You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/311780294

Attentional Focus and Cueing for Speed Development

Article  in  Strength and Conditioning Journal · February 2018


DOI: 10.1519/SSC.0000000000000266

CITATIONS READS

14 21,111

1 author:

Nick Winkelman
Irish Rugby Football Union
11 PUBLICATIONS   136 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

American Football Book Chapter View project

Sprinting Performance in Elite Rugby Sevens View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Nick Winkelman on 03 March 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Attentional Focus and
Cueing for Speed
Development
Nicklaas C. Winkelman, PhD
Head of Athletic Performance & Science, Irish Rugby Football Union, Dublin, Ireland

ABSTRACT have learned” (42). This quote suggests focus adopted by the athlete (23,55). Part
a distinction between teaching and of this misunderstanding comes from the
STRENGTH AND CONDITIONING
learning, supposing that teaching does
PROFESSIONALS COMMONLY commonly held belief that the act of
not always result in learning. As it turns
DEPEND ON EVIDENCE TO GUIDE coaching is an art rather than a science.
out, Wooden’s intuition and insights
“WHAT THEY COACH” (E.G., PRO- were quite accurate, as research has For example, Stoszkowski and Collins
GRAMMING), BUT THE SAME CAN- shown that the acute motor skill per- (63) evaluated the preferred methods
NOT ALWAYS BE SAID FOR THEIR formance expressed in a practice con- for acquiring knowledge from 320
APPROACH TO “HOW THEY text is not necessarily indicative of the coaches that spanned 26 different coun-
COACH” (E.G., INSTRUCTION). retention and transfer (i.e., learning) of tries and 30 different sports. The results
CONSEQUENTLY, PROFESSIO- those motor skills in a future practice or showed that 92.6% of coaches preferred
NALS HAVE ADOPTED THE CON- sporting context (24,61). Conse- informal learning environments, with
VENTIONAL VIEW THAT quently, it is important for strength 41.5% of coaches preferring peer discus-
COACHING IS MORE ART THAN and conditioning (SC) coaches to dis- sion and 1.8% of coaches preferring to
SCIENCE, DESPITE THE FACT THAT sociate between performance and get their information from academic
THERE ARE DECADES OF learning, as performance “refers to the journals. It is disconcerting to consider
temporary fluctuations in [motor skill] that the highest level of evidence (i.e.,
RESEARCH ELUCIDATING THE
research) ranks last on the preferred
SCIENCE UNDERPINNING THIS SO- behavior that can be observed and
methods for coaches to acquire knowl-
CALLED ART. IN LIGHT OF THIS measured during or immediately after
edge, with YouTube (2.9%), social net-
GAP, THIS ARTICLE WILL DELVE the [skill] acquisition process,” whereas
works (4.9%), and websites (9.1%) all
INTO THIS UNDERPINNING SCI- learning “refers to the relatively perma-
achieving higher usage marks. Further-
ENCE AND PROVIDE AN EVIDENCE- nent changes in [motor skill] behavior
more, 66% of coaches noted that peda-
BASED FRAMEWORK FOR that support long-term retention and
gogy, which includes topics such as
INSTRUCTION AND CUEING transfer” (61). Thus, coaches should
coaching methods, communication,
THROUGH THE LENS OF LINEAR be cautious not to assume learning
and skill acquisition, was the last area
SPEED DEVELOPMENT. has occurred because they observed
where they had learned something that
an immediate change in motor skill
was useful to their coaching practice,
performance; rather, coaches must be
whereas only 8.1% had noted learning
INTRODUCTION patient, waiting to see if the initial
something important from psychology,
ohn Wooden is not only consid- change in motor skill performance is

J
physiology, or biomechanics.
ered by most to be one of the best retained during subsequent practices
or within the context of competition. This evidence suggests that coaches
coaches of all time but also
are interested in the coaching methods
remembered for his love of teach- Although learning is mediated by a num-
that mediate motor skill learning; how-
ing. Wooden’s appreciation for teach- ber of factors, such as the differential use
ever, they are emphasizing the experi-
ing is best exemplified by his famous of blocked versus random practice
ence of other coaches rather than the
words, “you haven’t taught until they schedules to promote skill learning (i.e.,
contextual interference) (50,53), one of
Address correspondence to Dr. Nicklaas C. KEY WORDS:
Winkelman, 10–12 Lansdowne Road, Balls- the most misunderstood factors impact-
coaching; instruction; cueing; atten-
bridge, Dublin 4, Ireland, Nick.Winkelman@ ing motor skill learning is the influence of tional focus; acceleration; sprinting
IRFU.ie. instruction and cueing on the attentional

Copyright  National Strength and Conditioning Association 1


Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Speed Development

assimilation of scientific evidence and (e.g., “push the ground away” or “cut physical therapists were providing inter-
the systematic application of such evi- and change direction as fast as you nally focused instruction 67% of the
dence within their personal coaching can”), adopt an internal focus on body time. Based on the aforementioned stud-
practice. This presents a problem, movements associated with the motor ies, there is an apparent discontinuity
because research has already suggested skill (e.g., “push off of the inside edge of between current practices and the best
that coaches preferentially adopt your foot” or “rapidly extend your hip, practices that should be used to encour-
a method of instruction and cueing knee and ankle”) (70,72,76), or in the age motor skill learning (61).
that is not always optimal for motor case of no instruction being provided,
Wulf et al. (72) (experiment 1) were the
skill learning (55). Thus, there is a mis- adopt what can otherwise be consid-
first to evaluate the influence of atten-
alignment between the type of infor- ered the athlete’s normal focus, which is
tional focus on practice performance
mation coaches’ want (or find useful) likely going to result in internally or
and motor skill learning. The research-
and their preferred method for acquir- externally directed thoughts (55). From
ers found that instruction encouraging
ing such information. Consequently, a practical perspective, a coach teach-
an external focus opposed to an internal
the instruction and cueing methods ing a defensive back in American foot-
focus of attention led to better perfor-
that are currently adopted by coaches ball how to improve their backpedal
mance and motor skill learning during
to teach motor skills, for which peers speed, emphasizing increased leg fre-
a ski-simulator task in novice partici-
are motivated to learn from one quency, could provide an external
pants. Specifically, the internal focus
another, are not likely to optimize focus cue by encouraging the athlete
group was “instructed to exert force
motor skill learning. For this reason, to focus on “pushing off the ground
on the outer foot” and the external focus
it is important that coaches understand as rapidly as they can” or they could
group was “instructed to exert force on
the science of instruction and cueing, provide an internal focus cue by
the outer wheels” while a control group
while being provided with an instruc- encouraging the athlete to focus on
received no additional instruction. The
tional framework that can be applied in “extending their knees as rapidly as
results showed that the external focus
a practical context with the explicit they can.” Although the essence of
group was more effective than the
goal of promoting motor skill learning. the 2 cues is similar (i.e., fast leg action),
internal focus group during practice (i.
the external focus references “pushing
e., greater amplitude and frequency of
ATTENTIONAL FOCUS: INTERNAL off the ground” and the internal focus
FOCUS VERSUS EXTERNAL movement). More importantly, the
references “extending [the] knees.”
FOCUS external focus group was significantly
Furthermore, coaches may see this dif-
Just as Stoszkowski and Collins (63) more effective than the internal focus
ference as trivial, with the likely belief
reported that 66% of coaches had and control groups during a delayed
being that a variety of factors (e.g.,
recently found information within the retention test (i.e., day 1—Practice; day
motor skill type, skill level of athlete,
category of pedagogy to be most helpful 2—Practice; day 3—Retention Test)
specific motor skill error) influence the
in their current practice, there has been where no instruction was provided dur-
differential use of internal versus exter-
a recent increase in SC publications ing the same ski-simulator task. This
nal focus cues; however, the available
emphasizing the influence of attentional provides evidence that an external
evidence suggests that coaches would
focus on skill learning as mediated by focus leads to superior skill learning
be more likely to encourage motor skill
coaching instructions and cues compared with an internal focus and
learning if they used instruction and
(6,21,30,48,49,52,56). The role of atten- control condition within a novice pop-
cues that directed attention externally
tional focus, or an athlete’s focus of ulation learning a dynamic balance task.
opposed to internally (70). Contrary to
attention, has emerged as an important this evidence, it is common for coaches In a follow-up experiment, Wulf et al.
mediator of motor skill learning (70). and physical therapists to preferentially (72) (experiment 2) evaluated the ef-
From a motor skill learning standpoint, use internally directed instruction and fects of instruction on balance in
attentional focus can be defined as the cues to teach motor skills. For example, another group of novices. An internal
conscious effort of an individual to Porter et al. (2010) (55) found that of the focus group was asked to “keep their
focus their attention through explicit 13 track and field athletes questioned at feet at the same height” while an exter-
thoughts in an effort to execute a motor the USA Track and Field Outdoor nal focus group was asked to “keep the
skill with superior performance (6). National Championship, 84.6% reported red markers [on the balance platform]
From a coaching perspective, instruc- that their coaches use internal focus cues at the same height.” The results
tions and cues are used to focus an during practice, with 69% of those ath- showed no difference between groups
athlete’s attention on the most impor- letes reporting that they use internal during practice; however, the external
tant feature of the motor skill being focus cues during competition. Similar focus group outperformed the internal
learned before motor skill execution. results were seen in a group of 8 physical focus group during a delayed retention
Specifically, an athlete can be encour- therapists who were working with post- test where no additional instruction
aged to adopt an external focus on the stroke patients on gait rehabilitation, was provided. Thus, the benefit of an
intended movement effect or outcome with the results showing that the external focus of attention was not

2 VOLUME 0 | NUMBER 0 | MONTH 2016

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
observed until the delayed retention test. ATTENTIONAL FOCUS: SPRINTING the internal focus and control condi-
This is important, as coaches may Ille et al. (22) were the first to evaluate tion (56). Conversely, Porter and Sims
observe that an internal cue is effective the differential effects of an internal (2013) (54) found no difference
during the skill acquisition process; how- focus and external focus on 10-m sprint between conditions for the 0 to 10-yd
ever, the mechanism by which an inter- performance in a group of expert and and 0 to 20-yd splits; however, the
nal focus cue encourages an acute novice sprinters. Participants per- football players were significantly faster
change in performance does not align formed 10-m sprints from a block start over the 10- to 20-yd split under the
with the mechanisms associated with under an external focus, internal focus, control condition compared with the
long-term motor skill learning. Similar and a control condition. During the internal focus and external focus con-
results have been reported elsewhere internal focus condition, the partici- ditions, which were not different from
(20,74), supporting the supposition that pants were asked to “push quickly on one another. Porter and Sims (2013)
coaches should assess learning separate your legs and keep going as fast as pos- (54) concluded that whereas novices
from the context where the initial skill sible while swinging both arms back benefit from an external focus, trained
learning took place (i.e., future practice and forth and raising rapidly your athletes might benefit from adopting
session or competition). There has since knees.” During the external focus con- a normal focus of attention (i.e., control
been extensive research confirming the dition, the participants were asked to condition).
various performance and learning “get off the starting blocks as quickly as The research described above shows
benefits of an external focus of attention possible, head towards the finish line that both novices and experts benefit
for balance and suprapostural tasks rapidly and cross it as soon as possible.” from adopting an external focus of
(8,37,60,72,78), neuromuscular expres- The results showed that compared attention over various sprint distances
sion of force and velocity (19,21,34,65), with an internal focus, the external (22,56). Additionally, research has also
discrete sport skills with an implement focus condition resulted in faster total shown that experts benefit from adopt-
(e.g., golf, tennis, and soccer) (73,75,77), sprint times, faster reaction times, and ing simple external focus cues that
discrete sport skills without an imple- faster running times (i.e., total sprint relate to different phases of the 100-
ment (e.g., vertical and horizontal jump- time minus reaction time and block m sprint compared with their baseline
ing) (52,69,71,79), and continuous sport clearance). or normal focus (i.e., control) (31).
skills (e.g., swimming, running, sprinting, More recently, Porter et al. (56) evalu- However, there is also evidence sug-
and agility) (22,51,58,62). ated 84 novices and 9 trained collegiate gesting that those with an intermediate
Building on the evidence noted above, football players (i.e., intermediate skill level of sprinting experience perform
the following sections will discuss the level for sprinting) (54) in a 20-m and equally well across internal focus,
influence of attentional focus on sprint 20-yd sprint task under an external external focus, and control conditions
performance. Sprinting, which will be focus, internal focus, and a control con- for certain sprint distances (i.e., 0–10
discussed in terms of acceleration and dition. Both studies used similar cues, and 0–20 yd), while performing better
absolute speed, was selected for 3 pri- with the participants being told to under a control condition (i.e., normal
mary reasons. First, an athlete’s ability focus on “running the 20 meter/yard focus) for others (i.e., 10–20 yd). Con-
to accelerate has broad application and dash with maximum effort” during the sequently, although more research is
importance to a variety of sports control condition, focus on “running required to fully understand the influ-
(14,16,29). Second, although not as the 20 meters/yards with maximum ence of attentional focus on sprint per-
prevalent as acceleration, absolute effort, [and] focus on gradually raising formance and learning, when
speed (or maximal velocity) plays an your body level. Also, focus on power- considered in the context of the avail-
important role within many team fully driving one leg forward while able evidence (see Ref. 70 for a detailed
sports (e.g., soccer and rugby) (7,13) moving your other leg and foot down review), it can still be recommended
and also represents the most common and back as quickly as possible” during that coaches should encourage an
cause of hamstring injuries, for which the internal focus condition, and to external focus opposed to an internal
technique has been identified as one of focus on “running the 20 meters/yards focus of attention when instructing
the underpinning causes (2). Finally, with maximum effort, [and] focus on sprinting.
there is an emerging body of evidence gradually raising up. Also, focus on Finally, there are 2 relevant limitations
elucidating the biomechanical powerfully driving forward while claw- within the studies noted above that
determinants of effective sprinting ing the floor as quickly as possible” should be considered in a practical
(9–11,40,57) and the role of attentional during the external focus condition. context. First, all 3 studies (22,54,56)
focus (22,54,56), which collectively The results showed that novices were used cues that included more than 1
provides the necessary insights significantly faster over 20-m under the focus point. This creates a memory
required to build an evidence-based external focus condition compared recall problem, because it would be
framework around instruction and with the internal focus and control difficult for the participants to focus
cueing for sprinting. conditions, with no difference between on all cues simultaneously during such

3
Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Speed Development

a high-intensity activity. Thus, it is advis- to an internal focus when teaching Example: “Focus on exploding towards
able for coaches to limit the substance of a motor skill, there is also evidence the cone as if your were chasing an
their instructions or cues to 1 or 2 focus that is pointing to the mediating role opponent.”
points, because this aligns with known of certain cue characteristics (e.g., dis- Again, it is evident that all 3 D’s are
limitations in short-term memory recall tance; Ref. 37). Specifically, there are 3 present within this external focus cue.
(38) and the vast majority of the current attentional focus cue characteristics This time the portion of the cue stating
literature on attentional focus (70). Sec- that likely influence the benefit of “towards the cone” captures the dis-
ond, the studies noted above did not adopting an external focus of attention. tance and direction. Because the cone
directly assess learning through the use Those characteristics include focus dis- is far away (e.g., 10 m), the distance
of delayed retention and transfer tests, as tance, focus direction, and the intent would be considered more distal,
this is a known limitation of using that is encouraged by the focus descrip- whereas the direction would be toward.
within-subject designs. Thus, the current tion. Generally speaking, the focus dis- Moreover, the description in this cue
evidence only provides insights concern- tance can be proximal or distal to uses an action verb and a clarifying anal-
ing acute practice performance rather a fixed point (e.g., proximal: “drive ogy. Specifically, “focus on exploding”
than long-term skill learning. Future the barbell away from the bench”; dis- calibrates intent and intensity, whereas
studies, using a between-subject design, tal: “drive the barbell toward the ceil- “as if you were chasing an opponent”
should not only assess practice perfor- ing”), the focus direction can be provides an analogous scenario where
mance but also assess sprint skill learn- toward or away from a fixed point the motor skill being taught would nor-
ing through the use of delayed retention (e.g., away: “sprint away from the start mally be performed.
and transfer tests. Moreover, longitudi- line as fast as you can”; toward: “sprint
nal studies examining the differential The following sections will provide
toward the finish line as fast as you
impact of internal and external focus further insights into the available evi-
can”), and the focus description can
cues would likely provide the most prac- dence and practical recommendations
be created through the use of action
tically relevant evidence for coaches. for manipulating distance, direction,
verbs (e.g., “snap, spring, or bounce off
and description.
In summary, although the evidence for the ground”) or analogies (e.g., “drop
using an external focus of attention to into the cut like you are trying to sit
under a low roof”). To provide an DISTANCE
optimize sprint performance and
learning is still emerging, when consid- example, consider the following exter- The distance the external focus en-
ered in terms of the extant literature nal focus cue intended to improve courages has been shown to mediate
(70), it can be recommended that sprint speed: the benefit of an external focus of
coaches should instruct and cue in attention. The influential role of dis-
Example: “Focus on driving the ground
a way that encourages athletes to tance was first observed when Wulf
back as explosively as you can.”
adopt an external focus. However, et al. (2000) (75) found that novice golf-
In analyzing the substance of this ers benefited from adopting a proximal
the recommendation to use instruc-
external focus cue, it is evident that it external focus (i.e., club) opposed to
tions and cues that encourage an exter-
contains all 3 D’s (i.e., distance, direc- a distal external focus (i.e., ball trajec-
nal focus is only a starting point,
tion, and description) noted above. tory). The impact of distance was later
because there seems to be a series of
That is, both distance and direction assessed by McNevin et al. (2003) (37),
factors that potentially mediate the
are noted with the portion of the cue who found that balance was improved
effectiveness of a given external cue
stating, “focus on driving the ground when novices either focused on keep-
(68). Therefore, the next section will
back.” Specifically, the ground would ing a set of markers that were inside
discuss the characteristics of an exter-
be considered a proximal focus, while (i.e., far-inside) or outside (i.e., far-
nal cue that can be manipulated to
encouraging the individual to drive outside) of their feet parallel (i.e., exter-
ensure that the athlete’s focus of atten-
away from the ground. Similarly, the nal foci) opposed to focusing internally
tion is directed at the most relevant
description of the focus intent is cap- (i.e., “keep feet parallel”) or using a close
feature of the to-be-learned motor skill
(45), while identifying the subcharac- tured by the portion of the cue stating, external focus (i.e., “keep a set of
teristics within an instruction or cue “focus on driving . as explosively as markers in front of your feet parallel”).
that can be manipulated to ensure indi- you can.” The action verb “explosively” Focus distance has since been evalu-
vidualized effectiveness (35). is used to express the intent and inten- ated in dart throwing (1,36), putting
sity with which the sprint should be (25,59), golf (5,44), rowing (43), hori-
ATTENTIONAL FOCUS: CUE performed. Similarly, the action verb zontal jumping (48,49), a dynamic bal-
CHARACTERISTICS “driving” is used to promote a fast ance task (17), and playing the piano
Although the evidence presented and forward action, as would be ex- (15). Generally speaking, a distal exter-
above clearly shows the benefit of pected when accelerating onto a free- nal focus seems to be more beneficial
using instructions and cues that way, for example. To provide further than a proximal, especially as expertise
encourage an external focus opposed context, consider another example: increases (5) or if the requirement of

4 VOLUME 0 | NUMBER 0 | MONTH 2016

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
the task is to produce maximal power DESCRIPTION between a human accelerating and
(i.e., broad jump) (48); however, novi- The description presented within an a jet taking off. First, this analogy sug-
ces are more likely to benefit from external focus cue can be considered gests that an angled body position
a proximal external focus, especially the most important source of meaning, with a progressive rise is important,
for tasks involving an implement and as it defines the spatiotemporal (i.e., as a jet will initially have a low angle
accuracy (i.e., throwing, hitting, and space and time) aspects of the move- that progressively rises through take-
striking) (77). ment. For example, consider the action off. Second, this analogy also suggests
verbs push and punch. Although both that this movement should be done
DIRECTION words suggest impact between 2 per- very fast, as anyone who has seen
The direction of focus encouraged by sons, the word push would not be asso- a jet takeoff can attest. Thus, the jet
an external focus of attention has not ciated with the same level of intensity analogy and the movement being
been explicitly examined within the as the word punch. Specifically, pushing taught share specific spatiotemporal
attentional focus research. However, someone is associated with a longer characteristics, which is why this anal-
a series of studies by Porter et al. and slower action than punching some- ogy would work well for teaching
(48,49) can be recontextualized to pro- one. Thus, if a SC coach wants an ath- acceleration. Conversely, had the anal-
vide insights concerning the influence lete to spend more time on the ground ogy required the learner to “focus on
of focus direction. Specifically, in both during a sprint, then they could suggest driving off the start line like a helicopter
studies, participants were asked to use that the athlete “focus on pushing the taking off,” there would have been a dis-
a proximal external focus (i.e., “when ground away”; conversely, if that same parity between the spatiotemporal
you jump, focus on jumping as far past coach wants the athlete to spend less characteristics needed for effective
the start line as possible”) and a distal time on the ground during the sprint, acceleration and those suggested by
external focus (i.e., “when you jump, then they could suggest that the athlete the helicopter analogy. Therefore,
focus on jumping as close to the cone analogies allow a coach to convey
“focus on punching the ground away.”
as possible”). The results showed that important information about goal-
Although the fundamental movement
the distal external focus resulted in the relevant features of a movement with-
encouraged by the 2 focus cues is the
farthest jump distances, followed by out needing to use overly complex or
same, the manner with which the
a proximal external focus and then an internally directed language.
movement is performed (i.e., slower
internal focus. Although these results In summary, the distance, direction,
or faster) will likely be different.
can be examined as proximal and dis- and description encouraged by an
tal, they can also be evaluated in terms Finally, although not explicitly defined
external focus of attention will have
of toward and away. More specifically, as an external focus of attention, anal-
a direct impact on how a movement
the proximal external focus references ogies can be considered a type of exter-
is performed. Although there are vary-
“jumping as far past the start line as nal focus, because they do not explicitly
ing levels of evidence supporting the
possible,” which can be considered an call attention to the body and associ- differential impact of the 3 D’s on per-
“away-focus” because the participant is ated movement process (i.e., internal formance and learning, the theoretical
encouraged to jump as far away from focus). Moreover, analogies have been rationale described above serves as
the start line as possible. Conversely, shown to improve skill learning to a practical framework for practitioners
the distal external focus references a greater degree than providing explicit looking to optimize the external focus
“jumping as close to the cone as instruction (26,27,46); however, this cues they provide their athletes
possible,” which can be considered effect is likely mediated by culture (Figure).
a “toward-focus” because the partici- (47), because culture influences the
pant is encouraged to jump toward types of analogies that are familiar
the cone. Thus, these results can be and relevant to the athlete. Thus, anal- INSTRUCTIONAL FRAMEWORK
reconceptualized to suggest that ogies should be culturally and genera- The evidence and practical insights
a “toward” external focus resulted in tionally relevant, while drawing presented above provide a clear frame-
superior performance compared with associations that help the learner under- work that coaches can use to optimize
an “away” external focus. Considering stand goal-relevant features of the their instruction and cueing. By direct-
the emphasis on jumping far, whether movement being taught. For example, ing an athlete’s attention externally
thought of as a distal external focus or when teaching a novice how to accel- toward the movement outcome,
a “toward” external focus, it makes erate from a 2-point stance, a practi- opposed to internally toward the
sense that an external focus that tioner could use the following analogy: movement of the body, the coach en-
made the distance goal more salient sures that attention is directed at the
would result in superior performance Example: “Focus on driving off the primary movement goal (e.g., jumping
compared with the alternatives (i.e., start line like a jet taking off.” high) rather than a subservient process
proximal external focus or “away” This analogy imparts 2 important goal (e.g., extending the hip). However,
external focus). goal-relevant features that are shared if the information provided by the

5
Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Speed Development

qualities, which encourage a coordina-


tion profile that optimizes horizontal
force production, is desirable.
Although there is a scarcity of kine-
matic data on the optimization of the
acceleration phase of sprinting, the
fieldwork and research done by Mann
provide a strong basis for understand-
ing the technical qualities associated
with elite acceleration and absolute
speed as discussed in the next section.
In his book, The Mechanics of Sprinting
and Hurdling, Mann discusses the kine-
matics associated with what he refers to
as critical positions. One such position
for acceleration is toe-off or the position
Figure. Focus cue characteristics: distance, direction and description. Reprinted with the body is in right before leaving the
permission from IDEA Health & Fitness, Inc., www.ideafit.com. ground into flight. During the first step
out of a start position, effective sprinters
will achieve a low trunk angle relative to
coach, irrespective of whether it is determinants of the movement, while
the ground (;48–538), an extended
external or internal, is not directed at associating externally focused cue
back leg (i.e., “push leg”; ;08 @ hip;
task-relevant features of the motor skill, examples that can be modified in terms
;154–1598 @ knee), and flexed front
then the cue is not likely to be as effec- of distance, direction, and description.
leg (i.e., “punch leg”; ;80–908 @ hip; ;
tive (45). Thus, it is important for
#908 @ knee). These positions will then
coaches to consider the kinematic INSTRUCTIONAL FRAMEWORK:
rapidly reverse as the athlete transitions
and kinetic determinants of the move- ACCELERATION
into their second step, with the back leg
ments they are teaching when selecting Sprint acceleration, or the ability to rap-
punching forward and the front leg
the most precise instruction or cue. For idly increase velocity (m/s2) over short
pushing back. This transition is often
example, if while working with a rugby to moderate distances (i.e., 5–10 m), is
called “piston-like leg action,” because
wing, a coach identified that the athlete one of the most important speed quali-
the legs move forward and back in
was below positional norms for relative ties required in sport (e.g., Ref. 18). Until
straight lines with minimal cyclical
lower-body strength, relative power, recently, the kinetic and kinematic de-
action (i.e., up and down action as if
and 10-m sprint speed, then the coach terminants of acceleration were not well
cycling a bike), especially in the first 2–3
may decide to emphasize the develop- understood beyond the practical experi-
steps of acceleration from a static start
ment of strength qualities that are asso- ence of coaches. Rabita et al. (57) pro-
(32). Note that these positions are tar-
ciated with the expression of sprint duced the first overground sprinting
gets rather than absolutes, as many
speed (3,28). Conversely, if an athlete study to examine the determinants of
team sport athletes will not be able to
is “strong enough,” then it may be ben- effective acceleration based on a popula-
achieve the same positions as the elite
eficial to emphasize sprint-specific tion of elite and subelite sprinters. The
sprinters for which these recom-
work, as additional strength develop- results clearly showed that the elite
mendations are based. Despite this, the
ment may not support further changes sprinters could produce nearly 20%
norms described above will provide the
to speed (12). In this scenario, it would more horizontal force relative to body
be important to prioritize the common coach with insights into where their
weight than the subelite sprinters (9.59
kinematic or technical errors, which if versus 7.74 N/kg, respectively), allowing athlete can improve, in a relative sense,
modified, would support improved the elite sprinters to leave the blocks 0.44 to maximize their acceleration.
sprint ability. For this reason, it is m/s faster than the subelite sprinters, From an instruction and cueing stand-
important that coaches have a clear which accounted for 80% of the point, there are 4 general categories
understanding of the kinematic and between-group velocity differences for that emerge from the limited research
kinetic determinants of the movements the 40-m sprint (8.16 versus 7.59 m/s, available on acceleration. Generally,
they are teaching. In line with this respectively) (9). From an attentional coaches label these 4 categories as pos-
view, the following sections will focus perspective, the goal is to get the ture or trunk position, front-side leg
provide an instructional framework athlete to “push back” with as much action or the punch leg, backside leg
for acceleration and absolute speed. force as possible (39–41,57). Conse- action or the push leg, and arm action
Each instructional framework will quently, instructing or cueing technical (64). These 4 categories have become
emphasize the kinetic and kinematic attributes, or developing neuromuscular ubiquitous coaching references across

6 VOLUME 0 | NUMBER 0 | MONTH 2016

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Table 1
Instructional framework for acceleration

Acceleration Distance Direction Description

Motor Skill Characteristic Internal Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus
(Close) (Far) (Away) (Toward) (Action Verb) (Analogy 1) (Analogy 2)

Posture Minimize trunk “Focus on “Stay long & “Stay long & “Get long as “Get long as “Project low “Project out “From head to
flexion, achieve keeping low as you low as you you drive you drive and get from the heel,
spinal neutral, a neutral drive the explode away from toward the long as you start line as imagine your
and project spine while ground toward the the start line set of cones explode off if the top of body is
forward and you sprint back as set of cones as rapidly as on the 10- the start your head a chain
upward at a low forward at explosively on the 10- possible” meter line” line” was being pulled
angle relative to a low body as you can” meter line” a battering in both
the ground angle” ram directions -
smashing snap the
through chain as you
a door” project out
from the
start line”
Place pieces of athletic tape on each knee cap as reference point (4)
Leg action: Optimize hip, knee “Focus on “Accelerate the “Accelerate the “Drive the “Drive the “Repeatedly “Drive your “Drive your
front side and ankle flexing your pieces of pieces of pieces of pieces of explode knees knees
flexion at the hip, knee tape up & tape toward tape away tape toward through the forward as if forward as if
Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com

point of toe-off, and ankle away from the top of from the the top of pieces of you were you were
emphasizing as you the ground the fence start line as the fence tape as fast repeatedly continuously
a forwardly bring your as rapidly as line in front fast as you line as fast as you can” punching shattering
directed leg leg you can” of you as can” as you can” through panes of
action forward” rapidly as sparing glass”
you can” mitts”
Leg action: Optimize hip, knee “Focus on “Push the “Push past the “Sprint away “Sprint toward “Hammer the “Explode out “Explode out
backside and ankle extending ground timing gates from the the timing ground and up like and up like
extension at the your hip, back with as with as start line as gates as fast back as hard a cheetah is a jet taking
point of toe-off, knee and much force much speed fast as as possible” and fast as 2 steps off from an
emphasizing ankle as as possible” as possible” possible” you can” behind you” aircraft
a backwardly you bring carrier”
directed leg your leg
action backward”

(continued)
7
Speed Development

acceleration and absolute speed (64);

are hammers

shins are the

through the
“Imagine your “Imagine that however, the evidence presented above

smash the
your arms

and your
provides general support for their

hammer
nails—
importance, especially as the collective

nails”
coordination profile can be optimized
to encourage horizontal force produc-
tion. Table 1 provides a detailed
—open and
is the hinge

door as fast
as possible”
your elbow
a door and
forearm is

description of recommended external


close the

cues relative to the 4 identified


categories.

Critical Position: Toe-Off; Objective: Maximize Horizontal Force Production; General Instruction: “The goal is to sprint 10-meters as fast as you can.”
Place pieces of tape on the front of the shoulder and the ulnar side of the wrist as

INSTRUCTIONAL FRAMEWORK:
pockets as
your wrist
past your

ABSOLUTE SPEED
possible”
“Throw the
tape on

Absolute speed, or the maximal veloc-


fast as

ity (m/s) that can be achieved by an


athlete, is an important athletic quality
that is commonly expressed when an
athlete is required to sprint beyond
20 m. Unlike acceleration, there is
a larger body of evidence that provides
insights into the kinetic and kinematic
determinates of successful maximal
velocity sprinting. Weyand et al.
& away from
one another
tape toward

aggressively
as possible”

(2000) (67), in their seminal study, pro-


pieces of
“Drive the

duced the first experiment to show that


(continued )

mass-specific vertical force production


as
Table 1

clearly differentiated fast and slow


sprinting. Specifically, the results
as possible”
“Continuously “Continuously

back at the
wall as fast

showed that faster sprinters (11.1 m/s)


of tape on
the pieces

your wrist
accelerate

produced 1.26 times greater vertical


force than their slower counterparts
reference points (4)

(6.2 m/s). Furthermore, these forces


were generated with significantly
shorter ground contacts in the fast
one another
away from
the pieces
accelerate

versus slow sprinting. Thus, the collec-


as fast as
possible”
of tape

tive data support the notion that


creating a large force in a short period
of time is critical to producing large
top-speeds (66,67).
alternating

elbow and
extending
your arms
by flexing

shoulder”

More recently, Clark and Weyand


“Focus on

at your

(2014) (11) added to the research


and

described above showing that faster


sprinters are not only producing
Theoretical rationale for using tape (4).

greater forces than their slower coun-


the arm moving

flexing at elbow

extending at the
action whereby
a reciprocating

terparts but also they are doing so dur-


while the arm
and shoulder,

backward is

ing the first half of the stance phase


elbow and
forward is

(2.65 6 0.05 versus 2.21 6 0.05 body


shoulder
Emphasize

moving

weight, respectively), with no differen-


ces seen during the second half of the
stance phase (1.71 6 0.04 versus 1.73 6
0.04 body weight, respectively). In
describing the kinematic features that
likely support a large mass-specific
Arms

force during absolute speed, Clark


and Weyand (2014) (11) note the

8 VOLUME 0 | NUMBER 0 | MONTH 2016

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Table 2
Instructional framework for absolute speed

Absolute Speed Distance Direction Description

Motor Skill Characteristic Internal Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus External Focus
(Close) (Far) (Away) (Toward) (Action Verb) (Analogy 1) (Analogy 2)

Place a piece of athletic tape near the belly button or front of hips as a reference
point (tape goes on clothes) (4)
Posture Minimize trunk “Focus on “Focus on “Focus on “Explode away “Explode “Spring off the “Imagine that “Imagine you
flexion, achieve keeping getting tall getting tall from the toward the ground in you are are in a wind
spinal neutral, a neutral and leading and leading ground as 40-meter an effort to wearing tunnel.
and continuously spine while with the with the you rise to line as you project up a large belt Sprint with
rise until almost you rise to tape as you tape as you a vertical rise to and forward buckle. Get a slight lean
vertical relative a vertical push away project up sprint a vertical with each long and and stay
to the ground body from the toward the position” sprint step” lead with long as if
posture” ground” sky” position” the belt supported
buckle as by the wind”
you sprint”
Place pieces of athletic tape on each knee cap and behind the heel of each shoe as
reference points
Leg action: Optimize hip, knee “Focus on “Accelerate up “Accelerate up “Drive the “Drive the “Smash the “Imagine you “Imagine the
front side and ankle flexion flexing your and away towards the pieces of pieces of tape toward are in shin ground is
at the point of hip, knee from the sky by tape away tape the sky with high water. hot and you
Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com

toe-off, and ankle ground by rapidly from the vertically the Sprint with are bare foot.
emphasizing an as you rapidly bring the ground as toward the intention to the Sprint with
upwardly bring your bringing the pieces of fast as you sky as fast as lift & fly” intention of the intention
directed leg leg up & pieces of tape can” you can” stepping up of lifting off
action forward” tape together” and out of the ground
together” the water” as fast as you
can”
Leg action: Optimize hip, knee, “Focus on “Push off the “Push past the “Treat the “Treat the “Hammer the “Hit the “Repeatedly
backside and ankle extending ground as cones as fast ground like ground like ground ground as if explode off
extension at the your hip, fast as as possible” rubber— rubber— down & to spin the the ground
point of toe-off, knee and possible” bounce off bounce up back as hard earth as if
emphasizing ankle as you the ground toward the and fast as backward” sprinting up
a down and back bring your as rapidly as sky as you can” a steep
leg action leg down & possible” rapidly as flight of
back” possible” stairs”

(continued)
9
Speed Development

following when referencing Mann

shins are the


nails - smash
the hammer
through the
“Imagine your “Imagine that (2011, p. 613) (32):
your arms

hammers
and your

Critical Position: Toe-Off; Objective: Maximize Vertical Force Production; General Instruction: “The goal is to sprint 40-m as fast as you can.” Theoretical rationale for using tape (4).
First, the knee elevation sprinters achieve

nails
late in the swing phase appears to contrib-
are

ute to early stance ground force application


by allowing greater limb velocities to be
is the hinge

door as fast
as possible”
your elbow

- open and
a door and

achieved prior to foot-ground impact. Sec-


forearm is

close the
ond, the erect stance-phase posture sprint-
ers adopt likely contributes to the stiffness
required to decelerate the limb and body
relatively quickly after the instant of foot-
Place pieces of tape on the front of the shoulder and the ulnar side of the wrist as

ground impact.
pockets as
your wrist
past your

possible”
“Throw the
tape on

Further support for the importance of


fast as

knee elevation or hip flexion comes


from Mann and Herman (1985) (33).
Specifically, the researchers note the
following when discussing kinematic
factors differentiating gold, silver, and
bronze medalist sprinters (p. 160):
The most consistent success factor identi-
fied in the sprint results of elite athletes
& away from
one another
tape toward

aggressively

is the action of the upper leg. Better sprint-


as possible”
pieces of

ers end ground contact early and quickly


“Drive the

begin leg recovery. This abbreviated leg


(continued )

extension is one major factor in decreasing


as
Table 2

the critical ground contact time. During


the recovery phase, all 3 sprinters pro-
as possible”
“Continuously “Continuously

back at the
wall as fast
of tape on
the pieces

your wrist
accelerate

duced similar full extension, followed by


excellent flexion (high knee) positions. This
flexion result is critical in initiating the
reference points (4)

production of upper leg velocity into and


during ground contact.
one another
away from
the pieces
accelerate

Thus, athletes who can effectively ter-


as fast as
possible”
of tape

minate the stance phase, allowing


enough time to achieve an optimal
high knee position, are able to gener-
ate greater forces, especially during the
alternating

elbow and

first half of the subsequent stance


extending
your arms
by flexing

shoulder”
“Focus on

phase. Therefore, the ability to gener-


at your

ate cues that encourage these positions


and

and coordinative capacities will result


in an improved ability to express abso-
at the elbow and
forward is flexing

extending at the

lute speed.
the arm moving

the arm moving


action whereby

shoulder, while
a reciprocating

Mann has provided important techni-


backward is

elbow and

cal recommendations for optimizing


shoulder
Emphasize

absolute speed. Specifically, the critical


position or “golden position” that rep-
resents an optimized coordination pro-
file is that of toe-off, which is the same
phase of the sprint as discussed in the
previous section (32). Mann describes
Arms

this as the position where maximum


upper-leg hip flexion (i.e., ;808) is

10 VOLUME 0 | NUMBER 0 | MONTH 2016


Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
achieved, which should occur no later modify language in accordance with 4. Becker J and Wu WF. Integrating
than 0.033 s after toe-off. This position the physical and psychological develop- biomechanical and motor control principles
in elite high jumpers: A transdisciplinary
is associated with a relatively vertical ment of the athlete relative to the motor
approach to enhancing sport performance.
body position, an extended back leg skill being learned. J Sport Health Sci 4: 341–346, 2015.
(i.e., “push leg”; ; 0–108 @ hip; As a final suggestion, this process 5. Bell JJ and Hardy J. Effects of attentional
;150–1558 @ knee) and a flexed front should not intimidate coaches, espe- focus on skilled performance in golf. J Appl
leg (i.e., “punch leg”; ;80–908 @ hip; ; cially if they are used to primarily giv- Sport Psychol 21: 163–177, 2009.
#908 @ knee). ing athletes internal focus cues. 6. Benz A, Winkelman N, Porter J, and
Similar to the last section, coaches Rather, coaches should look at this Nimphius S. Coaching instructions and
should instruct and cue posture, front-side as a long-term transformation that is cues for enhancing sprint performance.
leg action, backside leg action, and arm guided by daily noticing, implying Strength Cond J 38: 1–11, 2016.
action to optimize the coordination pro- that coaches should reflect during 7. Bradley PS, Sheldon W, Wooster B, Olsen P,
file associated with absolute speed (64). and after a training session and simply Boanas P, and Krustrup P. High-intensity
Table 2 provides a detailed description start to notice the language they use running in English FA premier league
soccer matches. J Sports Sci 27: 159–168,
of recommended external cues relative relative to the results they see. If
2009.
to the 4 identified categories. a coach notices he or she is using an
internal cue, then the suggestion 8. Chiviacowsky S, Wulf G, and Wally R. An
external focus of attention enhances
would be to try an external cue on
SUMMARY balance learning in older adults. Gait
the subsequent rep(s). Over time, the Posture 32: 572–575, 2010.
In summary, to optimize motor skill coach’s language will evolve and
performance and learning, it is neces- this will be met with a systematic
9. Clark K and Weyand P. Sprint running
sary to use instruction and cues that research speeds up: A first look at the
improvement in their athlete’s motor mechanics of elite acceleration. Scand J
encourage the athlete to adopt an skill performance and learning. Med Sci Sports 25: 581–582, 2015.
external focus of attention. By adopt-
ing an external focus of attention, the Conflicts of Interest and Source of Funding: 10. Clark KP, Ryan LJ, and Weyand PG. Foot
athlete will perform better during the The author reports no conflicts of interest speed, foot-strike and footwear: Linking
and no source of funding. gait mechanics and running ground
context of practice, while encouraging reaction forces. J Exp Biol 217: 2037–
the retention and transfer of the prac- 2040, 2014.
ticed motor skill. To optimize the
Nicklaas C. 11. Clark KP and Weyand PG. Are running
impact of an external focus cue, it will speeds maximized with simple-spring
Winkelman is
be important for coaches to consider 2 stance mechanics? J Appl Physiol 117:
the Head of
factors. First, it is necessary to identify 604–615, 2014.
Athletic Perfor-
the primary limitation associated with 12. Cronin J, Ogden T, Lawton T, and Brughelli M.
mance & Science,
the movement skill being taught. If the Does increasing maximal strength improve
Irish Rugby
primary limitation is coordinative in sprint running performance? Strength Cond J
Football Union.
nature, then effective cueing is likely to 29: 86–95, 2007.
make a distinct impact; however, if the 13. Cunniffe B, Proctor W, Baker JS, and
problem lies within a lack of physical Davies B. An evaluation of the physiological
strength, or some other underpinning demands of elite rugby union using global
physical quality, then the effectiveness positioning system tracking software.
REFERENCES J Strength Cond Res 23: 1195–1203, 2009.
of the cue will be limited until the
1. Abdollahipour R, Psotta R, Palomo Nieto M,
underpinning problem is resolved. Sec- Rouzbahani M, Nikdast H, and Bahram A.
14. Di Salvo V, Baron R, González-Haro C,
ond, once the primary coordinative lim- Gormasz C, Pigozzi F, and Bachl N.
Effects of attentional focus instructions on
Sprinting analysis of elite soccer players
itations have been identified and the learning of a target task: A moderation
during European Champions League and
prioritized, then the coach will provide role of visual feedback. Kinesiology 46:
UEFA Cup matches. J Sports Sci 28:
the athlete with an individualized exter- 210–217, 2014.
1489–1494, 2010.
nal focus cue. By referencing the frame- 2. Askling CM, Tengvar M, Saartok T, and
15. Duke RA, Cash CD, and Allen SE. Focus of
work described earlier, the coach can Thorstensson A. Acute first-time hamstring
attention affects performance of motor
manipulate distance, direction, and strains during high-speed running
skills in music. J Res Music Educ 59: 44–
a longitudinal study including clinical and
description until the most effective cue 55, 2011.
magnetic resonance imaging findings. Am J
has been identified. This 2-step process Sports Med 35: 197–206, 2007. 16. Duthie GM, Pyne DB, Marsh DJ, and
provides coaches with a systematic Hooper SL. Sprint patterns in rugby union
3. Baker D and Nance S. The relation
approach to selecting the instruction between running speed and measures of
players during competition. J Strength
or cue most likely to encourage desired strength and power in professional rugby
Cond Res 20: 208–214, 2006.
coordinate changes. Moreover, this pro- league players. J Strength Cond Res 13: 17. Flores FS, Schild J, and Chiviacowsky S.
cess allows the coach to evolve and 230–235, 1999. Benefits of external focus instructions on

11
Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Speed Development

the learning of a balance task in children of 30. Makaruk H and Porter JM. Focus of 44. Perkins-Ceccato N, Passmore SR, and
different ages. Int J Sport Psychol 46: attention for strength and conditioning Lee TD. Effects of focus of attention
311–320, 2015. training. Strength Cond J 36: 16–22, depend on golfers’ skill. J Sports Sci 21:
18. Gabbett TJ. Sprinting patterns of national 2014. 593–600, 2003.
rugby league competition. J Strength Cond 31. Mallett CJ and Hanrahan SJ. Race 45. Polsgrove MJ, Parry TE, and Brown NT.
Res 26: 121–130, 2012. modeling: An effective cognitive strategy Poor quality of instruction leads to poor
19. Greig M and Marchant D. Speed for the 100 m sprinter? Sport Psychol 11: motor performance regardless of internal or
dependant influence of attentional focusing 72–85, 1997. external focus of attention. Int J Exerc Sci
instructions on force production and 9: 10, 2016.
32. Mann R. The Mechanics of Sprinting and
muscular activity during isokinetic elbow Hurdling. CreateSpace, 2011. 46. Poolton J, Masters R, and Maxwell J. The
flexions. Hum Move Sci 33: 135–148, influence of analogy learning on decision-
33. Mann R and Herman J. Kinematic analysis
2014. making in table tennis: Evidence from
of Olympic sprint performance: Men’s 200
20. Hadler R, Chiviacowsky S, Wulf G, and behavioural data. Psychol Sport Exerc 7:
meters. Int J Sport Biomech 1: 151–162,
Schild JFG. Children’s learning of tennis 677–688, 2006.
1985.
skills is facilitated by external focus 47. Poolton JM, Masters RS, and Maxwell JP.
34. Marchant DC, Greig M, and Scott C.
instructions. Motriz: Revista de Educação The development of a culturally appropriate
Attentional focusing instructions influence
Fı´sica 20: 418–422, 2014. analogy for implicit motor learning in
force production and muscular activity
21. Halperin I, Williams KJ, Martin DT, and a Chinese population. Sport Psychol 21:
during isokinetic elbow flexions. J Strength
Chapman DW. The effects of attentional 375–382, 2007.
Cond Res 23: 2358–2366, 2009.
focusing instructions on force production 48. Porter JM, Anton PM, Wikoff NM, and
35. Maurer H and Munzert J. Influence of
during the isometric mid-thigh pull. Ostrowski JB. Instructing skilled athletes to
attentional focus on skilled motor
J Strength Cond Res 30: 919–923, 2016. focus their attention externally at greater
performance: Performance decrement
22. Ille A, Selin I, Do MC, and Thon B. distances enhances jumping performance.
under unfamiliar focus conditions. Hum
Attentional focus effects on sprint start J Strength Cond Res 27: 2073–2078,
Move Sci 32: 730–740, 2013.
performance as a function of skill level. 2013.
36. McKay B and Wulf G. A distal external
J Sports Sci 31: 1705–1712, 2013. 49. Porter JM, Anton PM, and Wu WF.
focus enhances novice dart throwing Increasing the distance of an external focus
23. Johnson L, Burridge JH, and Demain SH. performance. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol 10:
Internal and external focus of attention of attention enhances standing long jump
149–156, 2012. performance. J Strength Cond Res 26:
during gait re-education: An observational
study of physical therapist practice in 37. McNevin NH, Shea CH, and Wulf G. 2389–2393, 2012.
stroke rehabilitation. Phys Ther 93: 957– Increasing the distance of an external focus 50. Porter JM and Magill RA. Systematically
966, 2013. of attention enhances learning. Psychol increasing contextual interference is
Res 67: 22–29, 2003. beneficial for learning sport skills. J Sports
24. Kantak SS and Winstein CJ. Learning-
performance distinction and memory 38. Miller G. The magical number seven, plus Sci 28: 1277–1285, 2010.
processes for motor skills: A focused or minus two: Some limits on our capacity 51. Porter JM, Nolan RP, Ostrowski EJ, and
review and perspective. Behav Brain Res for processing information. 1956. Psychol Wulf G. Directing attention externally
228: 219–231, 2012. Rev 101: 343–352, 1994. enhances agility performance: A qualitative
25. Kearney PE. A distal focus of attention 39. Morin JB, Bourdin M, Edouard P, Peyrot N, and quantitative analysis of the efficacy of
leads to superior performance on a golf Samozino P, and Lacour JR. Mechanical using verbal instructions to focus attention.
putting task. Int J Sport Exerc Psychol 13: determinants of 100-m sprint running Front Psychol 1: 1–7, 2010.
371–381, 2015. performance. Eur J Appl Physiol 112: 52. Porter JM, Ostrowski EJ, Nolan RP, and
3921–3930, 2012. Wu WF. Standing long-jump performance
26. Lam WK, Maxwell JP, and Masters RSW.
Analogy versus explicit learning of 40. Morin JB, Edouard P, and Samozino P. is enhanced when using an external focus
a modified basketball shooting task: Technical ability of force application as of attention. J Strength Cond Res 24:
Performance and kinematic outcomes. a determinant factor of sprint performance. 1746–1750, 2010.
J Sports Sci 27: 179–191, 2009. Med Sci Sports Exerc 43: 1680–1688, 53. Porter JM and Saemi E. Moderately skilled
2011. learners benefit by practicing with systematic
27. Liao CM and Masters RS. Analogy
learning: A means to implicit motor 41. Morin JB, Slawinski J, Dorel S, Couturier A, increases in contextual interference. Int J
learning. J Sports Sci 19: 307–319, 2001. Samozino P, Brughelli M, and Rabita G. Coaching Sci 4: 61–71, 2010.
Acceleration capability in elite sprinters 54. Porter JM and Sims B. Altering focus of
28. Lockie RG, Murphy AJ, Knight TJ, and de
and ground impulse: Push more, brake attention influences elite athletes sprinting
Jonge XAJ. Factors that differentiate
less? J Biomech 48: 3149–3154, 2015. performance. Int J Coaching Sci 8: 41–51,
acceleration ability in field sport athletes.
J Strength Cond Res 25: 2704–2714, 42. Nater S and Gallimore R. You Haven’t 2013.
2011. Taught Until They Have Learned: John 55. Porter JM, Wu W, and Partridge J. Focus of
Wooden’s Teaching Principles and attention and verbal instructions:
29. Lockie RG, Murphy AJ, Schultz AB,
Practices. Morgantown, WV: Fitness Strategies of elite track and field coaches
Jeffriess MD, and Callaghan SJ. Influence
Information Technology, 2010. and athletes. Sport Sci Rev XIX: 199–211,
of sprint acceleration stance kinetics on
velocity and step kinematics in field sport 43. Parr R and Button C. End-point focus of 2010.
athletes. J Strength Cond Res 27: 2494– attention: Learning the “catch” in rowing. 56. Porter JM, Wu WFW, Crossley RM, and
2503, 2013. Int J Sport Psychol 40: 616–635, 2009. Knopp SW. Adopting an external focus of

12 VOLUME 0 | NUMBER 0 | MONTH 2016


Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
attention improves sprinting performance coaches’ technical knowledge. J Sports internal versus external focus of attention.
in low-skilled sprinters. J Strength Cond Sci 27: 855–861, 2009. J Mot Behav 30: 169–179, 1998.
Res 29: 947–953, 2015. 65. Vance J, Wulf G, Tollner T, McNevin N, and 73. Wulf G, McConnel N, Gartner M, and
57. Rabita G, Dorel S, Slawinski J, Sàez-de- Mercer J. EMG activity as a function of the Schwarz A. Enhancing the learning of sport
Villarreal E, Couturier A, Samozino P, and performer’s focus of attention. J Mot Behav skills through external-focus feedback.
Morin JB. Sprint mechanics in world-class 36: 450–459, 2004. J Mot Behav 34: 171–182, 2002.
athletes: A new insight into the limits of 66. Weyand PG, Sandell RF, Prime DN, and 74. Wulf G and McNevin N. Simply
human locomotion. Scand J Med Sci Bundle MW. The biological limits to running distracting learners is not enough: More
Sports 25: 583–594, 2015. speed are imposed from the ground up. evidence for the learning benefits of an
58. Schucker L, Hagemann N, Strauss B, and J Appl Physiol 108: 950–961, 2010. external focus of attention. Eur J Sport
Volker K. The effect of attentional focus on 67. Weyand PG, Sternlight DB, Bellizzi MJ, and Sci 3: 1–13, 2003.
running economy. J Sports Sci 27: 1241– Wright S. Faster top running speeds are 75. Wulf G, McNevin NH, Fuchs T, Ritter F,
1248, 2009. achieved with greater ground forces not and Toole T. Attentional focus in complex
59. Shafizadeh M, McMorris T, and Sproule J. more rapid leg movements. J Appl Physiol skill learning. Res Q Exerc Sport 71: 229–
Effect of different external attention of focus 89: 1991–1999, 2000. 239, 2000.
instruction on learning of golf putting skill. 68. Winkelman N. The effect of attentional 76. Wulf G and Prinz W. Directing attention to
Perceptual Mot skills 113: 622–670, 2011. focus on sprint performance. In: Health movement effects enhances learning: A
60. Shea CH and Wulf G. Enhancing motor Promotion and Wellness. Provo, UT: review. Psychon Bull Rev 8: 648–660,
learning through external-focus instructions Rocky Mountain University of Health 2001.
and feedback. Hum Move Sci 18: 553– Professions, 2016.
77. Wulf G and Su J. An external focus of
571, 1999. 69. Wu WF, Porter JM, and Brown LE. Effect of attention enhances golf shot accuracy in
61. Soderstrom NC and Bjork RA. Learning attentional focus strategies on peak force and beginners and experts. Res Q Exerc Sport
versus performance: An integrative review. performance in the standing long jump. 78: 384–389, 2007.
Perspect Psychol Sci 10: 176–199, 2015. J Strength Cond Res 26: 1226–1231, 2012.
78. Wulf G, Weigelt M, Poulter D, and
62. Stoate I and Wulf G. Does the attentional 70. Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor McNevin N. Attentional focus on
focus adopted by swimmers affect their learning: A review of 15 years. Int Rev suprapostural tasks affects balance
performance? Int J Sports Sci Coaching 6: Sport Exerc Psychol 6: 77–104, 2013. learning. Q J Exp Psychol 56: 1191–1211,
99–108, 2011. 71. Wulf G and Dufek JS. Increased jump 2003.
63. Stoszkowski J, and Collins D. Sources, height with an external focus due to 79. Wulf G, Zachry T, Granados C, and
topics and use of knowledge by coaches. enhanced lower extremity joint kinetics. Dufek J. Increases in jump-and-reach
J Sports Sci 34: 794–802, 2016. J Mot Behav 41: 401–409, 2009. height through an external focus of
64. Thompson A, Bezodis IN, and Jones RL. An 72. Wulf G, Hoss M, and Prinz W. Instructions attention. Int J Sports Sci Coaching 2:
in-depth assessment of expert sprint for motor learning: Differential effects of 275–284, 2007.

13
Strength and Conditioning Journal | www.nsca-scj.com

Copyright ª National Strength and Conditioning Association. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

View publication stats

You might also like