You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jappdp

Linking academic worries and youth academic adjustment: The role of


parental involvement
Tianying Cai *, Kelly M. Tu
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, United States of America

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Using a multi-informant, longitudinal design, we investigated the independent and interactive associations be­
Academic performance tween youth academic worries and parental involvement (parent-teacher relationship quality, academic so­
Academic worries cialization, schoolwork assistance) before the middle school transition as predictors of youth engagement and
Adolescence
academic performance after the middle school transition among 100 youth (53% boys; Mage = 11.05 years; 57%
Engagement
Middle school transition
White). We found that maternal academic socialization moderated the prospective association between youth
Parental involvement academic worries and academic adjustment, such that youth who experienced more academic worries coupled
with higher maternal academic socialization had higher academic performance and engagement; no association
emerged for lower maternal academic socialization. Further, maternal schoolwork assistance before middle
school directly predicted better academic performance during middle school. Findings highlight the importance
of maternal schoolwork assistance in promoting youths’ academic performance, as well as maternal academic
socialization in promoting higher academic performance and engagement for youth experiencing more academic
worries.

Introduction independence) are disrupted by or at odds with the changes in the school
environment (e.g., increasing academic expectations, teacher views of
The transition to middle school has long been recognized as an student ability; Eccles & Roeser, 2009). Yet, the effect of youths’ aca­
exciting but challenging time in adolescents’ lives (Lord, Eccles, & demic worries about the middle school transition is less well understood
McCarthy, 1994). Compared to elementary school, middle school is and warrant attention with some evidence that worries and anxieties,
characterized by changes to the school environment, with different especially about academics, are linked to poorer academic performance
teachers by subject and subjects separated by ability, as well as (e.g., for meta-analysis, see von der Embse, Jester, Roy, & Post, 2018).
increasing academic expectations and demands placed on youth Additionally, parents could potentially ameliorate the negative effect
(Duchesne, Ratelle, & Roy, 2012). Perhaps not surprisingly, a majority of youths’ worries about the middle school transition, particularly via
of youth often report being worried or concerned about the transition to parental involvement, which is a reliable and consistent predictor of
middle school (e.g., academic expectations, workload, navigating new academic adjustment (Barger, Kim, Kuncel, & Pomerantz, 2019; Hill &
school environment; Duchesne, Ratelle, Poitras, & Drouin, 2009; Tyson, 2009). However, the role of parental involvement in buffering
Duchesne et al., 2012; Odegaard & Heath, 1992). The middle school the effects of youth academic worries on their engagement and perfor­
transition itself has been negatively linked to youths’ engagement and mance has received less attention. Thus, extending the literature, the
performance in middle school (Coelho, Marchante, & Jimerson, 2017; present study examined the independent and interactive contributions
Duchesne et al., 2012; Goldstein, Boxer, & Rudolph, 2015; Odegaard & of youth academic worries and parental involvement before middle
Heath, 1992; Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, & Kindermann, 2008). This school on youth engagement and academic performance after the mid­
negative association may be explained in part by the stage-environment dle school transition.
fit theory (Eccles et al., 1993; Midgley, Feldlaufer, & Eccles, 1989) in
which youths’ developmental needs (e.g., gains in autonomy and

* Corresponding author at: Department of Human Development and Family Studies at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 234 Bevier Hall, 905 South
Goodwin Avenue, Urbana, IL 61801, United States of America.
E-mail address: tc13@illinois.edu (T. Cai).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2021.101325
Received 10 December 2020; Received in revised form 29 August 2021; Accepted 10 September 2021
Available online 17 September 2021
0193-3973/© 2021 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

Academic adjustment and worries about the middle school transition related to better academic adjustment, performance, and engagement
among middle and high school students (Grolnick, Raftery-Helmer,
Academic performance and engagement during the middle school Flamm, Marbell, & Cardemil, 2015; Ratelle, Duchesne, & Guay, 2017).
years are critical in shaping the trajectory of youths’ future achieve­ Parental involvement in academics can also be a way that parents sup­
ment, such that middle school achievement is linked with youths’ high port youths’ needs of competence and relatedness by helping youth to
school achievement (Balfanz, 2009; Duchesne, Larose, Guay, Vitaro, & develop skills to succeed academically and connect parents with their
Tremblay, 2005). Additionally, high school academic achievement has youths’ school life, respectively.
longer-term implications for future success, including employment op­ Overall, a large body of literature has demonstrated the generally
portunities and mental health (e.g., Lansford, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, positive links between parental involvement and children’s academic
2016; Te Wang & Peck, 2013). Thus, identifying contributing factors to adjustment (e.g., Wei et al., 2019; for a meta-analysis, see Barger et al.,
youth academic engagement and performance are important for sup­ 2019). Parental involvement in academics and schooling can be cate­
porting youths’ future well-being and success. gorized into two broad forms: school-based involvement and home-
Despite a majority of youth reporting worries or concerns about the based involvement (Barger et al., 2019). School-based involvement in­
transition to middle school (Coelho et al., 2017; Duchesne et al., 2009, cludes parents’ direct participation in school activities (e.g., attending
2012; Odegaard & Heath, 1992), as well as a number of programs that parent-teacher conferences, volunteering in school events) and parental
have been developed to help prepare students for the middle school contact and relationship with teachers (Green, Walker, Hoover-
transition (for a systematic review, see van Rens, Haelermans, Groot, & Dempsey, & Sandler, 2007; Grolnick, Kurowski, Dunlap, & Hevey,
Maassen van den Brink, 2018), the effects of youths’ worries about 2000; Hughes & Kwok, 2007). Parents’ home-based involvement in­
middle school on their engagement and academic achievement has cludes parents’ academic-related activities at home, such as academic
received relatively limited attention compared to studies investigating socialization (e.g., communicating with youth about the importance of
the effects of the middle school transition itself (e.g., Dotterer, McHale, getting a good education) and school work assistance (e.g., helping
& Crouter, 2009; Eccles et al., 1993; Goldstein et al., 2015; Gutman, youth to complete homework; Wang, Hill, & Hofkens, 2014; for a meta-
Sameroff, & Cole, 2003; Herbers, Reynolds, & Chen, 2013). The number analysis see Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008). The current study fo­
of existing middle school transition programs is particularly noteworthy cuses on mother-teacher relationship quality, academic socialization,
(see van Rens et al., 2018) in recognizing the potential challenges and schoolwork assistance.
accompanying the middle school transition. The importance of parental involvement in academics for facilitating
The few existing studies examining youths’ worries about middle adolescents’ academic achievement has been well-recognized, such that
school found that youth reported a range of worries, including worries both reviews and meta-analyses support a small, yet positive association
about academic performance (e.g., challenging schoolwork), changes in between parental involvement and academic adjustment (Barger et al.,
the school environment (e.g., deal with multiple teachers), and re­ 2019; Hill & Tyson, 2009; Hughes & Kwok, 2007). For example, high-
lationships with others (e.g., friends, peers, teachers; e.g., Duchesne quality parent-teacher relationships can help parents to monitor
et al., 2012; Odegaard & Heath, 1992). Further, in one study, youth who youths’ academic progress, provide parents with information about
reported more worries, representing a combination of academic and when and how to intervene, and/or reflect parents’ support of youths’
social worries (e.g., difficulty of schoolwork and peer problems, academics more generally (Hassrick & Schneider, 2009; Hughes &
respectively) before the middle school transition experienced poorer Kwok, 2007). Further, when parents engage in academic socialization
school adjustment in middle school (Duchesne et al., 2012). Addition­ (e.g., discussing what youth are learning in school) and offer assistance
ally, some relevant literature on youth anxiety, including general anxi­ with schoolwork, youth have opportunities to receive messages about
ety, worries, and test anxiety, revealed negative associations with the importance or value of education as well as the chance to enhance
youths’ academic performance (for meta-analyses, see Seipp, 1991 and their academic skills through practice and proper instruction, respec­
von der Embse et al., 2018), especially in the middle school grades (von tively (Hill & Tyson, 2009). Academic socialization has also been pro­
der Embse et al., 2018). However, others have argued that anxiety or posed to be a more effective form of involvement, especially during early
worries could serve as a motivating factor to enhance performance (e.g., adolescence, because it allows parents to maintain involvement while
Eysenck & Calvo, 1992; Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007), meeting adolescents’ needs of autonomy and independence (Hill &
such as preparing for increasing academic demands. Collectively, there Tyson, 2009).
is some evidence that youth worries can disrupt youths’ engagement and Conversely, the benefit of schoolwork assistance is less clear as some
academic performance in middle school, with effect sizes ranging from studies found schoolwork assistance to be less effective than other forms
small to moderate (Duchesne et al., 2012; Goldstein et al., 2015; Skinner of involvement, non-significant, or even negatively associated with in­
et al., 2008). dicators of academic adjustment (for meta-analyses, see Barger et al.,
Furthermore, a few studies examining protective factors against the 2019; Hill & Tyson, 2009). Specifically, a meta-analysis by Hill and
negative effects of academic worries revealed some evidence that both Tyson (2009) found a negative association between schoolwork assis­
classroom (e.g., teacher emphasis on student engagement to develop tance and academic achievement among middle school students with a
competencies) and family (e.g., attachment) factors were protective small effect size (r = − 0.11). The authors explained that schoolwork
against or reduced academic and school transition worries, respectively assistance can potentially interfere developmentally with youths’ au­
(e.g., Duchesne et al., 2009, 2012). Of particular focus in our study is the tonomy development and increase academic pressure (Hill & Tyson,
integral role of parents as a means to alleviate the potential negative 2009), highlighting the need to consider the manner in which parents
effects of academic worries on youths’ academic adjustment (i.e., provide assistance (e.g., in an autonomy supportive or controlling
engagement, academic performance) in middle school. manner; Ratelle et al., 2017). Others have also proposed a remediation
effect such that youth who are struggling in school are more likely to
Parental involvement and academic adjustment during early adolescence receive assistance from parents (Barger et al., 2019).
Although it is generally well-established that parental involvement
According to self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000), parents has direct implications for youths’ academic adjustment (Barger et al.,
(and other influential adults) play a key role in supporting youths’ 2019; Hill & Tyson, 2009), it is less clear how parental involvement
developmental needs of autonomy, competence, and relatedness to help could help to reduce the potential negative effects that youths’ academic
youth achieve developmental milestones and/or overcome develop­ worries may have on their academic adjustment, especially during a
mental challenges. For instance, studies have found that parents’ pro­ school transition with increasing academic demands and expectations,
visions of academic autonomy support and schoolwork assistance were as well as uncertainty around a new school environment. Parental

2
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

involvement has the potential to alleviate adolescents’ worries and 2020; Tu & Ravindran, 2020). Families were recruited from the Mid­
enhance academic motivation and engagement, such as helping to western United States across two consecutive cohorts, spaced one year
reinforce or enhance youths’ value of school and/or motivation to apart (2017 and 2018). At T1 (Spring 2017 and 2018), informational
overcome their worries and concerns to succeed in school (Cheung & letters/emails were distributed to parents of all 5th grade students at
Pomerantz, 2015; Te Wang & Sheikh-Khalil, 2014). Additionally, participating elementary schools and in local communities. Parents who
parental involvement may be beneficial for adolescents who have expressed interest were contacted by phone to learn about the details of
greater concerns about academic expectations or challenges—by getting the study and scheduled for a laboratory visit. Written consent and
the support they need and also gaining the skills to manage future ac­ assent from mothers and youth, respectively, were obtained during the
ademic challenges, which can enhance their sense of control over their visit. Exclusion criteria included limited English proficiency and youth
learning experiences (Cheung & Pomerantz, 2012; Grolnick & Slow­ with a diagnosis of developmental delays or chronic illness due to lab­
iaczek, 1994). oratory procedures of the larger project not examined in this study. At
T2 (Fall 2017 and 2018), families were contacted to participate again by
The current study giving permission to contact teachers to complete surveys.
At T1, youth and their mothers participated in a laboratory session
Towards extending the literatures on worries about the middle during which they completed surveys in separate rooms. At T1 and T2,
school transition and parental involvement, we investigated the pro­ researchers sought permission from parents to contact youths’ elemen­
spective association between youths’ academic worries about the up­ tary and middle school teachers, respectively. At both waves, teachers
coming transition to middle school during their last year of elementary were contacted to complete surveys online about youths’ performance
school (5th grade; Time 1 or T1) and their academic adjustment in and behavior in school.
middle school (6th grade; Time 2 or T2) using a multi-informant t-tests and χ 2 tests across primary study variables and covariates
approach (i.e., adolescents, mothers, and teachers). Further, we inves­ revealed that, compared to participants with T1 teacher reports of
tigated whether indices of parental involvement (i.e., mother-teacher engagement and academic performance, those without T1 teacher re­
relationship quality, academic socialization, schoolwork assistance) in ports had mothers who reported lower mother-teacher relationship
the last year of elementary school (T1) served as moderators of the as­ quality, Mno teacher report = 2.78, Mwith teacher report = 3.32, t (24.57) =
sociation between youth academic worries and academic adjustment. − 2.07, p = .050, and earned less than bachelor’s degree, χ 2 (1) = 6.29, p
Based on empirical findings, we expected that T1 youth academic = .012. All other comparisons between participants with and without T1
worries would be negatively associated with their T2 academic adjust­ teacher reports of engagement/academic performance were not signif­
ment (Duchesne et al., 2012), but that higher levels of T1 parental icant. Further, t-tests and χ 2 tests were conducted for participants with
involvement via higher mother-teacher relationship quality and aca­ and without T2 teacher reports and no significant differences emerged
demic socialization would attenuate this association (Barger et al., 2019; across primary study variables and covariates. Participating families and
Hughes & Kwok, 2007; Miller, Valentine, Fish, & Robinson, 2016). teachers were compensated at each wave for their time and effort.
However, because of the mixed findings regarding the association be­
tween schoolwork assistance and academic adjustment (Barger et al., Measures
2019; Hill & Tyson, 2009) we had no a priori hypothesis about the
moderating role of T1 maternal schoolwork assistance in the association Youth academic worries (T1)
between T1 youth academic worries and T2 academic adjustment. To assess youth academic worries, youth were asked, “What are your
top 3 concerns or worries about going to middle school?” Similarly,
Methods mothers were asked, “What are your child’s top 3 concerns about middle
school?” The total number of academic worries reported by youth and
Participants mothers ranged from 1 to 4. Their responses to the open-ended question
were then coded by two independent research assistants for worries
Data for the current study come from a longitudinal study focused on about: academic expectations (e.g., amount of schoolwork, grades, strict
mother-adolescent interactions about adolescent stress across the tran­ teachers, time management, not doing well in classes), school environ­
sition to middle school (Tu, Li, & Cohen, 2019; Tu & Ravindran, 2020). ment (e.g., navigating classes, changing bell schedule, unstructured
At Time 1 (T1) during the spring of 5th grade, participants included 100 time), peers (e.g., peer pressure, bullying) and friendships (e.g., friends
young adolescents (53% boys; Mage = 11.05 years, SD = 0.33), their not in the same schools). The two coders had high interrater reliability
mothers (Mage = 41.25 years, SD = 6.22; 96.0% biological), and 78 (Kappas ranged from 0.73 to 1.00). We focused specifically on academic
teachers (78% reports obtained in total; 98% of mothers provided expectation worries, which was calculated as a proportion score of ac­
parental permission). At Time 2 (T2) during the fall of 6th grade, par­ ademic expectation worries out of all the worries named (e.g., if youth
ticipants included 89 out of 100 youth (Mage = 11.65 years, SD = 0.32), reported 3 worries, then the number of academic expectations worries
their mothers (Mage = 41.56 years, SD = 5.59; 95.6% biological), and 76 out of 3 worries was calculated). The use of a proportion score, as
teachers (85% reports obtained in total; 98% of mothers provided compared to the use of raw scores (frequency, sum), allows us to
parental permission). contextualize youths’ academic expectation worries relative to other
The youth in the sample included 57% White/European Americans, worries reported and allowed us to fit more parsimonious models (single
14% Hispanic/Latino/a, 11% Black/African Americans, 11% multira­ variable vs inclusion of multiple raw score variables to control for other
cial, 6% Asian/Asian American, and 1% other. About 76% of mothers worries). Separate proportion scores by informant were used. To
had a bachelor’s degree or higher. Approximately 4% of families re­ simplify terminology, the proportion of academic expectation worries is
ported an annual household income below $25,000; 14% reported be­ referred to as academic worries.
tween $25,000 and $50,000; 19% reported between $50,000 and
$75,000; and 63% reported above $75,000. Parent-teacher relationship quality (T1)
Mothers reported on their relationship quality with their child’s
Procedures teacher (7 items; Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 1991).
Items (e.g., “You feel comfortable talking with your child’s teacher
The Institutional Review Board at University of Illinois at Urbana- about your child”; “You think your child’s teacher is interested in getting
Champaign approved the study; only pertinent procedures are dis­ to know you”) were rated on a 5-point scale (0 = not at all to 4 = a great
cussed (for additional procedures, see Cai & Tu, 2020; Tu, Cai, & Li, deal). The scale demonstrated high reliability (α = 0.91).

3
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

Parental home involvement (T1) (Arbuckle, 2017), which uses full information maximum likelihood
Youth and mothers completed the youth and mother versions of the estimation to handle missing data (Acock, 2005). First, covariates,
parental home involvement scale, respectively (Wei et al., 2019). Items which included youth gender, age, and race; family income; maternal
were identical with the exception of wording modifications to reflect education level; cohort; and corresponding T1 academic variables were
youth vs parent perspectives. The parental home involvement scale has entered. Next, the main effects of T1 academic worries was entered,
two subscales, academic socialization (8 items; e.g., “My child and I talk followed by the main effect of T1 parental involvement. Lastly, the
about the importance of getting a good education”; “When I am with my interaction term between academic worries and parental involvement
child, we discuss how s/he can best manage her/his study time”; “My was entered. Separate models were fitted for each index of parental
child and I talk about different strategies that s/he can use to do her/his involvement. Further, to provide a rigorous test of associations and
schoolwork”) and schoolwork assistance (6 items; e.g., “I assist my child determine replicability, one set of models tested youth-reported aca­
with her/his schoolwork by helping her/him find information or ma­ demic worries (predictor), mother-reported parental involvement
terials for her/his projects”; “When my child needs help on her/his (moderator), and teacher-reported engagement and academic perfor­
schoolwork, I am there to help her/him myself”; “I spend time with my mance (outcomes); another set of models tested mother reports of youth
child on things related to her/his schoolwork”). Although the two sub­ academic worries (predictor), youth-reported parental involvement
scales are moderately correlated, they are conceptually different (Hill & (moderator), and teacher-reported engagement and academic perfor­
Tyson, 2009; Wei et al., 2019). Academic socialization refers to mance (outcomes). Significant interactions were probed using tests of
communication about academic expectations, learning strategies, and simple slopes (Aiken & West, 1991; Preacher et al., 2006), which yielded
messages about the value and utility of education, whereas schoolwork intercepts and slopes representing the relationship between youth aca­
assistance refers to parenting practices or behaviors where parents are demic worries and academic adjustment at low (− 1 SD) and high (+1
directly involved in helping children with their homework or preparing SD) levels of parental involvement. Regions of significance of the
for tests (Wei et al., 2019). Thus, we examined both subscales separately. moderator were also probed to determine the specific values of the
The items were rated on a 7-point Likert-like scale (1 = never to 7 = very moderator (i.e., parental involvement) at which the regression of aca­
much). Both subscales demonstrated good internal consistency for youth demic adjustment indices on youth academic worries changed from non-
and mother reports (αs range from 0.81 to 0.89). significant to significant (Preacher et al., 2006).
Further, effect sizes using Pearson correlation r (0.10 and 0.30
Engagement (T1-T2) indicating small and medium effect size, respectively) and f2 (= Rfull2 -
Teachers reported on youths’ engagement, which includes behav­ Rpartial2/ 1 – Rfull2; 0.02 and 0.15 indicating small and medium effect
ioral engagement, emotional engagement, and general school engage­ sizes, respectively; Cohen, 1988) for regression models are reported. We
ment. Behavioral engagement (5 items; e.g., “This student tries hard to reported f2 for significant main and interaction effects; Rfull2 refers to the
do well in school”; “In class, this student works as hard as he/she can”; R2 of the model including the significant variable (i.e., main or moder­
Skinner et al., 2008) was measured using a 4-point Likert-like scale (1 = ator variable, interaction term); Rpartial2 refers to the R2 of the model
not at all true to 4 = very true). Emotional engagement (5 items; e.g., without the significant variable.
“When we work on something in class, this student seems interested”;
“This student enjoys learning new things in class; Skinner et al., 2008) Results
was measured using a 4-point Likert-like scale (1 = not at all true to 4 =
very true). General engagement (3 items; e.g., “This student does more Preliminary analysis
than required”; “In my class, this student seems tuned in”; Institute for
Research and Reform in Education, 1998) were rated on a 4-point Likert- Descriptive statistics and correlations between primary study vari­
like scale (1 = not at all true to 4 = very true). The scores of the three ables are summarized in Table 1 and Table 2, respectively. Missing
scales were highly correlated (rs ranged from 0.75 to 0.90); scores were values analysis revealed that data were missing completely at random,
standardized and composited to create an overall engagement score. All χ 2 (209) = 240.94, p = .06. As seen in Table 2, T1 youth- and mother-
measures were reliable at T1 and T2 (αs range from 0.70 to 0.90). reported academic worries were positively correlated, as were youth
and mother reports of indices of parental involvement. Further, T1
Academic performance (T1-T2) youth-reported academic worries was negatively associated with T1
At each wave, teachers reported on youths’ academic performance youth-reported maternal academic socialization and schoolwork assis­
using the Mock Report Card (Pierce, Hamm, & Vandell, 1999). Teachers tance, and the latter two were positively correlated with each other. T1
rated youth in five areas (e.g., reading, math, science; 1 = failing to 5 = youth-reported maternal schoolwork assistance was also positively
excellent); items were composited for an overall academic performance associated with T1 teacher-reported academic performance and T2
score (αs = 0.97 and 0.96 at T1 and T2, respectively). teacher-reported engagement. T1 mother-reported academic worries
was positively correlated with T2 teacher-reported academic perfor­
Covariates (T1) mance. T1 mother reports of relationship quality with teacher, academic
Covariates included youth gender (coded 0 = girls, 1 = boys), youth socialization, and schoolwork assistance were positive associated with
age, annual family income (1 ≤ $15,000 to 5 ≥ $75,000), mother edu­ each other. Teacher-reported engagement and academic performance
cation level (coded 0 = less than bachelor’s degree, 1 = bachelor’s degree or were positively correlated with each other across T1 and T2. Regarding
higher), youth race/ethnicity (coded 0 = European American, 1 = racial/ effect sizes based on correlations, results linking youth academic
ethnic minorities), and cohort (coded 0 = cohort 1, 1 = cohort 2). worries, indices of parental involvement, and parent-teacher relation­
ship quality with youth engagement and academic performance
Plan of analysis revealed no effects to small or medium effects in a few instances.
Among covariates, compared with girls, boys had lower engagement
Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine descriptive statis­ and academic performance at T1. Higher family income was associated
tics and bivariate correlations among study variables. Missing data with higher T1 mother-reported youth academic worries, and higher T2
range from 1% to 24%, with the latter due to a combination of lack of teacher-reported engagement and academic performance. Higher
parental permission for teacher contact and participants who did not maternal education was associated with higher T1 mother-reported
return at T2. Missing value analysis was also conducted to determine the youth academic worries, greater T1 mother-reported schoolwork assis­
pattern of missingness. tance, and higher T2 teacher-reported academic performance. Cohort 2
A series of step-wise regression models were conducted in AMOS mothers reported higher T1 mother-teacher relationship quality, T1

4
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

Table 1 academic socialization, and T1 schoolwork assistance. Youth in Cohort 2


Descriptive statistics of academic worries, parental involvement and academic had lower teacher-reported academic performance at T2. No significant
performance. correlations emerged for youths’ racial/ethnic minority status or age.
Variables Mean or % for SD N Min Max
code of 1 Academic worries and parental involvement predicting engagement
1. T1 YR Youth academic 0.39 0.28 87 0.00 1.00
worries As shown in Table 3, among covariates for youth- and mother-
2. T1 YR Academic 3.49 0.88 100 1.13 5.00 reported academic worries models, youth from higher income families
socialization
and Cohort 1 had higher engagement at T2. Further, engagement was
3. T1 YR Schoolwork 3.61 0.83 99 1.33 5.00
assistance relatively stable from T1 to T2.
4. T1 MR Youth academic 0.30 0.29 95 0.00 1.00
worries Youth-reported academic worries x mother-reported parental involvement
5. T1 MR Academic 5.17 1.11 99 1.88 7.00
As shown in Table 3, no main effect emerged for youth-reported
socialization
6. T1 MR Schoolwork 5.07 1.19 99 1.17 7.00 academic worries or indices of parental involvement. However,
assistance maternal academic socialization emerged as a moderator of the associ­
7. T1 MR Mother-teacher 3.20 0.85 99 0.71 4.00 ation between T1 youth-reported academic worries and T2 teacher-
relationship reported engagement. The interaction term explained 4% of the
8. T1 TR Engagement 0.00 0.95 78 − 3.00 0.81
unique variance (small effect size, f2 = 0.10). A follow-up simple slopes
9. T1 TR Academic 4.19 0.88 78 2.00 5.00
performance analysis revealed that T1 academic worries were positively associated
10. T2 TR Engagement 0.00 0.93 76 − 3.34 0.90 with T2 teacher-reported engagement at higher levels of maternal aca­
11. T2 TR Academic 4.03 0.84 75 1.60 5.00 demic socialization, B (SE) = 0.64 (0.33), p = .052, but not at lower
performance
levels of maternal academic socialization, B (SE) = − 0.39 (0.33), p =
12. Youth gender (1 = boys) 53%
13. T1 Youth age 11.04 0.33 100 10.08 12.17
.234 (see Fig. 1). Regions of significance test revealed that the lower and
14. T1 Family income 4.39 0.92 97 1.00 5.00 upper bounds of the regions of significance tests were − 2.65 and 1.14
15. T1 Maternal education (1 75% SDs, respectively, from the mean of maternal academic socialization.
≥ Bachelor’s) Thus, the positive association between youth academic worries and
16. Youth minority status (1 43%
academic performance became significant when maternal academic
= minority)
17. Cohort (1 = cohort 2) 48% socialization was above 1.14 SD of the mean (14.1% of youth in the
sample). Maternal schoolwork assistance and the mother-teacher rela­
Note. T1 = Time 1. T2 = Time 2. YR = Youth report. MR = Mother report. TR =
tionship did not moderate the association between T1 youth academic
Teacher report.
worries and T2 engagement.

Mother-reported academic worries x youth-reported parental involvement


As shown in Table 3, no main effects emerged from mother-reported

Table 2
Correlations between academic worries, parental involvement and academic performance.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. T1 YR Youth academic worries –


2. T1 YR Maternal academic socialization − 0.25* –
3. T1 YR Maternal schoolwork assistance − 0.26* 0.63*** –
4. T1 MR Youth academic worries 0.41*** − 0.33*** − 0.22* –
5. T1 MR Maternal academic socialization − 0.15 0.40*** 0.35*** − 0.09 –
6. T1 MR Maternal schoolwork assistance − 0.13 0.28** 0.35*** − 0.13 0.63*** –
7. T1 MR Mother-teacher relationship 0.02 0.11 0.13 − 0.04 0.20* 0.28**
8. T1 TR Engagement − 0.02 0.01 0.19 0.10 − 0.07 0.08 0.09
9. T1 TR Academic performance − 0.12 0.00 0.25* 0.05 − 0.12 − 0.10 0.10 0.72***
10. T2 TR Engagement − 0.06 0.06 0.27* 0.04 0.01 0.18 0.17 0.68*** 0.54***
11. T2 TR Academic performance 0.08 − 0.06 0.08 0.23* − 0.10 0.05 0.04 0.57*** 0.57***
12. Youth gender (1 = boys) 0.12 0.01 − 0.19 0.13 − 0.18 − 0.12 0.09 − 0.26* − 0.27*
13. T1 Youth age − 0.03 0.08 0.05 0.12 0.05 0.02 − 0.06 0.16 0.14
14. T1 Family income 0.09 − 0.05 0.11 0.25* 0.00 0.14 0.10 0.22 0.21
15. T1 Maternal education (1 ≥ Bachelor’s) 0.09 0.07 0.09 0.23* 0.09 0.21* 0.18 0.13 0.00
16. Youth minority status (1 = minority) − 0.10 0.12 0.02 0.01 0.12 − 0.07 0.02 − 0.05 − 0.10
17. Cohort (1 = cohort 2) 0.07 0.15 0.06 0.00 0.29** 0.27** 0.25* 0.09 0.00

10 11 12 13 14 15 16

10. T2 TR Engagement –
11. T2 TR Academic performance 0.69*** –
12. Youth gender (1 = boys) − 0.04 0.07 –
13. T1 Youth age 0.15 0.12 − 0.17 –
14. T1 Family income 0.39*** 0.44*** 0.06 0.07 –
15. T1 Maternal education (1 ≥ Bachelor’s) 0.20 0.25* 0.06 0.04 0.40*** –
16. Youth minority status (1 = minority) − 0.08 − 0.15 0.01 − 0.06 − 0.24* − 0.01 –
17. Cohort (1 = cohort 2) − 0.10 − 0.27* − 0.06 0.11 0.03 0.05 0.10

Note. T1 = Time 1. T2 = Time 2. YR = Youth report. MR = Mother report. TR = Teacher report.


*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

5
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

Table 3
Regression coefficients linking academic worries and parental involvement with adolescent engagement and academic performance.
Models YR academic worries x MR involvement MR academic worries x YR involvement

T2 engagement T2 academic performance T2 engagement T2 academic performance

B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) β B (SE) β

Covariates
Gender (1 = boys) 0.15 (0.14) 0.08 0.31*(0.14) 0.19 0.15 (0.14) 0.08 0.31*(0.14) 0.19
T1 Youth age 0.11 (0.21) 0.04 0.15 (0.20) 0.06 0.11 (0.21) 0.04 0.15 (0.20) 0.06
T1 Family income 0.31 (0.07)*** 0.31 0.25 (0.07)** 0.27 0.31 (0.07)*** 0.31 0.25 (0.07)** 0.27
T1 Maternal education (1 ≥ Bachelor’s) − 0.17 (0.16) − 0.08 0.15 (0.16) 0.08 − 0.17 (0.16) − 0.08 0.15 (0.16) 0.08
Minority status (1 = minority) 0.11 (0.14) 0.06 − 0.09 (0.14) − 0.05 0.11 (0.14) 0.06 − 0.09 (0.14) − 0.05
Cohort (1 = cohort 2) − 0.30 (0.14)* − 0.16 − 0.45 (0.14)*** − 0.27 − 0.30 (0.14)* − 0.16 − 0.45 (0.14)*** − 0.27
T1 Engagement/Academic performance 0.69 (0.07)*** 0.71 0.56 (0.08)*** 0.59 0.69 (0.07)*** 0.71 0.56 (0.08)*** 0.59
R2 0.65 0.55 0.65 0.55

Main predictor
T1 Youth academic worries 0.12 (0.26) 0.04 0.40 (0.25) 0.14 − 0.29 (0.24) − 0.09 0.28 (0.24) 0.10
R2/ ΔR2 0.65/0.00 0.58/0.03 0.66/0.01 0.55/0.00

Academic socialization model


T1 Maternal academic socialization − 0.00 (0.06) − 0.00 0.04 (0.06) 0.05 0.00 (0.08) 0.00 0.04 (0.08) 0.04
R2/ ΔR2 0.65/0.00 0.59/0.01 0.66/0.00 0.55/0.00
Academic worries x socialization 0.46 (0.19)* 0.18 0.47 (0.18)** 0.20 0.21 (0.30) 0.05 0.72 (0.29)* 0.20
R2/ ΔR2 0.69/0.04 0.65/0.06 0.66/0.00 0.60/0.05

Schoolwork assistance model


T1 Maternal schoolwork assistance 0.03 (0.06) 0.04 0.13(0.05)* 0.17 0.14 (0.08) 0.12 0.12 (0.08) 0.12
R2/ ΔR2 0.65/0.00 0.63/0.05 0.64/− 0.02 0.57/0.02
Academic worries x assistance 0.30 (0.18) 0.13 0.18 (0.17) 0.09 0.12 (0.29) 0.03 0.23 (0.28) 0.07
R2/ ΔR2 0.63/− 0.02 0.64/0.01 0.65/0.01 0.57/0.00

Mother-teacher relationship model


T1 Mother-teacher relationship 0.12 (0.08) 0.11 0.07 (0.08) 0.07 N/A
R2/ ΔR2 0.65/0.00 0.58/0.00
Academic worries x mother-teacher relationship 0.45 (0.30) 0.11 0.46 (0.29) 0.13
R2/ ΔR2 0.65/0.00 0.61/0.03

Note. Coefficients reported are from step of entry. In final model with all variables, the coefficients for the main effect of youth academic worries were nearly identical
to those reported in the table at step of entry.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

Fig. 1. The association between T1 youth-reported


academic worries and T2 teacher-reported engage­
ment at lower (− 1 SD) and higher (+1 SD) levels of
maternal academic socialization.
Note. T1 = Time 1. T2 = Time 2. YR = Youth report.
MR = Mother report. TR = Teacher report.
*p = .05.

academic worries or any of the parental involvement indices. Further, no Academic worries and parental involvement predicting academic
index of parental involvement moderated the association between T1 performance
mother-reported worries and T2 teacher-reported engagement.
As shown in Table 3, among covariates for youth- and mother-
reported academic worries models, boys and youth from higher

6
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

income families and Cohort 1 had higher academic performance at T2. youth-reported maternal academic socialization. Thus, the positive as­
Further, academic performance was relatively stable from T1 to T2. sociation between mother reports of youth academic worries and aca­
demic performance became significant when youth-reported maternal
Youth-reported academic worries x mother-reported parental involvement academic socialization was above 0.09 SD of the mean (49% of youth in
As shown in Table 3, no main effect emerged for youth-reported the sample).
academic worries. Of the indices of parental involvement, T1 maternal
schoolwork assistance predicted better T2 teacher-reported academic Discussion
performance, which explained 5% of the unique variance (medium ef­
fect size, f2 = 0.15). No other main effects emerged. Utilizing a multi-informant, longitudinal design, findings from the
Further, the interaction between T1 youth-reported academic present study revealed that, although no main effects emerged for youth
worries and T1 maternal academic socialization predicted T2 academic academic worries, maternal academic socialization served as a moder­
performance. The interaction term explained 6% of the unique variance ator of the association between youth academic worries before middle
(medium effect size, f2 = 0.16). A follow-up simple slopes analysis school and youths’ academic performance after the transition to middle
revealed that T1 academic worries were positively associated with T2 school. Specifically, youth academic worries was positively associated
teacher-reported academic performance at higher levels of maternal with academic performance when mothers engaged in more academic
academic socialization, B (SE) = 0.97 (0.31), p = .002, but not at lower socialization, but not less academic socialization, and this pattern of
levels of maternal academic socialization, B (SE) = − 0.08 (0.31), p = findings was replicated across two multi-informant models. Similarly, a
.807 (see Fig. 2). Further, the lower and upper bounds of the regions of positive association emerged between youth-reported academic worries
significance tests were − 4.63 and 0.06 SDs, respectively, from the mean and teacher-reported engagement when mothers reported more aca­
of maternal academic socialization. Thus, the positive association be­ demic socialization, although this was not replicated in the mother-
tween youth academic worries and academic performance became sig­ reported academic worries model. Further, a main effect emerged for
nificant when maternal academic socialization was above 0.06 SD of the maternal schoolwork assistance (but not mother-teacher relationship
mean (56% of youth in the sample). quality or maternal academic socialization), such that mother-reported
schoolwork assistance before the middle school transition predicted
Mother-reported youth academic worries x youth-reported parental better teacher-reported academic performance after middle school
involvement transition. Thus, in our study, different patterns of findings emerged for
As shown in Table 3, no main effect emerged for mother-reported the three types of parental involvement examined (i.e., academic so­
academic worries and youth-reported parental involvement. However, cialization as moderator, schoolwork assistance main effect, and no ef­
similar to the youth-reported academic worries model, the interaction fect for mother-teacher relationship quality).
between mother-reported academic worries and youth-reported Surprisingly, main effects of youth- and mother-reported academic
maternal academic socialization predicted T2 academic performance, worries on youth academic adjustment in middle school did not emerge,
which explained 5% of the unique variance in academic performance which is inconsistent with some prior studies (e.g., Duchesne et al.,
(small effect size, f2 = 0.11). Follow-up simple slopes analysis revealed 2012; Goldstein et al., 2015). One potential explanation may be differ­
that T1 academic worries were positively associated with T2 teacher- ences in assessments of worries. Whereas Duchesne et al. (2012)
reported academic performance at higher levels of maternal academic examined youth reports about the extent to which they experienced
socialization, B (SE) = 1.02 (0.34), p = .003, but not at lower levels of worries in response to a range of academic and social issues (e.g., aca­
mother academic socialization, B (SE) = − 0.24 (0.34), p = .482 (see demic demands; relationships with teachers, peers) in the prediction of
Fig. 3). Further, the lower and upper bounds of the regions of signifi­ general school adjustment, we examined youth reports of their top
cance tests were − 3.02 and 0.09 SDs, respectively, from the mean of worries about the middle school transition and categorized each worry

Fig. 2. The association between T1 youth-reported academic worries and T2 teacher-reported academic performance at lower (− 1 SD) and higher (+1 SD) levels of
maternal academic socialization.
Note. T1 = Time 1. T2 = Time 2. YR = Youth report. MR = Mother report. TR = Teacher report. **p < .01.

7
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

Fig. 3. The association between T1 mother-reported youth academic worries and T2 teacher-reported academic performance at lower (− 1 SD) and higher (+1 SD)
levels of youth-reported maternal academic socialization.
Note. T1 = Time 1. T2 = Time 2. YR = Youth report. MR = Mother report. TR = Teacher report. **p < .01.

in order to calculate a proportion of academic worries out of all worries middle school transition. This finding is somewhat inconsistent with
reported. findings from a meta-analysis (Hill & Tyson, 2009), which found that
However, despite non-significant main effects of youth academic schoolwork assistance had negative associations with academic perfor­
worries, maternal academic socialization (but not mother-teacher rela­ mance (although a small effect size). The studies included in the meta-
tionship quality or schoolwork assistance) moderated the association analysis examined associations between schoolwork assistance and
between academic worries and academic performance among youth, youth academic adjustment in middle school (Hill & Tyson, 2009). In
partially supporting our hypothesis. Specifically, a positive association contrast, the current study examined associations across the transition
emerged between proportion of academic worries and youths’ academic from elementary to middle school. Some reasons that parental
performance in middle school, but only when mothers engaged in more involvement in the form of schoolwork assistance prior to middle school
academic socialization (e.g., communication or discussions about may be beneficial for later academic achievement is that it may help to
schoolwork and the importance of education). We speculate that equip youth with problem-solving skills that could be applied to future
maternal academic socialization could help to reinforce or enhance academic challenges through parents’ direct instruction and/or
youths’ value of school and/or potentially use their worries to serve as modeling (e.g., Tu et al., 2020), as well as help youth develop a routine
motivation to succeed academically, resulting in better academic per­ of doing their schoolwork. Mothers’ provision of schoolwork assistance
formance, despite academic worries and concerns (Cheung & Pomer­ could also help to promote youths’ sense of competence (e.g., Doctoroff
antz, 2015; Hill & Tyson, 2009). & Arnold, 2017; Grolnick et al., 2015; Ratelle et al., 2017). Additionally,
In contrast, when the proportion of youth academic worries was maternal schoolwork assistance likely conveys messages about the
lower, higher levels of maternal academic socialization (as compared to importance and value of education to youth (Pomerantz, Qin, Wang, &
lower levels) appeared to have counter-productive effects. When youth Chen, 2011) and could contribute to youths’ sense of connection and
are less concerned about academics (and more worried about other relatedness to mothers (Doctoroff & Arnold, 2017), both of which have
areas, such as peer relationships), but are met with more maternal ac­ benefits for youths’ academic adjustment (Pomerantz et al., 2011).
ademic socialization, the mismatch between youths’ needs and Neither main nor interaction effects emerged for mother-teacher
parenting behaviors may result in poorer academic performance. relationship quality, suggesting that parental school involvement in
Indeed, other concerns could also interfere with academic performance elementary school may not have an effect on youths’ academic perfor­
(e.g., peer concerns; Juvonen, Wang, & Espinoza, 2011), and youth may mance in middle school. This is somewhat consistent with prior studies
need parental support in those areas as well. Collectively, the patterns of that found smaller effects of school-based, as compared to home-based,
associations appear to be consistent with the protective-enhancing involvement (Barger et al., 2019). Further, the relationship that mothers
model of resilience (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000), such that a develop with elementary school teachers may differ from those of
protective moderator (i.e., maternal socialization) attenuated or pro­ middle school teachers because of the number of teachers youth have in
moted competence (i.e., academic performance) at higher levels of risk middle school compared to elementary school. It is also possible that
(i.e., more academic worries), but not at lower levels of risk (i.e., less mothers’ relationship with middle school teachers as compared with
academic worries). Notably, the pattern of associations was replicated elementary school teachers have more bearing on youths’ academic
across youth-reported academic worries x mother-reported academic so­ adjustment in middle school. Additionally, as youth get older, teachers
cialization and mother-reported academic worries x youth-reported aca­ tend to communicate more directly with youth and less with parents
demic socialization models, highlighting the robustness of this finding. (Adams & Christenson, 2000), potentially explaining the non-significant
Although maternal schoolwork assistance did not moderate the findings for mother-teacher relationship quality in the present study.
prospective association between academic worries and youth academic Lastly, more limited support emerged in the prediction of youth
adjustment, more mother-reported schoolwork assistance before middle engagement in middle school. Only one finding emerged, such that
school was directly related to better academic performance after the youth-reported academic worries predicted more engagement in middle

8
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

school when mothers utilized more academic socialization. When youth upcoming school changes. Schoolwork assistance and academic social­
report more academic worries, maternal academic socialization could ization also provide opportunities for parents to connect with youth to
help to motivate youths’ efforts in school, resulting in higher levels of help them develop competence and relatedness, which can also support
engagement at school (Eysenck et al., 2007). Conversely, main and their autonomy as they transition to middle school. Yet, our findings also
interaction effects of youth academic worries and other indices of suggest that parents need to recognize that youth may have different
parental involvement did not significantly predict youths’ engagement types of worries that also warrant attention and parental support. Thus,
in middle school. Perhaps youth worries and parental involvement have parents should adjust their approaches and behaviors to meet youths’
less of an impact on the extent to which youth are engaging at school. developmental needs during the transition to middle school.
Other factors, such as the classroom environment (e.g., sense of safety,
being bored), teacher classroom management and responsiveness to­ Author note
wards students, and/or classmates’ behaviors and engagement may
have a greater influence on youth engagement at school (Skinner et al., This research was supported by the USDA National Institute of Food
2008). Further, in thinking about the context of the present study, school and Agriculture, Hatch Project (ILLU-793-344) awarded to Kelly M. Tu.
transitions are disruptive and have been linked with general declines in
engagement (Eccles et al., 1993; Skinner et al., 2008).
The current study is not without limitations. First, the study is con­ Declaration of Competing Interest
ducted with a community sample of adolescents, with relatively higher
levels of parental involvement and student academic performance and The authors have no conflict of interest to report.
engagement. Although we had a range of racial/ethnic minority youth
(comprising 43% of the sample), a majority of the sample was White/ Acknowledgements
European American and additional approaches are needed to unpack
between and within group differences among underrepresented racial/ The authors would like to thank the families and teachers for their
ethnic groups. Further, a majority of youth were from families with high participation in this study. We would also like to thank the school ad­
maternal education. Future studies with more diversity in family so­ ministrators for their assistance in recruitment. Lastly, we would like to
cioeconomic status and race/ethnicity would provide more insight acknowledge the project staff, especially Saira Gonzales, Kelsie Olsen,
about the extent to which parents’ home- and school-based involvement and Brett Cohen, for their involvement with data coding.
can alleviate academic worries, especially among at-risk groups or for
underrepresented youth (Barger et al., 2019) given some evidence that
References
early adolescents from lower socioeconomic status and minority or un­
derrepresented backgrounds are more likely to become disengaged from Acock, A. C. (2005). Working with missing values. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67(4),
school (Li & Lerner, 2011). Second, we measured the proportion of 1012–1028. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2005.00191.x
youths’ academic worries, out of their top three worries about middle Adams, K. S., & Christenson, S. L. (2000). Trust and the family-school relationship
examination of parent-teacher differences in elementary and secondary grades.
school, but not the severity of academic worries and/or their self- Journal of School Psychology, 38(5), 477–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405
evaluation of ability in relation to academic worries. The severity of (00)00048-0
academic worries may have different implications for academic adjust­ Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.
Sage.
ment as high levels of stress or anxiety could impede youths’ engage­ Arbuckle, J. L. (2017). IBM SPSS Amos 25 User’s guide (IBM SPSS).
ment in schoolwork (Goldstein et al., 2015; Skinner et al., 2008). Balfanz, R. (2009). Putting middle grades students on the graduation path: A policy and
Additionally, future studies might consider accounting for the value practice brief. June (pp. 1–16). National Middle School Association https://www.
amle.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/04/Policy_Brief_Balfanz.pdf.
youth place on engagement and academic performance and examine
Barger, M. M., Kim, E. M., Kuncel, N. R., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2019). The relation
how it corresponds with their self-evaluations of their abilities in between parents’ involvement in children’s schooling and children’s adjustment: A
informing youths’ worries to provide more information to better un­ meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 145(9), 855–890. https://doi.org/10.1037/
bul0000201
derstand some of the inconsistent findings and propositions in the
Cai, T., & Tu, K. M. (2020). Linking parental monitoring and psychological control with
literature (e.g., Lauermann, Eccles, & Pekrun, 2017; Pekrun, 2006). internalizing symptoms in early adolescence: The moderating role of vagal tone.
Third, although we measured youths’ academic worries prior to the Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 48(6), 809–821. https://doi.org/10.1007/
middle school transition and their engagement and academic perfor­ s10802-020-00631-w
Cheung, C. S. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2012). Why does parents’ involvement enhance
mance after the middle school transition, the study had a relatively short children’s achievement? The role of parent-oriented motivation. Journal of
time span (two waves spaced 7 months apart). Future studies with Educational Psychology, 104(3), 820–832. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027183
additional waves of assessments can provide more information about the Cheung, C. S. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2015). Value development underlies the benefits of
parents’ involvement in children’s learning: A longitudinal investigation in the
longer-term effects of academic worries. United States and China. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 309–320. https://
Despite these limitations, the current study highlights the impor­ doi.org/10.1037/a0037458
tance of maternal schoolwork assistance in promoting youths’ academic Coelho, V. A., Marchante, M., & Jimerson, S. R. (2017). Promoting a positive middle
school Ttansition: A randomized-controlled treatment study examining self-concept
performance in middle school, as well as maternal academic socializ­ and self-esteem. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(3), 558–569. https://doi.org/
ation in promoting higher academic performance and more engagement 10.1007/s10964-016-0510-6
in the face of academic worries. The findings from the current study are Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
informative for parents in recognizing how their involvement at home
Doctoroff, G. L., & Arnold, D. H. (2017). Doing homework together: The relation between
can help to promote youths’ academic performance during a potentially parenting strategies, child engagement, and achievement. Journal of Applied
disruptive time in early adolescence with the transition to middle Developmental Psychology, 48, 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
appdev.2017.01.001
school. In providing schoolwork assistance, parents can equip their
Dotterer, A. M., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2009). The development and correlates
youth with important skills that can go beyond a specific assignment. of academic interests from childhood through adolescence. Journal of Educational
Youth may gain a broader repertoire of strategies for how to address or Psychology, 101(2), 509–519. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013987
handle academic challenges or problems from direct instruction from Duchesne, S., Larose, S., Guay, F., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, R. E. (2005). The transition
from elementary to high school: The pivotal role of mother and child characteristics
parents and observing how parents are helping them with their work. in explaining trajectories of academic functioning. International Journal of Behavioral
Further, academic socialization or discussions about youths’ schoolwork Development, 29(5), 409–417. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250500206067
or study strategies more generally and communication about the value Duchesne, S., Ratelle, C. F., Poitras, S.-C., & Drouin, E. (2009). Early adolescent
attachment to parents, emotional problems, and teacher-academic worries about the
and importance of education also appears to have benefits for youth who middle school transition. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 29(5), 743–766. https://
may be more worried or anxious about their academics in relation to the doi.org/10.1177/0272431608325502

9
T. Cai and K.M. Tu Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 76 (2021) 101325

Duchesne, S., Ratelle, C. F., & Roy, A. (2012). Worries about middle school transition and Journal of Early Adolescence, 14(2), 162–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/
subsequent adjustment: The moderating role of classroom goal structure. Journal of 027243169401400205
Early Adolescence, 32(5), 681–710. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431611419506 Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical
Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2009). Schools, academic motivation, and stage- evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71(3), 543–562.
environment fit. In R. M. Lerner, & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00164
psychology: Individual bases of adolescent development (pp. 404–434). John Wiley & Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Eccles, J. S. (1989). Student/teacher relations and attitudes
Sons Inc. toward nathematics before and after the transition to junior high school. Child
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, M., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., & Development, 60(4), 981. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131038
Mac Iver, D. (1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage- Miller, H., Valentine, J. L., Fish, R., & Robinson, M. (2016). Is the feeling mutual?
environment fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in families. Examining parent-teacher relationships in low-income, predominantly Latino
American Psychologist, 48(2), 90–101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.2.90 schools. American Journal of Education, 123(1), 37–67. https://doi.org/10.1086/
von der Embse, N., Jester, D., Roy, D., & Post, J. (2018). Test anxiety effects, predictors, 688167
and correlates: A 30-year meta-analytic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 227 Odegaard, S. L., & Heath, J. A. (1992). Assisting the elementary school student in the
(November 2017), 483–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.048 transition to a middle level school. Middle School Journal, 24(2), 21–25. https://doi.
Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing org/10.1080/00940771.1992.11495163
efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6(6), 409–434. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). Parent involvement in homework: A
02699939208409696 research synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 78(4), 1039–1101. https://doi.
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive org/10.3102/0034654308325185
performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336–353. https://doi.org/ Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions,
10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational
Goldstein, S. E., Boxer, P., & Rudolph, E. (2015). Middle school transition stress: Links Psychology Review, 18(4), 315–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9
with academic performance, motivation, and school experiences. Contemporary Pierce, K. M., Hamm, J. V., & Vandell, D. L. (1999). Experiences in after-school programs
School Psychology, 19(1), 21–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40688-014-0044-4 and children’s adjustment in first-grade classrooms. Child Development, 70(3),
Green, C. L., Walker, J. M. T., Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., & Sandler, H. M. (2007). Parents’ 756–767. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00054
motivations for involvement in children’s education: An empirical test of a Pomerantz, E. M., Qin, L., Wang, Q., & Chen, H. (2011). Changes in early adolescents’
theoretical model of parental involvement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), sense of responsibility to their parents in the United States and China: Implications
532–544. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.532 for academic functioning. Child Development, 82(4), 1136–1151. https://doi.org/
Grolnick, W. S., Kurowski, C. O., Dunlap, K. G., & Hevey, C. (2000). Parental resources 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01588.x
and the transition to junior high. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10(4), 465–488. Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational Tools for Probing
https://doi.org/10.1207/SJRA1004_05 Interactions in Multiple Linear Regression, Multilevel Modeling, and Latent Curve
Grolnick, W. S., Raftery-Helmer, J. N., Flamm, E. S., Marbell, K. N., & Cardemil, E. V. Analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31(4), 437–448. https://doi.
(2015). Parental provision of academic structure and the transition to middle school. org/10.3102/10769986031004437
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25(4), 668–684. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Ratelle, C. F., Duchesne, S., & Guay, F. (2017). Predicting school adjustment from
jora.12161 multiple perspectives on parental behaviors. Journal of Adolescence, 54, 60–72.
Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents’ involvement in children’s van Rens, M., Haelermans, C., Groot, W., & Maassen van den Brink, H. (2018).
schooling: A multidimensional conceptualization and motivational model. Child Facilitating a successful transition to secondary school: (How) does it work? A
Development, 65(1), 237–252. systematic literature review. Adolescent Research Review, 3(1), 43–56. https://doi.
Gutman, L. M., Sameroff, A. J., & Cole, R. (2003). Academic growth curve trajectories org/10.1007/s40894-017-0063-2
from 1st grade to 12th grade: Effects of multiple social risk factors and preschool Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions
child factors. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 777–790. https://doi.org/10.1037/ and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67. https://doi.
0012-1649.39.4.777 org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1020
Hassrick, E. M., & Schneider, B. (2009). Parent surveillance in schools: A question of Seipp, B. (1991). Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis of findings.
social class. American Journal of Education, 115(4), 671. https://doi.org/10.1086/ Anxiety Research, 4(1), 27–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/08917779108248762
599787 Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement and
Herbers, J. E., Reynolds, A. J., & Chen, C. C. (2013). School mobility and developmental disaffection in the classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic? Journal of
outcomes in young adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 25(2), 501–515. Educational Psychology, 100(4), 765–781. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012840
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412001204 Wang, M. T., Hill, N. E., & Hofkens, T. (2014). Parental involvement and African
Hill, N. E., & Tyson, D. F. (2009). Parental involvement in middle school: A meta-analytic American and European American adolescents’ academic, behavioral, and emotional
assessment of the strategies that promote achievement. Developmental Psychology, 45 development in secondary school. Child Development, 85(6), 2151–2168. https://doi.
(3), 740–763. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015362 org/10.1111/cdev.12284
Hughes, J., & Kwok, O. M. (2007). Influence of student-teacher and parent-teacher Te Wang, M., & Peck, S. C. (2013). Adolescent educational success and mental health
relationships on lower achieving readers’ engagement and achievement in the vary across school engagement profiles. Developmental Psychology, 49(7),
primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(1), 39–51. https://doi.org/ 1266–1276. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030028
10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.39 Te Wang, M., & Sheikh-Khalil, S. (2014). Does parental involvement matter for student
Juvonen, J., Wang, Y., & Espinoza, G. (2011). Bullying experiences and compromised achievement and mental health in high school? Child Development, 85(2), 610–625.
academic performance across middle school grades. Journal of Early Adolescence, 31 https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12153
(1), 152–173. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431610379415 Tu, K. M., Cai, T., & Li, X. (2020). Adolescent coping with academic challenges: The role
Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2016). A public health of parental socialization of coping. Journal of Adolescence, 81(March), 27–38.
perspective on school dropout and adult outcomes: A prospective study of risk and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2020.03.008
protective factors from age 5 to 27 years. Journal of Adolescent Health, 58(6), Tu, K. M., Li, X., & Cohen, J. R. (2019). The “Heart” of depression during early
652–658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.01.014 adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology, 61(8), 1168–1179. https://doi.org/
Lauermann, F., Eccles, J. S., & Pekrun, R. (2017). Why do children worry about their 10.1002/dev.21862
academic achievement? An expectancy-value perspective on elementary students’ Tu, K. M., & Ravindran, N. (2020). Getting under the skin: Maternal social coaching and
worries about their mathematics and reading performance. ZDM - Mathematics adolescent peer adjustment. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 66, 101091.
Education, 49(3), 339–354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-017-0832-1 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2019.101091
Li, Y., & Lerner, R. M. (2011). Trajectories of school engagement during dolescence: Wei, J., Pomerantz, E. M., Ng, F. F. Y., Yu, Y., Wang, M., & Wang, Q. (2019). Why does
Implications for grades, depression, delinquency, and substance use. Developmental parents’ involvement in youth’s learning vary across elementary, middle, and high
Psychology, 47(1), 233–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021307 school? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 56, 262–274. https://doi.org/
Lord, S. E., Eccles, J. S., & McCarthy, K. A. (1994). Surviving the junior high school 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.12.007
transition family processes and self-perceptions as protective and risk factors. The

10

You might also like