hang onto possessions because "they might books, music, and other created works. The associa-
160 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

tion of these artifacts with various group levels of self Self-extension occurs through control and mastery
provides a sense of community essential to group har- of an object, through creation of an object, through
mony, spirit, and cooperation. In addition, natural knowledge of an object, and through contamination
wonders can be incorporated into extended self such via proximity and habituation to an object. The ex-
that we enhance feelings of immortality and having a tended self operates not only on an individual level,
place in the world. This is not to suggest that extend- but also on a collective level involving family, group,
ing self into material possessions has only positive subcultural, and national identities. These additional
effects. Research on materialism suggests some of the levels of self were posited to account for certain be-
negative consequences of relying on possessions to haviors that might be seen as selfless in the narrower
provide meaning in life. But the construct of ex- individual sense of self.
tended self also suggests that possessions can make a In addition to the use of commonly purchased con-
positive contribution to our identities. sumer goods as possessions comprising extended self,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


Research involving levels of extended self seems several frequently used but seldom researched types
likely to provide a more macro perspective that re- of possessions were considered: collections, money,
lates consumer behavior to a broader portion of hu- other people, pets, and body parts. In each case, there
man life (Belk 1987a). By considering the role of con- is evidence of a relationship between these posses-
sumption in providing meaning in life, we may de- sions and one's sense of self. Implications were then
velop a stronger vision of the significance of derived involving gift-giving, vicarious consumption,
consumer research. Consumption is a central facet of care of possessions, organ donation, product disposi-
contemporary life, but it has seldom been considered tion and disuse, and the role of extended self in creat-
from this broader perspective. The construct of ex- ing meaning in life.
tended self offers some promise for cultivating such a A broad array of evidence and theory, drawing on
broadened appreciation of the potential significance a variety of fields of investigation, support the impor-
of consumer research. tance of extended self as a central construct that can
explain a variety of consumer and human behaviors.
CONCLUSIONS The construct offers a way to account for gift-giving
without necessarily resorting to the cynical premises
This article began by suggesting that we are what we of the exchange paradigm, and also offers perspec-
have and that this may be the most basic and powerful tives on the relatively unexplored consumption areas
fact of consumer behavior. A number of lines of evi- of pets, care and maintenance of consumption goods,
dence were presented in support of this contention. product loss and disposition, organ donation, shar-
The limited research that has addressed directly the ing, collective consumption of objects, and collection
"things" that are viewed to comprise self generally of consumption objects. It raises important issues
has found that possessions follow body parts and concerning the role of possessions at each stage of the
mind in their centrality to self. Evidence supporting life course. And, it offers a more promising way of
the general premise that possessions contribute to considering the symbolic importance of consumption
sense of self is found in a broad array of investiga- in our lives than does prior product and self-concept
tions, including reactions to the loss of possessions, research. In light of the scope and importance of such
treatment of grave goods, self-perception, and theo- issues, their relative neglect in consumer behavior re-
ries of property rights. search, and the diverse and compelling evidence sup-
In considering the functions of extended self, dis- porting the role that extended self may play in shed-
cussion was directed toward the relative roles that ding light on these issues, the concept of possessions
having, doing, and being play in our lives and identi- as extended self has much to offer the quest for an
ties. It seems an inescapable fact of modern life that understanding of consumer behavior.
we learn, define, and remind ourselves of who we are
by our possessions. Developmental evidence suggests [Received April 1987. Revised February 1988.]
that this identification with our things begins quite
early in life as the infant learns to distinguish selffrom REFERENCES
environment and then from others who may envy our
possessions. Emphasis on material possessions tends Abelson, Robert P. (1986), "Beliefs are Like Possessions,"
to decrease with age, but remains high throughout life Journalfor the Theory ofSocial Behaviour, 16 (3), 223-
as we seek to express ourselves through possessions 250.
and use material possessions to seek happiness, re- - - and Deborah A. Prentice (forthcoming), "Beliefs as
mind ourselves of experiences, accomplishments, Possessions-a Functional Perspective," Attitude
Structure and Function, eds. Anthony R. Pratkanis et
and other people in our lives, and even create a sense aI., Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, in
of immortality after death. Our accumulation ofpos- press.
sessions provides a sense of past and tells us who we Albury, Rebecca (1984), "Who Owns the Embryo?" Test-
are, where we have come from, and perhaps where we Tube Women: What Futurefor Motherhood?, eds. Rita
are going. Arditti et aI., London: Pandora, 54-67.
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 161

Alekshin, V.A. (1983), "Burial Customs as an Anthropo- - - (1984b), "Explanations for Congruence Between Pa-
logical Source," Current Anthropology, 24 (April), tron Stereotypes and Patron Self-Concepts," Proceed-
137-150. ings o/the 1983 Convention o/the American Psycholog-
Allport, Gordon W. (1937), Personality: A Psychological ical Association, ed. Dennis Anderson, Washington,
Interpretation, New York: Henry Holt. D.C.: American Psychological Association (Division
- - (1955), Becoming, New Haven, CT: Yale University 23),93-95.
Press. - - (1984c), "Cultural and Historical Differences in the
Ames, Kenneth L. (1984), "Material Culture as Nonverbal Concept of Self and Their Effects on Attitudes Toward
Communication: A Historical Case Study," American Having and Giving," Advances in Consumer Research,
Material Culture: The Shape o/Things Around Us, ed. Vol. 11, ed. Thomas Kinnear, Ann Arbor, MI: Associa-
Edith Mayo, Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green Uni- tion for Consumer Research, 291-297.
versity Popular Press, 25-47. - - (1985), "Materialism: Trait Aspects of Living in the
Atkin, Ron (1981), Multidimensional Man, Middlesex, En- Material World," Journal 0/ Consumer Research, 13

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


gland: Harmondsworth. (December), 265-280.
Ausubel, David P., Edmund V. Sullivan, and S. William - - (1986a), "Symbolic Consumption of Art and Cul-
Ives (1980), Theory and Problems 0/ Child Develop- ture," Artists and Cultural Consumers, eds. Douglas V.
ment, New York: Grune & Stratton. Shaw et aI., Akron, OH: Association for Cultural Eco-
Bakker, Cornelius B. and Marianne K. Bakker-Rabdau nomics, 168-178.
(1973), No Trespassing! Explorations in Human Terri- - - (1986b), "Yuppies as Arbiters of the Emerging Con-
toriality, San Francisco, CA: Chandler and Sharp. sumption Style," Advances in Consumer Research,
Barnett, Mark A., Michael Klassen, Vera McMinimy, and Vol. 13, ed. Richard J. Lutz, Provo, UT: Association
Laurel Schwarz (1987), "The Role of Self- and Other- for Consumer Research, 514-519.
Oriented Motivation in the Organ Donation Deci- - - (1987a), "ACR Presidential Address: Happy
sion," Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 14, eds. Thought," Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 14,
Melanie Wallendorf and Paul Anderson, Provo, UT: eds. Melanie Wallendorf and Paul Anderson, Provo,
Association for Consumer Research, 335-337. UT: Association for Consumer Research, 1-4.
Basch, Samuel H. (1973), "The Intrapsychic Integration of - - (1987b), "Possessions and Extended Sense of Self," .
a New Organ: A Clinical Study of Kidney Transplanta- Marketing and Semiotics: New Directions in the Study
tion," Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 42 (3), 364-384. o/Signs/or Sale, ed. Jean Umiker-Sebeok, Berlin, Ger-
Bateson, Gregory (1982), "Difference, Double Description many: Mouton de Gruyter, 151-164.
and the Interactive Designation of Self," Studies in - - (1988), "Property, Persons, and Extended Sense of
Symbolism and Cultural Communication, ed. F. Allan Self," Proceedings 0/ the Division 0/ Consumer Psy-
Hanson, Manhattan, KS: University of Kansas Publi- chology, American Psychological Association 1987 An-
cations in Anthropology, 3-8. nual Convention, New York, ed. Linda F. Alwitt, Wash-
Battin, M. Pabst (1979), "Exact Replication in the Visual ington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 28-
Arts," Journal 0/ Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 38 (2), 33.
153-158. - - and Mark Austin (1986), "Organ Donation Willing-
ness as a Function of Extended Self and Materialism "
Beaglehole, Ernest (1932), Property: A Study in Social Psy-
Advances in Health Care Research, 1986 Proceedings,
chology, New York: Macmillan.
eds. M. Venkatesan and Wade Lancaster, Toledo, OH:
Belk, Russell W. (1978), "Assessing the Effects of Visable Association for Health Care, 84-88.
Consumption on Impression Formation," Advances in - - , Kenneth Bahn, and Robert Mayer (1982), "Devel-
Consumer Research, Vol. 5, ed. H. Keith Hunt, Ann opmental Recognition of Consumption Symbolism,"
Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, 39- Journal o/Consumer Research, 9 (June), 4-17.
47.
- - , John Sherry, and Melanie Wallendorf (1988), "A
- - (1982a), "Acquisitiveness and Possessiveness: Criti- Naturalistic Inquiry into Buyer and Seller Behavior at
cisms and Issues," Proceedings o/the 1982 Convention a Swap Meet," Journal 0/ Consumer Research, 14
o/the American Psychological Association, ed. Michael (March),449-470.
B. Mazis, Washington, D.C.: American Psychological - - and Melanie Wallendorf (1988), "Sacred and Pro-
Association (Division 23), 70-73. fane Aspects of Money," paper presented at Sociology
- - (1982b), "Acquiring, Possessing, and Collecting: of Consumption Conference, Oslo, Norway.
Fundamental Processes in Consumer Behavior," Mar- - - , Melanie Wallendorf, John Sherry, Morris Hol-
keting Theory: Philosophy o/Science Perspectives, eds. brook, and Scott Roberts, (1988), "Collectors and Col-
Ronald F. Bushard and Shelby D. Hunt, Chicago, IL: lecting," Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 15, ed.
American Marketing Association, 185-190. Michael Houston, Provo, UT: Association for Con-
- - (1982c), "Effects of Gift-Giving Involvement on Gift sumer Research, Vol. 15, ed. Michael Houston, Provo
Selection Strategies," Advances in Consumer Research, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 548-553. '
Vol. 9, ed. Andrew Mitchell, Ann Arbor, MI: Associa- Bell?w, ~aul (1975), Humbolt's Gift, New York: Viking.
tion for Consumer Research, 408-412. Ben]amm, Walter (1955), "Unpacking My Library: A Talk
- - (1984a) "Three Scales to Measure Constructs Re- about Book Collecting," Illuminations, ed. Hannah
lated to Materialism: Reliability, Validity, and Rela- Arendt, trans. Harry Zohn, New York: Harcourt, Brace
tionships to Measures of Happiness, " Advances in Con- &World,59-67.
sumer Research, Vol. 11, ed. Thomas Kinnear, Ann Berg, Richard L. (1975), "Land: An Extension of the Peas-
Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research, 291- ant's Ego," Antropological Quarterly, 48 (January), 4-
297. 13.
162 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Bergler, Edmund (1959), Money and Emotional Conflicts, Campbell, Colin (1987), The Romantic Ethic and the Spirit
New York: Pageant. of Modern Consumerism, Oxford, England: Basil
Berscheid, Ellen and Jack Fei (1977), "Romantic Love and Blackwell.
Sexual Jealousy," Jealousy, eds. Gordon Clanton and Campbell, Keith (1984), Body and Mind, Notre Dame, IN:
Lynn G. Smith, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, University of Notre Dame Press.
101-109. Caplow, Theodore, Howard M. Bahr, Bruce A. Chadwrick,
Biorck, Gunnar and Gosta Magnusson (1968), "The Con- Reuben Hill, and Margaret H. Williamson (1982),
cept of Self as Experienced by Patients with a Trans- Middletown Families: Fifty Years of Change and Con-
planted Kidney," Acta Medica Scandinavia, 183, 191- tinuity, Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota
192. Press.
Bloch, Peter (1982), "Involvement Beyond the Purchase Carmack, Betty J. (1985), "The Effects on Family Members
Process: Conceptual Issues and Empirical Investiga- and Functioning After the Death of a Pet," in Pets and
tion," Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 9, ed. An- Family, Marvin B. Sussman, New York: Haworth,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


drew Mitchell, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Con- 149-162.
sumer Research, 413-417. Carrithers, Michael, Steven Collins, and Steven Lukes, eds.
Boorstin, Daniel (1973), The Americans: The Democratic (1985), The Category ofthe Person: Anthropology, Phi-
Experience, New York: Random House. losophy, History, Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Univer-
Bordewich, Fergus M. (1986), "Dowry Murders," The At- sity Press.
lantic, 258 (1), 21-27. Castelneovo-Tedesco, Pietro, ed. (1971), "Cardiac Sur-
Borneman, Ernest (1976), "Introduction," The Psycho- geons Look at Transplantation-Interviews with Drs.
analysis ofMoney, New York: Urizen, 1-70. Cleveland, Cooley, DeBakey, Hallman, and Rochelle,"
Bowlby, John (1969), Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1, Lon- in Psychiatric Aspects of Organ Transplantation, New
don: Hogarth. York: Grune & Stratton, 5-16.
Bremer, Frances and Emily Vogl (1984), Coping With His - - (1973), "Organ Transplant, Body Image, Psycho-
Success: A Survivor's Guide for Wives, New York, sis," Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 42 (3), 349-363.
Harper & Row. Chalfen, Richard (1987), Snapshot Versions of Life, Bowl-
Brinberg, David and Pat Castell (1982), "A Resource Ex- ing Green, OH: Bowling Green State University Press.
change Theory Approach to Interpersonal Interac- Chaturvedi, Shailendra K. and V. L. Pant (1984), "Objec-
tions," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, tive Evaluation of Body-Image of Renal Transplant
43 (8), 260-269. Recipients," Journal of Psychological Researches, 28
(1),4-7.
- - and Ronald Wood (1983), "A Resource Exchange
Theory Analysis of Consumer Behavior," Journal of Cheal, David J. (1986), "The Social Dimensions of Gift Be-
havior," Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
Consumer Research, 10 (December), 330-338. 3 (December), 423-439.
Brown, Barbara B. (1982), "House and Block as Territory," - - (1987), "'Showing Them You Love Them': Gift
paper presented at the 1982 Conference of the Associa- Giving and the Dialectic ofIntimacy," Sociological Re-
tion for Consumer Research, San Francisco, CA. view, 35 (1),150-169.
- - and Carol M. Werner (1985), "Social Cohesiveness, Chester, C. Ronald (1976), "Perceived Relative Depriva-
Territoriality, and Holiday Decorations: The Influence tion as a Cause of Property Crime," Crime & Delin-
of Cul-de-Sacs," Environment and Behavior, 17 (Sep- quency, 22 (1), 17-30.
tember), 539-565. Cialdini, Robert B., Richard J. Borden, Avril Thorne, Mar-
Brownmiller, Susan (1975), Against Our Will: Men, cus R. Walker, Stephen Freeman, and Lloyd R. Sloan
Women, and Rape, New York: Simon & Schuster. (1976), "Basking in Reflected Glory: Three (Football)
Burgess, Ann and Lynda Holmstrom (1976), "Coping Be- Field Studies," Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
havior of the Rape Victim," American Journal of Psy- chology, 34 (September), 366-375.
chiatry, 133 (4),413-417. Clanton, Gordon and Lynn G. Smith (1977), "The Self-In-
Cain, Ann O. (1985), "Pets as Family Members," in Pets flicted Pain of Jealousy," Psychology Today, 3
and the Family, ed. Marvin B. Sussman, New York: (March),45-47.
Haworth, 5-10. Cleveland, Sidney (1975), "Personality Characteristics,
Callender, Clive 0., James A. Bayton, Curtis Yeager, and Body Image and Social Attitudes of Organ Transplant
John E. Clark (1982), "Attitudes Among Blacks To- Donors versus Nondonors," Psychomatic Medicine, 37
ward Donating Kidneys for Transplantation," Journal (July/August),313-319.
ofthe National Medical Association, 74 (8), 807-809. Clifford, James (1985), "Objects and Selves-An After-
Cameron, Paul (1977), The Life Cycle: Perspective and word," Objects and Others: Essays on Museums and
Commentary, Oceanside, NY: Dabor Science Publica- Material Culture, History of Anthropology, Vol. 3,
tions. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press, 236-246.
- - , Carol Conrad, Dave D. Kirkpatrick, and Robert Ba- Clodd, Edward (1920), Magic in Names and Other Things,
teen (1966), "Pet Ownership and Sex as Determinants London: Chapman & Hall.
of Stated Affect Toward Others and Estimates of Oth- Codere, Helen (1950), Fighting with Property, New York:
ers' Regard of Self," Psychological Reports, 19 (3, Part Augustin.
I), 884-886. Cooper, Clare (1972), "The House as Symbol," Design and
- - and Michael Mattson (1972), "Psychological Corre- Environment, 3 (Fall), 3-37.
lates of Pet Ownership," Psychological Reports, 30 - - (1974), "The House as a Symbol of the Self," in
(February),286. Designing for Human Behavior, eds. Jon Lang et aI.,
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 163

Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, 130- - - and Nancy G. Duncan (1976), "House as Presenta-
146. tion of Self and the Structure of Social Networks," in
Cottle, Thomas J. (1981), "Two Against the Tide," Psy- Environmental Knowing: Theories, Research, and
chology Today, 1 (January), 18. Methods, ed. Reginald G. GoUege, Stroudsburg, P A:
Cowles, Kathleen V. (1985), "The Death ofa Pet: Human Dowden, Hutchinson, & Ross, 247-253.
Responses to the Breaking of the Bond," Pets and the Dworkin, Andrea (1981), Pornography: Men Possessing
Family, ed. Marvin B. Sussman, New York: Haworth, Women, New York: Perigee.
135-148. Dyke, C. (1981), Philosophy of Economics, Englewood
Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly (1982), "The Symbolic Function Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
of Possessions: Towards a Psychology of Materialism, " Edney, Julian J. (1972), "Property, Possession and Perfor-
paper presented at 90th Annual Convention of the mance: A Field Study of Human Territoriality," Jour-
American Psychological Association, Washington, nal ofApplied Social Psychology, 2 (3), 275-282.
D.C. - - (1975), "Territoriality and Control: A Field Experi-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


- - and Eugene Rochberg-Halton (1981), The Meaning ment," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
of Things: Domestic Symbols and the Self, Cambridge, 31 (6),1108-1115.
MA: Cambridge University Press. Ellis, Lee (1985), "On the Rudiments of Possessions and
Davis, Fred (1979), Yearning for Yesterday: A Sociology of Property," Social Science Information, 24 (March),
Nostalgia, New York: Free Press. 113-143.
Davis, Kingsley (1949), "Jealousy and Sexual Property: An Englehardt, H. Tristram, Jr. (1973), Mind-Body: A Categor-
Illustration," Human Society, New York: Macmillan, ical Relation, The Hague, The Netherlands: Martinus
175-194. Nijhoff.
Davis, Nancy J. and Robert V. Robinson (1988), "Class Erikson, Erik (1959), "Identity and the Life Cycle," Psycho-
Identification of Men and Women in the 1970s and 10gicalIssues, 1 (1),1-171.
1980s," American Sociological Review, 53 (February), Erikson, Kai T. (1976), Everything in Its Path: Destruction
103-112. of Community in the Buffalo Creek Flood, New York:
Derdeyn, Andre P. (1979), "Adoption and Ownership of Simon & Schuster.
Children," Child Psychiatry and Human Development, Van Esterick, Penny (1986), "Generating Status Symbols:
9 (Summer), 215-226. You Are What You Own," paper presented at Annual
Dichter, Ernest (1964), Handbook of Consumer Motiva- Conference of the Association for Consumer Research,
tions: The Psychology ofConsumption, New York: Mc- Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Graw-Hill. Farb, Peter and George Armelagos (1980), Consuming Pas-
Diener, Ed (1984), "Subjective Well-Being," Psychological sions: The Anthropology of Eating, Boston, MA:
Bulletin, 95 (3), 542-575. Houghton Mifflin.
Dietze, Gottfried (1963), In Defense of Property, Chicago, Feibleman, James K. (1975), The Stages of Human Life: A
IL: Henry Regnery. Biography of Entire Man, The Hague, The Nether-
Dillon, Wilton (1968), Gifts and Nations: The Obligation to lands: Martinus Nijhoff.
Give, Receive, and Repay, The Hague, The Nether- Farmer, Val (1986), "Broken Heartland," Psychology To-
lands: Mouton. day, 20 (4),54-63.
Dixon, John W., Jr. (1973), "The Erotics of Knowing," An- Feirstein, Bruce (1986), "Me, My Things and I," New York
Times Magazine, (May 4), 18.
glican Theological Review, 56 (1),3-16.
Feldman, Saul D. (1979), "Nested Identities," in Studies in
Dixon, S.C. and J.W. Street (1975), "The Distinction Be- Symbolic Interaction, ed. Norman K. Denzin, Green-
tween Self and Non-Selfin Children and Adolescents," wich, CT: JAI, 399-418.
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 127,157-162.
Fellner, Carl H. and John R. Marshall (1981), "Kidney Do-
Doka, Kenneth J. (1986), "Loss Upon Loss: The Impact of nors Revisited," in Altruism and He/ping Behavior,
Death After Divorce," Death Studies, 10 (5), 441-449. eds. J. Phillipe Rushton and Richard M. Sorrentino,
Donne, John (1623), "Mediation XVII," in The Norton An- Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 351-366.
thology of English Literature, Vol. 1 (1962) ed. M.H. Ferenczi, Sandor (1914), "The Ontogenesis of Money," in
Abrams, New York: W.W. Norton, 794-796. The Psychoanalysis of Money, ed. Ernest Borneman,
Donner, Dory (1985), "Bike Theives Take More Than Just New York: Urizen, 81-90.
Metal; They Steal a Big Part ofSomeone's Life," Daily Fingert, Hyman M. (1952), "Comments on the Psychoana-
Utah Chronicle, 94 (October 30), 11. lytic Significance of the Fee," Bulletin ofthe Menninger
Douglas, Mary (1966), Purity and Danger: An Analysis of Clinic, 16 (2),98-104.
Pollution and Taboo, London: Routledge & Kegan Fisher, Jeffrey D. and Reuben M. Baron (1982), "An Eq-
Paul. uity-Based Model of Vandalism," Population and En-
- - and Baron Isherwood (1979), The World of Goods: vironment, 5 (Fall), 182-200.
Towards an Anthropology ofConsumption, New York: Foa, Edna and Uriel G. Foa (1974), Societal Structures of
W.W. Norton. the Mind, Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Duncan, Otis D. (1975), "Does Money Buy Satisfaction?" Fogle, Bruce (1981), Interrelations Between People and
Social Indicators Research, 2 (2), 267-274. Pets, Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Duncan, James S. (1976), "Landscape and the Communi- Foote, Nelson (1956), "A Neglected Member of the Fam-
cation of Social Identity," The Mutual Interaction of ily," Marriage and Family Living, 28 (3), 213-218.
People and Their Built Environment, eds. James S. Fried, Marc (1963), "Grieving for a Lost Home," in The
Duncan and Nancy C. Duncan, The Hague, The Neth- Urban Condition: People and Policy in the Metropolis,
erlands: Mouton, 391-401. ed. Leonard J. Duhl, New York: Basic Books, 151-171.
164 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Friedmann, Erika and Sue A. Thomas (1985), "Health Ben- Hickrod, Lucy J. and Raymond L. Schmitt (1982), "A Nat-
efits of Pets for Families," Pets and the Family, ed. uralistic Study ofInteraction and Frame: The Pet," Ur-
Marvin B. Sussman, New York: Haworth, 191-204. ban Life, 11 (April), 55-77.
Fromm, Erich (1976), To Have or To Be, New York: Harper Hobart, Charles W. (1975), "Ownership of Matrimonial
& Row. Property: A Study of Practices and Attitudes," Review
Fulton, Julie, Robert Fulton, and Roberta Simmons (1977), and Analysis of Sociology and Anthropology, 12 (4),
"The Cadaver Donor and the Gift of Life," Gift ofLife: Part I, 440-452.
The Social and Psychological Impact of Organ Trans- Holman, Rebecca (1981), "Product Use as Communica-
plantation, eds. Roberta G. Simmons et aI., New York: tion," in Review of Marketing, eds. Ben M. Enis and
John Wiley, 338-376. Kenneth J. Roering, Chicago, IL: American Marketing
Furby, Lita (1978), "Sharing: Decisions and Moral Judg- Association, 106-119.
ments About Letting Others Use One's Possessions," Horn, Jack C. and Jeff Meer (1984), "The Pleasure of Their
Psychological Reports, 43 (2), 595-609. Company," Psychology Today, 18 (August), 52-58.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


- - (1980), "The Origins and Early Development ofpos- Horney, Karen (1964), The Neurotic Personality of Our
sessive Behavior," Political Psychology, 2 (1),30-42. Time, New York: W.W. Norton.
- - (1982), "Some Aspects of Possessive Behavior Dur- Howell, Sandra C. (1983), "The Meaning of Place in Old
ing the Second Year of Life," paper presented at Asso- Age," in Aging and Milieu: Environmental Perspectives
ciation for Consumer Research Annual Conference, on Growing Old, eds. Graham D. Rowles and Russell J.
San Francisco, CA. Ohta, New York: Academic Press, 97-107.
- - and Mary Wilke (1982), "Some Characteristics of Isaacs, Susan (1933), Social Development in Young Chil-
Infants' Preferred Toys," Journal of Genetic Psychol- dren, London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
ogy, 140 (June), 207-219. - - (1935), "Property and Possessiveness," British Jour-
Furnham, Adrian and Alan Lewis (1986), The Economic nal ofMedical Psychology, 15 (1), 69-78.
Mind: The Social Psychology of Economic Behavior, Jacobson, Eugene and Jerome Kossoff (1963), "Self-Per-
New York: St. Martin's. cept and Consumer Attitudes Toward Small Cars,"
Geist, Christopher D. (1978), "Historic Sites and Monu- Journal ofApplied Psychology, 47 (August) 242-245.
ments as Icons," in Icons of America, eds. Ray B. Jager, Bernd (1983), "Body, House, City or the Intertwin-
Browne and Marshall Fishwick, Bowling Green, OH: ings of Embodiment, In Habitation," in The Changing
Bowling Green University Popular Press, 57-66. Reality ofModern Man, ed. Dreyer Kruger, Pittsburgh,
Gerson, K., C.A. Stueve, and Claude S. Fischer (1977), "At- PA: Dusquene University Press, 51-59.
tachment to Place," in Network and Places: Social Re- James, William (1890), The Principles of Psychology, Vol.
lations in the Urban Setting, eds. Claude S. Fischer et 1, New York: Henry Holt.
aI., New York: Free Press.
Jones, Ernest (1948), "Anal-Erotic Character Traits," Pa-
Goffman, Erving (1961), Asylums, New York: Doubleday. pers on Psycho-Analysis, London: Maresfield Reprints,
- - (1971), Relations in Public: Microstudies ofthe Pub- 413-437.
lic Order, New York: Basic Books.
Goldberg, Herb and Robert T. Lewis (1978), Money Mad- Kant, Immanuel (1798), Anthropologie in Pragmatisher
ness: The Psychology ofSaving, Spending, Loving, and Hinsicht, I. XXXII., Konigsberg: F. Nicolovins.
Hating Money, New York: New American Library. Kastenbaum, Robert (1977), "Memories of Tommorrow:
Goldner, Fred N. (1982), "Pronoia," Social Problems, 30 On the Interpretations of Time in Later Life," in The
(October), 82-91. Personal Experience of Time, eds. Bernard S. Gorman
Goldstein, Judith L. (1987), "Lifestyles of the Rich and Ty- and Alden E. Wessman, New York: Plenum, 193-214.
rannical," American Scholar, 56 (2), 235-247. Kates, Don B., Jr. and Nicole Varzos (1987), "Aspects of
Graves, Robert (1965), Mammon and the Black Goddess, the Priapic Theory of Gun Ownership," paper pre-
Garden City, NY: Doubleday. sented at the 1987 Popular Culture Association Meet-
Greenbaum, Paul E. and Susan D. Greenbaum (1965), ings, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
"Territorial Personalization: Group Identity and So- Katz, Judith M. (1976), "How Do You Love Me? Let Me
cial Interaction in a Slavic-American Neighborhood," Count the Ways (The Phenomenology of Being
Environment and Behavior, 13 (September), 574-589. Loved)," Sociological Inquiry, 46 (1),17-22.
Grubb, Edward L. and Gregg Hupp (1968), "Perception of Keddie, Kenneth (1977), "Pathological Mourning after the
Self, Generalized Stereotypes, and Brand Selection," Death of a Domestic Pet," British Journal of Psychia-
Journal ofMarketing Research, 5 (1), 58-63. try, 131 (1),21-25.
Hansen, William B. and Irwin Altman (1976), "Decorating Kidd, Aline H. and Robert M. Kidd (1980), "Personality
Personal Places: A Descriptive Analysis," Environment Characteristics and Preferences in Pet Ownership,"
and Behavior, 8 (December), 491-504. Psychological Reports, 46 (3, Part 1),939-949.
Hart, Roger (1979), Children's Experience of Place, New Klein, Melanie (1957), Envy and Gratitude, London: Tavis-
York: Irvington. tock.
Heiman, Marcel (1967), "Man and His Pet," in Motiva- Klein, Susan D. and Roberta G. Simmons (1977), "The
tions in Play, Games, and Sports, eds. Ralph Slovenko Psychosocial Impact of Chronic Kidney Disease in
and James A. Knight, Springfield, IL: Charles C Children," in Gift ofLife: The Social and Psychological
Thomas, 329-348. Impact of Transplantation, eds. Roberta G. Simmons
Herskovitz, Melville J. (1952), Economic Anthropology: et aI., New York: John Wiley, 89-118.
The Economic Life ofPrimitive Man, New York: W. W. Kline, Linus W. and C.J. France (1899), "The Psychology
Norton. of Ownership," Pedagogical Seminary, 6 (4), 421-470.
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 165

Knight, James A. (1968), For Love of Money: Human Be- Lynes, Russell (1980), The Tastemakers: The Shaping of
havior and Money, Phildelphia, P A: J.B. Lippincott. American Popular Taste, New York: Dover.
Kopytoff, Igor (1986), "The Cultural Biography of Things: Macfarlane, Allan (1978), The Origin of English Individu-
Commoditization as Process," in The Social Life of alism: The Family, Property and Social Transitions,
Things: Commodities in Cultural Perspective, ed. Ar- Oxford, England: Blackwell.
jun Appadurai, Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- Macke, Anne S., George W. Bohrnstedt, and Ilene N. Bern-
versity Press, 64-91. stein (1979), "Housewives' Self-Esteem and Their Hus-
Korosec-Serfaty, Perla (1984), "The Home From Attic to bands' Success: The Myth of Vicarious Involvement,"
Cellar," Journal of Environmental Psychology, 4 (2), Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41 (February),
303-321. 51-57.
Kron, Joan (1981), Home Psych: The Social Psychology of Maguire, Mike (1980), "The Impact of Burglary Upon Vic-
Home and Decor, New York: Clarkson N. Potter. tims," British Journal of Criminology, 20 (July), 261-
Krueger, David W., ed. (1986), The Last Taboo: Money as 275.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


Symbol and Reality in Psychotherapy and Psychoanal- Maines, David H. (1978), "Bodies and Selves: Notes on a
ysis, New York: Brunner/Mazel. Fundamental Dilemma in Demography," in Studies in
Kutash, Irwin L., Samuel B. Kutash, Louis B. Schlesinger, Symbolic Interaction, ed. Norman K. Denzen, Green-
and Associates (1978), Violence: Perspectives on Mur- wich, CT: JAI, 241-265.
der and Aggression, San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Marchand, Roland (1985), Advertising and the American
La Branche, Anthony (1973), "Neglected and Unused Dream: Making Way for Modernity, 1920-1940,
Things: Narrative Encounter," Review of Existential Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 12 (2), 163-168. Maringer, Johannes (1960), The Gods of Prehistoric Man,
Leach, Robin (1986), Lifestyles of the Rich and Famous, trans. Mary Ilford, New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Marris, Peter (1986), Loss and Change, London: Routledge
Leaky, Richard E. (1981), The Making of Mankind, New & Kegan Paul.
York: E.P. Dutton. Martinez, Robin L. and Aline H. Kidd (1980), "Two Per-
Lessing, Alfred (1965), "What is Wrong with Forgery?," sonality Characteristics of Adult Pet Owners," Psycho-
Journal ofAesthetics and Art Criticism, 23 (Summer), logical Reports, 47 (September), 318.
461-471. Marx, Karl (1964; original 1848), Communist Manifesto,
Levi-Strauss, Claude (1963), Totemism, Boston, MA: Bea- New York: Washington Square Press.
con. - - (1967; original 1842), "The Centralization Ques-
Levinson, Borris M. (1972), Pets and Human Development, tion," Writings of the Young Marx on Philosophy and
Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas. Society, trans. Lloyd D. Easton and Kurt Guddat, Gar-
Lewinsky, Jan, St. (1913), The Origin of Property: and the den City, NY: Anchor.
Formation of the Village Community, London: Con- - - (1975; original 1844), "Critique of Hegelian Philos-
stable. ophy of the Right (Introduction)," Deutsch-Fau-
Lewis, Michael and Jeanne Brooks (1978), "Self, Other, zoeischwe Yahrbuecher/Karl Marx: Early Writings,
New York: Vantage.
and Fear: Infants' Reactions to People," in The Origins
of Fear, eds. Michael Lewis and Leonard A. Rosen- - - (1978; original 1867), Capital: A Critique ofPolitical
baum, New York: John Wiley, 165-194. Economy, Vol. 1, trans. Ben Fawkes, Harmondsworth,
England: Penguin.
Ley, David and Roman Cybriwsky (1974), "Urban Graffiti
as Territorial Markers," Annals of the Association of Mason, Roger (1981) Conspicuous Consumption: A Study
American Geographers, 6 (December), 491-505. of Exceptional Consumer Behavior, Westmead, En-
gland: Gower.
Lifton, Robert J. (1973), "The Sense of Immortality: On
Death and the Continuity of Life," American Journal Mazur, Ronald (1977), "Beyond Jealousy and Possessive-
ness," Jealousy, eds. Gordon Clanton and Lynn G.
ofPsychoan lays is, 33 (1), 3-15.
Smith, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 181-189.
Lindgren, Henry C. (1980), Great Expectations: The Psy- McCarthy, E. Doyle (1984), "Toward a Sociology of the
chologyofMoney, Los Altos, CA: William Kaufmann. Physical World: George Herbert Mead on Physical Ob-
Litwinski, Leon (1956), "Belongingness as a Unifying Con- jects," in Studies in Symbolic Interaction, ed. Norman
cept in Personality Investigation," Acta Psychologica, K. Denzen, Greenwich, CT: JAI, 105-121.
12 (July), 130-135. McClelland, David (1951), Personality, New York: Holt,
Locke, John (1690), Two Treatises of Government, Oxford, Rinehart, & Winston.
England: Oxford University Press. McCracken, Grant (1986), "Culture and Consumption: A
Lowenthal, David (1975), "Past Time, Present Place: Land- Theoretical Account of the Structure and Movement
scape and Memory," Geographical Review, 65 (1), 1- of the Cultural Meaning of Consumer Goods," Journal
36. ofConsumer Research, 13 (June), 71-84.
Luft, Joseph (1957), "Monetary Value and the Perception - - (1987a), "Culture and Consumption Among the El-
of Persons," Journal ofSocial Psychology, 46 (Novem- derly: Three Research Objectives in an Emerging
ber), 245-251. Field," Aging and Society, 7 (2), 203-224. .
Lurie, Alison (1981), The Language of Clothes, New York: - - (1987b), '''Homeyness' Culture Made Material in
Random House. the Modern North American Home," Working Paper
Lyman, Stanford M. and Marvin B. Scott (1967), "Territo- No. 87-105, Department of Consumer Studies, Uni-
riality: A Neglected Sociological Dimension," Social versity of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada N 1G
Problems, 15 (Fall), 236-249. 2W1.
166 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

- - (ed.), "The Evocative Power of Things: Consumer Paap, Warren R. (1981), "Being Burglarized: An Account
Goods and the Preservation of Hopes and Ideals," in of Victimization," Victimology: An International Jour-
Culture and Consumption: New Approaches to the nal, 6 (1-4), 297-305.
Symbolism of Consumer Goods and Activities, Bloom- Parker, Robert (1982), "The Brand Finale," American Way
ington, IN: Indiana University Press, 104-107. (July),37-40.
McIntyre, Pat, Mark A. Barnett, Richard J. Harris, James Parkes, Colin M. (1972), Bereavement: Studies of Grief in
Shanteau, JoJ;m Skowronski, and Michael Klassen Adult Life, New York: International Universities Press.
(1987), "Psychological Factors Influencing Decisions Paul, Ellen F. and Jeffrey Paul (1979), "Self-Ownership,
to Donate Organs," in Advances in Consumer Re- Abortion, and Infantacide," Journal ofMedical Ethics,
search, Vol. 14, eds. Melanie Wallendorf and Paul An- 5 (2),133-138.
derson, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Re- Piaget, Jean (1932), The Moral Judgment ofthe Child, Lon-
search, 331-334. don: Routlege & Kegan Paul.
McLeod, Beverly (1984), "In the Wake of Disaster," Psy- Pessemier, Edgar A., Albert C. Bemmaor, and Dominique

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


chology Today, 18 (October), 54-57. M. Hanssens (1977), "Willingness to Supply Human
Meer, Jeff (1984), "Pet Theories," Psychology Today, 18 Body Parts: Some Empirical Results," Journal ofCon-
(August), 60-67. sumer Research, 4 (December), 131-140.
Milgram, Stanley (1976), "The Image-Freezing Machine," Plugge, Herbert (1970), "The Ambiguity of Having and Be-
Society, 14 (1), 7-12. ing a Body," Human Inquiries, 10 (1-3),132-139.
Montemayor, Raymond and Marvin Eisen (1977), "The Pogrebin, Mark R., Eric D. Poole, and Robert M. Regoli
Development of Self-Conceptions from Childhood to (1986), "Natural Death and Unknown Persons: The
Adolescence," Developmental Psychology, 13 (4), 314- Process of Creating Identity," Social Science Journal,
319. 23 (4), 391-396.
Mueller, Edward (1978), "(Toddlers + Toys) = (An Auton- Prelinger, Ernst (1959), "Extension and Structure of the
omous System)," in The Social Network ofthe Develop- Self," Journal ofPsychology, 47 (January), 13-23.
ing Infant, eds. Michael Lewis and Leonard A. Rosen- Proshansky, Harold W. (1978), "The City and Self-Iden-
blum, New York: Plenum, 169-194. tity," Environment and Behavior, 10 (June), 147-169.
- - and Jeffrey Brenner (1977), "The Origins of Social Prottas, Jeffrey M. (1983), "Encouraging Altruism: Public
Skills and Interaction Among Playgroup Toddlers," Attitudes and the Marketing of Organ Donation,"
Child Development, 48 (September), 854-861. Health and Society, 61 (2),278-306.
Mukerji, Chandra (1978), "Artwork: Collection and Con- Rathje, William L. and Randall H. McGuire (1982), "Rich
temporary Culture," American Journal of Sociology, Men ... Poor Men," American Behavioral Scientist,
84 (8), 348-365. 25 (6), 705-715.
Myers, Elizabeth (1985), "Phenomenological Analysis of Rapoport, Amos (1981), "Identity and The Environment:
the Importance of Special Possessions: An Exploratory A Cross-Cultural Perspective," in Housing and Iden-
Study," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 12, tity: Cross Cultural Perspectives, ed. James Duncan,
eds. Elizabeth C. Hirschman and Morris B. Holbrook, London: Croom Helm, 6-35.
Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 560- Richins, Marsha and Peter Bloch (1986), "After the New
565. Wears Off: The Temporal Context of Product Involve-
New Yorker (1975), "Male Potency and the Dow Jones In- ment," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (Septem-
dustrial Average," New Yorker, 51 (October 20), 10- ber),280-285.
12. Rigby, Douglas and Elizabeth Rigby (1949), Lock, Stock
Niederland, William G. (1967), "Clinical Aspects of Cre- and Barrel: The Story of Collecting, Philadelphia, PA:
ativity," American Imago, 24 (1-2), 6-34. J.B. Lippincott.
- - and Bahman Sholevar (1981), "The Creative Pro- Robin, Michael and Robert Bensel (1985), "Pets and the
cess-A Psychoanalytic Discussion," The Arts in Psy- Socialization of Children," in Pets and the Family, ed.
chotherapy, 8 (1), 71-10 1. Marvin B. Sussman, New York: Haworth, 63-78.
Nuttin, Jozef M., Jr. (1987), "Affective Consequences of Rochberg-Halton, Eugene (1984), "Object Relations, Role
Mere Ownership: The Name Letter Effect in Twelve Models, and Cultivation of the Self," Environment and
European Languages," European Journal of Social Behavior, 16 (3), 335-368.
Psychology, 17 (4), 381-402. - - (1985), "Life in the Treehouse: Pet Therapy as Fam-
Olson, Clark D. (1981), "Artifacts in the Home and Rela- ily Metaphor and Self-Dialogue," in Pets and the Fam-
tional Communication: A Preliminary Report," milS- ily, ed. Marvin B. Sussman, New York: Haworth, 175-
ter's thesis, Department of Psychology, University of 190.
Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84060. - - (1986), Meaning and Modernity, Chicago, IL: Uni-
- - (1985), "Materialism in the Home: The Impact of versity of Chicago Press.
Artifacts on Dyadic Communication," in Advances in Rook, Dennis (1985), "Body Cathexis and Market Segmen-
Consumer Research, Vol. 12, eds. Elizabeth C. Hirsch- tation," The Psychology of Fashion, ed. Michael R.
man and Morris B. Holbrook, Provo, UT: Association Solomon, Lexington, MA: Lexington, 233-242.
for Consumer Research, 388-393. Rooney, Andrew A. (1984), Pieces ofMy Mind, New York:
O'Reilly, Lynn, Margaret Rucker, Rhonda Hughes, Marge Atheneum, 3-42.
Gorang, and Susan Hand (1984), "The Relationship of Rosenbaum, Jean (1972), Is Your Volkswagen a Sex Sym-
Psychological and Situational Variables to Usage of a bol? New York: Hawthorn.
Second-Order Marketing System," Journal of the Rosenblatt, Paul c., R. Patricia Walsh, and Douglas A.
Academy ofMarketing Science, 12 (3),53-76. Jackson (1976), Grief and Mourning in Cross-Cultural
POSSESSIONS AND THE EXTENDED SELF 167

Perspective, New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Snyder, Eldon E. (1972), "High School Student Perceptions
Files. of Prestige Criteria," Adolescence, 7 (Spring), 129-136.
Rozin, Paul, Linda Millman, and Carol Nemeroff (1986), Snyder, Mark, Ellen Berscheid, and Peter Glick (1985),
"Operation of Laws of Sympathetic Magic in Disgust "Focusing on the Interior and the Exterior: Two Inves-
and Other Domains," Journal of Personality and So- tigations of Personal Relationships," Journal of Per-
cial Psychology, 50 (April), 703-712. sonality and Social Psychology, 48 (6), 1427-1439.
Rubinstein, Carin (1981), "Money & Self-Esteem, Rela- Solomon, Michael (1986a), "Deep-Seated Materialism:
tionships, Secrecy, Envy, Satisfaction," Psychology The Case of Levi's 501 Jeans," in Advances in Con-
Today, 15 (May), 29-32 and additional pages. sumer Research, Vol 13, ed. Richard J. Lutz, Provo,
Rudmin, Floyd, Russell W. Belk, and Lita Furby (1987), UT: Association for Consumer Research, 520-521.
Social Science Bibliography on Property, Ownership - - (1986b), "The Missing Link: Surrogate Consumers
and Possession: 1580 Citations from Psychology, An- in the Marketing Chain," Journal ofMarketing, 50 (4),
thropology, Sociology, and Related Disciplines, Monti- 208-218.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


cello, IL: Vance Bibliographies. - - and Punam Anand (1985), "Ritual Costumes and
Saarinen, Aline B. (1958), The Proud Possessors, New Status Transition: The Female Business Suit as To-
York: Random House. temic Emblem," in Advances in Consumer Research,
Sacks, Oliver (1985), The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Vol. 12, eds. Elizabeth C. Hirschman and Morris B.
Hat and Other Clinical Tales, New York: Summit Holbrook, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Re-
Books. search,315-318.
Saile, David G. (1985), "The Ritual Establishment of the - - and Henry Assael (1988), "The Forest or the Trees?
Home," in Home Environments, eds. Irwin Altman A Gestalt Approach to Symbolic Communication," in
and Carol Werner, New York: Plenum, 87-111. Marketing and Semiotics: New Directions in the Study
Sanders, Clinton (1988), "Marks of Mischief: Becoming of Signs for Sale, eds. Jean Umiker-Sebeok and Sidney
and Being a Tattooed Person," Journal of Contempo- J. Levy, Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press,
rary Ethnography, 16 (January), 395-432. 189-218.
Sartre, Jean-Paul (1943), Being and Nothingness: A Phe- Sommer, Robert (1971), "Spatial Parameters in Naturalis-
nomenological Essay on Ontology, New York: Philo- tic Social Research," in Behavior and Environment:
sophical Library. The Use of Space by Animals, ed. Aristide H. Esser,
Schilder, Paul (1950), The Image and Appearance of the New York: Plenum, 281-290.
Human Body, New York: International Universities Sontag, Susan (1973), On Photography, New York: Farrar,
Press. Straus, and Giroux.
Secord, Paul F. (1968), "Consistency Theory and Self-Ref- Starobinski, Jean (1966), "The Idea of Nostalgia," Dio-
erent Behavior," Theories of Cognitive Consistency: A genes, 54 (Summer), 81-103.
Sourcebook, eds. Robert P. Ableson et aI., Chicago, IL: Stein, Benjamin (1985), "The Machine Makes This Man,"
Rand McNally, 349-354. Wall Street Journal, 205 (June 13),30.
- - and Sidney M. Jourard (1953), "The Appraisal of Steinbeck, John (1939), Grapes of Wrath, New York: Vik-
Body-Cathexis: Body-Cathexis and the Self," Journal ing.
ofConsulting Psychology, 17 (5),343-347.
Stewart, Susan (1984), On Longing: Narratives of the Min-
Seligman, Martin E.P. (1975), Helplessness, San Francisco,
iature, the Gigantic, the Collection, Baltimore, MD:
CA: Freeman.
John Hopkins.
Sherman, Edmund and Evelyn S. Newman (1977), "The
Meaning of Cherished Personal Possessions for the El- Tanay, Emanuel with Lucy Freeman (1976), The Murder-
derly," International Journal ofAging and Human De- ers, Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill, 25-43.
velopment, 8 (2), 181-192. Thukral, Vinod K. and Gaylord Cummins (1987), "The Vi-
Simmel, Georg (1950), The Sociology of Georg Simmel, tal Organ Shortage," in Advances in Nonprofit Market-
trans. Kurt H. Wolff, Glencoe, IL: Free Press. ing, Vol. 2, ed. Russell W. Belk, Greenwich, CT: JAI,
Simmons, Roberta L., Diane Bush, and Susan Klein (1977), 159-174.
"The Nondonor: Motives and Characteristics," in Gift Titcomb, Margaret (1969), Dog and Man in the Ancient Pa-
of Life: The Social and Psychological Impact of Organ cific, Honolulu, HI: Bernice P. Bishop Museum Special
Transplantation, eds. Roberta G. Simmons et aI., New Publication, 59.
York: John Wiley, 198-232. Titmus, Richard M. (1970), The Gift Relationship, Lon-
- - , Susan D. Klein, and Richard L. Simmons, eds. don: Allen and Unwin.
(1977), "Social and Psychological Rehabilitation of the Tonnies, Ferdinand (1957), Community and Society, trans.
Adult Transplant Patient," Gift ofLife: The Social and C. Loomis, East Lansing, MI: Michigan State Univer-
Psychological Impact of Organ Transplantation, New sity Press.
York: John Wiley, 48-70. Tournier, Paul (1957), The Meaning of Persons, trans. Ed-
Sirgy, Joseph (1982), "Self-Concept in Consumer Behavior: win Hudson, New York: Harper & Row.
A Critical Review," Journal of Consumer Research, 9 Treas, Charles E. and Dalton E. Brannen (1976), "The
(December),287-300. Growing Collector Market," Proceedings, eds. Henry
Smith, Janet F. (1983), "Parenting and Property," in Moth- W. Nash and Donald P. Robin, Mississippi State, MS:
ering: Essays in Feminist Theory, ed. Joyce Trebilcot, Southern Marketing Association, 234-236.
Totowa, NJ: Rowman & Allanheld, 199-212. Trivers, Robert L. (1971), "The Evolution of Reciprocal Al-
Snyder, C.R. and Howard L. Fromkin (1981), Uniqueness: truism," The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46 (4), 35-
Human Pursuit ofDifference, New York: Plenum. 39.
168 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Tuan, Yi-Fu (1978), Space and Place: The Perspective of Weisberg, Paul and James E. Russell (1971), "Proximity
Experience, Minneapolis, MN: University of Minne- and Interactional Behavior of Young Children to Their
sota Press. 'Security' Blankets," Child Development, 42 (Novem-
- - (1980), "The Significance of the Artifact," Geo- ber),1575-1579.
graphical Review, 70 (4),462-472. Weisner, ThomasandJoanC. Weibel (1981), "Home Envi-
- - (1984), Dominance & Affection: The Making ofPets, ronments and Lifestyles in California," Environment
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. and Behavior, 13 (July), 417-460.
Turner, Bryan S. (1984), The Body and Society: Explora- White, Robert W. (1959), "Motivation Reconsidered: The
tions in Social Theory, Oxford, England: Basil Black- Concept of Competence," Psychological Review, 66
well. (5),297-333.
Unruh, David R. (1983), "Death and Personal History: Whitehurst, Robert N. (1977), "Jealousy and American
Strategies of Identity Preservation," Social Problems, Values," in Jealousy, eds. Gordon Clanton and Lynn
30 (3), 340-351. G. Smith, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 136-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcr/article/15/2/139/1841428 by guest on 02 November 2021


Veblen, Thorstein (1898), "The Beginnings of Ownership, " 139.
American Journal ofSociology, 4 (3),352-365. Whiting, John W. (1960), "Resource Mediation and Learn-
- - (1899), The Theory ofthe Leisure Class, New York, ing by Identification," Personality Development in
MacMillan. Children, eds. Ira Iscoe and Harold W. Stevenson, Aus-
Veevers, Jean E. (1985), "The Social Meaning of Pets: Al- tin, TX: University of Texas Press, 113-125.
ternative Roles for Companion Animals," Pets and Wicklund, Robert A. and Peter M. Gollwitzer (1982), Sym-
Family, ed. Marvin B. Sussman, New York: Haworth, bolic Self Completion, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
11-30. baum Associates.
Volkan, Vanik D. (1974), "The Linking Objects ofPatho- Wiggins, James W. (1974), "The Decline of Private Prop-
logical Mourners," Normal and Pathological Re- erty and the Diminished Person," in Property in a Hu-
sponses to Bereavement, eds. John Ellard et aI., New mane Economy, ed. Samuel L. Blumenfeld, LaSalle,
York: MSS Information Corporation, 186-202. IL: Open Court, 71-84.
Wallendorf, Melanie and Eric Arnould (1988), "My Favor- Wikse, John R. (1977), About Possession: The Self as Pri-
ite Things: A Cross-Cultural Inquiry into Object At- vate Property, University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State
tachment, Possessiveness and Social Linkage," Journal University Press.
of Consumer Research, 14 (March), 531-547. Wilms, Grant, Stephen W. Kiefer, James Shanteau, and Pa-
- - and Russell Belk (1987), "Deep Meaning in Posses- tricia McIntyre (1987), "Knowledge and Image of
sions," video, Cambridge, MA: Marketing Science In- Body Organs: Impact on Willingness to Donate," in
stitute. Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 14, eds. Melanie
Wasson, R. Gordon (1972), Soma and the Fly-Agaric, Wallendorf and Paul Anderson, Provo, UT: Associa-
Cambaridge, MA: Botanical Museum of Harvard Uni- tion for Consumer Research, 338-341.
versity. Winicott, D.W. (1953), "Transitional Objects and Transi-
Weathers, Mary B. (1978), "Perceptions of Prestige Bor- tional Phenomena," International Journal of Psycho-
rowing in Relationship to Occupational Aspiration and analysis, 34 (2), 89-97.
Career Commitment in College Senior Women," un- Wiseman, Thomas (1974), The Money Motive, New York:
published dissertation, University of Miami, Coral Ga- Random House.
bles, FL 33124. Yamauchi, Kent T. and Donald I. Templer (1982), "The
Weiland, J. Hyman (1955), "The Adolescent and the Auto- Development of a Money Attitude Scale," Journal of
mobile," Chicago Review, 9 (Fall), 61-64. Personality Assessment, 46 (5), 522-528.

You might also like