You are on page 1of 15

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012

doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x

CRITICAL REVIEW AND INVITED COMMENTARY

Music and epilepsy: A critical review


Melissa Jane Maguire

Department of Neurology, Leeds General Infirmary, Leeds, United Kingdom

minergic system modulation and the ambivalent action of


SUMMARY
cognitive and sensory input in ictogenesis may provide
The effect of music on patients with epileptic seizures is possible theories for the dichotomous proconvulsant and
complex and at present poorly understood. Clinical stud- anticonvulsant role of music in epilepsy. The effect of
ies suggest that the processing of music within the human antiepileptic drugs and surgery on musicality should not
brain involves numerous cortical areas, extending beyond be underestimated. Altered pitch perception in relation
Heschl’s gyrus and working within connected networks. to carbamazepine is rare, but health care professionals
These networks could be recruited during a seizure mani- should discuss this risk or consider alternative medication
festing as musical phenomena. Similarly, if certain areas particularly if the patient is a professional musician or
within the network are hyperexcitable, then there is a native-born Japanese. Studies observing the effect of epi-
potential that particular sounds or certain music could act lepsy surgery on musicality suggest a risk with right tem-
as epileptogenic triggers. This occurs in the case of musi- poral lobectomy, although the extent of this risk and
cogenic epilepsy, whereby seizures are triggered by correlation to size and area of resection need further
music. Although it appears that this condition is rare, the delineation. This potential risk may bring into question
exact prevalence is unknown, as often patients do not whether tests on musical perception and memory should
implicate music as an epileptogenic trigger and routine form part of the preoperative neuropsychological workup
electroencephalography does not use sound in seizure for patients embarking on surgery, particularly that of the
provocation. Music therapy for refractory epilepsy right temporal lobe.
remains controversial, and further research is needed to KEY WORDS: Musicogenic, Therapy, Carbamazepine,
explore the potential anticonvulsant role of music. Dopa- Temporal lobectomy.

Music is an integral part of everyday life and culture. For epilepsy may have the potential to alter a patient’s musical-
most people, listening or playing music is a pleasurable ity, which could have disastrous consequences.
experience that may evoke a memory or emotion. Advances This review aims to summarize the current body of evi-
in technology have improved our access to all kinds of dence on music and its association with epilepsy. It outlines
music, with many people now downloading and sharing our current understanding of musical processing in the
digitized music files and using portable devices to play and human brain, and how this process could be disrupted in the
store large collections of music. production and propagation of seizures. Also discussed are
Although the environmental link between photosensitivity ictal musical phenomena, music therapy in refractory epi-
and epilepsy is well known, the interaction between music lepsy, and the effect of antiepileptic medication and epi-
and epilepsy is less well perceived or understood. It appears lepsy surgery on musicality.
some patients with seizures might gain benefit from musical
exposure, whereas other patients may experience an exacer-
bation of their seizures. The dichotomous effect of music on
Musical Processing and the
epilepsy is an intriguing yet poorly understood phenomenon Human Brain
and the subject of ongoing research and debate. In addition, The relation between music and the brain has been exten-
the effects of medications and surgical procedures used in sively researched over the past century, from the authorita-
tive work published by Critchley and Henson (1977) titled
‘‘Music and the Brain’’ to a modern update of the literature
Accepted April 3, 2012; Early View publication March 21, 2012. from intervening decades by Stewart et al. (2006).
Address correspondence to Melissa Maguire, Consultant Neurologist,
Leeds General Infirmary, Great George Street, Leeds, West Yorkshire LS1
During the late 19th century, German researchers pub-
3EX, U.K. E-mail: maguirem@doctors.org.uk lished numerous studies analyzing the disturbance of musical
Wiley Periodicals, Inc. functioning in patients with brain damage, observing how
ª 2012 International League Against Epilepsy focal lesions affect musical activities. In 1888, Knoblauch

947
948
M. J. Maguire

introduced the term ‘‘amusia’’ meaning impaired musical Musical recognition and emotion are thought to involve
capabilities. He described a sensory (receptive) amusia orbitofrontal areas and the limbic system, which may
whereby affected patients cannot hear, read, or understand serve to store past auditory memory and emotional evalu-
music, and motor (expressive) amusia whereby patients ation of a musical stimulus (Dellacherie et al., 2009). For
have an inability to sing, write, or play music (Knoblauch, example, chord dissonance per se may be detected via the
1888). Several other authors explored lesional conse- auditory association cortex, but the evoked unpleasant
quences on music perception (Head, 1926; Kleist, 1962; emotional response appears to be mediated via limbic
Luria et al., 1965), and a plethora of interesting discoveries structures.
were made. Although many cases of amusia were related to An extensive review by Stewart et al. (2006)) analyzed
an abnormality within the right temporal lobe, this was by 38 case reports and 27 case series reporting on symptom-
no means true in all reported cases and some lesions lateral- atic musical listening deficits, with associated structural
ized to the left hemisphere. Thalamic lesions in association imaging findings. The authors summarized anatomic areas
with a hemiparesis were reported to cause a peculiar alter- that are pertinent to pitch processing, temporal processing,
ation in musical perception on the affected side. One patient musical memory, and emotional responses to music using
seemed unable to tolerate hymns in church on his affected five cartoons. Anatomic areas implicated in 50% or more
side, causing him to rub the affected hand (Head, 1920). of the studies for a particular function were mapped out
Although these studies provided some evidence on the ana- using colored dots, with the size of the dot representing
tomic components of musical perception, the cases were the proportion of studies (Fig. 1). The review ascertained
confined to patients with damaged brains, and it is possible that the anatomic areas implicated in musical listening
that the underlying pathologic process may have affected deficits were central, affecting a number of regions
the results. beyond Heschl’s gyrus, with right-sided predominance.
Our modern understanding of the mechanisms involved Half of cases were associated with coexisting problems of
in musical processing has been aided by advances in func- speech perception.
tional imaging studies, using positron emission tomography Determinants of musicality have also been explored over
(PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) the years by examining environmental and genetic factors.
techniques. These techniques display the changes in hemo- Musicality is likely to be a polygenic trait and dependent on
dynamic response to mean synaptic firing rates in the brain, home environment and parental attitudes to the develop-
and have allowed researchers to delineate the anatomic ment of musical talent in children.
areas pertinent to the processing of music. Electroencepha- Studies have also identified structural differences in the
lography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) stud- brains of musicians versus nonmusicians in a number of
ies have also been employed to record postsynaptic anatomic areas including auditory (Schneider et al.,
potentials during listening to music. Recent studies have 2002), motor (Amunts et al., 1997), somatosensory, supe-
also identified the structural and functional organization of rior parietal (Gaser & Schlaug, 2003), callosal (Schlaug
the brain in musicians. et al., 1995), and cerebellar (Hutchinson et al., 2003)
Musical processing encompasses brain mechanisms in areas. The larger size of the corpus callosum in musicians
musical perception, recognition, and emotion. Musical per- may indicate increased cross-communication between
ception requires the decoding of a musical stimulus within both sides of the brain, merging spatial-emotional process-
the primary auditory cortex in Heschl’s gyrus and the associ- ing of the right brain with linguistic analytical processing
ation cortex in the superior temporal gyrus (planum tempo- of the left brain. People with absolute pitch engage left
rale). The primary auditory cortex is thought to receive dorsofrontal regions, whereas those lacking absolute pitch
thalamic afferents from the medial geniculate nucleus, appear to have a so-called working memory for pitch
which in turn connect through networks to the association located within inferior frontal areas. The age of onset of
cortex, mesolimbic systems, and other multisensory cortices musical training, that is, younger than 4 years old, appears
(Stewart et al., 2006). The primary auditory cortex appears to be a key predictor of absolute pitch, suggesting that this
especially sensitive to tone, whereas the auditory associa- ability develops during a critical period for imprinting
tion cortex is thought to perceive pitch (Penagos et al., (Sergeant & Roche, 1973).
2004) and perform more complex musical processing tasks
relating to linear stimuli, for example, melodies (Liegeois-
Chauvel et al., 1998) and nonlinear stimuli, for example,
Music and Epilepsy
chords and consonances. Similar areas within the secondary It is possible that brain mechanisms involved in musical
auditory cortex are also activated in speech (Price et al., processing may be involved in the generation and propaga-
2005). The perception of rhythms, with no particular melo- tion of seizures, manifesting with musical semiology.
dic content, is thought to involve activation of the cerebel- Hyperexcitable cortical areas may also become sensitized
lum and basal ganglia as well as the superior temporal to specific musical triggers and may explain the basis of
lobes, suggesting a motor aspect to rhythm perception. musicogenic epilepsy.
Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012
doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
949
Music and Epilepsy

Figure 1.
Critical brain substrates for musical
listening disorders according to
pitch processing, temporal process-
ing, musical memory, and emotional
responses to music. The presence
of a colored circle corresponding to
a particular function in a region indi-
cates that at least 50% of studies of
the function implicate that region.
The size of each circle is scaled
according to the proportion of stud-
ies of the function implicating that
region. Meter is not represented, as
no brain area was implicated in 50%
or more of cases. amyg, amygdala;
aSTG, anterior superior-temporal
gyrus; bg, basal ganglia; cc, corpus
callosum; fr, frontal; hc, hippocam-
pal; HG, Heschl’s gyrus; ic, inferior
colliculi; i, inferior; ins, insula;
l, lateral; m, medial; thal, thalamus;
PT, planum temporale; TG, tempo-
ral gyrus. Reprinted from Stewart
et al. (2006) with permission from
Oxford Journals.
Epilepsia ILAE

Musicogenic epilepsy has an estimated prevalence of one


Musicogenic epilepsy
case per 10,000,000 population. It is classified as a rare form
Musicogenic epilepsy was first coined by Critchley,
of complex reflex epilepsy by the International League
1937, who described a rare form of epilepsy in which
Against Epilepsy (ILAE) (Berg et al., 2010), with seizures
seizures, typically simple or complex partial types, are
induced by listening to music in most cases, but playing,
triggered by music. However, descriptions of possible
thinking, or dreaming of music have all been cited. In
music-induced seizures had been reported in the literature
reported cases, seizures following a musical stimulus can
as early as 1841. The Chinese poet Jichin Kyo, quoted by
often be delayed by several minutes. During this latent per-
Fujinawa et al. (1977) stated:
iod, patients may experience distress, agitation, tachycardia,
Since my remote boyhood I have always been absent and rapid breathing building up to the seizure, although this
minded while hearing the sound of a street vendor’s flute. is inconsistently reported. The seizures may not all be exclu-
I fall sick when I hear the sound of the flute in the evening sively stimulated by music, and there is reported variability
sun, although I do not know the reason. in the form of musical stimulus. For example some patients

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
950
M. J. Maguire

report seizures according to type (jazz, classical, choral, eral temporal lobe, right mesial temporal lobe, and bilateral
popular), instrument (organ, flute, piano), emotional content temporal lobes (Tayah et al., 2006). A third study used flu-
of the music (sad, sentimental, upbeat), or even composer orodeoxyglucose (FDG)–PET with invasive techniques
(Wagner, Beethoven, Beatles). Some patients have also (subdural grid and depth electrodes) and identified a focus
found strategies that enable them to abort a seizure. These within the right mesial temporal lobe with propagation to
features have led one author to question whether musico- the lateral temporal cortex, Heschl’s gyrus, insula, and fron-
genic epilepsy is a true form of reflex epilepsy (Vizioli, tal lobes (Mehta et al., 2009).
1989). These clinical studies suggest an affective role in musico-
In many cases, however, the seizure has been reported to genic seizures, as evidenced by mesial temporal activity.
occur within seconds and following a very specific stimulus, Authors speculate that emotion triggered by music is the
for example church bells (Poskanzer et al., 1962) or the causal factor rather than the auditory content of the music
melody of the Marseillaise (Vercelletto, 1953). Other per se. Merely thinking about music has been reported as a
reports cite the incorrect positioning of the larynx leading to trigger, suggesting the importance of musical memory and
a husky or ‘‘metallic’’ quality to singing causing seizures emotion.
(Brien & Murray, 1984), whereas others cite cognitive pro- Responses to limbic stimulation in subjects with epilepsy
cessing (Ogunyemi & Breen, 1993), playing a certain hymn are thought to depend on widespread neuronal matrices
(Sutherling et al., 1980), or improvisation (Le Chevalier linked through connections that have become strengthened
et al., 1985) as triggering events. through repeated use (Gloor, 1990). This is of interest given
A recent review reported on 110 cases of music-evoked that the onset of seizures often predates the sensitivity to
seizures published between 1884 and 2007 (Pittau et al., music.
2008). The mean age of onset of musicogenic seizures was However, the imaging studies provide less obvious clues
28 years, with a female predominance. Patients more often as to why a subset of patients may develop seizures on expo-
than not had high musicality. Seizures induced exclusively sure to a simple sound or tone, where there may be a less
by music occurred in 34 patients, with the majority also obvious emotional component (Wieser et al., 1997). In
reporting spontaneous seizures. Autonomic auras were addition, musicogenic seizures have since been reported in
common with oroalimentary automatisms occurring in more children as young as 6 months old (Lin et al., 2003), sug-
than one fourth of cases. In 60 patients with ictal electroen- gesting musicogenic seizures are likely to be epileptiform
cephalography (EEG), the focus of activity localized to the and not functional in nature.
temporal lobe with right-sided predominance. Ictal single Treatment of musicogenic seizures usually comprises
photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) in six avoidance of the musical triggers together with antiepileptic
cases demonstrated abnormalities within the right temporal medication. In the past, various behavioral therapies have
lobe involving mesial structures in four and within the left been tried in patients with high emotional states. Joynt et al.
temporal lobe in two cases. The musical stimulus varied (1962) suggested the use of sensory extinction, by playing
greatly, with a small proportion identifying certain tones or innocuous music or small snatches of noxious music prior to
sounds causing seizures and others reporting that any form playing the seizure-producing music, the idea being that
of music triggered events. The review also reported an fMRI cells typically initiating seizure activity would be set into
study on a patient with typical music-induced seizures activity by similar combinations of tones and chords, ren-
played ‘‘neutral’’ music and ‘‘emotionally charged melody’’ dering them less susceptible to the actual seizure-triggering
music. It revealed that during the playing of neutral music, melody. Thereby, excitable nerve cells are ‘‘usurped’’ by a
only acoustic areas were highlighted, but during emotional competing stimulus. Other methods of psychotherapy and
melodies, the activity also involved frontooccipital areas. deconditioning techniques have been used as treatment in
This broadly correlates with findings by Mrocz et al. musicogenic epilepsy patients (Daly & Barry, 1957; Forster
(2003), who demonstrated early activation of the right et al., 1965).
frontoorbital area prior to seizure activity from the left tem- At present the exact pathophysiology of musicogenic sei-
poral lobe. However, Mrocz et al. (2003) suggested that zures is undetermined. Questions around what make a cer-
activity provided evidence of emotional arousal and mem- tain type or facet of music epileptogenic to a particular
ory related to the music rather than to seizure activity per se, person remain unanswered, but it is possible that hyperex-
whereas in the review, frontal cortical activity was thought citable cortical areas could be stimulated to different
to represent the beginning of paroxysmal discharges unde- degrees and extents by different musical stimuli. These
tected by scalp EEG. questions remain an intriguing subject of ongoing research.
Invasive monitoring of seizure propagation in musico-
genic seizures has been reported in three studies. Subdural Musical ictal phenomena
recordings from one patient revealed cortical dysplasia Music may manifest as part of ictal semiology in associa-
within the superior temporal gyrus (Trevathan et al., 1999). tion with temporal lobe epilepsy. The phenomena could
One study using a subdural grid showed foci in the right lat- be either positive, as in musical auditory hallucinations,
Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012
doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
951
Music and Epilepsy

musicophilia, ictal singing or whistling, or negative as in There have been other fascinating accounts of patients
ictal aprosody and amusia. reporting musicophilia and musical inspiration in associa-
tion with altered emotional perception: following electrocu-
Musical hallucinations tion by lightning and following surgery for a right temporal
Some patients may develop musical auditory hallucina- oligodendroglioma (Sacks, 2008). The underlying neural
tions as part of temporal lobe epilepsy. In comparison to mechanisms of musicophilia in these cases remain unex-
musicogenic epilepsy, there is a less clear association with plained.
high musicality, and the emotional associates of temporal
lobe attacks with musical components are varied including Ictal singing, humming, and whistling
anger, fear, and panic. In a cohort of 666 patients with tem- The act of singing involves multiple cortical areas includ-
poral lobe epilepsy, around 16% had auditory hallucinations ing left superior temporal and parietal regions as well as
that took the form of sounds such as ticking or banging or both left and right premotor cortex, anterior superior tempo-
more complex forms such as a specific melody or orchestral ral gyrus, and planum polare (Callan et al., 2006).
piece (Currie et al., 1971). These seizures were associated Singing as an ictal phenomena is rare. There have been
with activation of the superior temporal gyrus with right- nine cases of ictal singing reported in the literature (Enatsu
sided predominance. In addition, some patients who experi- et al., 2011). Four had frontal lobe epilepsy, three had tem-
enced voices rather than music as a feature of their seizures poral lobe epilepsy, and in two cases the epileptogenic zone
had stimulation within the same cortical regions as those could not be localized. There has also been variability as to
triggered by a musical stimulus. More recent research in whether the ictal focus emanates from a dominant or non-
subjects with musical hallucinations implicated an autono- dominant focus. In one case the authors were able to distin-
mous network of nonprimary auditory areas in generating guish different localizations depending on whether the
abnormal neural activity that sustains musical hallucina- patient was humming or singing. Humming appeared to
tions (Griffiths, 2000). occur due to a temporal focus, whereas singing occurred
due to a focus within the frontal lobe, specifically within the
Musicophilia right prefrontal cortex (Bartolomei et al., 2007).
Musicophilia or musical craving is an extremely rare phe- Authors have speculated that ictal singing is a form of
nomenon. It has been reported in a patient with a stroke automatism that occurs either from a release phenomenon,
causing damage to the left hemisphere (Jacome, 1984) and or by activation of learned motor patterns or memories
also in frontotemporal dementia (Boeve & Geda, 2001). (Meierkord & Shorvon, 1991). Because the action of sing-
Rohrer et al. (2006) described an interesting case of a ing involves multiple regions, it suggests that ictal singing
65-year-old woman with right-sided temporal lobe epilepsy may occur through recruitment of a propagation network
who developed musical craving following commencement mimicking musical action via numerous cortical areas,
of lamotrigine for complex partial seizures. Before intro- rather than it occurring via a specific cortical focus.
duction of treatment she had been indifferent to music, There have been five cases of ictal whistling reported
avoiding music where possible and never attending con- within the literature. (Lazzarino & Valassi, 1982; Tan et al.,
certs. She had no musical training. Within weeks of treat- 1990; Loring et al., 1994; Raghavendra et al., 2010). Later-
ment she was actively listening to classical music stations alization has not been consistent among cases. One report
for several hours a day, and demanding to attend musical suggested a frontal focus, whereas the remaining cases
concerts. She described listening to music as an intensely reported this rare automatism with temporal lobe epilepsy.
pleasurable and highly emotional experience. There were In two cases, ictal whistling was abolished by temporal lobe
no other changes to her behavior or personality. surgery. The act of whistling requires the utilization of
In this case the musicophilia was driven by the pleasur- perioral, oral, and respiratory muscles and the recruitment
able emotional response derived from the music. The of a complex neuronal network. Functional imaging has
authors speculated that emotional responses to music demonstrated multiple areas involved in whistling including
might have been altered by functional reorganization of inferior rolandic cortex, cingulate cortex, basal ganglia,
neocorticolimbic interactions as a consequence of long- amygdala, thalamus, and cerebellum (Dresel et al., 2005).
standing seizures. Treatment might enhance these altered
emotional responses by restoring flow within the reorga- Ictal aprosody and amusia
nized sensory-limbic networks. This theory relates to the Disorders of prosody refer to abnormalities in the affec-
described phenomenon of ‘‘forced normalization,’’ tive components of speech including intonation, melody,
whereby patients may develop behavioral changes, for pitch, and gestures. This abnormality is considered either
example depression in the context of improved seizure expressive or receptive and may occur due to lesions in the
control. This is thought to occur due to electrophysiologic nondominant hemisphere. Amusia, as described previously,
and neurochemical alterations involving limbic circuitry and aprosody are considered to be negative phenomena
(Krishnamoorthy et al., 2002). often occurring as a result of stroke. However, there has
Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012
doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
952
M. J. Maguire

been one reported case of expressive amusia and aprosody included 58 Taiwanese children with partial epilepsy.
manifesting as an ictal feature of seizures emanating from a Continuous EEG monitoring occurred before, during, and
right temporooccipital lobe focus (Bautista & Ciampetti, after eight minutes of exposure to the Sonata. In 81% of
2003). On commencing antiepileptic treatment, the clinical patients there was a reduction in interictal discharges by an
events ceased and the patient’s musical capabilities were average of 33%, with the greatest reduction in those patients
restored. Negative ictal phenomena are poorly understood with generalized discharges. However, around 20% showed
and can include ictal blindness, paralysis, speech arrest, and an increase in interictal discharges by an average of 14%.
loss of hearing. They are thought to occur either via stimula- No significant differences were found according to gender,
tion of inhibitory cortical regions, dampening of receptive IQ, or number of antiepileptic drugs and response to music.
abilities of sensory regions, or inhibition of spinal motor The reduction in interictal discharges was not dependent on
neurons. the level of alertness or specific emotional response (Lin
et al., 2010). The second study included 11 Taiwanese
children aged 2–14 years old with refractory epilepsy. Two-
Music Therapy for Epilepsy thirds had generalized seizures of symptomatic causes, and
The therapeutic potential of music has largely been the majority (70%) had learning difficulties. Seizure fre-
explored in cognitive science. The cognitive effects of quency was observed for 6 months before music and during
music are well documented in the literature, although these 6 months of Mozart K448 exposure. The study found that
effects have been subject to scrutiny. Rauscher et al. (1993) 73% of patients had a 50% or greater reduction in their sei-
observed an immediate enhancement in spatial-temporal zure frequency, with two patients becoming seizure free
reasoning in college students exposed to 10 minutes of the during exposure to music. There were no significant
Mozart Sonata K448 (Rauscher et al., 1993). The authors differences in response status according to seizure type, IQ,
subsequently coined the term the ‘‘Mozart effect.’’ Further etiology, or gender (Lin et al., 2011).
studies in Parkinson’s disease, senile dementia, and atten- There have also been reports of a reduction in interictal
tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder also described cognitive discharges when exposed to Mozart K448 in patients with
benefits from listening to music (Pacchetti et al., 2000; rolandic types of seizures (Turner, 2004). Reductions in
Foster & Valentine, 2001; Rickson & Watkins, 2003; ictal spiking have been reported in subjects in coma and
Turner, 2004). refractory nonconvulsive status following exposure to
Rauscher et al. went on to devise an animal model of the Mozart Sonata K448 (Hughes et al., 1998; Lahiri &
‘‘Mozart effect’’ and demonstrated that rats exposed to long- Duncan, 2007; Kuester et al., 2010), and Johann Sebastian
term Mozart in utero and also postpartum performed better Bach (Miranda et al., 2010). Authors suggest a direct corti-
in negotiating a T maze than control rats that had been cal response to Mozart as opposed to responses relying on
exposed to silence, white noise, or the music of Philip Glass. emotion or level of awareness.
The effect lasted for at least 4 h after months of exposure Neuroscientists have also published on possible theories
(Rauscher et al., 1998). Further studies in mice showed sim- for the anticonvulsant effect of music. Hughes et al. (1998)
ilar enhanced maze skills for up to 24 h having been discussed the Trion model and that exposure to a highly pat-
exposed to 12 h of the Mozart sonata but not with mice terned or superorganized stimulus (in space and time) may
exposed to Beethoven (Aoun et al., 2005). Animal studies lead to learning of innate memory patterns, which in turn
have also shown that music exposure may enhance dendritic may decrease the excitability of an epileptogenic focus.
branching, cell proliferation, and neurogenesis in the hippo- Other authors have implicated mirror neurons in mediat-
campus and amygdala (Kim et al., 2006). ing the anticonvulsant effect. Mirror neurons discharge or
The reported musical effects on cognition have been are modified when an individual is performing an action
viewed with scepticism among neuroscientists. Concerns while exposed to musical or visual stimulation (Molnar-
have arisen from the lack of matched musical controls Szakacs & Overy, 2006). Musical activities, such as dancing
within experiments. It could be argued that Hayden is a to music, playing musical instruments, and moving mouths
better musical match to Mozart in terms of structure than and larynxes to sing are examples in which music and motor
Beethoven or Glass. In addition, the short duration of effect functioning are connected. Authors speculate that mirror
and difficulty in controlling for affective responses to music neurons mediate neuronal activity by linking auditory stim-
in experiments has attracted criticism. ulation directly to the motor cortex. Motor system modula-
The evidence for music as an anticonvulsant in human tion in transcranial magnetic stimulation and in behavioral
epilepsy is limited as is our understanding of the brain mech- studies has been observed to change during auditory stimu-
anisms involved. The clinical evidence is confined mainly lation (Buccino et al., 2005).
to Mozart music and in Taiwanese children with generalized A further theory proposes modification of the dopamine
seizures. Two small studies by the same author reported on neurotransmitter pathways in the effect of Mozart K.448 on
the effect of Mozart K448 on epileptiform discharges and epilepsy. Exposure to music is known to increase the
seizure frequencies in Taiwanese children. The first study expression of dopamine in the brain (Sutoo & Akiyama,
Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012
doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
953
Music and Epilepsy

2004). In recent years, dopamine has been reported to have et al., 2000). Playing music may promote dopamine
an important role in the pathophysiology of epilepsy. The release thereby flooding the dopaminergic systems and
reduced binding capacity of dopamine receptors in the basal upregulating D2 receptors. In patients with temporal lobe
ganglia has been hypothesized to contribute to seizures epilepsy, dopamine flooding may potentially behave in an
in autosomal dominant frontal lobe epilepsy (ADNFLE) anticonvulsant way through D2 receptor activation. In
(Fedi et al., 2008) and juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME) musicogenic epilepsy, it is possible that the emotional
(Landvogt et al., 2010). An FDG-PET study in seven effect of the musical trigger could lead to increased dopa-
patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy identified mine within the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Kaneyuki
reduced D2/D3 receptor binding (Werhahn et al., 2006). In et al., 1991). This increase in dopaminergic activity within
a study in animals, pilocarpine-induced seizures altered the the PFC could in turn suppress the limbic dopaminergic
binding capacity of dopaminergic receptors in striatal and response and result in the propagation of seizures. The
hippocampal areas, and facilitated the propagation and limbic-PFC circuit has also been implicated in an animal
maintenance of seizures (Mendes de Freitas et al., 2005). It model of anxiety (Bishop, 2007). This is interesting given
is possible that listening to music modifies dopaminergic that anxiety correlates with a lack of seizure control in
pathways within subcortical structures to influence tha- JME, and a risk of >20 lifetime generalized tonic–clonic
lamocortical projections. seizures (De Araujo Filho et al., 2006). Further research is
In summary, further research is needed to explore the needed into the role of dopamine pathways in epilepsy
potential anticonvulsant role of music both at the basic sci- and the interaction with music.
ence and clinical level. The research to date has focused Another interesting area of research is the ambivalent
principally on Mozart music without a good understanding action of sensory and cognitive input in ictogenesis. This is
of the basic science behind the effect. There is limited evi- relevant to understanding the effect of music in epilepsy
dence of an effect with Mozart on seizures in the clinical set- given that limbic and multisensory cortices are involved in
ting. It may be that some aspects of musical stimuli will musical processing.
ultimately prove to have therapeutic potential, but for the A study by Guaranha et al. (2009) observed the effect
moment these remain largely unknown and unproven and of cognitive tasks on epileptiform discharges in 76
our understanding of the brain mechanisms involved are patients, aged 12–53 years old with JME. The study used
limited. a video-EEG protocol recording brain activity during
action programming tasks, for example, spatial construc-
Mechanisms for the Proconvulsant tion, and thinking tasks, for example, mental calculation.
and Anticonvulsant Effects of The recording was performed over 4–6 h, and EEG dis-
charge rates during specific tasks were compared to a
Music in Epilepsy baseline awake EEG recording. The study observed a pro-
To evaluate the therapeutic potential of music we need vocative effect on discharges during cognitive tasks in 29
to try and understand why music might behave as anticon- patients and an inhibitory effect in 28 patients. In 30
vulsant in some patients with epilepsy yet proconvulsant patients there was no effect on discharges during cogni-
in others. Understanding why this paradox occurs may be tive tasks. Action programming tasks, particularly manual
explained through the dichotomous effect of dopamine on praxis, were the most provocative, whereas thinking tasks,
receptors in the brain. Previous studies have shown that in particular mental calculation, were the most inhibitory.
the anticonvulsant action of dopamine has been attributed The effect of cognitive tasks on epileptiform discharges
to D2 receptor stimulation in the forebrain, whereas selec- appeared independent of drug treatment, seizure control,
tive D1 receptor activation appears to lower the seizure or age of patient. These findings broadly correlate with
threshold both clinically and in animal models (Al-Tajir & other studies (Matsuoka et al., 2002; Matsuoka et al.,
Starr, 1990; Starr, 1996). Studies in mesial temporal lobe 2005). The authors speculated that cognitive tasks recruit-
epilepsy have identified reduced D2/D3 binding capacity ing motor pathways were more epileptogenic, whereas
in the striatum. This may be a consequence of receptor spatial thinking involving activation of parietal regions
downregulation, due to ongoing seizures (Starr, 1996). without motor recruitment may act to dampen activity in
Striatal dopamine receptor downregulation might disin- neighboring motor cortices and thus seizure activity.
hibit thalamocortical connections by means of inhibition Music may act as anticonvulsant by activating and
of the substantia nigra, thereby further enhancing cortical enhancing areas of cortex involved in spatial cognitive
hyperexcitability. This has been explored in animal mod- processes that produce greater inhibition, either directly
els of absence epilepsy (Deransart et al., 2000) A further on surrounding motor areas or via inhibitory corticotha-
study has ascertained that signaling through D2 receptors lamic feedback loops. Similarly music may act as procon-
could play a neuroprotective role against pathologies vulsant in musicogenic epilepsy where anticipation of
involving glutamate-induced neurodegeneration (Bozzi music and memory responses may enhance activity in the

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
954
M. J. Maguire

PFC and associated frontostriatal connections (Leaver Studies investigating the effect of surgery on musical
et al., 2009). emotion report conflicting results. Some studies report
impaired recognition of scary music following anterome-
The Effect of Antiepileptic dial temporal lobe resection (Khalfa et al., 2008; Gosselin
et al., 2011), whereas another study found that patients
Therapies on Musicality who underwent unilateral medial temporal lobe surgery
Music exposure may thus influence seizures, but the were able to judge emotional dissimilarities in music as
treatment of seizures may affect musicality. This could have well as healthy participants (Dellacherie et al., 2011).
potentially disastrous effects, particularly if the patient These discrepancies may relate to the amount of surgical
relies on his or her musical skills for employment. resection, the methodologic differences in assessing emo-
tional responses, or the cognitive processes underlying the
Antiepileptic drugs emotional responses.
Carbamazepine has been associated with a reversible Studies examining musical recognition broadly agree
disturbance of pitch perception. Since 1993, there have and report impairment in recognizing previously pre-
been 26 reported cases of this adverse effect reported in sented tunes following temporal lobe surgery but with a
the literature (Tateno et al., 2006). Of interest is that all right-sided predominance (Shankweiler, 1966; Samson &
but one case have involved Japanese patients. Patients Zatorre, 1991, 1992). The outcomes suggest a bitemporal
were aged between 4 and 42 years old with a mean age of role in musical recognition and possibly for emotion as
19 years and most were female. Twenty of 26 patients well.
were prescribed carbamazepine for epilepsy. Symptoms of Studies examining pitch processing report impairments
altered pitch perception seem to develop 2 h to 2 weeks in pitch direction, pitch pattern, timbre, and tonal structure
after the administration of carbamazepine. On reduction in patients who have undergone right temporal lobectomy
in dose or discontinuation of carbamazepine, the abnor- including Heschl’s gyrus (Milner, 1962; Zatorre, 1985;
mality appeared to fully reverse. Patients perceived a Samson & Zatorre, 1988; Zatorre, 1988; Zatorre & Samson,
lower pitch than usual and in the majority of cases by a 1991; Kester et al., 1991; Zatorre & Halpern, 1993; Samson
semitone. Many of the patients were musical and had & Zatorre, 1994; Johnsrude et al., 2000; Warrier & Zatorre,
studied an instrument. 2004). Similarly, studies exploring the effect on rhythm and
The mechanism of pitch disturbance with carbamaze- time intervals show impairment in patients who have under-
pine is unknown. Authors speculate that carbamazepine gone right temporal lobectomy, including the superior tem-
acts at a local level, either by changing the mechanics of poral gyrus (Milner, 1962; Liegeois-Chauvel et al., 1998).
the organ of Corti (Chaloupka et al., 1994), or by affect- It appears that patients undergoing right temporal lobec-
ing the sarcolemma of the stapedius muscle and altering tomy are more at risk than those undergoing left temporal
the tension on the tympanic membrane receiving sound. lobectomy in acquiring postoperative problems with musi-
Alternatively carbamazepine may act at a central level. cal perception.
Carbamazepine-treated patients have been found to have
prolonged I–III and I–V latencies of the auditory brain-
stem response (Medaglini et al., 1988). However, auditory Preoperative and Postoperative
brainstem responses in patients with abnormal pitch per- Evaluation of Musical Functioning
ception have been reported to be normal. Acoustic effects in Patients with Epilepsy
of lamotrigine in pediatric patients with epilepsy have
been assessed in a recent study. The authors examined Published studies suggest a bitemporal role for musical
speaking rate, pitch, and articulation rate, finding no evi- recognition and possibly emotion, but a right temporal role
dence of adverse effect with lamotrigine (Yun et al., for musical perception. The potential risks to musical pro-
2011).There have not been any reports of studies on other cessing should be discussed with patients, particularly those
antiepileptic drugs and effect on musicality. undergoing right temporal lobectomy. It would be interest-
ing to ascertain current practices in discussing musical
Temporal lobe surgery implications in neurosurgical clinics.
The effect of temporal lobe surgery for intractable epi- Preoperative neuropsychological evaluation of musical
lepsy on musical processing is an important issue and the processing should also be considered in temporal lobe resec-
consequences for patients could be devastating. Tables 1 tive patients. There have been many tests devised over the
and 2 highlight published studies examining the effect of years, the detail of which is beyond the scope of this review.
temporal lobe surgery on musical emotion, recognition, and However, two internationally recognized assessment tools
perception. Most of these studies have involved small num- are mentioned briefly here.
bers of surgical patients, and the methods used for assessing The Seashore Measures of Musical Talent was a musical
musical processing have varied considerably. aptitude test developed in 1919 and modified in 1960 to

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
Table 1. Summary of studies investigating the effect of temporal lobe surgery for refractory epilepsy on musical emotion and recognition
Author and year Resection No Age, years (gender) Musical education Test Key finding
Musical emotion
Dellacherie et al. (2011) Controls (C) 12 36 (m) All groups had limited or no Multidimensional scaling No difference in ability to assess
Medial temporal, sparing STG (L) 10 38 (m) formal musical training analysis on emotional emotional dissimilarity in
Medial temporal, sparing STG (R) 9 37 (m) dissimilarity resection groups versus (C)
Gosselin (2005) Controls (C) 16 27–47 Patient group: None Emotional task Impaired rating of fearful music in
Medial temporal, sparing STG (L) 8 20–60 Control group: Some 5 years Error detection task both patient groups. Ability to
Medial temporal, sparing STG (R) 8 20–60 training recognize happy and sad music
not impaired
Khalfa et al. (2008) Controls (C) 60 32 (m) 30% control group and Musical excerpts Impaired sadness recognition and
Anterior temporal including STG 12 29 (m) 50–60% of patient groups from Montreal Battery of dissonance in both resection
(L) with 5 years musical training Evaluation of Amusia groups versus (C)
Anterior temporal including STG 14 34 (m) Impaired happiness recognition in
(R) (L) versus (R) and (C)
Musical recognition
Samson and Zatorre (1991) Controls (C) 20 31 (m) 30% of each group with Song excerpts Impaired recognition of
(Experiment 1) Anterior temporal (including HG) 22 27 (m) musical training (Serafine et al., 1984) previously presented tunes in
(L) patient groups
Anterior temporal (including HG) 21 30 (m)
(R)
Samson and Zatorre Controls (C) 10 28 (m) Unknown Song excerpts Impaired recognition of
(1991) Anterior temporal (including HG) 12 30 (m) (Serafine et al., 1984) previously presented tunes
(Experiment 2) (L) without lyrics in (R) group
Anterior temporal (including HG) 12 29 (m)
(R)
Samson and Zatorre (1992) Controls (C) 15 28 (m) 10% of each group with 112 melodic patterns Impaired recognition in
Anterior temporal (including HG) 20 30 (m) musical training determining which tune had
(L) previously been presented in
Anterior temporal (including HG) 20 30 (m) both patient groups compared
(R) to (C)
Shankweiler (1966) Controls (C) 20 – Unknown Dichotic presentation of Impaired recognition of
Anterior temporal (including HG) 20 – tunes pre- and postsurgery previously presented tunes
(L) postsurgery for (R) group versus
Anterior temporal (including HG) 20 – (C) and (L)
(R)
M, mean value; STG, superior temporal gyrus; HG, Heschl’s gyrus; R, right; L, left; C, controls.
Music and Epilepsy

doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012
955
956

Table 2. Summary of studies investigating the effect of temporal lobe surgery for epilepsy on musical perception
Author and Year Resection No Age (years) Musical education Test Key finding
Musical perception
Johnsrude et al. (2000) Controls (C) 14 22–52 Unknown Pitch discrimination and Impaired pitch direction task in right
M. J. Maguire

Anterior temporal sparing HG (L) 12 22–53 direction tasks temporal resection including HG group
Anterior temporal sparing HG 5 22–53
(R)

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


Anterior temporal including HG 6 22–53
(L)
Anterior temporal including HG 8 22–53

doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
(R)
Kester et al. (1991) Controls (C) 12 27 (m) 100% of study participants Musical aptitude profile Pitch pattern and meter impaired in right
Anterior temporal, sparing HG 9 27 (m) <1 year of training Seashore tonal memory test temporal lobectomy versus controls
(L) and left temporal lobectomy
Anterior temporal, spaing HG (R) 12 32 (m)
Koike et al. (1996) Temporal, including STG (R) 10 28 (m) Unknown Seashore Measures of Musical No impairment of any of the six subsets
Temporal, sparing STG (R) 10 28 (m) Talents in any of the resection groups
Temporal, sparing STG (L) 9 37 (m)
Liegeois-Chauvel et al. Controls (C) 24 32 (m) 10% of each group with Temporal organization task Pitch interval impaired in all resective
(1998) Temporal, sparing STG (L) 14 31 (m) musical training groups particularly posterior STG
Temporal, sparing STG (R) 19 30 (m) group versus controls
Anterior STG (L) 5 39 (m) Rhythm impaired only in posterior STG
Anterior STG (R) 8 30 (m) group
Including posterior STG (L aid R) 8 – Meter impaired in anterior STG
resections
Milner (1962) Temporal, including HG (L) 16 – Unknown Seashore Measures of Musical Pitch pattern, timbre, loudness, and time
Temporal, including HG (R) 11 – Talent pre- and postsurgery subtests impaired in (R)
Samson and Zatorre Controls (C) 20 29 (m) 25% of each group with Melodic and chord Impaired pitch pattern in right temporal
(1988) Anterior temporal including HG 28 28 (m) musical training discrimination tasks lobectomy resections including HG
(L) versus (C) and (L)
Anterior temporal including HG 26 29 (m) No difference in tonal structure versus
(R) controls in all resection groups
Frontal (R) 14 31 (m)
Frontotemporal (R) 9 25 (m)
Samson and Zatorre Controls (C) 15 26 (m) 25% of each group with 48 pairs of digtized sounds (see Impaired timbral change in tone pairs in
(1994) Anterior temporal including HG 15 28 (m) musical training paper) right temporal lobectomy group versus
(L) (C) and (L)
Anterior temporal including HG 15 30 (m)
(R)
Samson et al. (2001) Controls (C) 14 – Unknown Psychophysical task Impaired detection of temporal
Anterior temporal (L) 11 – Tempora vaidion task irregularity in familiar tunes in (L) versus
Anterior temporal (R) 11 – (see paper) (C) and (R)
Samson et al. (2002) Controls (C) 15 26 (m) 25% of each group 36 stimulus pairs played on Impared judgment in timbral dissmilarity
with musical training different timbres in pairs of melodies in both resective
groups versus (C)
Continued
Table 2. Continued
Author and Year Resection No Age (years) Musical education Test Key finding
Anterior temporal (L) 15 28 (m)
Anterior temporal including HG 15 30 (m)
(R)
Warrier and Zatorre Controls (C) 12 36 (m) 20% of each group with See paper for methods Impaired judgment of final note tuning in
(2004) Anterior temporal including HG 18 35 (m) musical training right temporal resection group versus
(L) (C)
Anterior temporal including HG (R) 18 37 (m)
Zatorre (1985) Controls (C) 20 32 (m) Unknown Six tonal melodies altered by Impaired discrimination of pitch direction
Anterior temporal including HG 28 27 (m) contour or scale in right temporal lobectomy including
(L) HG versus (L) and (C)
Anterior temporal including HG 30 29 (m)
(R)
Zatorre (1988) Controls (C) 18 25 (m) Unknown Pairs of tones with altered Impaired discrimination of pitch direction
Anterior temporal sparing HG (L) 15 27 (m) pitch direction in right temporal lobectomy including
Anterior temporal sparing HG 15 25 (m) HG
(R)
Anterior temporal including HG (L) 16 30 (m)
Anterior temporal including HG (R) 18 31 (m)
Zatorre and Halpern Controls (C) 14 31 (m) A few participants in each Seven songs used to assess Pitch height of two lyrics within familiar
(1993) Anterior temporal (L) 14 33 (m) group with musical pitch height songs impaired in (R) versus (C)
Anterior temporal including HG 14 32 (m) training
(R)
Zatorre and Samson Controls (C) 18 27 (m) No professional musicians Pitch discrimination assessed Impaired discrimination of pitch
(1991) Anterior temporal including HG 26 28 (m) included by target tone variation and difference between two tones with
(L) distracter tones intervening distracter tones in
Anterior temporal including HG 26 29 (m) right temporal lobectomy group
(R)
Frontal (R) 13 31 (m)
Frontotemporal (R) 7 26 (m)
M, mean value; STG, superior temporal gyrus; HG, Heschl’s gyrus; R, right; L, left; C, controls.
Music and Epilepsy

doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012
957
958
M. J. Maguire

assess musical abilities in preschool children (Seashore


et al., 1960). The test consists of six measures: pitch, loud-
Conclusions
ness, rhythm, time, timbre, and tonal memory. The test The dichotomous proconvulsant and anticonvulsant role of
requires a person to compare a pair or short series of tones music in epilepsy is a fascinating and at present poorly under-
produced by a beat frequency oscillator. This test is based stood phenomenon. Dopaminergic system modulation may pro-
on the psychophysical measures of auditory perception with vide one possible explanation, but this theory needs further
little recognizable musical content and has been reported to exploration. It is clear from clinical studies that musical percep-
be sensitive to right temporal lobe dysfunction (Milner, tion occurs via a complex network, which if recruited or hyper-
1962). However, the test has been criticized for being excitable may manifest as seizures. What makes a particular
‘‘atomistic’’ and ‘‘unmusically orientated’’ (Mursell, 1937). sound or sequence of sounds epileptogenic may be explained by
Other authors have criticized its localizing value (Steinmeyer, the stimulation to varying effect of different areas involved
1984; Boone & Rausch, 1989). The tool was also used to within the music network. Musicogenic epilepsy may be more
assess professional musicians. The musicians scored higher common than we suspect as routine EEG does not use sound in
on only three of the six measures compared to population seizure provocation, and there may be a delay in linking a partic-
scores, suggesting the Seashore assumptions about the ular musical trigger to seizures.
requirements for high musical talent may be incorrect There is limited evidence of an anticonvulsant effect with
(Henson & Wyke, 1984). These more recent studies cast music in epilepsy. The therapeutic potential remains for the
uncertainty on the validity of this test in the preoperative moment largely unknown and unproven and our under-
setting. standing of the brain mechanisms involved are very limited.
The Montreal Battery for the Evaluation of Amusia Altered pitch perception in relation to carbamazepine is
(Peretz et al., 2003) is a more recent test that uses musical rare, but health care professionals should discuss this risk or
material as the stimulus. The test assesses melodic and tem- consider alternative medication, particularly if the patient is
poral processing of musical perception and memory via six a professional musician or native-born Japanese. The effect
tests on contour, interval, scale, meter, and memory using of epilepsy surgery on musicality is a potential and signifi-
thirty musical phrases. The musical excerpts can be found at cant issue especially with right temporal lobectomy,
http://www.brams.umontreal.ca/plab/research. The test has although the extent of this risk and any correlation to size
a high sensitivity even on retesting at 4 months. The test and area of resection need further delineation. This potential
correlated well with another widely used aptitude test, risk may bring into question whether musical perception,
Gordon’s musical aptitude profile (Gordon, 1965). The and memory testing should form part of the preoperative
authors stated that the advantage of this tool over the Sea- neuropsychological workup for patients embarking on sur-
shore measures or Gordon’s musical aptitude profile was gery, particularly on the right temporal lobe.
that having been more recently devised it better reflected
the current concepts of musical perception and memory.
The test was more likely to pinpoint specific deficiencies in Acknowledgments
components of musical perception compared to Gordon’s I would like to thank Dr Michael Johnson, Consultant Neurologist at
musical aptitude profile, which was designed to assess Leeds General Infirmary for his kind donation of Critchley & Henson’s
musical aptitude in children and tests structural components Music and the Brain, a fascinating book for all who are working within the
neurologic sciences.
all at once. The test better reflected melody discrimination
and ordinary musical ability compared to the Seashore mea-
sures. This tool therefore may be more suited compared to Disclosure
previously devised tools in diagnostic work looking at base-
line musical functioning and evaluation of deficits. The MJ Maguire has no conflict of interest to disclose. I confirm that I have
read the Journal’s position on issues involved in ethical publication and
website above has a tool that can be used to evaluate musical affirm that this report is consistent with those guidelines.
emotion. In summary this test may be more suitable in the
presurgical evaluation setting, although further work is
required in this area. References
Further research is needed into the neuropsychological
Al-Tajir G, Starr MS. (1990) Anticonvulsant action of SCH 23390 in the
consequences of temporal lobe surgery on musical process- striatum of the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 191:329–336.
ing. This could be achieved by collaboration of profession- Amunts K, Schlaug G, Jancke L, Steinmetz H, Schleicher A, Dabringhaus
als within the cognitive sciences and neurosurgeons A. (1997) Motor cortex and hand motor skills: structural compliance in
the human brain. Hum Brain Mapp 5:206–215.
specializing in epilepsy surgery. The trend for referring Aoun P, Jones T, Shaw GL, Bodner M. (2005) Long-term enhancement of
increasing numbers of refractory epilepsy patients for sur- maze learning in mice via a generalized Mozart effect. Neurol Res
gery could potentially expose increasing numbers of indi- 27:791–796.
Bartolomei F, McGonigal A, Guye M, Guedj E, Chauvel P. (2007) Clinical
viduals to this problem. Understanding the consequences of and anatomic characteristics of humming and singing in partial sei-
surgery on musical processing is therefore paramount. zures. Neurology 69:490–492.

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
959
Music and Epilepsy

Bautista RE, Ciampetti MZ. (2003) Expressive aprosody and amusia as a Gosselin N, Peretz I, Noulhiane M, Hasboun D, Beckett C, Baulac M,
manifestation of right hemisphere seizures. Epilepsia 44:466–467. Samson S. (2005) Impaired recognition of scary music following
Berg AT, Berkovic SF, Brodie MJ, Buchhalter J, Cross JH, van Emde unilateral temporal lobe excision. Brain 128:628–640.
Boas W, Engel J, French J, Glauser TA, Mathern GW, Mosh SL, Gosselin N, Peretz I, Hasboun D, Baulac M, Samson S. (2011) Impaired
Nordli D, Plouin P, Scheffer IE. (2010) Revised terminology and recognition of musical emotions and facial expressions following ante-
concepts for organization of seizures and epilepsies: report of the ILAE romedial temporal lobe excision. Cortex 47:1116–1125.
Commission on Classification and Terminology, 2005–2009. Epilepsia Griffiths TD. (2000) Musical hallucinosis in acquired deafness. Phenome-
51:676–685. nology and brain substrate. Brain 123:2065–2076.
Bishop SJ. (2007) Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative Guaranha MS, da Silva Sousa P, de Araffljo-Filho GM, Lin K, Guilhoto LM,
account. Trends Cogn Sci 11:307–316. Caboclo LO, Yacubian EM. (2009) Provocative and inhibitory effects
Boeve BF, Geda YE. (2001) Polka music and semantic dementia. Neurol- of a video-EEG neuropsychologic protocol in juvenile myoclonic
ogy 57:1485. epilepsy. Epilepsia 50:2446–2455.
Boone KB, Rausch R. (1989) Seashore Rhythm Test performance in Head H. (1920) Studies in neurology, Vol. 2. Oxford Medical Publication,
patients with unilateral temporal lobe damage. J Clin Psychol 45:614– Oxford.
618. Head H. (1926) Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. Cambridge
Bozzi Y, Vallone D, Borrelli E. (2000) Neuroprotective role of dopamine University Press, Cambridge.
against hippocampal cell death. J Neurosci 20:8643–8649. Henson RA, Wyke MA. (1984) The performance of professional musicians
Brien SE, Murray TJ. (1984) Musicogenic epilepsy. Can Med Assoc J on the Seashore measures of musical talent: an unexpected finding.
13:1255–1258. Cortex 18:153–157.
Buccino G, Riggio L, Melli G. (2005) Listening to action-related sentences Hughes JR, Daaboul Y, Fino JJ, Shaw GL. (1998) The ‘‘Mozart effect’’ on
modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and epileptiform activity. Clin Electroencephalogr 29:109–119.
behavioural study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 24:355–363. Hutchinson S, Lee LH, Gaab N, Schlaug G. (2003) Cerebellar volume of
Callan DE, Tsytsarev V, Hanakawa T. (2006) Song and speech: brain musicians. Cereb Cortex 13:943–949.
regions involved with perception and covert production. Neuroimage Jacome DE. (1984) Aphasia with elation, hypermusia, musicophilia
31:1327–1342. and compulsive whistling. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 47:308–
Chaloupka V, Mitchell S, Muirhead R. (1994) Observation of a reversible, 310.
medication-induced change in pitch perception. J Acoust Soc Am Johnsrude IS, Owen AM, White NM, Zhao WV, Bohbot V. (2000)
96:145–149. Impaired preference conditioning after anterior temporal lobe resection
Critchley M. (1937) Musicogenic epilepsy. Brain 60:13–27. in humans. J Neurosci 20:2649–2656.
Critchley M, Henson RA. (1977) Music and the brain: studies in the neurol- Joynt RJ, Green D, Green R. (1962) Musicogenic epilepsy. JAMA
ogy of music. Heinemann, London. 179:501–504.
Currie S, Heathfield KW, Henson RA, Scott DF. (1971) Clinical course and Kaneyuki H, Yokoo H, Tsuda A, Yoshida M, Mizuki Y, Yamada M. (1991)
prognosis of temporal lobe epilepsy: a survey of 666 patients. Brain Psychological stress increases dopamine turnover selectively in meso-
94:173–190. prefrontal dopamine neurons of rats: reversal by diazepam. Brain Res
Daly DD, Barry MJ. (1957) Musicogenic epilepsy: report of three cases. 557:154–161.
Psychosom Med 19:399. Kester DB, Saykin AJ, Sperling MR, O’Connor MJ, Robinson LJ, Gur RC.
De Araujo Filho GM, Pascalicchio TF, Lin K, Sousa PS, Yacubian EM. (1991) Acute effect of anterior temporal lobectomy on musical process-
(2006) Neuropsychiatric profiles of patients with juvenile myoclonic ing. Neuropsychologia 29:703–708.
epilepsy treated with valproate or topiramate. Epilepsy Behav 8:606– Khalfa S, Guye M, Peretz I, Chapon F, Girard N, Chauvel P, Ligeois-
609. Chauvel C. (2008) Evidence of lateralized anteromedial temporal struc-
Dellacherie D, Pfeuty M, Hasboun D, Lefvre J, Hugueville L, Schwartz DP, tures involvement in musical emotion processing. Neuropsychologia
Baulac M, Adam C, Samson S. (2009) The birth of musical emotion: a 46:2485–2493.
depth electrode case study in a human subject with epilepsy. Ann N Y Kim H, Lee MH, Chang HK, Lee TH, Lee HH, Shin MC, Shin MS, Won R,
Acad Sci 1169:336–341. Shin HS, Kim CJ. (2006) Influence of prenatal noise and music on the
Dellacherie D, Hasboun D, Baulac M, Belin P, Samson S. (2011) Impaired spatial memory and neurogenesis in the hippocampus of developing
recognition of fear in voices and reduced anxiety after unilateral tempo- rats. Brain Dev 28:109–114.
ral lobe resection. Neuropsychologia 49:618–629. Kleist K. (1962) Sensory aphasia and amusia: the myeloarchitectonic
Deransart C, Riban V, LÞ B, Marescaux C, Depaulis A. (2000) Dopamine basis. Oxford Pergamon Press, Oxford.
in the striatum modulates seizures in a genetic model of absence epi- Knoblauch A. (1888) Uber Storungen der musikalischen Leistungsfahigkeit
lepsy in the rat. Neuroscience 100:335–344. infolge von Gehirnlasionen. Dt Arch Klin Med 43:331–352.
Dresel C, Castrop F, Haslinger B, Wohlschlaeger AM, Hennenlotter A, Koike A, Shimizu H, Suzuki I, Ishijima B, Sugishita M. (1996) Preserved
Ceballos-Baumann AO. (2005) The functional neuroanatomy of musical abilities following right temporal lobectomy. J Neurosurg
coordinated orofacial movements: sparse sampling fMRI of whistling. 85:1000–1004.
Neuroimage 28:588–597. Krishnamoorthy ES, Trimble MR, Sander JW, Kanner AM. (2002) Forced
Enatsu R, Hantus S, Gonzalez-Martinez J, So N. (2011) Ictal singing due normalization at the interface between epilepsy and psychiatry.
to left frontal lobe epilepsy: a case report and review of the literature. Epilepsy Behav 3:303–308.
Epilepsy Behav 22:404–406. Kuester G, Rios L, Ortiz A, Miranda M. (2010) Effect of music on the
Fedi M, Berkovic SF, Scheffer IE, O’Keefe G, Marini C, Mulligan R, Gong S, recovery of a patient with refractory non-convulsive status epilepticus.
Tochon-Danguy H, Reutens DC. (2008) Reduced striatal D1 receptor Epilepsy Behav 18:491–493.
binding in autosomal dominant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy. Neurol- Lahiri N, Duncan JS. (2007) The Mozart effect: encore. Epilepsy Behav
ogy 71:795–798. 11:152–153.
Forster FM, Hallgrim K, Peterson WG, Bengzon ARA. (1965) Modifica- Landvogt C, Buchholz HG, Bernedo V, Schreckenberger M, Werhahn KJ.
tion of musicogenic epilepsy by extinction technique. Trans Am Neurol (2010) Alteration of dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding in patients with
Assoc 90:179. juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsia 51:1699–1706.
Foster NA, Valentine ER. (2001) The effect of auditory stimulation on Lazzarino LG, Valassi F. (1982) Whistling as a manifestation of epilepsy.
autobiographical recall in dementia. Exp Aging Res 27:215–228. Riv Neurobiol 128:127–130.
Fujinawa A, Kawai I, Ohashi H, Kimura S. (1977) A case of musicogenic Le Chevalier B, Eustache F, Rossa Y. (1985) Les troubles de la perception
epilepsy. Folia Psychiatr Neurol 31:463–472. de la musique d’origine neurologique. Rapport de Neurologie. Masson,
Gaser C, Schlaug G. (2003) Brain structures differ between musicians and Paris.
non-musicians. J Neurosci 23:9240–9245. Leaver AM, Van Lare J, Zielinski B, Halpern AR, Rauschecker JP. (2009)
Gloor P. (1990) Experiential phenomena of temporal lobe epilepsy. Facts Brain activation during anticipation of sound sequences. J Neurosci
and hypotheses. Brain 113:1673–1694. 29:2477–2485.
Gordon E. (1965) Musical aptitude profile. GIA, Chicago.

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
960
M. J. Maguire

Liegeois-Chauvel C, Peretz I, Babai M, Laguittin V, Chauvel P. (1998) Rickson DJ, Watkins WG. (2003) Music therapy to promote prosocial
Contribution of different cortical areas in the temporal lobes to music behaviors in aggressive adolescent boys: a pilot study. J Music Ther
processing. Brain 121:1853–1867. 40:283–301.
Lin KL, Wang HS, Kao PF. (2003) A young infant with musicogenic epi- Rohrer JD, Smith SJ, Warren JD. (2006) Craving for music after treatment
lepsy. Pediatr Neurol 28:379–381. for partial epilepsy. Epilepsia 47:939–940.
Lin LC, Lee WT, Wu HC. (2010) Mozart K.448 and epileptiform dis- Sacks O. (2008) Musicophilia: tales of music and the brain. Vintage Books,
charges: effect of ratio of lower to higher harmonics. Epilepsy Res New York.
89:238–245. Samson S, Zatorre RJ. (1988) Melodic and harmonic discrimination follow-
Lin LC, Lee WT, Wang CH, Chen HL, Wu HC, Tsai CL, Wei RC, Mok HK, ing unilateral cerebral excision. Brain Cogn 7:348–360.
Weng CF, Lee MW, Yang RC. (2011) Mozart K.448 acts as a potential Samson S, Zatorre RJ. (1991) Recognition memory for text and melody of
add-on therapy in children with refractory epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav songs after unilateral temporal lobe lesion: evidence for dual encoding.
20:490–493. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 17:793–804.
Loring DW, Hermann BP, Meador KJ. (1994) Amnesia after unilateral tem- Samson S, Zatorre RJ. (1992) Learning and retention of melodic and verbal
poral lobectomy: a case report. Epilepsia 35:757–763. information after unilateral temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia
Luria AR, Tsvetkova LS, Futer DS. (1965) Aphasia in a composer. J Neurol 30:815–826.
Sci 2:288–292. Samson S, Zatorre RJ. (1994) Contribution of the right temporal lobe to
Matsuoka H, Takahashi T, Sasaki M, Yoshida S, Numachi Y, Sato M. musical timbre discrimination. Neuropsychologia 32:231–240.
(2002) The long-term course of seizure susceptibility in two patients Samson S, Ehrl N, Baulac M. (2001) Cerebral substrates for musical tem-
with juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Seizure 11:126–130. poral processes. Ann N Y Acad Sci 930:166–178.
Matsuoka H, Nakamura M, Ohno T, Shimabukuro J, Suzuki T, Numachi Y, Samson S, Zatorre RJ, Ramsay JO. (2002) Deficits of musical timbre per-
Awata S. (2005) The role of cognitive-motor function in precipitation ception after unilateral temporal-lobe lesion revealed with multidimen-
and inhibition of epileptic seizures. Epilepsia 46:17–20. sional scaling. Brain 125:511–523.
Medaglini S, Filippi M, Smirne S, Ferini-Strambi L, Giusti MC, Poggi A. Schlaug G, Jancke L, Huang Y, Staiger JF, Steinmetz H. (1995) Increased
(1988) Effects of long-lasting antiepileptic therapy on brainstem audi- corpus callosum size in musicians. Neuropsychologia 33:1047–1055.
tory evoked potentials. Neuropsychobiology 19:104–107. Schneider P, Scherg M, Dosch HG, Specht HJ, Gutschalk A, Rupp A.
Mehta AD, Ettinger AB, Perrine K, Dhawan V, Patil A, Jain SK, Klein G, (2002) Morphology of Heschl’s gyrus reflects enhanced activation in
Schneider SJ, Eidelberg D. (2009) Seizure propagation in a patient with the auditory cortex of musicians. Nat Neurosci 5:688–694.
musicogenic epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 14:421–424. Seashore CE, Lewis D, Saetveit JG. (1960) Seashore measures of musical
Meierkord H, Shorvon S. (1991) Variations on a theme – singing as an epi- talents manual (Revised 1960). The Psychological Corp, New York.
leptic automatism. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 54:1114–1116. Serafine ML, Crowder RG, Repp BH. (1984) Integration of melody and text
Mendes de Freitas R, Aguiar LM, Vasconcelos SM. (2005) Modifica- in memory for songs. Cognition 16:285–303.
tions in muscarinic, dopaminergic and serotonergic receptors concen- Sergeant D, Roche S. (1973) Perceptual shifts in the auditory information
trations in the hippocampus and striatum of epileptic rats. Life Sci processing of young children. Psychol Music 1–2:39.
78:253–258. Shankweiler D. (1966) Effects of temporal-lobe damage of perception
Milner B. (1962) Laterality effects in audition. In Mountcastle VB (Ed) of dichotically presented melodies. J Comp Physiol Psychol 62:115–
Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance. The Johns Hop- 119.
kins Press, Baltimore, MD, pp. 177–195. Starr MS. (1996) The role of dopamine in epilepsy. Synapse 22:159–194.
Miranda M, Kuester G, Ros L, Basaez E, Hazard S. (2010) Refractory non- Steinmeyer CH. (1984) Are the rhythm tests of the Halstead-Reitan and
convulsive status epilepticus responsive to music as an add-on therapy: Luria-Nebraska batteries differentially sensitive to right temporal lobe
a second case. Epilepsy Behav 19:539–540. lesions? J Clin Psychol 40:1464–1466.
Molnar-Szakacs I, Overy K. (2006) Music and mirror neurons: from motion Stewart L, Von Kriegstein K, Warren JD, Griffiths TD. (2006) Music and
to ‘e’motion. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 1:235–241. the brain: disorders of musical listening. Brain 129:2533–2553.
Mrocz IA, Karni A, Haut S, Lantos G, Liu G. (2003) fMRI of triggerable Sutherling WW, Hershman LM, Miller JQ, Lee SI. (1980) Seizures induced
aurae in musicogenic epilepsy. Neurology 60:705–709. by playing music. Neurology 30:1001–1004.
Mursell J. (1937) The psychology of music. W.W. Norton & Co Inc, New Sutoo D, Akiyama K. (2004) Music improves dopaminergic neurotransmis-
York. sion: demonstration based on the effect of music on blood pressure reg-
Ogunyemi AO, Breen H. (1993) Seizures induced by music. Behav Neurol ulation. Brain Res 1016:255–262.
6:15–18. Tan E, Ciğer A, Zileli T. (1990) Whistling epilepsy: a case report. Clin
Pacchetti C, Mancini F, Aglieri R. (2000) Active music therapy in Parkin- Electroencephalogr 21:110–111.
son’s disease: an integrative method for motor and emotional rehabilita- Tateno A, Sawada K, Takahashi I, Hujiwara Y. (2006) Carbamazepine-
tion. Psychosom Med 62:386–393. induced transient auditory pitch-perception deficit. Pediatr Neurol
Penagos H, Melcher JR, Oxenham AJ. (2004) A neural representation of 35:131–134.
pitch salience in nonprimary human auditory cortex revealed with func- Tayah TF, Abou-Khalil B, Gilliam FG, Knowlton RC, Wushensky CA,
tional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 24:6810–6815. Gallagher MJ. (2006) Musicogenic seizures can arise from multiple
Peretz I, Champod AS, Hyde K. (2003) Varieties of musical disorders. The temporal lobe foci: intracranial EEG analyses of three patients. Epilep-
Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia. Ann N Y Acad Sci 999: sia 47:1402–1406.
58–75. Trevathan E, Gewirtz RJ, Cibula JE. (1999) Musicogenic seizures of the
Pittau F, Tinuper P, Bisulli F, Naldi I, Cortelli P, Bisulli A, Stipa C, right superior temporal gyrus origin precipitated by the theme song
Cevolani D, Agati R, Leonardi M, Baruzzi A. (2008) Videopolygraphic from ‘‘The X Files’’. Epilepsia 40:23.
and functional MRI study of musicogenic epilepsy. A case report and Turner RP. (2004) The acute effect of music on interictal epileptiform dis-
literature review. Epilepsy Behav 13:685–692. charges. Epilepsy Behav 5:662–668.
Poskanzer DC, Brown AE, Miller H. (1962) Musicogenic epilepsy caused Vercelletto P. (1953) Case of musicogenic epilepsy; presentation of a case of
only by a discrete frequency band of church bells. Brain 85:77. temporal seizures and discussion on the origin. Rev Neurol 88:379–382.
Price C, Thierry G, Griffiths T. (2005) Speech-specific auditory processing: Vizioli R. (1989) Musicogenic epilepsy. Int J Neurosci 47:159–164.
where is it? Trends Cogn Sci 9:271–276. Warrier CM, Zatorre RJ. (2004) Right temporal cortex is critical for utiliza-
Raghavendra S, Mirsattari S, McLachlan RS. (2010) Ictal whistling: a rare tion of melodic contextual cues in a pitch constancy task. Brain
automatism during temporal lobe seizures. Epileptic Disord 12:133– 127:1616–1625.
135. Werhahn KJ, Landvogt C, Klimpe S, Buchholz HG, Yakushev I, Siessmeier T,
Rauscher FH, Shaw GL, Ky KN. (1993) Music and spatial task perfor- Mller-Forell W, Piel M, Rçsch F, Glaser M, Schreckenberger M,
mance. Nature 365:611. Bartenstein P. (2006) Decreased dopamine D2/D3-receptor binding in
Rauscher FH, Robinson KD, Jens JJ. (1998) Improved maze learning temporal lobe epilepsy: an [18F] fallypride PET study. Epilepsia
through early music exposure in rats. Neurol Res 20:427–432. 47:1392–1396.

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x
961
Music and Epilepsy

Wieser HG, Hungerbuhler H, Siegel AM, Buck A. (1997) Musicogenic epi- Zatorre RJ. (1988) Pitch perception of complex tones and human temporal-
lepsy: review of the literature and case report with ictal single photon lobe function. J Acoust Soc Am 84:566–572.
emission computed tomography. Epilepsia 38:200–207. Zatorre RJ, Halpern AR. (1993) Effect of unilateral temporal-lobe excision
Yun M, Choi YM, Eun SH, Seol IJ, Kim SJ. (2011) Acoustic effects of on perception and imagery of songs. Neuropsychologia 31:221–232.
lamotrigine in pediatric patients with epilepsy. Brain Dev 33:374–378. Zatorre RJ, Samson S. (1991) Role of the right temporal neocortex in reten-
Zatorre RJ. (1985) Discrimination and recognition of tonal melodies after tion of pitch in auditory short-term memory. Brain 114:2403–2417.
unilateral cerebral excisions. Neuropsychologia 23:31–41.

Epilepsia, 53(6):947–961, 2012


doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03523.x

You might also like