You are on page 1of 8

Creativity Research Journal

ISSN: 1040-0419 (Print) 1532-6934 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcrj20

Evidence of the Psychometric Integrity of the


Creative Personality Scale using Item Response
Theory Models

Meihua Qian, Jonathan A. Plucker & Xiangdong Yang

To cite this article: Meihua Qian, Jonathan A. Plucker & Xiangdong Yang (2019): Evidence of
the Psychometric Integrity of the Creative Personality Scale using Item Response Theory Models,
Creativity Research Journal, DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2019.1647758

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2019.1647758

View supplementary material

Published online: 04 Aug 2019.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 17

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hcrj20
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 0(0), 1–7, 2019
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1040-0419 print/1532-6934 online
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2019.1647758

RESEARCH NOTE

Evidence of the Psychometric Integrity of the Creative


Personality Scale using Item Response Theory Models
Meihua Qian
Clemson University

Jonathan A. Plucker
Johns Hopkins University

Xiangdong Yang
East China Normal University

Gough’s Creative Personality Scale (CPS) has been very widely used to assess creative
personality characteristics, and many researchers have argued that it is associated with strong
reliability and validity evidence. However, findings vary considerably across the samples
used in each study, suggesting that an analysis using the item response theory framework
would provide more useful evidence of the instrument’s psychometric integrity. Results
suggest that the CPS is multidimensional, and some items have low discrimination indices.
Additionally, significant correlations between participants’ trait estimates (i.e., estimated
creative personality) and their engagement in a range of creative activities indicate that the
CPS is associated with convincing evidence of criterion-related validity.

The Creative Personality Scale (CPS; Gough, 1979) has examine a person’s self-concept. Later six subscales were
been extremely widely used, with this popularity due pri- developed to investigate creativity (e.g., MacKinnon,
marily to its ease of administration, ease of scoring, and 1963), but evidence of validity for these subscales was
generally convincing evidence of reliability and validity lacking (Gough, 1979). Therefore, with a sample of
(e.g., Hocevar, 1981; Kadusa & Schaefer, 1991; Sheldon, 1,701 architects, mathematicians, research scientists,
1995; Zhou & Oldham, 2001). But the evidence concerning senior engineers, and graduate students in psychology,
its reliability and validity varies considerably across the Gough used the ACL to develop another subscale, the
samples used in each study. CPS.
Specifically, the CPS was derived from the Adjective Gough first divided the subjects into four subgroups
Check List (ACL; Gough & Heilbrun, 1965), a self- based on gender and field in which each person worked,
report measure which itself was originally intended to and the subjects completed the ACL. Second, the corre-
lations between each item and the criterion ratings of
creativity in the subgroups were calculated. If the cor-
Address correspondence to Meihua Qian, College of Education, relations were statistically significant, the corresponding
Clemson University, 410 Tillman Hall, Clemson, SC 29634. E-mail:
mqian@g.clemson.edu
items would be included (Gough, 1979). For example,
The authors have no conflicts of interest to report and confirm that the the item “original” produced correlations with the var-
data used for this article have not been used in any previous reports. ious criterion ratings of creativity of .13, .06, .17 and
Meihua Qian, College of Education, Clemson University; Jonathan .11 in the four subgroups. Two of these coefficients
A. Plucker, Center for Talented Youth and School of Education, Johns were statistically significant (p < .05). Although the
Hopkins University; Xiangdong Yang, School of Education Science, East
other two coefficients were not significant, they were
China Normal University.
2 QIAN ET AL.

positive, and the item “original” is consistent with the difference between his/her creative personality trait level
definition of creative personality (Gough, 1979). For all (θs) and the item location parameter (bi), and the item
of these reasons, it was included in the CPS. Another 29 discrimination parameter (αi).
items were similarly selected for the final scale. Among IRT provides a more complete theoretical framework
the 30 items, there were 12 contraindicative and 18 than CTT for checking items’ properties. However, the
indicative adjectives. Internal consistency estimates, as benefits of IRT techniques are associated with a close
measured by Cronbach’s Alpha, ranged from .73 to .81 match between the model and data. Three fundamental
for the four subgroups. In addition, 10 out of 12 criter- assumptions underlie item response models. They are
ion validity coefficients for the CPS were significant at monotonicity, dimensionality, and local independence.
the 0.05 level, but all of these validity coefficients Monotonicity means that the higher the examinee’s abil-
varied from subgroup to subgroup, ranging widely ity, the greater the probability of getting an item correct.
from 0.15 to 0.42. As many tests inherently meet with this assumption, it is
This lack of invariance in reliability and validity esti- often assumed (Nandakumar & Ackerman, 2004).
mates across samples is a cause for concern and, given According to Embretson and Reise (2000), dimensional-
the continued popularity of the instrument, the motiva- ity and local independence are the two major assump-
tion for the current study. During many phases of the tions that should be checked. Local independence implies
instrument’s development, from the item selection pro- that for an examinee, his/her response to an item only
cess to the calculation of reliability and validity evi- depends on his or her trait level and has nothing to do
dence, sampling impacted the development of the CPS. with his or her response to other test items or other
In order to obtain item estimates independent of common factors. But actually item responses are condi-
a specific sample, the CPS was reexamined using the tionally independent. If the test is unidimensional, then
item response theory (IRT) framework. In IRT, item local independence is achieved by conditioning on
properties such as difficulty and discrimination can be a unidimensional latent trait. If the test is multidimen-
estimated independent of the ability level of the exam- sional, then local independence can be achieved by con-
inee (e.g., Nandakumar & Ackerman, 2004), given that ditioning on a multidimensional latent trait vector.
the corresponding IRT model fits the data. In other Therefore, local independence and dimensionality are
words, item statistics are invariant across samples. This two interrelated concepts, and only one of them needs
is a great advantage over the traditional classical test to be tested. In practice, local independence is usually
theory (CTT), in which items statistics rely heavily on assumed, and dimensionality is tested. The aim of this
the characteristics of each sample and thus might lack paper was to reexamine the psychometric properties of
invariance across samples (e.g., Embretson & Reise, the CPS using the IRT framework.
2000; Nandakumar & Ackerman, 2004).

STUDY 1: FACTOR AND IRT ANALYSIS STUDY


IRT models
There are a variety of IRT models. Based on various data Method
formats, there are dichotomous IRT models and polyto- Participants and procedure
mous IRT models. According to the number of latent
traits included in the model, there exist unidimensional Participants consisted of 220 undergraduate students and
and multidimensional IRT models. IRT models can also 139 graduate students who attended two leading universi-
be classified into one-parameter logistic (1PL) model, ties in East China. They (68% female) represented a wide
two-parameter logistic (2PL) model, or three-parameter range of majors, such as psychology, sociology, biology,
logistic (3PL) model based on the number of item para- chemistry, engineering, arts, and music.
meters included in the model. Since the CPS generates Students were first instructed to sign a consent form to
binary responses, and it is not an achievement test, a 2PL participate in the study, and then they were instructed to
IRT model for dichotomous data will be used to estimate complete the measures by one of the researchers in their
its item properties. The detailed model specification is as classrooms. In the meantime, students were reminded
follows: that their participation was completely voluntary and
they could withdraw from the study at any time.
PðXis ¼1j θs ; bi ; αi Þ¼ exp½1:7αi ðθs bi Þ=
Measures
f1 þ exp½1:7αi ðθs bi Þg :
The Creative Personality Scale (Gough, 1979) was
For any person s, the probability of endorsing employed in the study, which is one of the most widely
a particular CPS item (i.e., P (Xi = 1)) is based on the used instruments for measuring a person’s creative
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 3

personality (e.g., Sheldon, 1995). It consists of 18 posi- TABLE 1


tive and 12 negative items. All the items have binary Exploratory factor analysis of the CPS
responses. A participant scored 1 by endorsing a positive Standardized Factor Loading
item and scored −1 by endorsing a negative item. In
addition, items were translated into Chinese by a group Item Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3
of researchers who had years of experience of studying Inventive 0.74
creativity and were fluent in both Chinese and English. Individualistic 0.71
The conceptual equivalence of the English and Chinese Original 0.67
versions was examined and achieved through back- Sexy 0.60
Insightful 0.59
translation.
Confident 0.58
Clever 0.56
Informal 0.56
Results Intelligent 0.55
Unconventional 0.52
Dimensionality assessment Egotistical 0.51
Reflective 0.47
The unidimensionality of the CPS was examined via Humorous 0.42
exploratory factor analysis (EFA) using TESTFACT 4 Resourceful 0.41
software (e.g., Bock, Gibbons, & Murraki, 1988; Bock Self-confident 0.41
et al., 2002). As all the responses were binary, EFA was Interest-wide 0.37
Conservative 0.67
carried out based on the tetrochoric correlation matrix.
Conventional 0.66
The CPS appeared to be multidimensional because the Capable 0.65
first eigenvalue (6.16) was not five times as large as Honest 0.64
the second one (4.32), and the second and third eigenva- Well-mannered 0.62
lues (4.07) were very similar. In the end, three factors Cautious 0.52
Sincere 0.39
were extracted because the fourth eigenvalue (2.51)
Artificial 0.77
seemed to be much smaller than the third one. Snobbish 0.71
However, a further examination of several large residual Suspicious 0.68
correlations indicated that the addition of a fourth factor Submissive 0.57
might be necessary. Moreover, the chi-square decrease Dissatisfied 0.47
Narrow interest 0.35
(and the associated change in degrees of freedom) due to
Commonplace 0.32
adding the fourth factor was 68 (27), which was signifi-
cant at the .01 level. But after the fourth factor was
extracted, only two items loaded highly on it, and these
items also had nontrivial loadings on the third factor. In a unidimensional subscale, three 2PL IRT models were
other words, these two items did not seem to represent fitted to the data. Item parameters were estimated via the
a meaningful fourth dimension. Hence, the three-factor marginal maximum likelihood procedure, and the person
solution was adopted for subsequent analyses. Table 1 parameter was estimated through the expected
shows the factor loading of each item on the correspond- a posteriori approach.
ing factor. It seems factor 1 stands for positive creative All item characteristic curves (ICC) indicated excel-
personality characteristics (e.g., inventive, confident, and lent fit between observed values and predicted values
intelligent), factor 2 represents neutral creative personal- generated from the IRT model, with p-values much
ity characteristics (e.g., conventional, well-mannered, and greater than 0.05 for each chi-square test. In addition,
honest) and factor 3 represents negative creative person- concerning each model, both item parameter invariance
ality characteristics (e.g., snobbish, submissive, and nar- and person parameter invariance were achieved. For
row interest). instance, with regard to model one, b1 (i.e., the esti-
mated item location values based on the first random
half sample) was regressed on b2 (i.e., the estimated
IRT analyses
item location values based on the second random half
Since the pairwise correlations among the three sample), with a resulting standardized regression coeffi-
extracted factors were very weak (i.e., r < 0.1) and cient of 0.92. These results suggested that the estimates
were not statistically significant, unidimensional IRT of item location values were invariant across different
models instead of a multidimensional IRT model were samples. A similar strategy was used to examine item
employed to estimate each item’s discrimination and discrimination estimates, with a resulting standardized
difficulty (i.e., location) indices using the BILOG-MG3 regression coefficient of 0.89, indicating that the esti-
program. In other words, treating each factor as mates of item discrimination were also invariant across
4 QIAN ET AL.

different samples. Since item parameter invariance and by 1 over the square root of test information. The test
person parameter invariance for the other two IRT mod- information function (TIF) is the sum of the item infor-
els also appeared to hold, it seems reasonable to con- mation functions, and is a measure of how much infor-
clude a good fit between the constructed IRT model and mation a test provides at different trait levels. For
observed data was achieved (Hambleton, Swaminathan, a given item, its information function will reach its
& Rogers, 1991). maximum for individuals whose trait is at or near the
Table 2 displays the estimated item parameters. As the value of the location parameter.
participants’ trait estimates are not a focus of the present The TIF actually corresponds to reliability in the CTT
study, they are not listed below. Table 2 shows that most framework and the standard error of measurement.
items had moderate discrimination values, and eight items Reliability pertains to the precision of measurement,
(i.e., Egotistical, Reflective, Humorous, Resourceful, Wide and it is usually measured using a single index such as
Interests, Self-confident, Sincere, and Commonplace) had the ratio of true and observed score variance. This index
rather low discrimination values. According to Gough characterizes a test’s average reliability. But in the IRT
(1979), the first six items are positive and are consonant framework, the precision of measurement is not uniform
with the definition of creative personality, and the last two across the entire range of estimated trait levels.
items are contraindicative. Specifically, regarding subscale1, clearly items contribu-
The test information curve of each subscale can be ted the most information between theta values (i.e., esti-
found in the online supplemental materials. The standard mated creative personality trait) of −1.6 and 0, and little
error function was plotted on the same graph. It’s given information at the end points, where the standard error
rose sharply (see online supplemental materials). For
subscale 2, items offered the most information between
TABLE 2 theta values of −2.0 and −1.0, and less information
Item analyses of subscales 1–3 beyond this range (see online supplemental materials).
Item discrimination Item location Subscale 3 displays an interesting situation. The higher
the theta values were, the more information the items
a SE b SE provided (see online supplemental materials).
Subscale 1 Intelligent 1.53 0.40 −1.73 0.27
Inventive 1.28 0.41 −0.46 0.17
Original 1.20 0.35 −0.90 0.21 STUDY 2: VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY STUDY
Individualistic 1.00 0.23 −1.17 0.25
Clever 0.99 0.26 −1.53 0.35
Sexy 0.81 0.18 −0.03 0.19 Method
Confident 0.79 0.22 −1.09 0.32
Insightful 0.76 0.18 −0.87 0.24
Participants
Informal 0.71 0.16 −0.56 0.20 A subset of the participants described in study 1 were chosen
Unconventional 0.70 0.17 0.22 0.24
Egotistical 0.59 0.16 1.80 0.44
for study 2 because only 312 students completed both the CPS
Humorous 0.59 0.15 −1.44 0.42 and the Creative Behavior Inventory (Hocevar, 1979, 1980).
Reflective 0.58 0.13 −0.46 0.27
Self-confident 0.57 0.14 −0.80 0.32 Measures
Resourceful 0.52 0.14 −1.11 0.35
Wide-interests 0.51 0.14 −2.08 0.59 In addition to the CPS, participants were also asked to
Subscale 2 fill out the Creative Behavior Inventory (CBI; Hocevar,
Honest 1.22 0.36 −1.27 0.23
Conservative 1.09 0.40 0.72 0.19
1979, 1980) because the CBI and other similar measures
Capable 1.06 0.31 −1.95 0.35 have been widely used to measure students’ creativity (e.g.,
Conventional 0.80 0.21 1.27 0.28 Dollinger, 2003; Paek & Runco, 2017). This 77-item inven-
Well-mannered 0.69 0.17 −1.23 0.30 tory is a self-reported checklist of creative behaviors or
Cautious 0.67 0.16 −0.37 0.21 activities that each participants has been previously
Sincere 0.56 0.14 −1.45 0.37
Subscale 3
engaged in. Every item is on a scale from 0 (never) to 6
Artificial 1.34 0.50 1.65 0.30 (more than six times).
Snobbish 1.05 0.38 2.17 0.47
Suspicious 0.98 0.31 0.75 0.21
Narrow Interests 0.78 0.23 2.28 0.55 Results
submissive 0.74 0.24 1.93 0.50
Dissatisfied 0.69 0.19 1.07 0.31 Criterion-related validity
Commonplace 0.54 0.16 2.58 0.75
Since Gough (1979) selected the CPS items based upon
Note. a = item discrimination. b = item location. SE = standard error. the correlations between every item and various criterion
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 5

ratings of creativity, correlations between the CPS item positive correlations of subscale 1 with CBI scores were
scores and the CBI total score were calculated. As observed (see Table 4). CPS subscales 2 and 3 repre-
described above, the CBI is a self-report checklist that sented either neutral or negative creative personality
asks participants to indicate the frequency with which characteristics. Hence, they appeared to be significantly
they participate in a range of creative activities. Previous negatively correlated with CBI scores, which is basically
psychometric research has indicated that the CBI item consistent with Gough’s (1979) conclusion. Also, both
scores can be represented effectively with a one-factor CTT and IRT provided similar evidence of the CPS’s
model (Pluker, 1999). Correlations between the CPS item criterion-related validity.
scores and CBI scale score are included in Table 3. It seems
seven items did not correlate significantly with CBI scores. Internal consistency
Also, according to Gough (1979), sincere should be nega-
tively correlated with the criterion measure of creativity, The Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficients for the CPS and
while egotistical should be positively correlated with the subscales 1–3 were calculated to provide evidence of
criterion measure of creativity. internal consistency, and they were .70, .77, .61, and
In addition, for the three subscales, correlations .58, respectively. If those eight items with low discrimi-
between the total score of each subscale and the total nation indices (i.e., Egotistical, Reflective, Humorous,
score of CBI were calculated to show criterion-related Resourceful, Wide Interests, Self-confident, Sincere, and
validity under the CTT framework, whereas the correla- Commonplace) are deleted, the CPS will have 10 positive
tions between the participants’ trait estimates and the items and 12 neutral or negative items, with subscale 1
total score of CBI were calculated to show criterion- containing 10 positive items, subscale 2 having 6 neutral
related validity under the IRT framework. Specifically, items, and subscale 3 including 6 negative items. After
CPS subscale 1 included 16 positive items, so significant deletion, the Cronbach’s Alpha Coefficients for the CPS
and subscales 1–3 will be .60, .74, .61, and .60, respec-
tively. Therefore, removing those eight items with low
discrimination indices does not seem to dramatically
TABLE 3
Correlations between CPS item scores and CBI scale score change the Cronbach’s Alphas Coefficients.

Item Correlation with CBI total score

Original .27**
Discussion
Individualistic .20** Factor and IRT analyses
Clever .04
Inventive .22** As far as creativity assessment is concerned, CTT still
Confident .28** remains the dominant approach because it does not
Insightful .21**
Humorous .14*
require strong theoretical assumptions. However, the
Intelligent .19** major drawback of CTT is that the estimation of item
Wide-interests .31** properties such as item discrimination and item diffi-
Informal .29** culty/location is heavily influenced by the characteristics
Resourceful .26** of each sample and thus might lack invariance across
Self-confident .13*
Reflective .12*
samples (e.g., Embretson & Reise, 2000; Nandakumar
Sexy .35** & Ackerman, 2004). In contrast, under the IRT frame-
Unconventional .08 work, item statistics can be estimated independent of the
Egotistical −.08 ability level of the examinee (e.g., Nandakumar &
Artificial −.13* Ackerman, 2004), given that the corresponding IRT
Snobbish .01
Narrow Interests −.24**
model fits the data well. In other words, item parameter
Capable .14*
submissive −.12*
TABLE 4
Honest −.05
Correlations of CPS subscale scores with the CBI total score
Suspicious −.14*
Commonplace −.18** Correlation with CBI scores
Dissatisfied −.04
Well-mannered −.18* Scale Under IRT framework Under CTT framework
Sincere .16**
Cautious −.03 Subscale1 .38** .33**
Conservative −.33** Subscale 2 −.28** −.28**
Conventional −.22** Subscale 3 −.25** −.24**

Note. * p < .05. ** p < .01. Note. ** p < .01.


6 QIAN ET AL.

invariance can be achieved. In addition, CTT focuses on obvious inconsistency between what was found in this
test-level information (e.g., the total score), while IRT study and Gough’s (1979) findings might be due to the
emphasizes the item-level information, which also makes smaller sample size in the present study and a different
IRT more preferable than CTT. Therefore, three 2 PL criterion measure of creativity. But the validity analyses
IRT models were fitted to each subscale of the CPS. of the CPS subscales under both the IRT and CTT frame-
Findings suggest that although most of the CPS items work support that the CPS is associated with convincing
had moderate discrimination indices and could distin- evidence of criterion-related validity.
guish participants with different levels on the specific
creative personality trait, eight items had pretty low dis- Limitations and future directions
crimination indices, which means they are not able to
effectively differentiate people with various creative per- This study has several limitations. First, Gough (1979)
sonality trait levels. Since the internal consistency analy- developed the CPS with a sample of 1,701 architects,
sis in study 2 also indicates that deleting the eight items mathematicians, research scientists, senior engineers, and
with low discrimination indices does not seem to signifi- graduate students in psychology. However, the sample in
cantly change the Cronbach’s Alphas Coefficients, we this study consisted of 359 undergraduate and graduate
suggest those eight items be either removed from the students with various majors. In the future, if a similar
CPS or replaced by other new items. study can be conducted with a much larger and diverse
In addition to the estimated item statistics, the test group of participants, it may provide more useful evi-
information function acquired through IRT analyses dence for further examining and revising those poorly
reveals that the CPS subscales 1–3 provided different functioning items.
amount of information across the entire range of the Second, CBI scores were employed to investigate the
estimated creative personality trait levels. CPS’s criterion-related validity in this study. Since the
Zampetakis (2010) examined the factor structure of CBI is a self-report checklist that asks participants about
the CPS with a sample of 228 engineering students their engagement in a range of creative activities, another
from a small Technical University in Greece and argued way to examine the CPS’s criterion-related validity will
that the CPS was unidimensional. However, exploratory be to measure participants’ actual creative achievements,
factor analysis based on the tetrachoric correlation matrix and then analyze the relationship between their estimated
in study 1 suggests that the CPS was multidimensional, creative personality trait and their creativity levels. In
with factor 1 representing positive creative personality summary, more research seems to be needed to develop
characteristics, factor 2 standing for neutral creative per- a better version of CPS as it was created several decades
sonality characteristics, and factor 3 representing nega- ago, and it has been extremely widely used.
tive creative personality characteristics. More research is
needed to determine the exact reasons for this conflicting
finding. SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
Also, the CPS was developed empirically rather than
theoretically. Some items did not have promising item Supplemental data for this article can be accessed here.
properties partially because the selection of each item
was based solely on statistical significance, which was
further complicated by the sample-dependent nature of
CTT-based models. In the future, researchers may want DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
to develop measures of creativity based upon both the-
ories and empirical evidence. No potential conflict of interest was reported by the
authors.
Validity
Comparing the results of this study with Gough’s REFERENCES
(1979) findings, it shows that the correlations between
CPS items and the criterion measure of creativity varied Bock, R. D., Gibbons, R., Schilling, S. G., Muraki, E., Wilson, D. T., &
a lot with different samples under the CTT framework. Wood, R. (2002). TESTFACT 4 [Computer Program]. Chicago, IL:
Specifically, Gough (1979) found that all the items sig- Scientific Software International.
nificantly correlated with participants’ criterion ratings of Bock, R. D., Gibbons, R. D., & Murraki, E. (1988). Full information factor
creativity, whereas in this study seven items did not analysis. Applied Psychological Measurement, 12, 261–280.
doi:10.1177/014662168801200305
correlate with the CBI score. Moreover, sincere changed Dollinger, S. J. (2003). Need for uniqueness, need for cognition, and
from a negative item to a positive one, while egotistical creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 37, 99–116. doi:10.1002/
changed from a positive item to a negative one. The j.2162-6057.2003.tb00828.x
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL 7

Embretson, S. E., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory for psychol- honor of Henry A. Murray (pp. 251-278). New York, NY: Atherton
ogists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, Associates. Press.
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the adjective check list. Nandakumar, R., & Ackerman, T. (2004). Test modeling. In
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1398–1405. doi:10.1037/ D. Kaplan (Ed.), The Sage handbook of quantitative methodology
0022-3514.37.8.1398 for the social sciences (pp. 93–105). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Gough, H. G., & Heilbrun, A. B. (1965). The adjective check list manual. Publications.
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Paek, S. H., & Runco, M. A. (2017). Dealing with the criterion problem by
Hambleton, R. K., Swaminathan, H., & Rogers, H. J. (1991). measuring the quality and quantity of creative activity and
Fundamentals of item response theory. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. accomplishment. Creativity Research Journal, 29, 167–173.
Hocevar, D. (1979, April). The development of the creative behavior inventory. doi:10.1080/10400419.2017.1304078
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Rocky Mountain Psychological Plucker,J.A. (1999). Reanalyses of student responses to creativity check-
Association,Las Vegas, NV. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED lists:Evidence of content generality.The Journal of Creative Behavior,
170 350.). 33, 126–137. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.1999.tb01042.x
Hocevar, D. (1980). Intelligence, divergent thinking, and creativity. Sheldon, K. M. (1995). Creativity and self-determination in personality.
Intelligence, 4, 25–40. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(80)90004-5 Creativity Research Journal, 8, 25–36. doi:10.1207/
Hocevar, D. (1981). Measurement of creativity: A review and critique. s15326934crj0801_3
Journal of Personality Assessment, 45, 450–464. doi:10.1207/ Zampetakis, L. A. (2010). Unfolding the measurement of the creative
s15327752jpa4505_1 personality. Journal Of Creative Behavior, 44,105–123. doi:10.1002/
Kadusa, H., & Schaefer, C. (1991). Concurrent validity of the creative j.2162-6057.2010.tb01328.x
personality scale of the adjective check list. Psychological Reports, 69, Zhou, J., & Oldham, G. R. (2001). Enhancing creative performance:
601–602. doi:10.2466/pr0.1991.69.2.601 Effects of expected developmental assessment strategies and creative
MacKinnon, D. W. (1963). Creativity and images of the self. In personality. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 35, 151–167.
R. W. White (Ed.), The study of lives: Essays on personality in doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2001.tb01044.x

You might also like