You are on page 1of 50

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/326273971

Language Disorder in Children

Chapter · January 2017


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-57196-6

CITATIONS READS
5 1,194

4 authors:

Ronald B Gillam Sandra Laing Gillam


Utah State University Utah State University
135 PUBLICATIONS   5,283 CITATIONS    60 PUBLICATIONS   1,070 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Sarai Holbrook Carla Idalia Orellana


Utah State University Utah State University
3 PUBLICATIONS   18 CITATIONS    4 PUBLICATIONS   18 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Randomized Controlled Trial of the Supporting Knowledge in Language and Literacy (SKILL) Program for Children who are At-Risk for Language and Literacy Difficulties.
View project

Validation of a brief prosody rating scale for children with autism spectrum disorder View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ronald B Gillam on 10 May 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 1

Language Disorder in Children

Ronald B. Gillam
Sandra L. Gillam
Sarai Holbrook
Carla Orellana

Utah State University

In S. Goldstein & M. DeVries (Eds.), Handbook of DSM-5 Disorders in Children and


Adolescents (pp. 57-76), NY: Springer https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57196-6
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 2

Abstract
Children with language disorder have unusual difficulties understanding and/or using

vocabulary or grammar in age-appropriate ways. Their difficulties with language development

cannot be attributed to underlying conditions such as intellectual disability, sensory disorders,

medical conditions, neurological disorders, or motor dysfunctions. Language disorder affects

approximately 7% of preschool and school-age children children. Researchers have hypothesized

neurophysiological, biological (genetic), and psychological (cognitive/linguistic) causes of

language disorder, but the precise etiology of language disorder has yet to be determined.

Preschoolers with language disorder often have difficulties using grammatical morphemes to

mark tense and agreement These grammatical difficulties may continue into the school-age

years, when children with language disorder exhibit difficulties understanding and producing

narratives and expository texts and often present reading and writing delays. A complete

assessment of language disorder should include a detailed case history, interviews with the

individual’s family and teachers, administration of ecologically valid standardized and non-

standardized measures of communication, and consideration of related areas (e.g., behavioral,

medical, educational status). Language intervention for children with language disorder involves

techniques designed to remediate or modify the factors that contribute to language disorder or

teaching compensatory strategies to reduce the impact of a language disorder on academic, social

and/or vocational functioning. In the DSM-V, communication disorders (including language

disorder) are categorized as neurodevelopmental disorders along with intellectual disability,

autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, specific learning disorder, and

motor disorders.
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 3

Keywords
Language development
Language impairment
Language disorder
Grammar
Vocabulary
Communication

Overview of Language Disorder and History in DSMs


Children with language disorder have unusual difficulties understanding and/or using

vocabulary or grammar in age-appropriate ways. Their language impairments cannot be

attributed to underlying conditions such as intellectual disability, sensory disorders (hearing or

vision), medical conditions, neurological disorders, or motor dysfunctions (American Psychiatric

Association [APA], 2013). For example, children who are born with Down syndrome, severe

hearing impairment, or fetal alcohol syndrome are likely to have problems learning and using

language, but these children should receive diagnoses that are based on their primary disorder.

Thus, the diagnosis of language disorder should be reserved for children whose receptive and/or

expressive language skills are significantly below that which would be expected for their age in

the absence of any other developmental disability that could cause the language difficulties.

Language disorder occur fairly commonly, affecting approximately 7% of preschool and

school-age children children (Tomblin et al., 1997). Children whose language learning

difficulties do not resolve by age 5 frequently have difficulties with social and academic

language during the elementary school years (Tomblin et al., 2003), and they are likely to

continue to exhibit social, academic, and vocational difficulties well into the high-school years

(Johnson et al., 1999; Stothard, Snowling, Bishop, Chipchase, & Kaplan, 1998). Because reading
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 4

and writing are language tasks (Kamhi & Catts, 1999), many school-age children with language

disorders present literacy learning problems (Catts, Fey, Weismer & Bridges, 2014). Terms such

as developmental language disorders, specific language impairment, and primary language

impairment are also used as diagnostic labels for unexplained developmental language problems

(Bishop, 2014).

History of Language Disorder

Until the late 1950’s, language disorders and speech disorders were collapsed into a

single category of, “speech disturbances” (DSM-1, 1952) or “other speech impediments” (ICD-7,

1957). The modern era of clinical research that focused on children who demonstrated

significant language delays without co-occurring mental retardation, hearing impairment, or

autism began in the 1960’s, when researchers like Jon Eisenson and his colleagues at Stanford

studied children he referred to as having developmental aphasia (Eisenson, 1966, 1968). About

that same time, Mildred Berry and her colleagues at Texas Women’s University and the

University of Wisconsin at Madison were studying children that they referred to as having

idiopathic language retardation or neurogenic learning disabilities (Berry, 1969). Most

researchers at the time assumed that such children had atypical neural organization, often

referred to as “minimal brain dysfunctions” (Clements, 1966) that interfered with language

learning.

The ICD-9 (1978) differentiated between speech and language disorders, but both types

of disorders were part of a single diagnostic category called “developmental speech/language

disorders.” It was not until the DSM-III in 1980 that children with language disorders had their

own diagnostic category (developmental language disorder – expressive and receptive type) that

was separate from a “developmental articulation disorder.” The designation was further divided
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 5

into developmental expressive language disorder and developmental receptive language disorder

in the DSM-III-R (1987).

In both the DSM-III and the DSM-III-R, developmental language disorders were part of

the category of Specific Developmental Disorders that included developmental reading disorder

(dyslexia), developmental arithmetic disorder, and mixed specific developmental disorder. The

key to the diagnosis was that the child had a particular skill that was impaired relative to age

expectations but overall development was within the normal range. Grouping developmental

reading disorder and developmental language disorder in the same category acknowledged the

close relationships between these conditions.

In the DSM-V, communication disorders (including language disorder) are categorized as

neurodevelopmental disorders along with intellectual disability, autism spectrum disorder,

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, specific learning disorder, and motor disorders. The

disorders in this grouping have similar genetic risk factors, shared neural substrates, and similar

clinical features such as cognitive processing problems, higher rates of co-morbidity with each

other, and a continuous course of development into adulthood (Andrews et al., 2009). It is

interesting that in some respects, the current characterization of language disorder as a

neurologically-based disorder is similar in nature to the neurological-oriented characterization

that was prevalent in the 1960’s, albeit with a much stronger scientific base.

Current Diagnostic Criteria for Language Disorder

Language disorder is characterized by impairments in vocabulary, sentence structure, and

discourse level communication (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; American Speech-

Language Hearing Association [ASHA], 1993). These difficulties should be apparent across

multiple modalities including speaking, writing, and/or sign language (American Psychiatric
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 6

Association, 2013; ASHA, 1993). Language disorder affects receptive language, expressive

language, or both. Receptive language relates to an individual’s ability to comprehend linguistic

information, whereas expressive language relates to an individual’s ability to formulate and

produce linguistic information. To be diagnosed with a language disorder, the child’s language

performance must be shown to be significantly below age expectations to the extent that it would

be likely to interfere with communicative, social, academic, and/or vocational functioning.

Finally, language disorder must be present from early development and must not be attributable

to intellectual impairment or global developmental delay. Language difficulties exist in the

absence of or to a greater degree than could be explained by other sensory, motoric, medical, or

neurological conditions that may be present (APA, 2013).

The DSM-5 does not list specific psychometric criteria for diagnosing language disorder.

To date, the only epidemiologically-derived diagnostic criteria comes from a series of

longitudinal studies of kindergarten children with specific language impairment by Tomblin and

his colleagues (Catts et al., 2008; Fey et al., 2004; Leonard et al., 2007; Tomblin et al., 2003).

Tomblin, Records and Zhang (1996) reported excellent sensitivity and specificity (values above

.9) for a standard score cut-off of 83 on a global language test that assessed receptive and

expressive language skills in the areas of vocabulary, syntax, and discourse processes or for

performance of -1.25 standard deviations or more below the mean on two or more composite

scores representing language comprehension, language production, vocabulary skill,

grammatical skill, and narrative skill. Looking across a number of studies of children with

language disorders, many investigators identify language disorder by performance that is -1 or

more standard deviations below the mean on multiple tests or subtests of receptive or expressive

language (Leonard, 2014; Spaulding, Plante & Farinella, 2006).


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 7

Assessment of Language Disorders

A complete assessment of language disorder should include a detailed case history,

interviews with the individual and his family, ecologically valid standardized and non-

standardized measures of communication, and consideration of related areas (e.g., behavioral,

medical, educational status). Thus, assessment of language disorder should be a multifaceted

process that involves consideration of the aspects of an individual’s life that are affected by

language. A single individual can collect this data, but the preference is for professionals from

different disciplines to work together in the assessment process. According to the World Health

Organization this practice model is called Interprofessional Practice (IPP). In IPP, professionals

work collaboratively in health care and education settings to learn about, from and with each

other (World Health Organization, 2010). When multiple service providers from different

professional backgrounds work together, they are better able to provide comprehensive

healthcare and educational services to individuals and the families they serve.

Infants and toddlers

Assessment of language disorders in infants and toddlers is primarily related to predicting

which children have significant delays in language development that will persist over time.

However, the diagnostic accuracy of identifying infants and toddlers with persistent language

disorder is relatively poor (Woods et al., 2014). For this reason, a diagnosis of language disorder

is rarely made before 24 months of age.

Test instruments and analyses used for infants and toddlers include items to assess

developmental milestones related to communication, motor, cognitive, sensory, and social-

emotional skills. An example of a norm-referenced test for measuring general development in

multiple domains is the Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition (BDI-2) (Newborg,
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 8

2007), which can be used to obtain developmental age scores for personal-social skills, adaptive

behaviors, communication skills and cognitive abilities. The Rosetti Infant Toddler Language

Scale (Rosetti, 2006) has items measuring gestures, play, language comprehension, and language

expression, but it does not compare toddlers to other children their same age. In addition,

informal observation and sampling may be used when the infant or toddler may not be able to

cooperate with a test procedure because of compliance issues or extreme disability.

Parent report is often the best way to obtain evidence about language and communication

skills in infants and toddlers. There are screening and assessment measures such as the

MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (CDI), in which parents of children

between 8 and 37 months of age report which words their child understands or uses. The scale

divides the words children understand and produce into semantic classes so that examiners can

get a sense for the concepts and categories of words that are present or need elaboration. In

addition, there is a section that asks parents to report the communicative and symbolic gestures

their child has attempted or mastered.

Preschool and School-age children

A combination of assessment tools and analyses are needed in order to gain a full

understanding of a preschool-aged child’s receptive and expressive language skills. Some

standardized tests assess multiple areas of language to provide a broad view of an individuals’

communication system. Two of these global language measures are the Clinical Evaluation of

Language Fundamentals – 5th Edition (CLEF-5; Wiig, Semel, & Secord, 2013) and the Test of

Language Development – 4th Edition which is available for two levels, primary (TOLD-P:4;

Hammill & Newcomer, 2008) and intermediate (TOLD-I:4; Hammill & Newcomer, 2008). Both

tests offer composites that represent overall language functioning as well as domain specific
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 9

skills. Adding the Test of Narrative Language (Gillam & Pearson, 2004) to either of these

measures would provide examiners with the type of composite scores used in the Tomblin

epidemiological studies: overall indices of expressive and receptive language as well as scores

representing comprehension and use of vocabulary, grammatical morphology, syntax, and

narration. Many other tests are available that assess specific areas of language skill. Examiners

should select standardized tests that are reliable and valid in relation to the child being tested,

particularly if the child is from a culturally or linguistically diverse background (Friberg, 2010;

Paul, 2012; Spaulding, Plante & Farinella, 2006).

Although standardized tests can give information about an individual’s communication

functioning relative to a normative population, non-standardized assessments of language

provide a rich description of an individual’s communication functioning in multiple contexts.

Examples of non-standardized assessments include language samples, interviews, and dynamic

assessment.

Examiners commonly collect a language sample, or segment of connected speech, from

children in natural environments. Language samples reveal information about the individual’s

language productivity (total number of words, number of different words, mean length of

utterance), language complexity (complex vocabulary and sentence structure), and accuracy

(grammatical acceptability and articulatory intelligibility) and may reveal information about the

individual’s communication that is not readily apparent in standardized measures (Costanza-

Smith, 2010). Language samples are collected using a variety of methods. Conversational

language samples, in particular, illuminate an individual’s ability to comprehend questions and

statements within the normal flow of discourse. These can be collected through direct

observation by the examiner or family members may provide videos of an individual’s


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 10

communication with friends, siblings, parents, and others, which can then be analyzed. Language

samples are particularly useful for assessing individuals of culturally and linguistically diverse

backgrounds (Laing & Kamhi, 2003).

Input from people who interact regularly with a child suspected of having a language

disorder further informs the diagnostic process. Parents and other family members provide

valuable information regarding birth and development of children; further, they provide critical

insight into how the child’s communication affects family life, social interactions, and academic

functioning. Teachers can also assist, giving information about academic and social functioning

within the school setting. As children mature, information from self-reports becomes

increasingly important to inform intervention as the individual’s priorities need to be considered.

Examiners may also use a process called dynamic assessment to improve diagnostic

accuracy. Dynamic assessment is a “test-teach-retest” method in which clinicians obtain

evidence of a child’s ability to perform a language task (such as narration), then observe the

child’s learning processes as they teach the skill during two teaching sessions, and then retest to

determine how much the child learned. This method does not rely on the prior experience of an

individual; it is designed to assess an individual’s ability to respond to teaching. Observations of

the child’s ability to modify their responses based on the clinician’s input is the key to making

correct judgments of language impairment (Peña & Gillam, 2000; Peña, Gillam & Bedore,

2014).

The assessment of language disorder involves a battery of assessment tools, both formal

and informal. Understanding an individual’s abilities compared to others of similar age and

experience is important for diagnosing language impairment accurately Collecting information


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 11

about the individual’s functional abilities, strengths, and weaknesses can be even more important

for developing intervention plans.

Etiology of Language Disorder

Researchers have hypothesized neurophysiological, biological (genetic), and

psychological (cognitive/linguistic) causes of language disorder. As with nearly all of the

neurodevelopmental disorders, the precise etiology of language disorder has yet to be

determined.

By definition, children with language disorders present language difficulties in the

absence of frank neurological impairments such as brain lesions. However, it is reasonable to

assume that there is a connection between neural functions and language ability. To date,

relatively little neuroimaging research has been conducted with this population. Some studies

have revealed neurological differences between children with language disorders and typically-

developing children. For example, structural differences have been noted in inferior frontal

cortex (Gauger, Lombardino & Leonard, 1997) and the temporal-parietal junction (Jernigan et

al., 1991) in children with language disorders. In addition, many of these children have been

shown to have atypical hemispheric asymmetries in which the perisylvian area (inferior frontal

gyrus back to the supramarginal gyrus) in the right hemisphere is larger than the same area in the

left hemisphere (Plante, Swisher, Vance & Rapcsak, 1991; Gauger, Lombardino & Leonard,

1997). This pattern is the opposite of the left lateralization that is seen in most typically-

developing children. However, fMRI studies (Ellis Weismer, et al, 2005) and functional Near

Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) studies (Gillam et al, 2015) have not revealed laterality

differences between children with and without language disorders as they were engaged in

memory or language tasks.


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 12

There have been many more electrophysiological studies of children with language

disorders using EEG and ERP techniques, primarily because this technology is relatively

inexpensive and data can be collected in more naturalistic settings. Results of a number of

electrophysiological studies suggest that attention processes for auditory nonspeech and speech

stimuli are unusual in children with language disorders (McArthur, Atinson & Ellis, 2009;

Weber-Fox, et al., 2010). Group differences have been detected for grammatical processing

(Shafer et al., 2000; Weber-Fox et al., 2010) but not for semantic processing (Fonteneau & van

der Lely, 2008; Weber-Fox et al, 2010). Importantly, it has been shown that ERP responses of

children with language disorder become more like their typically-achieving peers after

participating in a narrative language intervention (Popescu et al., 2009). The EEG and ERP

evidence suggests that neural processing differences contribute to language disorder. More

importantly, these processes can be altered through language intervention. Clearly, this is a

fertile area for future research.

There are genetic contributions to language disorder. A number of studies have shown

that children with language disorder have higher percentages of family members with a history

of language problems than control children without language disorder (Barry, Yasin & Bishop,

2007; Flax et al, 2003). Studies of twins consistently reveal higher concordance rates (both

twins presenting language disorder) for monozygotic twin pairs than for dizygotic twin pairs

(Bishop, North & Donlan, 1995; Tomblin & Buckwalter, 1998). Molecular genetic studies have

identified potential genetic variants that may contribute to language disorder including FOXP2

(Lai et al, 2001; Tomblin et al, 2009), CNTNAP2 (Vernes et al. 2008), and 6p22 (Rice, Smith, et

al., 2009). Rice (2012) has proposed that the start-up mechanisms responsible for gene

expression may be delayed in children with language disorder. This hypothesis is consistent
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 13

with evidence of slower growth trajectories in children with language disorders (McArthur &

Bishop, 2005; Rice, Wexler & Hershberger, 1998; Tomblin, et al. 2003).

Psychological hypotheses about the etiology of language disorder can be divided into

linguistic (domain-specific) and cognitive (domain-general) factors. Domain-specific

explanations of language disorder suggest that the difficulty children with language impairment

have is specifically related to the grammatical system. Most domain-specific accounts of

language disorder are related to the notion that some children treat certain linguistic rules as

optional rather than obligatory (Marinis & van der Lely, 2007; Friedman & Novogrodsky, 2007).

One early explanation is called the extended optional infinitive hypothesis proposed by Wexler

(1994). Under this account, language disorder is thought to arise from a specific difficulty in

understanding that one must mark tense in a main clause. Inconsistent marking of tense is typical

for all children as they acquire language, however students with language impairment continue to

do so long after their typically developing peers (Rice, Wexler & Hershberger, 1998).

Domain-general explanations of language disorder suggest that limitations in cognitive

processing interfere with one’s ability to learn language. Evidence has shown that children with

language impairment demonstrate slower response times during linguistic and nonlinguistic tasks

(Kail, 1994; Leonard et al., 2007), however some have argued that processing efficiency, rather

than processing speed, may better explain performance of children with language impairment on

these tasks (Gillam, Cowan & Day, 1995). It is likely that some combination of cognitive factors

including limited information processing capacity (including attention, perception, and memory)

and slowed processing speed play an important role in language disorder. For example, working

memory and processing speed together has been shown to account for 62% of the variance in the

composite language scores of adolescents with language disorder (Leonard et al., 2007).
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 14

Symptom Presentation, Course of the Disorder, and Developmental Changes

Preschool Years
During the preschool years, children should learn a variety of words that represent

different word classes (e.g., question words, colors, spatial terms), conjunctions (e.g.,

coordinating, subordinating), and words that may be used to categorize, and describe objects,

actions and events (Anderson, 2010). By 24 months of age, children developing typically are

using 2-word phrases with diverse and varied vocabulary, while children with a language

disorder may still be pointing at objects they want or using a few single words. Preschoolers who

do not learn words easily or who have difficulty expressing a range of semantic relationships

such as possession (e.g., mommy’s shoe), recurrence (e.g., mommy came back), and location

(e.g., mommy in the kitchen) may have a language disorder.

Preschoolers with language disorders often produce very few grammatical morphemes

(e.g., possessives, plurals) correctly and have difficulties using them to mark tense and

agreement (e.g., She walkded home. She my friend). They may also have difficulty

understanding and using complex sentences. Preschoolers developing typically generally

produce sentences that are grammatically correct (70%) at the age of 3, while preschoolers with

language disorder at the same age make significantly more errors (>70%).

School-age Children
School-age children may continue to make some grammatical errors in their sentences,

however no more than 20% of their sentences should be grammatically incorrect. In addition, the

conversational speech of school-aged children should contain a variety of complex sentences (at

least 20% in a 100 utterance sample). Children with language disorders tend to use fewer

complex sentences and tend to make more grammatical errors throughout the school-age years

(Gillam & Johnston, 1993)


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 15

Another sign of a potential language problem for school-age students includes delays or

deficits in phonological awareness (Gillon, 2005). Phonological awareness is the knowledge that

individual sounds make up the words in the language. Tasks that measure this awareness include

those that requires sound categorization (e.g., which word starts like leg; lamb, fox, ball),

blending (e.g., when you combine the sounds, /b/, /a/, /l/ they make the word?), and segmentation

(e.g., the word ball may be broken into what 3 sounds?) Problems with phonological awareness

may make it more difficult for children with language disorder to make the necessary

connections between letters and the sounds they represent, making word recognition difficult.

Narration (storytelling) is an important form of discourse during the school-age years.

Children developing typically produce narratives that are generally grammatical and contain an

adequate number of syntactically diverse and complex sentences (>20%). Their stories contain

basic story elements (settings, events, actions, endings) that are organized temporally and

logically. Children with language disorder are likely to produce narratives that are disorganized,

and that contain more ungrammatical sentences and fewer complex sentences (Gillam &

Johnston, 1993).

Finally, a hallmark of a language problem during the school-age years is difficulty

understanding and using higher-level skills such as inferences (Laing & Kamhi, 2002; Karasinski

& Ellis-Weismer, 2010). Inferences are assumption that are made about concepts or ideas that

were not stated. For example, in the paragraph, “The girl watched while the boy jumped onto the

steps. He slipped and fell. She laughed,” inferences are required to understand that the child who

“slipped and fell” was the boy, and that the girl was the one who laughed.

Secondary School
Children with a language disorder at the adolescent stage may demonstrate difficulty with

understanding and using figurative language, multiple meaning words, idioms and abstract or
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 16

curricular vocabulary. Much of the conversational and written discourse that is encountered

during the adolescent years contains morphologically complex words that require knowledge and

experience with word structure as well as mastery of sophisticated metalinguistic skills (Nippold

& Sun, 2008). Students with language impairment may demonstrate poor comprehension of

curricular texts presented orally or in written form identified by an inability to state main ideas,

and to recall and report facts about persons, dates and details, and to make logical generalizations

and conclusions (Gillam, Fargo & St. Clair, 2008). They may also have difficulties composing

book reports, essays, and term papers that require integration of information contained in oral

and written discourse.

Sociodemographic Trends in Language Disorder Identification

As mentioned earlier, Tomblin et al (1997) conducted an epidemiological study on the

prevalence of language disorders based on a stratified sample from regions of Illinois and Iowa.

They estimated an overall prevalence rate of 7.4%. They further estimated a prevalence rate of

6% in girls and 8% in boys, which differs from the the 2:1 male to female ratio that had been

previously reported in the literature. When race and ethnicity were taken into consideration,

variations across groups were found, with Native American children having the highest

prevalence rates, followed by African-American children, then Hispanic children. None of the

Asian American children were found to have a language disorder in that particular study.

Children who spoke another language than English or who heard another language spoken at

home were excluded from the study. The differences in prevalence rates could be attributed to

culturally and linguistically biased assessments. These differences were further confounded by a

socioeconomic factor, namely, that racial/ethnic background correlated with parental education,

which was also associated with language disorder.


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 17

Epidemiological studies reveal cracks in the referral and diagnosis system. Tomblin et al

(1997) found that only 29% of the parents of children identified with a language disorder had

been informed by a professional that their child had a speech or language problem. When

accounting for severity of the child’s disorder 39% of the parents of the more severely impaired

children had prior knowledge of their child’s language problems compared to 27% of the less

severely impaired children. Similarly, in a study of 513 at-risk, low SES children, King et al

(2005) found that at least one source (parent, primary care provider, or home visitor) reported

initial concerns about 60% of the more severely language impaired children but only 43% of the

less severely language impaired children.

Peña, Gillam, Bedore, and Bohman (2011) determined the risk for language disorder in a

sample of 1, 100 bilingual pre-kindergarten children in two states. Using the criteria of

performance at or below the 25th percentile on at least one Spanish measure and one English

measure, they found that 30.6% of 1029 Latino children were at-risk for language disorder.

There were no differences in the risk of language disorder for various sub-groups of children

with differing levels of exposure to English and Spanish when controlling for age. Unlike the

Tomblin et al (2007) study, there was a weak association between socioeconomic status and risk

for language disorder in this Latino population. When they followed 167 at-risk children over a

two-year period of time, they found that 12% of at-risk bilingual children were language

impaired (Gillam et al., 2013). This is consistent with the King et al. (2205) finding that 10% of

their at-risk population had language disorders. The availability of linguistically and culturally

appropriate assessments may impact accuracy of prevalence in diverse groups leading to both

overrepresentation and underrepresentation of these groups. It appears that a process that


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 18

involves universal screening for language ability followed by more complete language testing

yields a prevalence rate of 10 – 12% for samples of monolingual and bilingual children.

Associated Impairments
Problems with the acquisition and use of language are prominent characteristics of a

number of diagnostic conditions. Language disorder may result from deficits in the growth and

development of the brain. Language disorder is also associated with neurocognitive disorders

such as traumatic brain injuries or sensory disorders such as hearing impairment. Finally,

language disorder can be related to neglect and abuse, behavioral problems, and emotional

problems. The most common associated conditions are late talkers (early delayed language),

social communication disorder, intellectual disorder, and autism spectrum disorder.

Late Talkers

Children who start speaking very late (fewer than 50 words at 18 months of age) are often

referred to as late talkers. When these children have age-appropriate language comprehension

skills and no family history of language impairment, they are likely to catch up to their peers by

the time they reach kindergarten (Rescorla, 2002). They often perform within the normal range

on a variety of language measures by the time they reach high school (Rescorla, 2009). Late

talkers whose deficits persist into the school-age years are usually identified as having a

language disorder.

Social Communication Disorder

An important change in the DSM-V involves the distinction between disorders that

primarily affect semantic and syntactic aspects of language and those that primarily affect the

pragmatic aspect of language. Social (pragmatic) Communication Disorder (SCD) is defined as

a primary deficit in the social use of nonverbal and verbal communication as demonstrated by
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 19

impairments in social communication (greetings, sharing information), in the ability to meet

listener needs, and in such skills as taking turns in conversations. These problems limit the

ability to develop and maintain social relationships. Many children have symptoms that cross

the boundaries between SCD and language disorder. For example, children with impairments of

language form (syntax) and content (vocabulary) may have social communication difficulties as

well. However, in diagnosing a social communication disorder, the clinician must determine that

the child’s poor social language skills do not result primarily from vocabulary or grammar

impairments.

Intellectual Disability

An intellectual disability is indicated by severe deficits in intellectual functions (e.g.,

reasoning and problem solving) and adaptive behaviors (e.g., communication and activities of

daily living) that appear during childhood or adolescence. Children with intellectual disabilities

often present motor, language and social delays before 2 years of age. Later in development,

they earn scores below 70 on both IQ tests and measures of adaptive functioning. Intellectual

disabilities can be caused by genetic conditions such as Down Syndrome or Fragile X syndrome,

maternal infections during pregnancy such as rubella or cytomegalovirus, complications during

delivery resulting in anoxia, diseases such as measles and meningitis, or environmental factors

such as neglect or severe malnutrition. Children with intellectual disabilities almost always have

language impairments related to language content, and many also have impairments in the area

of language form (Rakhlin & Grigorenko, 2015). It is common for these children to be

significantly delayed in learning words, and to use short, simple sentences well into the school-

age years. Despite the fact that children with a language disorder do not have deficits in basic

intelligence, the nature of the language impairment demonstrated by children in both groups can
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 20

be quite similar (Laws & Bishop, 2004). The exception is that there is often a bigger

discrepancy between comprehension and production abilities in children with a language

disorder than in children with an intellectual disability (Polišenská, & Kapalková, 2014).

Autism Spectrum Disorder

Children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) must have pervasive and

sustained difficulties with reciprocal social communication and social interaction characterized

by severe problems with conversation, sharing of interests or emotions and initiating or

responding to social interactions. Children with ASD must also present restricted or repetitive

patterns of behavior, interests or activities such as repetitive motor movements (e.g., rocking or

rubbing their heads), insistence on sameness, and/or inflexible adherence to routines. These

symptoms are present from early childhood, and they interfere with daily activities and the

ability to learn through socialization. Language impairment accompanied by a lack of interest in

socializing (e.g., lack of eye contact), or unusual social interactions (e.g., not responding to

speaker or responding to anything said by saying the same words over and over) may be early

signs of ASD. Even though a number of individuals with ASD have intellectual disabilities

and/or language disorder, their key symptoms cannot be explained by these other deficits

(American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

In the past, ASD and language impairment have been viewed as separate and distinct

disorders, however, it has become increasingly clear that there are a number of areas in which the

two overlap in their language profiles (phenotype), as well as neural processing patterns and

genetic contributions (Ellis-Weismer, 2014). In terms of language profiles, children with ASD as

well as some with language impairment have been shown to demonstrate similar problems in

language use or pragmatics. While some have suggested that basic language skills such as
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 21

phonology, morphology and syntax remain intact in ASD, recent research has shown that many

students with ASD experience more basic language deficits involving all of the domains of

language. In fact, as many as 50% of students with ASD never develop functional

communication skills (Eigsti et al., 2011).

There are neurological and genetic differences between children with language disorders

and children with ASD. While some studies have shown rightward hemispheric asymmetry in

children with ASD and children with language disorder, children with language disorder are

more likely to have structural differences in inferior frontal cortex (Boca’s area) than children

with ASD. Genetic studies that have explored potential overlap between language disorder and

ASD have shown that the incidence of ASD is increased for both siblings of children with SLI

and ASD, however there is no clear indication of a common genetic etiology between ASD and

language disorder (Rakhlin & Grigorenko, 2015).

Comorbid Disorders
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is characterized by the presence of

developmentally inappropriate levels of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention across

multiple contexts and settings that affects 3-5% of the school age children (National Institutes of

Health [NIH] Consensus Development Panel, 2000). Children with ADHD may demonstrate

comorbid deficits in language but it is not uncommon for the presence of ADHD to result in

misdiagnosis of language disorder. Generally, children with ADHD without language

impairment do not demonstrate significant errors in marking tense while children with language

impairment do (Redmond, 2005). In addition, students with ADHD with no comorbid deficits in

language have been shown to perform similarly to typically developing peers in producing
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 22

organized stories that contained critical story elements (events, actions, endings), while students

with language impairments do not (Luo & Timler, 2008). In a recent study, Redmond et al.,

(2011) examined the extent to which tense marking, nonword repetition, sentence recall and

narrative proficiency accurately characterized language impairment in 60, 7-8 year old children.

Findings revealed that students with ADHD performed similarly to their typical peers on

measures of tense-marking, nonword repetition, and sentence recall while the students with

language impairment did not.

Specific Learning Disorder

Specific learning disorder (more commonly referred to as a learning disability) is

characterized by persistent difficulties learning academic skills. These children’s skills in the

areas of word decoding, reading comprehension, spelling, writing, number facts, and/or

mathematical reasoning are well below the expectations for their chronological age. These

learning disorders are “specific” because, like language disorder they cannot be explained by

intellectual disabilities, visual or hearing problems, other mental or neurological disorders, or

poor instruction.

Treatment for Children with Language Disorders


Language intervention for children with language disorder involves techniques designed to

remediate or modify the factors that contribute to language disorder or teaching compensatory

strategies to reduce the impact of a language disorder on academic, social and/or vocational

functioning. Clinicians design treatment programs that target deficiencies in language form,

content and use for preschoolers (ages 3-5), elementary students (ages 5-10), and adolescents

(11-21). In the next sections, we will discuss language targets (form, content and use) as well as

methods and techniques used to remediate, modify or help children compensate for factors that
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 23

contribute to difficulties in these language domains.

Intervention for Preschoolers

Targets. Intervention targeting language form in preschoolers typically focus on

improving morphological abilities such as the use of copula BE (am, is, are, was, were), auxiliary

BE (am, is, are, was, were), or auxiliary DO (do, does, did). Other forms that might be targeted

in intervention sessions include the use of possessive –s, 3rd person present –s, and irregular and

regular past tense verbs. Clinicians may work with preschoolers to increase the length and

complexity of the sentences they use and to use more diverse types of sentences such as

statements, commands, and interrogatives.

Intervention for preschoolers may also focus on the content of language. For example,

clinicians may work with children to increase the size of their vocabulary (e.g., basic concepts,

colors), their use of various semantic relationships (e.g., agent-action, agent-action-object), and

their understanding and production of verbs, pronouns and conjunctions.

Language use or pragmatic goals may also be a part of language intervention programs for

preschool children. Intervention for language use might include teaching children to be more

flexible in their use of language in different contexts, to use their imagination and/or to improve

their abilities to participate and contribute to conversations they have with parents, teachers and

their peers. Clinicians may help preschoolers develop their conversational skills by improving

their abilities in initiating and maintaining conversational topics, taking turns and in knowing

when and how to change topics. In addition, preschoolers with language disorders may also need

to learn how to let others know when they do not understand something and to clarify

information they provide during conversations that may be misunderstood by others (Brinton &

Fujiki, 1995).
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 24

Clinicians may also work with preschoolers on narrative comprehension and production.

Preschoolers with narrative language difficulties may work on their abilities to answer basic

questions about stories such as “who did what to whom?” Clinicians may work with preschoolers

to relate past experiences, to describe ongoing activities or routinized events or to recount or

generate fictional stories. All aspects of language form and content necessary for preschoolers to

understand and produce narratives would be integrated into this kind of intervention program.

Preschoolers with language disorders may develop problems in literacy so intervention might

also target emergent literacy skills such as print awareness, book awareness, and letter-sound

correspondence or activities designed to foster phonological awareness. Phonological awareness

lessons may involve teaching preschoolers to blend and segment spoken words, delete whole

words from compound words and to delete syllables and phonemes from spoken words, phrases

or sentences (Goldstein & Olszewski, 2015).

Methods and Techniques for improving language form, content and use for

preschoolers. There are 3 basic approaches for providing intervention services to young children

with language impairments. These include clinician centered, client-centered and hybrid

approaches (Fey, Long & Finestack, 2003). Clinician-centered approaches include very

structured techniques such as engaging children in drill and drill-play activities. Child-centered

techniques include indirect stimulation approaches. Hybrid approaches incorporate both

clinician-centered and child-centered techniques.

Clinicians may use various indirect language facilitation techniques and contingent responses

within each of these general approaches to model the forms, content and pragmatic skills the

child needs to learn. Indirect language facilitation techniques may include demonstrations,

expansions, expatiations, and vertical structures (See Cleave, Becker, Curran, Van Horne, &
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 25

Fey, 2015 for a meta-analysis of the efficacy of these techniques). Demonstrations are the

repeated but variable use of linguistic patterns during ongoing play activities, such as when

playing with a toy farm set. For example, to highlight the use of past-tense -ed, a clinician might

say, “The pig trotted into the barn. The cow trotted into the barn. The goat trotted into the barn.

They all trotted into the barn.”

Expansions are contingent verbal responses that increase the length or complexity of an

utterance produced by the child during the play interaction. For example, a child might say,

“Puppy” to which the clinician might say, “Yes, that is a puppy.” In this example, the utterance

“Yes that is a puppy” is used contingently by the SLP because it incorporates what the child said

(puppy) and it is an expansion because it incorporates the child’s utterance into a longer more

complex utterance from one (puppy) to five (yes, it is a puppy).

Expatiations are contingent verbal responses that add new but relevant information to the

child's utterance. For example, a child might say, “goat” to which the clinician may respond,

“That is a stinky goat.” The utterance provided by the clinician is contingent because it includes

what the child offered (goat) but it is also an expatiation because it adds new information (There

is a goat AND it is stinky).

Vertical structures are language facilitation techniques that involve asking children

questions to elicit at least two responses from them. These responses are then combined by the

clinician into one longer, more complex utterance. For example, consider the following

interaction targeting the causal connective “because”:

Clinician: The pig got a new hat. How do you think he feels?

Preschooler: Happy.

Clinician: Why does he feel happy?


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 26

Preschooler: Because he got a hat.

Clinician: The pig is happy because he got a new hat.

When using vertical structuring as a language facilitation technique, it is important to remember

that both utterances that are elicited from the preschooler must be combined into a complete

sentence (happy, because he got a new hat). Simple repetitions of each of the utterances

independently does not constitute a vertical structure.

Language Intervention during the elementary school years (ages 5-10)

Targets. Language Intervention during the elementary school years (ages 5-10) targets

similar language skills as those of the preschool years (e.g., morphology, sentence structure,

vocabulary, pragmatics and literacy) but may incorporate more advanced forms using

curriculum-based instructional materials (e.g., books, classroom conversation). For example,

elementary students may need morphological intervention to help them understand and use

advanced morphological forms (e.g., prophet/prophetic), prefixes and suffixes (e.g., dis-, inter-, -

ic, -tion) and to be more aware of grammatical errors they may be making in spoken and written

language. Further, students may need to learn and practice certain sentence structures such as

those that contain dependent clauses (e.g., prepositional phrases, adverbial clauses) so they that

may better understand and produce more complex sentence structures.

Language content or vocabulary instruction may target understanding and use of basic

word knowledge, abstract or figurative language, as well as literate language such as

coordinating and subordinating conjunctions, adverbs, metalinguistic and metacognitive verbs

and elaborated noun phrases. In addition to teaching students new words, Clinicians may also

assist them in learning strategies so they may learn new words on their own.
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 27

Intervention for language use (pragmatics) during the elementary school years involves

helping students to learn the discourse management skills they need to participate in

conversations and to repair conversational breakdowns. Other conversational skills that may be

part of an intervention program for elementary students with language disorder may include

teaching students to request clarification when they do not understand and to express their ideas

in polite or persuasive ways in a variety of contexts.

Students may also need instruction to help them understand and use narrative and

expository discourse skills. Fictional narratives contain characters whose goal motivated actions

become part of a story. Students with language disorders may need instruction to learn how to

identify the parts of a story (character, setting, initiating event, action, consequence) so they may

better organize their own stories and recall stories they have heard or read. Instruction may also

focus on assisting students in telling stories that contain multiple embedded or associated

episodes using coherent and organized linguistic form and content.

Expository discourse such as comparison, cause and effect or problem-solution becomes

important in academic settings during the elementary school years (Gillam, Fargo & Robertson,

2009). Clinicians may provide students with language disorder instruction on how to identify

features of the different types of expository structures. For example, to identify comparison texts

students would be taught signal words such as different from, same as, similar to, instead of, and

although. Students would then be taught how to use these words to compare how two or more

events, topics, concepts, theories, problems or objects compare.

Clinicians may focus on helping elementary students with language disorder to learn the

underlying language skills they need to become proficient readers. For example, students in the

early grades learn the difference between long and short vowels and how to blend sounds into
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 28

words while reading. Clinicians may design lessons to strengthen their phonological processing

skills. Higher-level phonemic awareness tasks such as deletion (e.g., deleting sounds from

clusters), and practice with multi-syllabic words (e.g., intellectual) might be part of such a

program.

Methods and Techniques for improving language form, content and use for school-age

students.

Several treatment procedures including the clinician, client-centered and hybrid

approaches used with preschoolers are appropriate to improve language form, content and use

with school-age children. Another popular approach to language intervention during the school-

age years incorporates aspects of modeling, practice, role play and discussion and is called

literature-based language intervention.

Literature based language intervention utilizes children’s literature as a context for

teaching specific form (morphology, syntax), content (semantics/vocabulary) use (narrative,

expository and conversational discourse) and literacy targets (phonological awareness) that

students may to work on to be successful in multiple settings. Literature –based activities

typically include clinician-led discussions about concepts and ideas that are contained in the

books. Clinicians may read and reread a story, pointing out specific aspects in the story that are

relevant to the goals the student is working on. For example, students may be working on

narrative discourse comprehension and retelling. In this case, the clinician might read a story,

highlight the story elements that are contained in the story (character, setting, events) and then

ask students to answer questions about the story elements. The clinician may re-read the story,

and then ask students to retell the story using the pictures in the book, and then without them, to
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 29

foster recall and memory specific details.

If language form is a target, the clinician may re-read the book while highlighting the

form that is the focus of instruction at that point in time, for example causal words such as

“because” or “so.” The clinician may use vertical structuring to facilitate students use of complex

sentences containing the words such as in the following example using the book “Tacky the

Penguin.”

Clinician: How did the penguins in the book feel about the hunters?

Student: The penguins were afraid of the hunters.

Clinician: Why were they afraid?

Student: Because the hunters might get them.

Clinician: the penguins were afraid because the hunters might get them.

Similarly, vocabulary is a frequent target of literature-based language intervention. Literature-

based vocabulary instruction is most effective when words are embedded in stories that are read

repeatedly, when the meaning of words are explicitly taught within the story context, and when

students are given opportunities to use the target vocabulary during their own discussions and

retellings about the text (Beimiller & Boote, 2006).

In one recent study, Gillam, Gillam and Reese (2012) found that literature-based

language intervention that incorporated these important aspects of instruction resulted in larger

narrative and vocabulary gains than more clinician-directed, drill based language intervention

procedures.

Language Intervention during the adolescent years (ages 11-21)


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 30

Targets. Adolescent language intervention often focuses on targeting skills to support

students so they may meet the increasing linguistic demands of middle and high school. This is

important if they are going to be ready to enter the workforce or to be prepared for college-entry

should they choose that path. Intervention programs for adolescents will target higher-level

language skills they need to understand curricular texts such as the use of inferencing and the

comprehension of figurative language and advanced vocabulary, including words they need to

understand to access their curricular materials. In addition, Clinicians will assist students in

learning the language skills they need to analyze and construct well-supported arguments.

Unfortunately, not all activities are designed to remediate or modify a language disorder

particularly in the adolescent years. Very often, Clinicians must teach compensatory strategies to

students at this stage so they are able to circumvent some of the underlying problems that may

not be remediable. Strategies include teaching students how to use graphic organizers, checklists,

and organizational techniques in order to help them recall and use information to answer

questions, recall information and construct spoken and written arguments. Strengthening

students’ metalinguistic and metacognitive skills will also be an important part of intervention

efforts during the adolescent years. Students may be taught specific strategies they may use to be

able to monitor their own performance during reading, writing, and even in social contexts. In

addition to teaching strategies and self-monitoring skills, Clinicians may need to put certain

modifications in place to help students to be more successful in the classroom such as

preferential seating and making recorded lectures available to them.

Methods and Techniques for improving language form, content and use for adolescents.

Many of the same methods and techniques used for younger school-age students may

also be effective for adolescents and include direct instruction during literature-based activities,
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 31

however, because adolescents are more cognitively sophisticated, they may take part in more

meta-cognitive activities that involve encouraging them to think about what they know, and how

they learn best. Role playing becomes an important intervention context at this stage and may

involve presenting students with situations where they must generate solutions that target form,

content or use goals that are important for them. For example, students may be asked to provide

a solution to a scenario that takes into consideration how characters feel and does not

compromise the relationship between the two characters. Further, students may be asked to

explain “why” they feel their solutions are appropriate in different situations. In particular,

adolescents must be able to generate long-term solutions to problems and discuss why short-term

solutions may not be sufficient. Any number of language targets, strategies, and self-monitoring

skills may be taught during role-playing with adolescents, and is a popular method for teaching

at this stage.

Contexts of Intervention

In years past, removing one or more students from the classroom has been the traditional

method for providing intervention to students of all ages. More recently, in light of the World

Health Organization’s call for Interprofessional Practice in health care and educational settings,

Clinicians and educators have become more comfortable working together, sometimes in regular

classroom, to provide services to children with language disorders. This approach to language

intervention makes it possible to integrate language-learning goals (complex sentence usage,

narrative abilities, phonological awareness, vocabulary, expository texts, learning strategies, etc.)

into aspects of the academic curriculum. Clinicians and classroom teachers may work together to

plan lessons and will either carry them out together with the whole class or one of them may lead

the class while the other facilitates student participation. For example, a clinician might conduct
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 32

lessons on creating narratives with the entire class all at once (Gillam, et al., 2014) while the

classroom teacher works with small groups of students in the classroom by helping them ask or

answer relevant questions.

Another possibility it to arrange the classroom into small “centers” that allow for groups

of children to rotate through them to practice various phonological awareness skills (van Kleeck,

Gillam & McFadden,1998) or even to develop new vocabulary (Gillam, S., 2007). For example,

students may participate in a listening center, where they hear definitions of new vocabulary pre-

recorded on a digital recorder that are relative to a curricular topic, such as volcanos. In the next

center, they may watch a short film about volcanos. In the last center, students may participate in

discussions with the clinician, and asked to provide definitions and explanations about the

concepts and ideas they are learning. Any number of language targets may be taught in this

format. The overwhelming findings suggest that speech-language pathology services may be

effectively provided in regular classroom contexts (Cirrin, et al., 2010; Hadley, Simmerman,

Long & Luna, 2000).

Prognosis and Outcomes of Children with Language Disorders


Unfortunately, symptoms related to language disorder tend to persist. There have been a

number of longitudinal studies that have followed children diagnosed with language disorders as

preschoolers or kindergartners into their teenage years. Approximately 70% of these children

continued to demonstrate language impairments as teenagers (Beitchman et al., 1996; Nippold et

al., 2009). The results of these studies suggest that teenagers with a history of language disorders

are highly likely to present with cognitive, linguistic, social and academic problems (Johnson et

al., 2009; Lindsey, Dockrell, Nippold, & Fujiki, 2012; Nippold et al, 2009; Tomblin & Nippold,

2014). Studies of adults with a history of language disorder during childhood indicate that they

often have persisting difficulties with measures of language and literacy, poor memory skills,
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 33

lower levels of employment, and fewer close friendships than individuals with no history of

language disorder (Clegg et al., 2005; Johnson, Beitchman & Brownlie, 2010). However, these

individuals are likely to perceive their quality of life in much the same way as their non-impaired

peers (Records, Tomblin & Freesse, 1992; Johnson Beitchman & Brownlie, 2010). Individuals

who had less severe impairments during childhood tend to have the best language outcomes at

high-school and into adulthood. Specifically, in a well-designed longitudinal study, language

ability during childhood was a significant predictor of educational attainment and occupational

SES during adulthood, especially when it was combined with teacher ratings of behavior, family

SES and reading ability (Johnson et al., 2010). It is clear that there is a strong need to develop

more effective interventions for children with language disorders and for teachers, parents, and

other professionals who work with these children to advocate for educational and vocational

programs that will promote better life-long outcomes.

Conclusion
Language disorder can be diagnosed when children’s receptive and/or expressive

language skills are (1) significantly poorer than the language that is typically expected for same-

age peers; (2) when his or her language difficulties interfere with the ability to participate in and

learn from everyday interactions with adults and other children; and (3) when the language

deficits cannot be attributed to any other disability. Neurophysiological, biological (genetic) and

psychological (cognitive/linguistic) factors contribute to language disorder.

Very young children with language disorder may not combine words to express

relationships until well after 2 years of age. During the preschool years, children with language

disorder may learn new words more slowly and often make more grammatical errors than their

typically developing peers. During the elementary school years, children with language disorders
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 34

have difficulty learning and using words that are important for functioning in their classrooms;

their conversational language may contain an unusually large percentage of ungrammatical

utterances; and they may experience difficulties understanding and creating coherent narratives.

Older school age children with language disorder do not comprehend and use morphologically

complex words like summarization and predictive that appear in the curricular materials they

encounter in school, and they have difficulty understanding the expository texts they read for

their courses.

Language disorder can be diagnosed reliably in the late preschool and early school-age

years. Clinicians assess language abilities with the use of standardized tests and informal

assessment procedures such as language sample analysis, observations of children as they are

conversing with others, and interviews with parents and teachers. Clinicians can conduct

assessment independently, or they can employ interprofessional approaches to assessment in

which professionals from different disciplines conduct evaluations.

Three basic models of language intervention services were described: child-centered,

clinician-centered and hybrid, as were facilitative techniques such as self-talk, parallel talk,

focused stimulation, expansion, and vertical structures. Many clinicians use book discussions as

the primary context for language intervention with school-age children. Clinicians who work in

public school settings sometimes conduct language intervention in the regular classroom. This

approach to language intervention is helpful when clinicians want to integrate their language-

learning goals (complex sentence usage, narrative abilities, phonological awareness, vocabulary,

expository texts, learning strategies, etc.) with the expectations of the academic curriculum.
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 35

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2004). Preferred practice patterns for the

profession of speech-language pathology (No. PP2004-00191). Rockville, MD. Retrieved

from http://www.asha.org/docs/html/PP2004-00191.html

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1993). Definitions of communication

disorders and variations [Relevant Paper]. Available from www.asha.org/policy.

Anderson, A. (2011). Linguistic specificity through literate language use in preschool-age

children with specific language impairment and typical language. Child Language

Teaching and Therapy, 27(1), 109–123. http://doi.org/10.1177/0265659010381691

Andrews, G., Pine, D. S., Hobbs, M. J., Anderson, T. M., & Sunderland, M. (2009).

Neurodevelopmental disorders: Cluster 2 of the proposed meta-structure for DSM-V and

ICD-11. Psychological Medicine, 39(12), 2013-2023.

Berry, M. F. (1969). Language Disorders of Children: The bases and diagnoses. NY: Appleton-

Century-Crofts.

Biemiller, A. & Boote, C. (2006) An effective method for building vocabulary in primary grades.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 44-62. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.44

Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained

language problems. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,

49(4), 381-397.
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 36

Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1995). Genetic basis of specific language

impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,

37, 56–71.

Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (1995). Conversational intervention with children with specific

language impairment. In M. E. Fey, J. Windsor, S. F. Warren (Eds.), Language

intervention: Preschool through the elementary years. (pp. 183–212). Baltimore, MD,

US: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

Catts, H. W., Bridges, M. S., Little, T. D., & Tomblin, J. B. (2008). Reading Achievement

Growth in Children with Language Impairments. Journal of Speech, Language &

Hearing Research, 51(6), 1569-1579.

Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Weismer, S. E., & Bridges, M. S. (2014). The relationship between

language and reading abilities. In Tomblin, J. B. & Nippold, M. A. (Eds.), Understanding

individual differences in language development across the school years (pp. 144–165).

New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Cirrin, F. M., Schooling, T. L., Nelson, N. W., Diehl, S. F., Flynn, P. F., Staskowski, M., Torrey,

T. Z., Adamczyk, D. F. (2010). Evidence-based systematic review: Effects of different

service delivery models on communication outcomes for elementary school–age children.

Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 41(3), 233-264. doi:10.1044/0161-

1461(2009/08-0128)

Clahsen, H. (1989). The grammatical characterization of developmental dysphasia. Linguistics,

27, 897-920.
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 37

Clegg, J., Hollis, C., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2005). Developmental language disorders - a

follow-up in later adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes. Journal of

Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 46(2), 128-149.

Clements, S. D. (1966). Minimal Brain Dysfunction in Children NINDB monograph No. 3.

Washington DC: US Department of Health, Education and Welfare.

Costanza-Smith, A. (2010). The clinical utility of language samples. Perspectives on Language

Learning and Education, 17(1), 9. doi: 10.1044/lle17.1.9 Dynamic Assessment. (n.d.).

Retrieved January 28, 2016, from

http://www.asha.org/practice/multicultural/issues/Dynamic-Assessment.htm

Eigsti, I.-M., de Marchena, A. B., Schuh, J. M., & Kelley, E. (2011). Language acquisition in

autism spectrum disorders: A developmental review. Research in Autism Spectrum

Disorders, 5(2), 681–691. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.09.001

Eisenson, J. (1966). Perceptual disturbances in children with central nervous system disfunctions

and implications for language development. British Journal of Disorders of

Communications, 1(1), 21-32.

Eisenson, J. (1968). Developmental Aphasia: A speculative view with therapeutic implications.

Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 33(1), 3 - 13.

Ellawadi, A. B., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2015). Using Spoken Language Benchmarks to

Characterize the Expressive Language Skills of Young Children with Autism Spectrum

Disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 24(4), 696.

http://doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-14-0190
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 38

Ellis Weismer, S., & Kover, S. T. (2015). Preschool language variation, growth, and predictors

in children on the autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(12),

1327–1337. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12406

Ellis Weismer, S., Plante, E., Jones, M., & Tomblin, J.B. (2005). A functional magnetic

resonance imaging investigation of verbal working memory in adolescents with specific

language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 405-425.

Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007).

MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical

Manual - Second Edition. Baltimore: Brookes Publishing.

Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and

Written Story Composition Skills of Children with Language Impairment. Journal of

Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1301-1318.

Fey, M. E., Long, S. H., & Finestack, L. H. (2003). Ten principles of grammar facilitation for

children with specific language impairments. American Journal of Speech-Language

Pathology, 12(1), 3. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2003/048)

Flax, J. F., Realpe-Bonilla, T., Hirsch, L. S., Brzustowicz, L. M., Bartlett, C. W., & Tallal, P.

(2003). Specific Language Impairment in Families: Evidence for Co-Occurrence with

Reading Impairments. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 46(3), 530-543. doi: 10.1044/1092-

4388(2003/043).

Friberg, J. C. (2010). Considerations for test selection: How do validity and reliability impact

diagnostic decisions? Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 26(1), 77–92.

doi:10.1177/0265659009349972
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 39

Fonteneau, E., & van der Lely, H. (2008) Electrical brain responses in language-impaired

children reveal grammar-specific deficits. PLoS ONE, 3(3).

Gauger, L. M., Lombardino, L. J., & Leonard, C. M. (1997). Brain Morphology in Children with

Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research,

40(6), 1272-1284. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4006.1272.

Gerber, S., Brice, A., Capone, N., Fujiki, M., & Timler, G. (2012). Language use in social

interactions of school-age children with language impairments: An evidence-based

systematic review of treatment. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools,

43(2), 235-249. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0047)

Gillam, R. B., Cowan, N., & Day, L. S. (1995). Sequential memory in children with and without

language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(2), 393-402. doi:

10.1044/jshr.3802.393.

Gillam, R. B., & Johnston, J. (1992). Spoken and written language relationships in

language/learning impaired and normally achieving school-age children. Journal of

Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1303-1315.

Gillam, R.B. & Pearson, N.L. (2004). Test of Narrative Language. Austin, Pro-Ed.

Gillam, R. B., Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., Bohman, T. M., & Mendez-Perez, A. (2013).

Identification of specific language impairment in bilingual children: I. Assessment in

English. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 56(6), 1813-1823. doi:

10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0056)

Gillam, R.B., Wan, N.J., Gillam, S.L., & Hancock, A.S. (2015). Neural activation in children

with SLI during complex sentence comprehension. Poster presented at the annual

meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, San Francisco, California, April.


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 40

Gillam, S. (2007). Intervention for text comprehension deficits. In A. G. Kamhi, J. J.

Masterson, & K. Apel (Eds.), Clinical decision making in developmental language

disorders (pp. 267-284). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Gillam, S., Fargo, J. D., & St. Clair Robertson, K. (2009). Comprehension of expository text:

Insights gained from think-aloud data. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology,

18(1), 82-94. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/07-0074)

Gillam, S. L., Gillam, R. B., & Reece, K. (2012). Language outcomes of contextualized and

decontextualized language intervention: Results of an early efficacy study. Language

Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 43(3), 276-291. doi:10.1044/0161-

1461(2011/11-0022)

Gillam, S. L., Olszewski, A., Fargo, J., & Gillam, R. B. (2014). Classroom-based narrative and

vocabulary instruction: Results of an early-stage, nonrandomized comparison study.

Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 45(3), 204.

doi:10.1044/2014_LSHSS-13-0008

Gillon, G. T. (2005). Epilogue: Phonological Awareness: Effecting Change Through the

Integration of Research Findings. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools,

36(4), 346.

Goldstein, H., & Olszewski, A. (2015). Developing a phonological awareness curriculum:

Reflections on an implementation science framework. Journal of Speech Language and

Hearing Research, 58(6), S1837-S1850. doi:10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0351

Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F., & Pena, E. (2001). Dynamic assessment of diverse children: A tutorial.

Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32(4), 212–224.


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 41

Hadley, P. A., Simmerman, A., Long, M., & Luna, M. (2000). Facilitating language development

for inner-city children: Experimental evaluation of a collaborative, classroom-based

intervention. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 31(3), 280-295.

doi:10.1044/0161-1461.3103.280

Hammill, D. D. & Newcomer, P. L. (2008). Test of language Development-Intermediate (4th

ed.). Austin, TX: ProEd

Hammill, D. D. & Newcomer, P. L. (2008). Test of language Development-Primary (4th ed.).

Austin, TX: ProEd

Haynes, W. O. & Pindzola, R. H. (2008). Diagnosis and evaluation in speech pathology (7th ed.).

Boston, MA: Pearson

Jernigan, T. L., Hesselink, J. R., Sowell, E., & Tallal, P. A. (1991). Cerebral structure on

magnetic resonance imaging in language and learning-impaired children. Archives of

Neurology, 48, 539–545.

Johnson, C. J., Beitchman, J. H., & Brownlie, E. B. (2010). Twenty-year follow-up of children

with and without speech-language impairments: Family, educational, occupational, and

quality of life outcomes. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(1), 51–65.

Johnson, C. J., Beitchman, J. H., Young, A., Escobar, M., Atkinson, L., Wilson, B., & Lam, I.

(1999). Fourteen-year follow-up of children with and without speech/language

impairments: Speech/language stability and outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and

Hearing Research, 42, 744–760.

Kail, R. (1994). A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with

specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,

37(2), 418–421.
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 42

Kamhi, A. G., & Catts, H. W. (1999). Language and reading: Convergence and divergence. In H.

W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (pp. 1-24). Needham

Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Karasinski, C., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2010). Comprehension of inferences in discourse processing

by adolescents with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and

Hearing Research, 53(5), 1268-1279. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0006)

King, T. M., Rosenberg, L. A., Fuddy, L., McFarlane, E., Sia, C., & Duggan, A. K. (2005).

Prevalence and early identification of language delays among at-risk three year olds.

Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 26(4), 293-303.

Lai, C. S., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-

domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523.

Laing, S. P., & Kamhi, A. G. (2002). The use of think-aloud protocols to compare inferencing

abilities in average and below-average readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(5),

436–47. doi: 10.1177/00222194020350050401

Laing, S. P., & Kamhi, A. (2003). Alternative assessment of language and literacy in culturally

and linguistically diverse populations. Language Speech and Hearing Services in

Schools, 34(1), 44. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2003/005)

Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2000). Prevalence and natural history of

primary speech and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature.

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35(2), 165–188.

http://doi.org/10.1080/136828200247133
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 43

Laws, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in

Williams syndrome: A comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language

impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39(1), 45–

64. doi:10.1080/13682820310001615797

Leonard, L. (2014). Children with specific language impairment (2nd Edition). Cambridge,

MA, MIT Press.

Leonard, L. B., Weismer, S. E., Miller, C. A., Francis, D. J., Tomblin, J. B., & Kail, R. V.

(2007). Speed of Processing, Working Memory, and Language Impairment in Children.

Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 50(2), 21p. doi: 10.1044/1092-

4388(2007/029)

Lindsay, G., Dockrell, J. E., Nippold, M., & Fujiki, M. (2012). Longitudinal Patterns of

Behavioral, Emotional, and Social Difficulties and Self-Concepts in Adolescents with a

History of Specific Language Impairment. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in

Schools, 43(4), 445-460. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/11-0069)

Luo, F., & Timler, G. R. (2008). Narrative organization skills in children with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder and language impairment: Application of the causal network

model. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 22(1), 25–46. doi:10.1080/02699200701627430

Marshall, C. R., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). The impact of phonological complexity on past

tense inflection in children with Grammatical-SLI. Advances in Speech Language

Pathology, 9(3), 191–203. http://doi.org/10.1080/14417040701261509


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 44

McArthur, G., Atkinson, C., & Ellis, D. (2009). Atypical brain responses to sounds in children

with specific language and reading impairments. Developmental Science, 12(5), 768–783.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00804.x

McArthur, G. M., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Which people with specific language impairment

have auditory processing deficits? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21(1).

National Institutes of Health [NIH] Consensus Development Panel (2000). Diagnosis and

treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Journal of

Pharmaceutical Care in Pain & Symptom Control, 8(3), 75–89.

doi:10.1300/J088v08n03_09

Newborg, J. (2005). Battelle Developmental Inventory, Second Edition. Itasca, IL: Riverside

Publishing.

Nippold, M. A., Mansfield, T. C., Billow, J. L., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Syntactic development

in adolescents with a history of language impairments: A follow-up investigation.

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 18, 241-251.

Nippold, M. A., & Sun, L. (2008). Knowledge of morphologically complex words: A

developmental study of older children and young adolescents. Language, Speech, and

Hearing Services in Schools, 39(3), 365–373. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2008/034)

Paul, R., & Norbury, C. (2012). Language disorders from infancy through adolescence:

Listening, speaking, reading, writing, and communicating. St. Louis, Mo: Mosby.

Paul, R., & Roth, F. P. (2011). Characterizing and predicting outcomes of communication delays

in infants and toddlers: Implications for clinical practice. Language, Speech & Hearing

Services in Schools, 42(3), 331–340 doi:0161-1461(2010/09-0067)


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 45

Pena, E. D., & Gillam, R. B. (2000). Dynamic assessment of children referred for speech and

language evaluations. In C. Lidz (Ed), Dynamic assessment: Prevailing models and

applications Advances in cognition and educational practice, Vol 6 (pp. 543-575). New

York, NY: Elsevier Science.

Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., & Bedore, L. M. (2014). Dynamic Assessment of Narrative Ability in

English Accurately Identifies Language Impairment in English Language Learners.

Journal of Speech, Language, And Hearing Research: JSLHR.

Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., Bedore, L. M., & Bohman, T. M. (2011). Risk for Poor Performance

on a Language Screening Measure for Bilingual Preschoolers and Kindergarteners.

American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20(4), 302. doi:10.1044/1058-

0360(2011/10-0020)

Plante, E., Swisher, L., Vance, R., & Rapcsak, S. (1991). MRI findings in boys with specific

language impairment. Brain and Language, 41(1), 52–66. http://doi.org/10.1016/0093-

934X(91)90110-M

Polišenská, K., & Kapalková, S. (2014). Language profiles in children with Down Syndrome and

children with Language Impairment: Implications for early intervention. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 35(2), 373–382. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.11.022

Popescu, M., Fey, M. E., Lewine, J. D., Finestack, L. H., & Popescu, E.-A. (2009). N400

responses of children with primary language disorder: intervention effects. Neuroreport,

20(12), 1104–1108. http://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832e9c97

Rakhlin, N., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2015). (A)typical language development: Genetic and

environmental influences. In R. H. Bahr, E. R. Silliman, R. H. Bahr, & E. R. Silliman


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 46

(Eds.), Routledge handbook of communication disorders. (pp. 11-21). New York, NY,

US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.

Records, N. L., Tomblin, J. B., & Freesse, P. R. (1992). The quality of life of young adults with

histories of specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language

Pathology, 1, 44-53.

Redmond, S. M. (2005). Differentiating SLI from ADHD using children’s sentence recall and

production of past tense morphology. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 19(2), 109–127.

doi:10.1080/02699200410001669870

Redmond, S. M., Thompson, H. L., & Goldstein, S. (2011). Psycholinguistic profiling

differentiates specific language impairment from typical development and from

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing

Research, 54(1), 99. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0010)

Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of

Speech Language and Hearing Research, 45(2), 360-371. doi: 10.1044/1092-

4388(2002/028)

Rescorla, L. (2009). Age 17 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers: Support for

a dimensional perspective on language delay. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing

Research, 52(1), 16-30. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0171)

Rice, M. L. (2012). Toward epigenetic and gene regulation models of specific language

impairment: looking for links among growth, genes, and impairments. Journal of

Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 4(1), 1.


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 47

Rice, M. L., Smith, S. D., & Gayán, J. (2009). Convergent genetic linkage and associations to

language, speech and reading measures in families of probands with Specific Language

Impairment. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 1(4), 264–282.

http://doi.org/10.1007/s11689-009-9031-x

Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of

tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 11126-1145.

Rossetti, L. M (2006). The Rossetti Infant-Toddler Language Scale. East Moline, IL:

LinguiSystems.

Shafer, V. L., Schwartz, R. G., Morr, M. L., Kessler, K. L., & Kurtzberg, D. (2000). Deviant

neurophysiological asymmetry in children with language impairment. NeuroReport,

11(17).

Spaulding, T. J., Plante, E., & Farinella, K. A. (2006). Eligibility criteria for language

impairment: is the low end of normal always appropriate? Language, Speech and

Hearing Services in Schools, 37(1), 61-72.

Stothard, S. E., Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Chipchase, B. B., & Kaplan, C. A. (1998).

Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 407–418.

Swineford, L. B., Thurm, A., Baird, G., Wetherby, A. M., & Swedo, S. (2014). Social

(pragmatic) communication disorder: A research review of this new DSM-5 diagnostic

category. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 6(1), 41. doi:10.1186/1866-1955-6-

41
Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 48

Tomblin, J. B. & Buckwalter, P. R. (1998). Heritability of Poor Language Achievement Among

Twins. J Speech Lang Hear Res, 41(1), 188-199. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4101.188.

Tomblin, J. B., & Nippold, M. A. (2014). Features of language impairment in the school years.

In J. B. Tomblin, M. A. Nippold, J. B. Tomblin, & M. A. Nippold (Eds.), Understanding

individual differences in language development across the school years. (pp. 79-116).

New York, NY, US: Psychology Press.

Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997).

Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245

Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Buckwalter, P., & O'Brien, M. (2003). The stability of primary

language disorder: Four years after kindergarten diagnosis. Journal of Speech, Language

& Hearing Research, 46, 1283-1296.

van Kleeck, A., Gillam, R. B., & McFadden, T. U. (1998). A study of classroom-based

phonological awareness training for preschoolers with speech and/or Language

Disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7(3), 65.

doi:10.1044/1058-0360.0703.65

Vernes, S. C., Newbury, D. F., Abrahams, B. S., Winchester, L., Nicod, J., Groszer, M., …

Fisher, S. E. (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language

disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359(22), 2337–2345.

Weber-Fox, C., Leonard, L. B., Hampton, A., & Tomblin, B. J. (2010). Electrophysiological

correlates of rapid auditory and linguistic processing in adolescents with specific

language impairment. Brain and Language, 115, 162-181.


Running Head: LANGUAGE DISORDER 49

Westby, C., Burda, A., & Mehta, Z. (2003). Asking the right questions in the right ways:

Strategies for ethnographic interviewing. The ASHA Leader, 8(8), 4–17. doi:

org/10.1044/leader.FTR3.08082003.4

What Is Language? What Is Speech? (n.d.). Retrieved January 29, 2016, from

http://www.asha.org/public/speech/development/language_speech/

Wiig, E. H., Semel, E., & Secord, W. A. (2013). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals –
version 5. Bloomington, MN: Pearson.
Woods, P. L., Rieger, I., Wocadlo, C., & Gordon, A. (2014). Predicting the outcome of specific

language impairment at five years of age through early developmental assessment in

preterm infants. Early Human Development, 90(10), 613-619. doi:

10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.07.010

World Health Organization (2010). Framework for Action on Interprofessional Education &

Collaborative Practice. Geneva: WHO Press

View publication stats

You might also like