You are on page 1of 12

Applying Systemic Functional Linguistics to

Conversations with Dementia: The Linguistic


Construction of Relationships between
Participants
Nicole Müller, D.Phil./ and Zaneta Mok, Ph.D.^

ABSTRACT

Social isolation in dementia is a growing concern as the


incidence and prevalence of dementing conditions is on the rise in
many societies. Positive social interactions, which foster the construction
and enactment of positive interpersonal relationships and therefore
positive discursive identities, make an important contribution to emo-
tional well-being. In this article, we investigate how two women diag-
nosed with dementia of the Alzheimer's type use language to relate to
each other and two visiting graduate students. We use Systemic Func-
tional Linguistics as an analytical framework, specifically investigating the
use of vocatives and naming, and conversational moves and exchanges.

KEYWORDS: Dementia, conversation, Systemic Functional


Linguistics, interpersonal relationships

Learning Outcomes: As a result of this activity, the reader will be able to (1) apply introductory familiarity with
basic notions of Systemic Functional Linguistics, sufficient to pursue further study in the field, and (2) explain the
role of language in the construction and enactment of interpersonal relations in dementia.

SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL language use: when "doing SFL," linguists first


LINGUISTICS and foremost analyze texts, that is, language
Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) is a used by speakers in context. Halliday highlights
theory and toolkit of analytical procedures that that SFL "presents language as a semogenic
has become increasingly popular in the clinical (meaning-making) resource, one governed by
domain in the recent past. SFL is a theory of tendencies not rules, and whose categories (as is

'Department of Communicative Disorders, University of Discourse Across Disorders: Acquired Neurogenic Con-
Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, Louisiana; ^La Trobe ditions; Guest Editor, Elizabeth Armstrong, Ph.D.
University, Melbourne, Australia. Semin Speech Lang 2012;33:5-15. Copyright © 2012
Address for correspondence and reprint requests: by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue,
Nicole Müller, D.Phil., Department of Communicative New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662.
Disorders, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, P.O. DOL http://dx.doi.org/10.105S/s-0031-1301159.
Box 43170, Lafayette, LA 70504 (e-mail: nmueller@ ISSN 0734-0478.
louisiana.edu). '
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE/VOLUME 33, NUMBER 1 2012

typical of semiotic systems) are 'fuzzy' rather enacts a relationship to an addressee and the
than determinate. It is formulated in terms of world, and is tied into its linguistic context
strengths, rather than of deficits or constraints: (Mode). The categories Field (what is being
what the speaker can do (and what the language talked about). Tenor (relationship of the
'can do')." SFL's orientation is social-interac- speaker to the audience, and the message), and
tional and context-embedded. Linguistic re- Mode (the role of language in the discourse
sources are conceptualized within the analytic situation) are often referred to as register vari-
framework as networks of choices available to ables? In the rest of this article, we shall focus
language users at every level of language (pho- mainly on the interpersonal metafunction and
nology, lexicogrammar, discourse-semantics). on how conversation participants with and
These networks, or systems of choices, make without dementia construct and enact interper-
up one of the cornerstones of the SFL model. sonal relationships.
Note, however, that linguistic choices are not A thorough introduction to SFL would be
made consciously (although some levels of lin- far beyond the scope of this article. The current
guistic choice are accessible to metalinguistic standard reference work is HaUiday and Mat-
awareness). The other denning concept of SFL thiessen.^ Eggins wrote a briefer, and for the
is function: the focus is on meaning-making, beginner rather more user-friendly introduc-
that is, SFL is oriented toward language users tion. Eggins and Slade^ offer detailed guidance
employing the resources at their disposal to on analyzing conversations using SFL tools;
create meaning in context (as compared with a
Martin and White^. specifically deal with ap-
context-free perspective aiming to uncover as-
praisal systems. Togher and Ferguson edited a
pects of an underlying language capacity, or
special issue of Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics
underlying language deficits, for instance). Hal-
on SFL, to which Armstrong contributed an
liday and Matthiessen'^ summarize the basic
overview on an SFL perspective on language
functions, or metafancúons, of language as
disorder. A very thorough chapter-length in-
follows: Language construes human experience;
troduction to SFL in the speech-language
in other words, humans use linguistic resources
pathology context is provided by Ferguson
to construct meaning from their experiences of
and Thompson.^ Armstrong and Mortensen
the world. This fijnction is referred to as the
ideational function (with two subcomponents, apply the analysis of conversational moves to
experiential and logical), and relates to the Field the everyday talk of persons with aphasia.
of information, or, what is talked about. Simul-
taneously, language use also enacts relationships
with others and the world around us; this Data
enacting ñinction is referred to as the inter- In this article, we draw on a conversation be-
personal function, relating to the Tenor of tween four participants (all names are pseudo-
human relationships. A third function is recog- nyms): Ms. Frances (F) was, at the time the
nized, the textual metafiinction (relating to the conversation was recorded, in her early 90s, an
Mode of discourse), which is considered as African-American woman with a diagnosis of
enabling and facilitating the former two, inas- dementia of the Alzheimer's type (DAT). She
much as they depend on the language user's also used a wheelchair for mobility. Ms. Beatrice.
ability to create and structure text, to manage (B) was in her late 70s, Caucasian, and also had a
information flow and continuity. The term diagnosis of DAT; she was able to walk inde-
metafunction was adopted to "suggest that pendently. Both women were somewhat hard of
function was an integral component within hearing, but neither had a hearing aid. Ms.
the overall theory" of SFL, because "[ljanguage Frances and Ms. Beatrice -were residents in a
is as it is because of the ñinctions in which it has nursing home in South Louisiana, and Ms.
evolved in the human species"'^ Every instance Beatrice was in the habit of visiting Ms. Frances,
of language use simultaneously relates to Field, who could not leave her room unaided. Both Ms.
Tenor, and Mode, in that it expresses informa- Frances and Ms. Beatrice were natives of South
tion and thereby construes human experience. Louisiana. Neither had had recent assessments
of dementia severity, and neither had ever
APPLYING SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL LINGUISTICS TO DEMENTIA/MÜLLER, MOK

received any dementia-related intervention. tion of single-word utterances. The two resi-
Participant observation revealed that in both dents on whose home ground the conversation
women, dementia had progressed to the point takes place are the most active talkers, whereas
where they did not retain factual information Rose stays somewhat in the background, and
about their visitors (names, places of origin, Mary appears, according to this first glance at
marital status, for instance), despite repeatedly the conversation, to be better integrated into
eliciting this information during fortnightly the interaction. We shall refer to quantitative
visits over the course of a year. Mary and Rose aspects of speech function use later in this
were graduate students in speech-language article.
pathology at the time, and doing fieldwork
with the first author of this article. Mary had VOCATIVES AND NAMING EACH OTHER: "MS.,"
grown up in South Louisiana; Rose had come "SUGAR," "HONEY," "MY LITTLE MAMAN"
to Louisiana from China about a year previ- In this section, we take a brief look at how
ously. The setting for the conversation is Ms. participants express attitudes toward each
Frances' room in the nursing home; a few other. Speakers express varying degrees of fa-
minutes into the conversation, Ms. Beatrice miliarity, solidarity, and intimacy to each other,
comes to visit. The conversation was tape- and this is achieved largely through the use of
recorded with the approval of the participants, lexical choices.'' We shall look at how partic-
the nursing home management, and the Uni- ipants address each other (vocatives), and how
versity of Louisiana at Lafayette Institutional in particular one participant, Ms. Beatrice,
Review Board. expresses her attitude toward Ms. Frances by
using a specific label.
Mary and Rose invariably address Ms.
Interpersonal Relationships Frances and Ms. Beatrice by using the title
"Ms." plus first name. In the regional culture
WHO'S DOING THE TALKING? (the southern United States, specifically. South
A simple first step, according to Eggins,'' in Louisiana), this is considered an appropriate
teasing out dimensions of Tenor is to consider form used chiefly by younger participants to
who produces what proportion of the talk in address older ones: it signals more familiarity
any given situation. Table 1 summarizes some than title plus last name ("Ms. Boudreaux," for
quantitative aspects of the conversation. Ms. instance), but more respect than the first name
Frances contributes most of the talk, in terms only would convey. The title "Ms." is appro-
of numbers of words and turns produced, priately used irrespective of the addressee's
followed by Ms. Beatrice, Mary, and Rose, in marital status.
descending order. In terms of mean length of Ms. Frances uses two vocatives, "honey"
turn, the output produced by the two elderly and "sugar," as, in examples la and lb (the
ladies and Mary is very comparable, whereas transcript conventions used are those of Sys-
Rose's turns are on average considerably tematic Analysis of Language Transcripts
shorter. She also produces the largest propor- [SALT],^° with some minor editing for read-
abihty).
(la)
Table 1 A Simple Quantification of the
Participants' Contributions
833 F where you from honey?
834 R I/'m from overseas.
F B M R
835 R China,
Turns 169 108 107 87 (lb)
Words 1124 958 568 229 617 F I/'m an old lady sugar.
Different words 269 284 199 85 618 ;:08
M LU (words/turn) 6,6 8,87 5,3 2,6 619 F {light chuckle} I am {quiet, slow] 57.
1-word utterances 54 26 32 58
F, Ms, Frances; B, Ms, Beatrice; M, Mary; R, Rose, MLU, Ms. Frances uses both "honey" and "sugar"
mean length of utterance in words per turn. as vocatives for all three participants in this
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE/VOLUME 33, NUMBER 1 2012

conversation, but most often when addressing Ms. Beatrice's use of "my little maman" is
the two students. She doesn't use these terms somewhat incongruent with established usage,
to secure the addressee's attention; rather, they but she makes it clear that she uses maman
signal affiliation and friendliness. However, metaphorically to capture the relationship she
the speaker also gently expresses a status perceives herself to have with Ms. Frances. By
differential in that it would not, in the local using the verbal process call, she establishes that
culture, be appropriate for the students to use she doesn't confuse Ms. Frances with her own
these terms, but for the older woman to grandmother, but conveys a title on her; and
address the students in this way is perfectly she further establishes that she knows Ms.
acceptable. It is safe to assume that Ms. Frances' real name (note, though, that she
Frances is drawing on long-established habits qualifies the certainty of her assertion by using
of language use; however, her use of these the mental process and projection "I think").
terms of endearment also speaks to the inter- She presents her use of this term of endearment
actionally functional preservation of such hab- as grounded in her experience of loneliness and
its in dementia, in that they help to create and sadness when coming "here," that is, the nursing
sustain the interpersonal dynamic of the con- home, and of being consoled by Ms. Frances,
versation. who looked after her. The explanation given is
entirely cogent and presented cohesively. It is
Ms. Beatrice enters Ms. Frances' room,
also impossible that it is entirely based on actual
stating, "I have come to see my little maman"
experience: Ms. Frances is unable to leave her
(line 86). In the formerly francophone culture of
room independently, so would not have been
South Louisiana, the French maman is used in.
able to "come see about" Ms. Beatrice or to
English as a loanword to denote grandma rather
"come cover my feet." Thus, Ms. Beatrice has
than mom. Following Mary's somewhat amused
established in her mind an experiential reality
question "Why do you call her that?" (line 91),
that is not entirely founded upon fact; rather,
Ms. Beatrice proceeds to explain her reasons:
the presented-as-remembered reality is made to
(2)
fit and to express the friendship and gratitude
92 B (because she) she/'s so good to me [M, F
Ms. Beatrice feels for Ms. Frances. In the
chuckle}.
absence of conflicting evidence (no one contra-
93 B that/'s why I call her my little maman {M,
dicts her, and we may infer that she doesn't
F chuckle).
remember any differently), this alternate reality
95;:02
is discursively confirmed and accepted by her
96 B I think her name is F. conversation partners.
97 M < m h m > .
98 R <yea>.
99 F < that/'s> right. THE GIVE-AND-TAKE OF CONVERSATION:
100 F you know my name [chuckles}. CONSTRUCTING AND ENACTING
101 B now I call her my little maman. RELATIONSHiPS
102 F that/'s right. Many studies have shown that casual conversa-
103 B she/'s the first one that come see about tion is a powerful site for doing "social work,"
me when I got in here. and perhaps the most important site for the
104 B I was crying. construction and enactment of relationships
105 ;:02 with others (see, e.g., Eggins and Slade'* for a
106 B you know. much more in-depth discussion). The SFL
107 B I felt so alone. framework for the analysis of speech functions
108 R mhm. in conversation"''^'^ rests on the basic assump-
109 B X she come:02 cover my feet. tion that "dialogue is a process of exchange"^^
110 B she say "do/n't cry." in which either information or goods and
111 B "do/n't cry, things gonna get better" {M, services are exchanged and in which speakers
R chuckle}. adopt "roles associated with exchange rela-
112 B they did. tions: either giving or demanding."'' This
113 F {LV} it did get better huh? leads to a distinction between four basic
APPLYING SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL LINGUISTICS TO DEMENTIA/MÜLLER, MOK

attend

E
atie
I— open
initiate
initi, —

I— monitor
elaborate
prolong -\— extend
enhance

elaborate
'— append extend
enhance
sustain —
engage
support register
reply
— respond ~ develop

confront disengage
react — reply

support • track
• response
'— rejoin
confront - r ^ challenge
' response
Figure 1 System network diagram of speech function moves. Based on and abridged from components
in; Eggins S, Slade D. Analysing Casual Conversation. London, UK; Equinox; 2005.

speech functions, or moves, with which speak- declarative, interrogative, imperative, and
ers can initiate an exchange in dialogue, modulated interrogative mood types.
namely making statements and offers (giving In this section, we shall look at conversa-
information or goods/services, respectively), tional moves used by the participants in our
asking questions (demanding information), conversation, following the framework used by
or making commands (demanding goods/ Eggins and Slade.'*'' Fig. 1 diagrams move
services). An opening move by one speaker choices available to speakers in a conversation,
will constrain the choices available to a next in the form of a systeni network (note that in
speaker, such that adopting a giving role in an Eggins and Slade's framework, there is an
opening move sets up the expectation that the additional level of subcategorization, or deli-
responding move be one of accepting; by the cacy, at the right-hand end of the system net-
same token, an opening demand implies that work, which we have omitted from our
the next speaker adopt a giving role. Moves discussion because of space limitations). A
relate to clauses by way of expression, or system network diagram is a tool to map
realization: Clauses are grammatical units, paradigmatic ordering in language, that is, a
characterized by, in SFL terms, specific set of alternatives available to a language user,
mood structures (that is, configurations of together with a condition of choice, or entry
subject and verb group). Moves are considered point.
as units of discourse, expressed through We are not going to comment in detail on
clauses. Although any one move type can be all move types used by the participants,, but
expressed by more than one clause type, Halli- rather focus on some salient patterns. We shall
day and Matthiessen'^ suggest that there are define and illustrate the move types as we
unmarked associations of mood (clause) types proceed, using an extract from the conversation
and categories such that the unmarked, or as raw material (see example 3). The move
congruent, realizations of statements, ques- labels in the right column correspond to those
tions, commands, and offers are, respectively. in Fig. 1 and Table 2 (see below).
10 SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE/VOLUME 33, NUMBER 1 2012

(3)
628 M do you have any brothers or sisters? Open: Initiate: Question: Fact
629 F (uh):02 yes. React: Respond: Support: Reply
628 F they live in L(city). Continue: Prolong: Extend
629 ;:02
630 F they're not around here. Continue: Prolong: Elaborate
631 ;:03
632 F well they were in L(city). Continue: Prolong: Elaborate
633 F but theyiOl travel a lot. Continue: Prolong: Extend
634 F I do/n't know where they are now they> Continue: Prolong: Extend
635;:17
637 F but I have a sister Margot:03 and two brothers. Continue: Prolong: Extend
637 ;:07
638 F they'll X. • Unintelligible
639 ; : n
640 F {louder} y'all from where? Open: Initiate: Question: Fact
641 M l/'m from O(city). React: Respond: Support: Reply
642 F O(city)? React: Rejoin: Support: Track
643 M mhm. React: Rejoin: Support: Response
644 F that/'s not too very far from here. Continue: Append: Extend
645 M mhm. React: Respond: Support: Reply
646 F uhhuh I used to go to O(city) often. Qpen: Initiate: Statement
647 F I had a friend that lived:02 in O(city). Continue: Prolong: Elaborate

• In move analysis, the first distinction and a two-second pause, uses a reacting move
drawn is between opening and sustaining that is further subcategorized as a response and
moves. Attending opening moves prepare the a supporting reply. Supporting moves confirm
ground for the following talk, by securing expectations or content set up in initiating
another interactant's attention (by way of a moves (similar to what is labeled preferred
greeting, or a vocative, for instance, "hey responses or second pair parts in adjacency pairs
Sam"). Initiating opening moves, on the other in Conversation Analysis).'* Replying moves
hand, introduce material for negotiation, a negotiate a proposition introduced by a former
proposition or proposal. Our data do not con- speaker and most often, as in this case, take the
tain many attending moves (see Table 2), form of elliptical clauses. Eggins and Slade's"*
which indicates that the participants did not framework for move analysis further subdivides
often find it necessary to call each other's replies into accepting, complying, agreeing,
attention (back) to the conversation. In line answering, acknowledging, and affirming re-
628, Mary uses a factual question directed at plies. We shall not discuss this level of analytic
Ms. Frances to initiate an exchange. delicacy here for reasons of economy of
The basic function of sustaining moves is space.
to continue the negotiation of the material In theory, the negotiation initiated by
initiated in an opening move. When a different Mary's move in line 628 could have been
speaker makes a move, this is classified as a completed by Ms. Frances' minimal reply in
reacting move. These come in two further line 628. However, Ms. Frances chooses to use
types, responses and rejoinders. Responses are a prolonging continuing move: the same
moves that contribute to the completion of the speaker keeps talking and chooses to prolong
negotiation currently under way, whereas re- the exchange by adding information to the
joinders delay the completion of the negotia- ongoing negotiation set up in the opening
tion. In line 629, F, after a hesitation marker move. Prolonging continuing moves are further
APPLYING SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL LINGUISTICS TO DEMENTIA/MÜLLER, MOK 11

Table 2 Speech Function Moves


F B M R
Total moves 239 (NV 4) 175 142 (NV4) 109
Incomplete 5 1 0 0
(Unintelligible utterances) 9 3 0 0
Open 35 37 29 5
Attend 1 2 2 1
Command 7 0 1 0
Offer 0 2 0 0
Ouestion 20 14 18 4
Statement 7 19 8 0
Continue 66 73 33 13
Monitor 3 2 5 0
Prolong 55 61 21 11
Elaborate 21 9 9 7
Enhance 6 8 4 1
Extend 28 44 8 3
Append 8 10 7 2
Elaborate 2 1 4 1
Enhance 0 1 1 1
Extend 6 8 2 0
React: respond 92 37 51 65
S: register 37 7 8 19
S:engage 1 1 3 3
S: develop 17 10 10 1
Elaborate 7 2 6 0
Enhance 2 1 0 0
Extend 8 7 4 1
S: reply 31 16 22 29
C: reply 6 3 8 13
React: Rejoin 41 27 29 26
S: tracking 29 21 10 6
S: response 12 5 18 20
C:response 0 1 1 0
F, Ms. Frances; B, Ms. Beatrice; M, Mary: R, Rose; NV, nonverbal.

subcategorized into elaborations, extensions, which in turn is embedded in a specific context


and enhancements. The dividing lines be- of culture, as to Mary's initiating move. In a
tween these two subcategories appear to different situational context, say, a "fact-find-
blur a little at times; however, they are ing" interview by a member of the nursing home
defined as follows: Elaborations clarify or staff, a minimalist one-word answer might have
restate material that was introduced in a been entirely applropriate. However, in the con-
previous move, or give an example. Exten- text of a visit and a friendly chat, Mary's
sions introduce additional information, and initiating question is likely to imply an invita-
enhancements qualify previously introduced tion to share further details once the negotiation
material by giving circumstantial information of the initial proposition has been accom-
(time, place, cause, condition). By choosing to pushed, and Ms. Frances complies. In fact,
continue and prolong the exchange, Ms. Fran- she contributes a series of seven utterances
ces reacts as much to the context of situation, (reproduced as example 4):
12 SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE/VOLUME 33, NUMBER 1 2012

(4)
628 F they live in L(city), Continue; Prolong; Extend
629 :;02
630 F they're not around here, Continue; Prolong; Elaborate
631 ;;03
632 F well they were in Kcity), Continue; Prolong; Elaborate
633 F but they;01 travel a lot, Continue; Prolong; Extend
634 F I do/n't know where they are now they> Continue; Prolong; Extend
635;;17
637 F but I have a sister Margot;03 and two brothers, Continue; Prolong; Extend
637 ;;07
638 F they'll X, Unintelligible

Ms. Frances' extensions and elaborations Ms. Frances' voice gradually deteriorates, to
continue the conversational topic established the point where her utterance in line 638
by Mary's opening question. After each of Ms. becomes partially unintelligible (indicated by
Frances' continuing moves, other speakers an X in the transcript, following SALT con-
have the opportunity to take over, inasmuch ventions). This gradual deterioration could be
as the end of a move is a possible turn transfer interpreted as a sign of fatigue. However, Ms.
point, and "speaker change could occur with- Frances' next move indicates rather that she
out turn transfer being seen as an interrup- has run out of things to say about her siblings
tion."'* However, in this case, the reluctance of and needs someone else to make a contribu-
the other speakers to take over results in a tion: After yet another long pause, she takes
prolonged series of moves on Ms. Frances' charge of the conversation, and changes its
part: Rather than letting the conversation direction by opening another exchange, using
flounder, she continues. The number and a factual question (see example 5).

(5)
638 F they'll X, Unintelligible
639;;11
640 F (louder) y'all from where? Open; Initiate; Ouestion; Fact
, 641 M I/'m from O(city), React; Respond; Support; Reply
642 F O(city)? React; Rejoin; Support; Track
643 M mhm, React; Rejoin; Support; Response
644 F that/'s not too very far from here, Continue,; Append; Extend
645 M mhm, React; Respond; Support; Reply
646 F uhhuh I used to go to O(city) often, Open; Initiate; Statement
647 F I had a friend that lived;02 in O(city), Continue; Prolong; Extend

length of pauses between her turns indicate Ms. Frances' opening question in line 640
that she is encountering some difflculty in is produced in a noticeably louder and clearer
doing so, as does the abandoned move in voice than her previous continuing moves. In
line 634. However, even after a 17-second using an opening question, Ms. Frances firmly
silence, none of the other participants volun- transfers responsibility to her addressees, in this
teers a contribution, and Ms. Frances makes case the students, who are now obliged (if they
another continuing move, which is followed want the conversation to go on) to react and
by yet another long pause. During this ex- negotiate the material introduced by Ms. Fran-
tended series of continuations, the intensity of ces. Her question in line 640 is an illustration of
APPLYING SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL LINGUISTICS TO DEMENTIA/MÜLLER, MOK 13

a pattern that the first author observed in many toward completion. Tracking moves do not
conversations with Ms. Frances. She has a indicate confrontation or disagreement (a chal-
stock of repetitive questions she uses, most lenge on the part of the speaker of the material
often when there is a lull in the conversation presented for negotiation by another speaker),
(indicated by long interutterance pauses), or but rather seek to avoid potential misunder-
when she herself runs into trouble formulating standing by, for instance, eliciting a clarifica-
an utterance. In this particular conversation, tion or repetition of the previous speaker's
Ms. Frances initiates the negotiation of her contribution. Tracking moves account for the
conversation partners' origin (in all instances largest proportion of the rejoining moves in
but one, one or the other of the students) 11 this conversation, and also the larger part of the
times in total. This also occurred in other one-word utterances (Eggins and Slade'' flir-
conversations, along with other "favorite" ques- ther subcategorize tracking moves into check-
tions eliciting marital status, whether one had ing, confirming, clarifying, and probing
children, one's work, whether one was visiting, moves). Mary responds to Ms. Frances' track-
and liked it here.^^ ing move in line 643, which resolves any linger-
Multiple negotiations of the same factual ing doubt as to the information given in her
content of course lend a degree of repetitiveness response in 641, and which in turn leaves Ms.
to a conversation, and indeed repetitiveness is Frances the option to continue her earlier move
often cited as one of the hallmarks of discourse (642) in line 644, extending the information
involving participants with DAT^'': increasing thus far negotiated about Mary's hometown.
difficulties in laying down new memories, in- She often uses this utterance, or a close variant
cluding memories for very recent events, make it (suitably adapting "not very far" to "far away,"
hard for tlie person with dementia to keep track depending on the location negotiated), in ex-
of the content of an unfolding conversation; changes she initiates by "Where y'all from?" or
hence repetitive content. However, further in- "Where is your home?"; she does this six times
vestigation of Ms. Frances' use of these repet- in this particular conversation.
itive opening moves reveals considerable Mary's reaction to Ms. Frances' assertion is
conversational skill. The questions are not ran- a noncommittal supporting reply, which closes
domly placed (they don't, for instance, interrupt the present exchange. Ms. Frances makes an-
ongoing exchanges), but rather open new ex- other opening move, offering the statement
changes when an ongoing exchange "fizzles that she used to visit Mary's hometown, and
out," and when the conversation partners signal thus in terms of the Field negotiated presenting
reluctance to take over (by silence). In addition, related information. As none of her interloc-
her questions were always entirely contextually utors offers any comment, she follows up with a
and culturally appropriate: In the context of continuing move, which prolongs the ex-
situation (a visit by two friendly strangers) and change, extending the information by provid-
culture (including the norms regarding inter- ing an implicit reason for her having visited
generational interactions, which give Ms. Fran- O(city), that is, having had a friend there.
ces the right to, among other things, ask An analysis of conversational moves can be
personal questions of those friendly strangers), used in a move-by-move reconstruction of how
it is acceptable that Ms. Frances, the "host" of participants in a conversation offer information
the interaction, asks questions about one's place for negotiation and how through the process of
of origin, marital status, children, work, and so negotiation they construct and enact relation-
forth. ships with each other as the conversation pro-
In line 641, Mary's reacting move supplies gresses, by, for instance, either supporting or
the answer (a supporting reply) to Ms. Frances' challenging each other's contributions, and by
question. Ms. Frances in turn reacts with a taking charge or letting others determine the
tracking move. Tracking moves are a subcate- openings of exchanges. For example, example 3
gory of supporting rejoinders. Rejoining moves shows how Ms. Frances successfully rescues
are, as mentioned previously, defined as pro- the conversation from fioundering by changing
tracting an exchange, rather than moving it its direction and asking a question; she thus
14 SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE/VOLUME 33, NUMBER 1 2012

projects herself as being in control of the the two older women are tracking moves, that
situation. Also, her extended turn, consisting is, moves that delay the completion of an
of prolonging moves (one of them abandoned) exchange not by challenging a previous move,
separated by long pauses, shows that her inter- but rather attempt to (re)establish mutual
locutors are reluctant to take over and control understanding. Given that both older women
the content and direction of the interaction. In are somewhat hard of hearing, it is not surpris-
addition, a move analysis can be used to quan- ing to encounter many tracking moves.
tify patterns in a conversation, as in Table 2.
Ms. Frances overall contributes the larg-
est number of moves, but Ms. Frances and DISCUSSION AND OUTLOOK
Ms. Beatrice make approximately the same Social isolation is a pressing concern in demen-
number of opening moves, and also the ma- tia care. The opportunity to create and maintain
jority of the opening moves in the conversa- positive social relations becomes increasingly
tion. This indicates that the two older woman difficult as the cognitive-communicative de-
are not reluctant to offer material for nego- clines associated with DAT progress. This is a
tiation, either by way of factual questions (Ms. challenge particularly in residential care: Typi-
Frances' preferred technique) or statements cally, persons with dementia don't move out of
(slightly preferred by Ms. Beatrice). Rose their famUiar home environment and into a
stands out as the reluctant participant, less new, residential care environment until DAT
than 5% of her contributions are opening has progressed to the extent that they can no
moves. Continuing moves (most often pro- longer live independently. Given that it be-
longing moves) are also frequently used by the comes increasingly harder to lay down new
two older women and far outnumber their memories and therefore gradually accumulate
opening moves. This may indicate that once common ground with others (whether resi-
a speaker is making a contribution, others in dents, care staff, or visitors) that can serve as a
this conversation are reluctant to take a turn basis for positive long-term relationships, two
away from the present speaker (see example 3 aspects become increasingly important: the dis-
again). However, it likely also speaks to a cursive construction of interpersonal relation-
conversational situation where the two older ships in the moment of interaction, and
women are not reluctant to expand on infor- therefore the opportunity to engage in positive
mation they themselves have offered for ne- and productive social interaction.
gotiation: By continuing, a speaker expands Using extracts from one conversation as an
an exchange on,her or his own terms, thus example, we have illustrated the use of several
having a more controlling stake in the con- tools out of the SFL arsenal that can be used to
versation. This also indicates that the two investigate how people relate to each other in
women with DAT don't feel intimidated by conversation. The proportions of talk produced
their visitors, but feel encouraged to talk and by the two older woman with dementia, as
interact and to take (and keep) charge of the compared with the two students, show that
conversation. they perceived this as a situation conducive to
Reacting moves are more often responses talk. We briefly explored the use of address
rather than rejoinders in this conversation; this terms and saw that Ms. Frances' use of terms of
pattern is strongest with Ms. Frances and Rose. endearment enacts a friendly relationship be-
This indicates that the interlocutors by and tween the older and the two younger women, a
large successfiiUy collaborate in bringing the relationship that is not entirely symmetrical in
negotiation of material presented in an opening terms of power and/or status, in that it would
move to a timely close. The circumstance that not be appropriate, by local cultural norms, for
most responding moves are supporting rather a younger person to reciprocally use the same
than confronting moves also speaks to a con- terms of endearment. In turn, the consistent
versational atmosphere that is mutually sup- use of "Ms." plus first name on the part of
portive, rather than confrontational. By far the the students enacts the corresponding part of
largest proportion of rejoining moves used by that same relationship, as that of a younger
APPLYING SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL LINGUISTICS TO DEMENTIA/MÜLLER. MOK 15

participant who owes and expresses respect to interactions that permit persons with dementia
the older one. to enact, and mutually support, positive social
Ms. Beatrice's metaphor "my little maman" relationships, which in turn will be an impor-
(explicitly introduced by her as a metaphorical tant factor in counteracting social isolation.
expression) neatly captures a complex relation-
ship she feels she has with her friend. In
addition, we saw that she has constructed in REFERENCES
her mind a narrative representation of a remem- 1. Halliday MAK. Guest contribution. A note on
bered reality to sustain and explain both the systemic functional linguistics and the study of
metaphor and the relationship. Seen purely language disorders. Clin Linguist Phon 2005;19:
from a dementia-as-deficit perspective, such 133-135
confabulations can be dismissed as indications 2. Halliday MAK, Matthiessen CMIM. An Intro-
that a person loses her grip on reality. However, duction to Functional Grammar. 3rd ed. London,
UK: Arnold; 2004
viewed from an interpersonal perspective, we
3. Eggins S. An Introduction to Systemic Functional
find that both the metaphor and the narrative Linguistics. London, UK: Pinter; 1994
sustaining it speak to positive interpersonal 4. Eggins S, Slade D. Analysing Casual Conversa-
meanings, and thus can become an important tion. London, UK: Equinox; 2005
indicator of a person's emotional and social 5. Martin JR, White PR. The Language of
well-being. Evaluation: Appraisal in English. New York,
NY: Palgrave; 2005
A consideration of the conversational 6. Togher L, Ferguson A Eds. Special issue:
moves used by the participants showed Ms. Systemic Functional Linguistics and the study of
Frances and Ms. Beatrice largely in control of language disorders. Clin Linguist Phon 2005;19
the flow of conversation: they contributed the 7. Armstrong E. Language disorder: a functional
overall largest proportion of opening and con- linguistic perspective. Clin Linguist Phon 2005;19:
tinuing moves, thus negotiation of content 137-153
happened largely on their terms. In addition, 8. Ferguson A, Thomson J. Systemic functional
linguistics and communication impairment. In:
reacting moves were far more often responses, Ball MJ, Perkins MR, Müller N, Howard S, eds.
rather than rejoinders, which means that par- The Handbook of Clinical Linguistics. Oxford,
ticipants on the whole interacted coUaboratively UK: BlackweU; 2008:130-145
to smoothly advance the negotiation of propo- 9. Armstrong E, Mortensen L. Everyday talk: its role
sitions offered in opening moves. The circum- in assessment and treatment for individuals with
stance that there were far fewer confronting aphasia. Brain Impair 2006;7:175-189
than supporting moves also contributes to the 10. Miller JF, Iglesias A. Systematic Analysis of
Language Transcripts (SALT), Instructional
creation of a supportive and positive interaction. Version 2010 [Computer software]. Madison,
In research on the communicative skills WI: SALT Software, LLC; 2010
and deficits of persons with DAT, it is com- 11. Halliday MAK. Language as code and language as
monly accepted that structural, syntagmatic behaviour: a systemic-functional interpretation of
aspects of language (phonology and syntax) the nature and ontogenesis of dialogue. In:
tend to be well preserved until comparatively Fawcett R, HaUiday MAK, Lamb SM, Makkai
A, eds. The Semiotics of Language and Culture,
late in disease progression.^'' However, an
Vol. I: Language as Social Semiotic. London, UK:
analysis of interpersonal aspects of language Pinter; 1984:3-35
use also reveals skillful use of the relevant 12. Guendouzi JA, Müller N. Approaches to Dis-
paradigms. Given that language use patterns course in Dementia. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
that construct and enact relationships, status, Erlbaum Associates; 2006
and identities in conversation are the result of 13. MuUer N, Guendouzi JA. Order and disorder in
lifelong processes of socialization, it is safe to conversation: encounters with dementia of the
assume that the relevant memories are quite Alzheimer's type. Clin Linguist Phon 2005;19:
• 393-404
deeply embedded and habitual. This in turn
14. Kempler D. Language changes in dementia of the
means that enabling social interactions, such as Alzheimer's type. In: Lubinski R, ed. Dementia
casual conversations "staged" (if need be) in and Communication. San Diego, CA: Singular;
facultative contexts, is likely to give rise to 1995:98-114
Copyright of Seminars in Speech & Language is the property of Thieme Medical Publishing Inc. and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express
written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like