You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Veterinary Behavior xxx (2017) 1e7

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Veterinary Behavior


journal homepage: www.journalvetbehavior.com

Research

Assessment of canine behaviors using C-BARQ in a sample from


Northern Mexico
Mónica Teresa González-Ramírez*, Lucía Quezada-Berumen, René Landero-Hernández
Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León, UANL, Fac. de Psicología, San Nicolás de los Garza, Nuevo León, México

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Problematic behaviors in dogs can negatively affect dogs’ welfare and the welfare of their owners. The
Received 25 August 2016 purpose of this study was to evaluate the psychometric properties of the Mexican version of the Canine
Received in revised form Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire and identify the variables associated with behavioral
13 March 2017
problems. The study involved 483 owners who completed the Canine Behavioral Assessment and
Accepted 16 March 2017
Available online xxx
Research Questionnaire, the Dog-owner Compatibility Index of Activity Preferences, and answered
several questions about their dogs. The most frequent behavior concern reported was stranger-directed
aggression, which correlated with other types of aggression, with age of dog, and with the compatibility
Keywords:
dog behavior
between dog and owner in activity preferences. Our results suggest that the human-dog interaction
aggression could improve with a proper handling of the dog.
C-BARQ Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
human-dog interaction

Introduction through interviews or self-report questionnaires. Among the op-


tions of self-report questionnaires, the Canine Behavioral Assess-
Unwanted behaviors in dogs can have serious implications in ment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) can be mentioned. It
their quality of life, as well as the quality of life of their owners was developed in English by Hsu and Serpell (2003) and has been
(Pirrone et al., 2015). Domestic dogs can show a wide variety of be- translated into several languages and used in different countries,
haviors that are viewed as unpleasant or annoying by their owners. including Italy (Marshall-Pescini et al., 2008), China (Hsu & Sun,
Some of the consequences of canine behavior problems mentioned 2010), Japan (Nagasawa et al., 2011), and Iran (Tamimi et al.,
in previous research include a broken dog-owner bond and the 2015). No studies with this questionnaire were found in Mexico.
possibility that a dog could be abandoned or euthanized (Col et al., To identify variables related to canine behaviors, we reviewed
2016). More than 30% of abandoned dogs were abandoned due to studies from different countries, finding some studies only reported
behavioral problems (Wells & Hepper, 2000). Common behavioral results of specific subscales (e.g., Serpell & Hsu, 2005; Marshall-
reasons for relinquishment include aggression toward people, Pescini et al., 2008; van den Berg et al., 2010). Among the findings
aggression to other animals, and destruction (Patronek et al., 1996). of these studies, the following can be mentioned. Bennett and Rohlf
The probabilities of being adopted are low (Lepper et al., 2002). (2007), in Australia, found that the age of the dog was associated
According to the National Council on Pet Population Study and with aggression behaviors, anxiety, and nervousness. Disobedience
Policy, in 1997, the number of dogs euthanized annually in Amer- was positively correlated with family size, and small dogs were
ican shelters due to behavioral problems is estimated in the mil- considered more disobedient; whereas small and medium dogs
lions (American Humane Association, 2013). showed more excitability than big ones.
The assessment of unwanted behaviors in dogs can be per- In Germany, Kubinyi et al. (2009) found that owners who spent
formed with direct observation or reports from their owners more time with their dogs reported them as being calmer, more
trainable, and more sociable. Likewise, it was reported that the least
trainable, calm, and sociable dogs did not receive professional training.
* Address for reprint requests and correspondence: Mónica Teresa González- In Taiwan, Hsu and Sun (2010) found that higher scores of
Ramírez, Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León, UANL, Fac. de Psicología, Av. Uni-
versidad S/N, Ciudad Universitaria, San Nicolás de los Garza, Nuevo León C.P., 66451,
owner-directed aggression were associated with male older dogs
México. Tel: þ52 81 83294050; Fax: +52 81 83338222. that had been sterilized, whereas higher scores of dog-directed
E-mail address: monygzz77@yahoo.com (M.T. González-Ramírez). aggression were associated with living in houses with less space,

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.03.007
1558-7878/Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
2 M.T. González-Ramírez et al. / Journal of Veterinary Behavior xxx (2017) 1e7

sharing the house with more people, and less shared time with Questions gathered data about the dogs, such as breed, size, age,
the owner. sex, number of dogs living in the same household, reproductive
In Italy, Pirrone et al. (2015) found that although 65% of partic- status, training history, and whether the dog was walked at least
ipants reported unwanted behaviors in their dogs, these were not once a day. Participants were asked to answer the survey thinking
considered problematic, and owners were not interested to modify about 1 specific dog. If a participant wanted to, they could answer 1
them. These behaviors were variations of fear and anxiety; it was survey for each one of their dogs.
also found that smaller dogs barked the most. It is possible that The sample consisted of 483 people who responded to the
unwanted behaviors were not considered problematic because questionnaire, with a mean age of 31.5 years (standard deviation
owners did not think these were detrimental to the welfare of dogs [SD] ¼ 8.7), 82.2% (397) were women and 17.8% (86) were men.
or humans involved. However, an unwanted behavior can be With respect to marital status, 51.6% (249) were single, 33.3% (161)
considered a problematic behavior, even if the owner is not aware were married, 11.8% (57) were living together, and the rest was
of its negative effects. either separated or divorced.
Recently, Col et al. (2016) found the dog’s breed, sex, neuter Sixty-five different breeds were reported. Mixed breeds were
status, the owners’ socioeconomic status, and his work routine most common (85; 17.6%), followed by Chihuahuas (45; 9.3%),
were all associated with some canine behavior problems. schnauzers, and poodles (33; 6.8% each). Dog groups were analyzed
Bennett and Rohlf (2007) report that sterilized dogs were more by size rather than by breed because there were many breeds with
nervous or shy. Kubinyi et al. (2009) reported that sterilized dogs few dogs. Seventeen percent (82) were extra small dogs (3-5 kg),
were less calm, whereas Hsu and Sun (2010) found lower scores in 32.5% (157) were small (5-12 kg), 25.3% (122) were medium-sized
owner-directed aggression in sterilized dogs. For their part, Serpell (12-25 kg), 22.2% (107) were large (25-40 kg), and 3.1% (n ¼ 15)
and Hsu (2005) reported that reproductive status was associated were extra-large dogs (over 40 kg). The mean age of dogs was
with trainability in males of the Shetland sheepdog breed, but not 4.1 years (SD ¼ 3.1); 51.6% (249) were female and 47.8% (231) were
on females. male and 3 unknown. Participants who owned 1 dog accounted for
The previous studies are evidence of the current interest to 47.2% (228), 26.9% (130) reported having 2 dogs, 14.3% (69) reported
research variables related to canine behavior. Payne et al. (2015) having 3 dogs, and the rest (56) had between 4 and 9 dogs. In relation
indicate that human factors have been an interest element in to reproductive status, 60% (290) were not sterilized and 40% (193)
these researches. In this manner, Meyer and Forkman (2014) found, were sterilized. Regarding training history, 72.9% (352) did not
in a sample of owners of Danish dogs, a negative association be- receive formal training by a dog trainer, 27.1% (131) did receive formal
tween fear-related behavioral problems and the owner-dog rela- training by a dog trainer. Concerning the frequency of dog walking,
tionship. González-Ramírez et al. (2017) found a relationship 9.9% (48) reported never taking their dog for a walk, 28.6% (138) re-
between owner-dog relationship and compatibility; they measured ported they walked their dog sometimes, 32.5% (157) said most of the
compatibility using activity preferences as indicator of the adjust- time, and 28.4% (137) reported walking their dog at least once a day.
ment between the animal and its owner in the behavioral dimen-
sion; activity preference is to share the love for daily activities like Instruments
walking, interacting with others, and so on.
Thus, although there are results from studies carried out in The Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire,
different countries, Hsu and Sun (2010) indicate that the dog-owner C-BARQÓ, developed in English by Hsu and Serpell (2003) was used
interaction, the way to perceive this interaction, and the behavior of to evaluate the owners assessment of their dogs. The questionnaire
the dog, probably varies from one culture to another. For example, consists of 14 subscales about observable and specific behaviors
González-Ramírez and Landero (2014) mentioned that dogs with Likert scale response options of 5 points. The scale evaluates
frequently live indoors in many countries; however, in Mexico, a lot the intensity of behaviors related to aggression, fear, and excit-
of them live in backyards; this suggests that people spend less time ability, and the frequency of the rest of the behaviors (0 indicates
interacting with their dogs, even when they are at home. In absence of behavior, and 4 is the highest intensity or frequency.
Australia, Col et al. (2016) found that time spent at home is an Authorization to use the C-BARQ was provided with the most
important factor for minimizing the risk of behavior problems. recent, who version of C-BARQ with 100 items (J. Serpell, personal
Taking these differences into account, it is necessary to continue communication, 2014), and a Spanish version translated in Spain.
studies in this line of research, to identify common factors in However, this Spanish version was not used because several items
different countries that are associated with certain behaviors in were difficult to understand due to cultural differences between Spain
dogs. For this reason, the purpose of the study was to evaluate the and Latin America. Back translation was used to translate C-BARQ100.
psychometric properties of the Mexican version of the C-BARQ as The subscales of the C-BARQ are the following:
well as to identify variables associated with canine behavior
problems in a sample from Northern Mexico. 1. Stranger-directed aggression: the dog shows aggressive or
threatening behaviors toward strangers who approach to him,
to his owner, his territory, or home range.
Materials and methods 2. Owner-directed aggression: the dog shows aggressive or
threatening behaviors toward the owner or family members
Participants when they are scolded, mistreated, when stared, or when
approached while they eat or have an object in their possession.
The participants were residents from northern Mexico over the 3. Dog-directed aggression: the dog shows aggressive or threat-
age of 18 years. The only requirement to participate in the study ening behaviors when an unknown dog approaches directly.
was to own at least 1 dog. The questionnaire was in Spanish and 4. Dog rivalry: the dog shows aggressive or threatening behaviors
available online through SurveyMonkey.com. The questionnaire toward other familiar dogs in the same household.
link was shared across social networks. Snowball sampling was 5. Stranger-directed fear: the dog looks cautious or afraid when a
used, where each person was asked to invite other people to answer stranger approaches directly.
the questionnaire. Participants not completing all questions were 6. Nonsocial fear: the dog looks cautious or afraid of sudden or
excluded. All data were treated confidentially. loud noises, traffic, or unfamiliar objects or situations.
M.T. González-Ramírez et al. / Journal of Veterinary Behavior xxx (2017) 1e7 3

7. Dog-directed fear: the dog looks cautious or afraid when an association between the C-BARQ subscales and the size of the dog,
unknown dog approaches directly. their age, the age of owner, number of dogs living in the same
8. Touch sensitivity: the dog looks cautious or afraid toward household, dog walking routine, and the Dog-owner Compatibility
potentially painful or uncomfortable procedures, such as Index of Activity Preferences. Likewise, the Mann-Whitney test was
bathing, grooming, nail clipping, or vet examinations. used to evaluate the difference between subscales of the C-BARQ in
9. Separation-related behavior: the dog vocalizes and/or destroys relation to sex, reproductive status, and training history. The Kruskal-
things when they are separated from their owner, often Wallis test was used to evaluate differences between subscales of the
accompanied or preceded by behaviors and signs of anxiety such C-BARQ with respect to the size of the dogs. All analyses were per-
as restlessness, loss of appetite, tremors, or excessive salivation. formed using SPSS (version 20.0, IBM SPSS Statistics 20 software;
10. Attachment or attention seeking: the dog keeps a close prox- SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois, USA).
imity to their owner or other family members, seeks affection
or attention, and shows signs of agitation (whines, jumps up, Results
tries to intervene) when the owner pays attention to others.
11. Trainability: the dog shows willingness to comply and obey Exploratory factor analysis and reliability
simple instructions from owner. The dog is not easily distracted,
tends to learn fast, recover objects, and when is corrected or Factor analysis was performed first to group items or variables
punished respond, performing behavior previously asked. into dimensions. The solution was forced to 14 factors that
12. Chasing: the dog chases cats, birds, and/or small animals when explained 63.4% of the variance. The value of the explained variance
given the chance. is high, showing that 14 factors are sufficient to have most of the
13. Excitability: the dog overreacts to potentially exciting events, information collected by individual items. However, several factors
such as walks, car trips, hearing the doorbell, arrival of visitors, were residual. The first factor grouped the items corresponding to
or the owner arriving home, and has a difficulty to calm down stranger-directed aggression; factor 2 grouped the subscales dog-
after these events. directed fear and stranger-directed fear; factor 3 corresponded to
14. Energy: the dog is full of energy, always in movement or the subscale owner-directed aggression; factor 4 corresponded to
playing. the subscale dog-directed aggression, however, the items of this
subscale also showed high loading on factor 1. Factor 4 included
The C-BARQ also provides information on behavioral problems, items of the dog rivalry subscale. Factor 5 corresponded to the
ranging from coprophagia to repetitive behaviors such as spinning trainability subscale; item 8 (corresponding to fetching objects)
or chasing their tail. These questions are scored independently. showed a superior load on factor 12. Factor 6 corresponded to the
Hsu and Sun (2010) indicate that the items corresponding to subscale separation-related behavior. Factor 7 corresponded to the
each factor vary slightly in the different versions, so analyzing the subscale excitability. Factor 8 included items related to the chasing
factor structure of each new translation will ensure that each factor subscale. Factor 9 corresponded to items related to touch sensi-
is represented by the corresponding items. tivity. Factor 10 corresponded to attachment/attention seeking.
Owner-dog compatibility was evaluated using the Dog-owner Factor 11 grouped the items corresponding to nonsocial fear,
Compatibility Index of Activity Preferences by González-Ramírez although 3 of these items showed high loading on factor 2. Factor 12
et al. (2017). This questionnaire evaluates preferences of the grouped items related to energy level. The rest of the factors did not
owner and the perceived behavior of the dog in relation to 6 ac- present a clear clustering of items (Table 1).
tivities; each of the 6 activities has a section for the owner and a Internal consistency of the original subscales was analyzed,
section corresponding to the dog (i.e., section for the owner: On the finding adequate values (Table 1), so it was decided to continue the
weekend or a nonworking day, you would like to walk or to run in analysis with the original 14 subscales and not with the results of
the park, section for the dog: your dog enjoys long walks in the the factor analysis to obtain more specific information about the
park). The questionnaire is a 4-point Likert scale, with response classification of behavioral problems, rather than regrouping the
options ranging from completely disagree to completely agree. results according to the factor analysis.
The scoring of the compatibility index is as follows. It is first Table 2 presents descriptive statistics corresponding to the
determined if there is dog-owner compatibility for each of the 6 original subscales of the C-BARQ. The most frequent problematic
situations presented. Compatibility is considered when the answer behavior is stranger-directed aggression, followed by excitability.
for both the dog and the owner is 0 or 1 (response options completely The attachment/attention-seeking subscale shows central tendency
disagree or disagree), or 2 or 3 (response options completely agree or measures similar to excitability; however, it is not possible to assure
agree). If the answer for an activity is 0 or 1 for owner and 2 or 3 for that the items of this subscale are considered behaviors that bother
dog, it is not compatible. In addition, if the answer for an activity is 2 or annoy the owners. Similarly, a high level of trainability was re-
or 3 for owner and 0 or 1 for dog, it is not compatible. Afterward, the ported, which indicates that participants are satisfied with the
number of compatibilities is divided by 6 and multiplied by 100. The degree of obedience of their dogs.
authors reported a 0.82 Cronbach’s alpha. Table 3 shows that behavioral problems do not occur in an iso-
lated way. Correlations between the subscales of the C-BARQ occur
Statistical analysis for stranger-directed aggression with dog-directed aggression and
stranger-directed fear; owner-directed aggression with dog rivalry;
To assess the reliability of the questionnaires, Cronbach’s alpha dog-directed aggression with dog rivalry and chasing; dog-directed
coefficient was performed. An exploratory factor analysis was per- fear with stranger-directed fear, nonsocial fear and touch sensi-
formed by principal component analysis with varimax extraction on tivity; stranger-directed fear with nonsocial fear and touch sensi-
the C-BARQ. The analysis included only the items corresponding to tivity; nonsocial fear with touch sensitivity; as well as excitability
the 14 subscales, independently scored questions about behaviors with attachment/attention seeking.
were excluded from the analysis. Normality of the distribution of the Behaviors that are scored independently and had a mean equal
scores of the subscales was assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov to or higher than 1.5 were as follows: begs persistently for food
test, indicating that the scores were not normally distributed (P < when people are eating (mean [M] ¼ 1.9; SD ¼ 1.5), pulls exces-
0.05). The Spearman correlation coefficient was used to analyze the sively hard when on the leash (M ¼ 1.65; SD ¼ 1.4), and barks
4 M.T. González-Ramírez et al. / Journal of Veterinary Behavior xxx (2017) 1e7

Table 1
Factorial structure and reliability of the C-BARQ Mexico

Behavior Subscale Items in the subscale Factor loads Cronbach’s alpha Inter-item Comments
correlation

Aggressive Factor 1 ¼ stranger-directed 10 þ 11 þ 12 þ 15 þ 16 þ 18 þ 0.643-0.836 0.92 0.63-0.78


behavior aggression 20 þ21 þ 22 þ 28
Factor 3 ¼ owner-directed 9 þ 13 þ 14 þ 17 þ 19 þ 25 þ 30 þ 31 0.534-0.749 0.84 0.48-0.69
aggression
Factor 4 ¼ dog-directed 23 þ 24 þ 26 þ 29 0.428-0.485 0.87 0.66-0.82 All items showed high
aggression Item 24 < 0.300 loadings on factor
2 (0.520-0.575)
Factor 4 ¼ dog rivalry 32 þ 33 þ 34 þ 35 0.726-0.795 0.87 0.67-0.75
Fear Factor 2 ¼ dog-directed fear 45 þ 46 þ 52 þ 53 0.685-0.763 0.86 0.66-0.78
Factor 2 ¼ stranger-directed 36 þ 37 þ 39 þ 40 0.675-0.759 0.88 0.68-0.78
fear
Factor 11 ¼ nonsocial fear 38 þ 41 þ 42 þ 44 þ 47 þ 48 0.350-0.609 0.72 0.31-0.52 Items 41, 42 and 47 also
showed loading on factor
2 (0.600, 0.431, and 0.409)
Other behaviors Factor 5 ¼ trainability 1þ2þ3þ4þ5þ6þ7þ8 0.515-0.683 0.77 0.17-0.58 Item 8 loaded on factor
Item 12 < 0.300 Eliminating 0.41-0.59 12 (0.611) and its interitem
item 8, a ¼ 0.78 correlation was r ¼ 0.17
Factor 8 ¼ chasing 27 þ 74 þ 75 þ 76 0.317-0.842 0.72 0.28-0.67 Item 76 loaded higher on
Item 76 ¼ 0.317 Eliminating 0.49-0.70 factor 5 (0.462)
item 76, a ¼ 0.78
Factor 6 ¼ separation-related 54 þ 55 þ 56 þ 57 þ 58 þ 59 þ 60 þ 61 0.311-0.808 0.78 0.32-0.62 Item 55 also loaded on
behavior factor 13 (0.544)
Factor 7 ¼ excitability 62 þ 63 þ 64 þ 65 þ 66 þ 67 0.576-0.799 0.80 0.48-0.64
Factor 9 ¼ touch sensitivity 43 þ 49 þ 50 þ 51 0.419-0.774 0.76 0.45-0.62 Item 43 also loaded on
factor 11 (0.457)
Factor 10 ¼ attachment or 68 þ 69 þ 70 þ 71 þ 72 þ 73 0.539-0.797 0.69 0.06-0.53 Item 73 loaded on
attention seeking Item 73 load Eliminating 0.42-0.55 factor 8 (0.743)
< 0.300 item 73, a ¼ 0.75
Factor 12 ¼ energy level 91 þ 92 0.446 and 0.640 0.39 0.24

C-BARQ, Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire.

persistently (M ¼ 1.8; SD ¼ 1.5). These behaviors were included in directed aggression, dog-directed fear, stranger-directed fear,
the following analysis. touch sensitivity, and begging persistently for food.
Participants who reported more compatibility with their dogs
Relationships between variables and the subscales of the C-BARQ (Dog-owner Compatibility Index of Activity Preferences) also re-
ported less stranger, owner, and dog-directed aggression, less dog
Correlations between the subscales of the C-BARQ with age, size rivalry, and the more compatibility, more trainability. Similarly,
of the dog, and frequency of dog walking were analyzed. Several those who considered their dog to be high in trainability reported
significant correlations were identified, some of these were weak walking them at least once a day, whereas dogs with higher scores
(rs < 0.15), so they were not considered relevant. The results in in persistent bark were walked less.
Table 4 show that age of owner was negatively correlated with
separation-related behaviors and touch sensitivity, and positively
correlated with trainability. Age of dog was positively correlated Group differences
with stranger-directed aggression, dog-directed aggression, and
dog rivalry and was negatively associated with separation-related Training
behavior and energy level. The size of the dog was positively Although the correlations between the trainability subscale
correlated with trainability and negatively correlated with owner- and the rest of the subscales of the C-BARQ were lower than 0.25,
significant differences were found in relation to training history.
As was expected, dogs with a formal training history (131) scored
Table 2 higher in trainability (Z ¼ 6.883; P ¼ 0.001), they also obtained
Descriptive statistics for the subscales of the C-BARQ Mexico
lower scoring than dogs with no training (352) in the following
Behavior Subscale Median Mean Standard behaviors: owner-directed aggression (Z ¼ 2.060; P ¼ 0.039),
deviation nonsocial fear (Z ¼ 3.714; P ¼ 0.001), separation-related
Aggression Stranger-directed aggression 2.8 2.9 0.7 behaviors (Z ¼ 2.138; P ¼ 0.032), touch sensitivity (Z ¼ 3.106;
Owner-directed aggression 0.0 0.3 0.5 P ¼ 0.002), and begging persistently for food when people are
Dog-directed aggression 1.0 1.1 1.0 eating (Z ¼ 2.414; P ¼ 0.016). In relation to persistent barking,
Dog rivalry 0.2 0.6 0.8
Fear Dog-directed fear 0.5 0.9 0.9
the median of the dogs with no training was double than those
Stranger-directed fear 0.2 0.6 0.9 who did received training; however, no significant difference was
Nonsocial fear 0.8 1.0 0.7 found (Z ¼ 1.940; P ¼ 0.052).
Other behaviors Trainability 2.4 2.3 0.8
Chasing 1.0 1.0 0.9
Sex
Separation-related behavior 0.7 0.8 0.7
Excitability 2.2 2.2 0.9 When comparing males (n ¼ 231) and females (249), it was found
Touch sensitivity 0.5 0.9 0.9 that females showed significantly less owner-directed aggression
Attachment or attention seeking 2.3 2.2 0.8 (Z ¼ 2.491; P ¼ 0.013) and less dog rivalry (Z ¼ 2.459; P ¼ 0.014)
Energy level 1.5 1.5 0.9 than males. However, females showed higher scores in stranger-
C-BARQ, Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire. directed fear (Z ¼ 2.093; P ¼ 0.036) than males.
M.T. González-Ramírez et al. / Journal of Veterinary Behavior xxx (2017) 1e7 5

Table 3
Spearman’s correlations between subscales of the C-BARQ

Subscale a) b) c) d) e) f) g) h) i) j) k) l) m)

a) Stranger-directed aggression
b) Owner-directed aggression 0.273b
c) Dog-directed aggression 0.527b 0.250b
d) Dog-directed fear 0.200b 0.136b 0.207b
e) Dog rivalry 0.236b 0.300b 0.423b 0.126b
f) Trainability 0.098a 0.176b 0.103a 0.157b 0.077
g) Chasing 0.228b 0.072 0.348b 0.103a 0.154b 0.129b
h) Stranger-directed fear 0.363b 0.144b 0.190b 0.562b 0.107a 0.150b 0.083
i) Nonsocial fear 0.251b 0.170b 0.165b 0.375b 0.158b 0.193b 0.166b 0.440b
j) Separation-related behavior 0.112a 0.150b 0.079 0.281b 0.049 0.218b 0.155b 0.227b 0.283b
k) Touch sensitivity 0.176b 0.244b 0.153b 0.397b 0.105a 0.209b 0.089 0.333b 0.334b 0.244b
l) Excitability 0.224b 0.120b 0.148b 0.205b 0.016 0.041 0.244b 0.177b 0.191b 0.266b 0.267b
m) Attachment or attention seeking 0.158b 0.076 0.167b 0.185b 0.095a 0.064 0.233b 0.094a 0.162b 0.226b 0.182b 0.388b
n) Energy level 0.033 0.037 0.069 0.050 0.025 0.055 0.165b 0.027 0.106a 0.257b 0.062 0.210b 0.141b

C-BARQ, Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire.


a
The correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed).
b
The correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2 tailed).

Reproductive status Discussion


Mean age of spaying/neutering was 2.2 years (SD ¼ 2.2); 40%
(77) were sterilized under the age of 1 year, and 9.5% (18) over the The main purpose of the study was to adapt the C-BARQ to
age of 5 years. Sterilized (193) and nonsterilized dogs (290) were Mexican population and to analyze its psychometric properties.
compared, and differences were found in trainability (Z ¼ 4.065; Akin to the study of Tamimi et al. (2015), factor analysis showed a
P ¼ 0.001), separation-related behaviors (Z ¼ 2.678; P ¼ 0.007), similar structure to the original scale, with some variations. It was
attachment/attention seeking (Z ¼ 2.528; P ¼ 0.011), and energy decided to keep the original proposal of the item distribution in the
level (Z ¼ 2.207; P ¼ 0.027); with sterilized dogs being the ones subscales, due to the internal consistency and factor loading being
with better behavior. Differences in trainability remained after acceptable. Although the factor analysis suggested to simplify the
separating by sex of the dog, whereas the rest of the differences did factor structure of the questionnaire, in so doing, information
not remain. Nonsterilized male dogs (Z ¼ 2.072; P ¼ 0.038) would be lost about specific stimulus of the behaviors evaluated by
exhibited more separation-related behaviors. the C-BARQ. It is recommended that future studies perform internal
consistency analysis to adequate the items that better represent
Size each behavior in a particular sample.
Table 5 summarizes significant differences identified by the Among the items with high factor load in more than 1 subscale,
Kruskal-Wallis test between size of dogs and some subscales of the it is of interest to mention item 8 (Will “fetch” or attempt to fetch
C-BARQ. For owner-related aggression, small dogs presented higher sticks, balls, or objects), which was considered part of the train-
scores. For dog-directed fear, extra-small dogs showed higher ability subscale; in this sample, its factor load placed it in the
scores. For dog rivalry, medium-sized dogs obtained a higher score. subscale energy. For this sample, it seems that dogs with more
For trainability, the scores increase as the size of the dog increases. energy are the ones trained to fetch. However, the item remained in
For stranger-directed fear, non-social fear, touch sensitivity, and the subscale trainability because its elimination did not increase
begging persistently for food, extra-small dogs showed higher Cronbach’s alpha considerably.
scores. Last, the greater the size of dog, the more pulling excessively Dogs with higher scores in the trainability subscale were walked
on the leash. more often and were more compatible with their owners in the

Table 4
Spearman’s correlations between subscales of the C-BARQ and other variables

Subscale Age of owner Age of dog Size Walking the dog at least once a day Dog-owner compatibility index of activity preferences

Stranger-directed aggression 0.037 0.200a 0.067 0.067 0.193a


Owner-directed aggression 0.078 0.053 0.173a 0.102 0.193a
Dog-directed aggression 0.011 0.202a 0.052 0.060 0.214a
Dog-directed fear 0.043 0.083 0.148a 0.067 0.105
Dog rivalry 0.038 0.187a 0.076 0.113 0.203a
Trainability 0.171a 0.069 0.177a 0.218a 0.200a
Chasing 0.027 0.017 0.042 0.060 0.049
Stranger-directed fear 0.093 0.014 0.212a 0.080 0.124
Nonsocial fear 0.113 0.028 0.106 0.087 0.112
Separation-related behavior 0.247a 0.202a 0.090 0.099 0.007
Touch sensitivity 0.145a 0.003 0.262a 0.113 0.179
Excitability 0.066 0.004 0.023 0.031 0.067
Attachment/attention seeking 0.083 0.021 0.075 0.019 0.004
Energy level 0.017 0.249a 0.058 0.095 0.093
Begs for food 0.075 0.027 0.202a 0.067 0.098
Pulls on the leash 0.021 0.154 0.159 0.004 0.038
Persistent barking 0.152 0.044 0.097 0.163a 0.098

C-BARQ, Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire.


a
The correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2 tailed).
6 M.T. González-Ramírez et al. / Journal of Veterinary Behavior xxx (2017) 1e7

Table 5
Differences according to dog size

Subscale Extra small, 3-5 kg Small, 5-12 kg Medium, 12-25 kg Large, 25-40 kg Extra large, 40 kg Kruskal-Wallis
(n ¼ 82) (n ¼ 157) (n ¼ 122) (n ¼ 107) (n ¼ 15)

Median Median Median Median Median

Stranger-directed aggression 2.8 3.0 2.8 2.8 2.6 KW ¼ 9.461; P ¼ 0.051


Owner-directed aggression 0.0 0.1 0.0 0.0 0.0 KW ¼ 22.526; P ¼ 0.001
Dog-directed aggression 0.8 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.5 KW ¼ 6.253; P ¼ 0.181
Dog-directed fear 1.0 0.8 0.8 0.5 0.5 KW ¼ 11.348; P ¼ 0.023
Dog rivalry 0.3 0.3 0.5 0.0 0.0 KW ¼ 12.145; P ¼ 0.016
Trainability 2.1 2.3 2.4 2.5 2.6 KW ¼ 15.293; P ¼ 0.004
Chasing 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 0.5 KW ¼ 5.951; P ¼0.203
Stranger-directed fear 0.8 0.5 0.1 0.0 0.0 KW ¼ 22.215; P ¼ 0.001
Nonsocial fear 1.0 0.8 1.0 0.7 0.5 KW ¼ 13.230; P ¼ 0.010
Separation-related behavior 0.6 0.7 0.6 0.6 0.4 KW ¼ 6.903; P ¼ 0.141
Touch sensitivity 0.9 1.0 0.5 0.5 0.3 KW ¼ 36.354; P ¼ 0.001
Excitability 2.0 2.3 2.0 2.2 2.2 KW¼ 6.424; P ¼ 0.170
Attachment/attention 2.3 2.3 2.2 2.2 2.5 KW ¼ 7.224; P ¼ 0.125
Energy level 1.5 1.5 1.5 1.5 1.0 KW ¼ 5.442; P ¼ 0.245
Begs for food 3.0 2.0 2.0 1.0 2.0 KW ¼ 20.103; P ¼ 0.001
Pulls on the leash 1.0 1.0 2.0 2.0 2.0 KW ¼ 19.975; P ¼ 0.001
Barks persistently 2.0 2.0 2.0 1.0 2.0 KW ¼ 9.821; P ¼ 0.044

Bold italicized values correspond to higher scores when significant differences were found.

index of activity preferences. Similarly, the dogs that were walked received previous training or not. These last 2 coincide with the
more often had lower scores of persistent barking; it would be Australian study by Bennett and Rohlf (2007), and the association
interesting to consider the results of Marshall-Pescini et al. (2008) with the training variable coincides with the study by Kubinyi et al.
who found that higher levels of trainability helped improve (2009). As is suggested by Hsu and Sun (2010), certain behavioral
problem-solving ability in dogs, by being proactive in the interaction problems such as those related to aggression could be reduced with
with new objects. Marshall-Pescini et al. (2008) also found a negative a proper management of the dog which should include more
correlation between trainability and fear of strangers. Enhanced involvement of the owner in dog-related activities, which could
trainability may diminish/prevent behavioral problems and improve improve the owner-dog relationship (Meyer & Forkman, 2014).
owner-dog relationship. To improve trainability, we suggest an There are limitations to this study. As Bennett and Rohlf (2007)
environment of high stimulation, frequent walks, completion of an note, it is presumed that people who voluntarily participate in this
obedience training program, and owner-dog sharing activities. type of studies are committed to their dogs; therefore, it is expected
The highest scores in this sample were for stranger-directed that dogs present adequate behavior or that their owners are
aggression, followed by excitability (Table 2). These results differ satisfied with their behaviors. Nevertheless, it is also possible that
from those reported in Australia, where few dogs scored high in people committed to their dogs have dogs with serious behavior
aggressive behaviors (Bennett & Rohlf, 2007), although they are problems due to a lack of rules and structure in their daily routine.
similar to findings in Taiwan (Hsu & Sun, 2010), where stranger- In addition, most of the samples were women, a common occur-
directed aggression was one of the most frequent behaviors re- rence in these kinds of studies (e.g., Bennett & Rohlf, 2007); also,
ported, after dog-directed aggression. this is not a representative sample from Mexico.
One of the most frequently reported independently scored be- In conclusion, certain variables were identified to have associ-
haviors in this sample was persistent barking, results that differ ation with specific behaviors, including dog-owner compatibility of
from those presented by Bennett and Rohlf (2007) for an Australian activities, size of the dog, age of dog, and history of training. These
sample, where owners reported that their dogs generally did not results suggest that an early training, as well as a selection of a dog
bark persistently. In contrast, a recent report noted that barking was compatible with the level of activity of the owner, could benefit the
the second most common behavior problem reported in another dog-owner interaction. For future research, it would be interesting
Australian survey (Col et al., 2016). Pirrone et al. (2015) reported a to question owners about the type of method used for training and
correlation between persistent barking and size of the dog for their to compare their effect on dog behavior.
Italian sample which is similar to the present study, where smaller
dogs barked the most. References
Behavioral problems correlate with each other (Table 3), which
may indicate that dogs demonstrate the same type of behavior in American Humane Association, 2013. Animal Shelter Euthanasia. Available at: http://
different settings. This finding may be seen specifically in behaviors www.americanhumane.org/animals/stop-animal-abuse/fact-sheets/animal-
shelter-euthanasia.html . Accessed May 15, 2017.
of aggression and fear. Bennett, P.C., Rohlf, V.I., 2007. Owner-companion dog interactions: relationships
Significant differences between subscales of the C-BARQ were between demographic variables, potentially problematic behaviours, training
found when comparing sterilized and nonsterilized dogs. In this engagement and shared activities. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 102 (1-2), 65e84.
Col, R., Day, C., Phillips, C.J., 2016. An epidemiological analysis of dog behavior
sample, sterilized dogs presented fewer separation-related behav- problems presented to an Australian behavior clinic, with associated risk fac-
iors. This finding differs from that of Kubinyi et al. (2009), where tors. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 15, 1e11.
sterilized dogs were less calm, and that of Bennett and Rohlf (2007), González-Ramírez, M.T., Landero, R., 2014. Benefits of dog ownership: comparative
study of equivalent samples. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 9 (6), 311e315.
who reported sterilized dogs were more nervous.
González-Ramírez, M., Landero, R., Vanegas, M., 2017. Dog-owner compatibility
The behavior with the highest frequency was stranger-directed index of activity preferences. Hum. Anim. Interact. Bull. 5(1), 58-68.
aggression, and the only variable associated with this behavior Hsu, Y., Serpell, J.A., 2003. Development and validation of a questionnaire for
was age of the dog. Among the analyzed variables, the ones asso- measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc.
223 (9), 1293e1300.
ciated with more subscales of the C-BARQ were dog-owner Hsu, Y., Sun, L., 2010. Factors associated with aggressive responses in pet dogs. Appl.
compatibility of activities, size of the dog, and whether they Anim. Behav. Sci. 123 (3-4), 108e123.
M.T. González-Ramírez et al. / Journal of Veterinary Behavior xxx (2017) 1e7 7

Kubinyi, E., Turcsán, B., Miklósi, A., 2009. Dog and owner demographic character- Payne, E., Bennett, P., McGreevy, P., 2015. Current perspectives on attachment and
istics and dog personality trait associations. Behav. Processes. 81 (3), 392e401. bonding in the dog-human dyad. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 8, 71e79.
Lepper, M., Kass, P.H., Hart, L.A., 2002. Prediction of adoption versus euthanasia Pirrone, F., Pierantoni, L., Mazzola, S.M., Vigo, D., Albertini, M., 2015. Owner and
among dogs and cats in a California animal shelter. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 5 (1), animal factors predict the incidence of, and owner reaction toward, problematic
29e42. behaviors in companion dogs. J. Vet. Behav.: Clin. Appl. Res. 10 (4), 295e301.
Marshall-Pescini, S., Valsecchi, P., Petak, I., Accorsi, P.A., Previde, E.P., 2008. Does Serpell, J.A., Hsu, Y.A., 2005. Effects of breed, sex, and neuter status on trainability in
training make you smarter? The effects of training on dogs’ performance (Canis dogs. Anthrozoös 18 (3), 196e207.
familiaris) in a problem solving task. Behav. Processes 78 (3), 449e454. Tamimi, N., Jamshidi, S., Serpell, J.A., Mousavi, S., Ghasempourabadi, Z., 2015.
Meyer, I., Forkman, B., 2014. Dog and owner characteristics affecting the dog-owner Assessment of the C-BARQ for evaluating dog behavior in Iran. J. Vet. Behav.:
relationship. J. Vet. Behav. 9 (4), 143e150. Clin. Appl. Res. 10 (1), 36e40.
Nagasawa, M., Tsujimura, A., Tateishi, K., Mogi, K., Ohta, M., Serpell, J.A., Kikusui, T., van den Berg, S.M., Heuven, H.C., van den Berg, L., Duffy, D.L., Serpell, J.A., 2010.
2011. Assessment of the factorial structures of the C-BARQ in Japan. J. Vet. Med. Evaluation of the C-BARQ as a measure of stranger-directed aggression in three
Sci. 73 (7), 869e875. common dog breeds. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 124 (3-4), 136e141.
Patronek, G.J., Glickman, L.T., Beck, A.M., McCabe, G.P., Ecker, C., 1996. Risk factors for Wells, D.L., Hepper, P.G., 2000. Prevalence of behaviour problems reported by
relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 209 (3), owners of dogs purchased from an animal rescue shelter. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.
572e581. 69 (1), 55e65.

You might also like