You are on page 1of 12

Theoretical Perspectives on the

Cause of Stuttering
Nicoline Grinager Ambrose
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

F or some strange reason, we have never been able


to devise an agreeable definition of stuttering.
Demosthenes did not seem to have a problem
identifying stuttering—he knew that he did, and he knew
what could relieve it. Perhaps those seaside walks along the
Freud, who immediately interpreted the ocean as his
mother’s womb and the pebbles as an oral fixation. Soon
after, Skinner came knocking at the door. He laughed at
Freud. How ridiculous. It is obvious, he said, that our
friend Demosthenes here has practiced fluency with the
crashing waves kept his head clear. And, philosophers of that noise of the waves in his ears, and now the stimulus of the
time had heavier thoughts on their minds. But, I will not waves evokes a learned response and voila, fluent speech.
repeat here a review of the numerous commentaries of But Demosthenes was skeptical, thinking that he had
Wingate (1964), Perkins (1990), and others on the definition merely been walking and talking, nothing more. He
of stuttering and presence, or lack of, adequate theoretic grabbed his sandals and strode into town. Later that
underpinnings. I will leave the philosophizing to the ancient evening, at his Philosophy of Stuttering Support Group
Greeks and others. Instead of trying to determine who is meeting, he repeated the events of the day to his fellow
right and who is wrong, I will try to connect the disparate group members Moses and Darwin, and as the reader can
points of view about what type of disorder stuttering is. imagine, the issue of the cause of stuttering was far from
Anyway, Demosthenes enjoyed his fluent walks along the being resolved. Let’s leave them and return to reality.
beach and enjoyed experimenting with talking with pebbles Which one of the above gentlemen was correct in his
in his mouth. He went home, and who should be there but views? That is not an easy question to answer. It is not the
case that one must be right and the others wrong—any
ABSTRACT: The many theories of the cause of stuttering number of them could be onto the truth, just different
are too numerous to mention. Instead, this article aspects of it. How much we know about something drives
presents a brief background of theories in the realms of our thinking about its causes. Before Van Leeuwenhoek
psychology, learning theory, and biology. A representative discovered bacteria, or “animalcules,” in 1677, people had
sampling of older literature is discussed, followed by a
other ideas about the causes of illness. Before the design of
more detailed consideration of a few state-of-the-art
DNA was discovered, understanding of the biologic
studies. The article concludes that previously presented
general models can provide a basis for testable hypoth- transmission of traits was limited.
eses. Current research appears to indicate that there is a Humans have always wondered about their world, but it
genetic basis for stuttering, affecting both motor and is more than simple curiosity. Our development and
sensory systems in the brain in subtle ways, and that survival as a species has in large part been due to our
deficits are exacerbated by a host of both other genetic unique capabilities for mental reflection. This fundamental
as well as environmental factors. Genetic, physiologic, drive to explain, understand, predict, and control our
psychologic, and environmental influences guide the universe as we know it is strong. Long ago, natural
developmental pathway of stuttering. phenomena were explained as the work of deities. Greek
and Roman, African, Chinese, Native American, Egyptian,
KEY WORDS: stuttering, etiology, theory, speech disorder,
and Scandinavian people—all had belief systems that
childhood stuttering
explained how the world worked. We think of these ideas

80 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES INCONTEMPORARY


COMMUNICATION SCIENCE
ISSUES AND D ISORDERS
IN C OMMUNICATION • Volume
SCIENCE 31 • 80–91
AND D ISORDERS • Spring
• Volume 2004 • Spring 2004 © NSSLHA
31 • 80–91
1092-5171/04/3101-0080

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


as interesting and powerful, but many of them certainly not not attempt to review all of the relevant literature; rather,
“true” and certainly not “scientific” to our modern way of major trends are highlighted and supported by a few
thinking. But, given the observations that were available at samples of relevant studies.
the time, these theories made sense. Theoretical invention
tries to suit the data. Perhaps in a millennium or two, what Psychologic Causes
we now believe to be “real truth” will seem ridiculous,
naïve, outdated, or wrong. The writings of Sigmund Freud were in ascendance in the
People use whatever resources they have to explain early decades of the 1900s. In the 1920s and 1930s, one
occurrences of an almost infinite number of phenomena. It line of thinking regarding the origins of stuttering focused
has even been said that perhaps many beliefs come from the on personality conflict of the individual, referencing
left brain trying to explain what the right brain is experienc- Freud’s theory of psychoanalysis. Basically, this approach
ing. And after all, truth is relative. Think of patients with viewed stuttering as merely an overt symptom of something
various brain malfunctions. Sometimes people with aphasia else—unconscious, deep-seated neurotic disorder(s). Authors
have very interesting ideas about why they have difficulty purporting this point of view, however, differed in what they
with language. Children have interesting explanations for understood to be the specific nature of the conflict. Whereas
their behavior. The combination of natural curiosity and the Coriat (1928) asserted that the problem is characteristic of
need to explain is perhaps at the root of science. libido fixation at the oral stage of infant psychosexual
It follows that the history of ideas about the cause and development, Fenichel (1945) maintained that the conflict
development of stuttering has undergone many develop- originally occurred during the anal stage, lending it more
ments as our knowledge base and technology have im- aggressive characteristics. Blanton (1931) wrote that children
proved. It is also important to remember that science goes who began to stutter experienced fixation of the libido,
in circles. In other words, history repeats itself. Theories of thereby making the child overly sensitive and anxious. He
stuttering have gone from biologic to psychologic to agreed, however, that there were physiologic factors in
behaviorist and back to biologic to all three combined. addition to psychologic ones, such as being left-handed or
Regardless, several unusual phenomena must be incorpo- having weak muscles in the tongue and throat area. Brill
rated into, or must not contradict, any theory of the cause (1923) discussed stuttering as a psychoneurosis, although
of stuttering; namely, those that ameliorate stuttering. he also referred to physiologic factors as “hereditary
Stuttering diminishes, or even disappears, under a host of burden” (p. 135). These authors stressed family environ-
conditions—singing, speaking in unison/choral reading, ment as shaping stuttering. Although Brill claimed consider-
talking with a metronome, talking with noise or delayed able treatment success using classical Freudian psycho-
auditory feedback, and talking to pets or babies (quite analysis as the main vehicle of therapy, he conceded that a
similar, really, in that neither have read treatises on large number of his former patients eventually relapsed.
stuttering and do not realize that their caretakers talk The psychoanalytic point of view continued to be active,
differently than others). Conversely, stuttering tends to however, and in 1957, Travis wrote about “the unspeakable
worsen under various types of pressure (e.g., time) and feelings of people” who stutter. This was a complete
more complex linguistic demands. These facts need to be change from his earlier views on cerebral laterality. Travis
taken into account when we attempt to answer the follow- described stuttering as a vehicle that acts to inhibit
ing questions: What are we trying to explain? Where, in unconscious needs, or unacceptable statements of true inner
the fluency-generating system, does the breakdown occur? thoughts and feelings. The child wants to communicate,
Do breakdowns occur at multiple sites? Is it components or but, being afraid of revealing unspeakable thoughts and
connections that are aberrant? feelings, he emits stuttered speech that conceals them. Any
discomfort caused by the stuttering was, allegedly, more
than overcome by the relief of hiding unmentionables.
Glauber (1958) also saw stuttering as a neurotic disorder, a
ASPECTS OF CAUSATION personality disturbance reflected in speech. Even the
preponderance of stuttering in males was explained in
From early on, professionals in speech-language pathology psychologic terms in that boys faced more pressure to live
had very different views on the cause of stuttering. up to standards of aggression and had a greater ability to
Stuttering has, at various points in time, been considered to provide and take action. Females, so it was said, had only
be caused by deep-seated neuroses arising in infancy, to be passive creatures (Schuell, 1946).
purely by learning, and from a strictly structural flaw. From the 1920s through the 1980s, a large number of
There is general agreement today, however, that the personality studies of stutterers were undertaken. The
ultimate origin of stuttering is in large part biologic, shaped findings were mixed, but overall, it became clear that
by additional biologic, psychologic, and behavioral influ- people who stutter (PWS) are not any more neurotic than
ences. We begin the review of the various aspects of average (see reviews by Bloodstein, 1995; Goodstein, 1958;
causation of stuttering with a brief presentation of psycho- Sheehan, 1958). The only areas identified as problematic
logic and learning perspectives so as to provide a full view involved communication situations, difficulties that can be
of how far the field has come in its pursuit of truth in the explained as resulting from the stuttering rather than being
name of science. This is followed by a more in-depth a cause of it. Although today it appears perhaps illogical to
presentation of possible causative biologic factors. We shall consider stuttering part of a neurosis, amenable to

Ambrose: Theoretical Perspectives on the Cause of Stuttering 81

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


psychoanalytic treatment, interest in personality, especially overview of theories of stuttering that covers behaviorism
anxiety, has been renewed, as seen in a number of recent and learning. Any discussion of behaviorism and learning
studies. Craig, Hancock, Tran, and Craig (2003) investi- theory in relation to stuttering must mention Wendell
gated trait anxiety in a large sample of adults who stutter. Johnson’s diagnosogenic theory (Johnson et al., 1959),
Trait anxiety is a stable attribute, as opposed to state stating that stuttering begins in the ears of the parents when
anxiety, which occurs in certain circumstances. The persons they overreact to their child’s normal disfluencies. The child
who stutter did present higher trait anxiety levels, but the attempts to avoid the disfluencies and, in the struggle,
anxiety appeared to have developed as a result of the stuttering develops. Along similar lines, Wischner (1950)
stuttering and so would not be considered a causal factor. portrayed stuttering as emerging in accordance with a more
The issue is not that simple, however. As in the next study formal learning theory. Stuttering originates from a painful,
described below, it is likely that some individuals have a anxiety-producing stimulation in the form of parental
predisposition to develop trait anxiety, and it is not known disapproval of normal disfluency. In other words, the child
how this tendency might interact with other factors that anticipates negative, painful reactions to his or her stuttering
contribute to a predisposition to stutter. and so tries to avoid them. Initial successful avoidance
Recently, there has been another shift of interest in reduces the anxiety drive and thus reinforces the behavior
psychosocial aspects of stuttering, with a focus on tempera- that eventually becomes more complicated stuttering.
ment. Anderson, Pellowski, Conture, and Kelly (2003) Another concept is that stuttering results from an
investigated temperament factors in young children who approach–avoidance drive (Sheehan, 1953). In this concept,
stutter through a norm-referenced parent questionnaire. The the desire to communicate collides with the drive to avoid
temperament scale used contains nine subscales: activity, speech anxiety, which may have roots in either or both
adaptability, approach or withdrawal, quality of mood, personality and conditioning from prior negative experi-
intensity of reaction, distractibility, attention span, sensory ences with stuttering. Bloodstein’s (1958) notion that
threshold, and rhythmicity of physiologic functions. Com- stuttering is an anticipatory struggle arising from the belief
pared to the normally fluent children, the stuttering children that speech is difficult contains some similar elements.
were less adaptable to novel situations, less distractible, and Stuttering is seen as a reflection of tension and fragmenta-
less regular in daily physiologic functions. The differences tion in speech when the complexity of the act causes
were statistically significantly different, but the mean scale concern and feelings of being overwhelmed.
scores for both groups on all nine scales were within normal Still another view explains stuttering as a behavior that
limits. More children in the stuttering group had individual is acquired through operant conditioning, that is, a behavior
scores that exceeded 1 SD. The study also reported that there that is shaped by its own consequences. In a pioneer,
was no direct relationship between the time since onset of controlled experiment, Flanagan, Goldiamond, and Azrin
stuttering and scale scores, which indicates that the tempera- (1958) demonstrated that stuttering was reduced when
ment qualities measured may be relatively stable. The point stuttered events were immediately followed by punishment
of interest to the discussion here, however, lies in a chicken (loud noise); it was increased when stuttered events were
and egg question: Do the differing temperament dimensions immediately followed by the removal of punishment
precipitate or exacerbate stuttering, or does the stuttering (continuous tone). Later, Shames and Sherrick (1963)
reinforce the temperamental proclivities of the child? The theorized that stuttering was shaped out of normal dis-
authors concluded that “temperamental characteristics…could fluency as operant behavior, subject to a combination of
potentially contribute, in some as yet unknown fashion, to punishment, positive reinforcement, and negative reinforce-
the onset and development of stuttering” (Anderson et al., ment. Normal repetitions are reinforced when a parent
2003, p. 1230). Although their stipulations have not yet been eventually heeds the child’s request. But as repetitions
substantiated, the fact that there are differences means that become aversive, the parent indicates displeasure at
we cannot yet rule out that possibility. disfluency (in terms of operant behavior, punishment).
We have come a long way from thinking that neuroticism When the consequences of disfluency are negative reac-
causes stuttering, but this does not mean that psychosocial tions, the child may change disfluencies into struggle
factors must be relegated only to the shaping of stuttering behavior or silence in order to avoid the aversive conse-
independent of causative factors. As the genetic and quences (negative reinforcement for the parent). Thus,
environmental bases of personality reveal themselves, it stuttering emerges and is maintained by complex interaction
may become obligatory to add them to the complex of positive and negative reinforcement. Brutten and
equation for the etiology of stuttering. Psychologic causes Shoemaker (1967) developed a two-factor theory of
of a disorder are no longer necessarily distinct from stuttering. They believe that stuttering is initially estab-
organic causes. Overall, the renewed interest in this aspect lished through classical conditioning of emotional learning,
is a welcome development. It would seem to be particularly when anxiety causes disruptions in speech. Through
profitable to study the interaction of personality/tempera- generalization, many stimuli acquire the capability of
ment with other factors. triggering anxiety that results in fluency breakdowns—
stuttering. Secondary characteristics of stuttering designed
Behaviorism and Learning Theory to cope with the disfluencies are then developed through
operant conditioning.
The idea that stuttering is a learned behavior or a “bad In general, these ideas have been discarded as ultimate or
habit” is quite old. Bloodstein (1995) provided an excellent sufficient causes of stuttering. Instead, they are now viewed

82 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 80–91 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


as forces in its development (see Wingate, 1997, for a PWS or were normally fluent and reported subtle evidence
thorough discussion of the matter). The idea that stuttering of a central auditory problem in stuttering at the level of
develops out of normal disfluencies along a continuum the brainstem.
remains prevalent, although not supported by data, as Another method used to study the auditory system in
repeatedly pointed out in the Illinois studies by Yairi, stuttering is dichotic listening, where competing stimuli are
Ambrose, and colleagues (e.g., Ambrose & Yairi, 1999; presented to the left and right ears. Most normal right-
Yairi & Ambrose, 1999). Nevertheless, stuttering therapies handers show a right ear advantage for speech stimuli.
reflecting various learning models have been shown to be Some studies showed that PWS had the expected right ear
rather promising (Onslow, Packman, & Harrison, 2001; advantage, but Rosenfield and Goodglass (1980) found that
Ryan, 2001). more stutterers than controls failed to have the expected
advantage, and Blood and Blood (1984) reported that the
Biologic Causes magnitude of right ear preference was greater for those who
were normally fluent. A note of caution, however, must be
Bloodstein (1995) wrote that Aristotle believed that heeded. Dichotic listening has been described as having low
stuttering stemmed from a problem with the tongue, and in test–retest reliability; in fact, in a second report, Blood and
the 1840s, tongue reduction surgery was performed to Blood (1989) reanalyzed the same data using different
alleviate stuttering. The better known early biologic analysis methods and came up with varying results.
theories of stuttering, however, come from the 1920s and Older studies with conflicting findings have been
1930s. The first three years of the Journal of Speech replaced by current work with state-of-the-art technology
Disorders, 1936–1938, contained no fewer than nine yet again revealing a variety of differences, not always
articles devoted to physiologic aspects of stuttering. Topics consistent, in at least some groups of speakers. Probably
included conditioned reflexes (Moore, 1938), twinning among the most enlightening of the newer technologies is
(Berry, 1938), and heart rates (Palmer & Gillette, 1938; brain imaging. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) can be
Travis, Tuttle, & Cowan, 1936). There are a plethora of used to create a detailed view of brain morphology, and
publications regarding anatomic and physiologic bases of functional MRI and positron emission tomography (PET)
stuttering; only a representative few will be touched on give an eye to function in the brain. Another promising
here as a reflection of past and current work. They are methodology involves magnetoencephalography (MEG).
grouped loosely into four sections: sensory, central process- Although imaging yields poor resolution in time, but
ing, motor, and genetics. For each section, a brief back- excellent resolution in space, MEG is not as accurate in
ground will be presented, and a few current studies will be terms of place but gives much improved resolution in the
discussed in more detail to indicate the general direction of time dimension. A few studies with findings relating to the
cutting-edge research. auditory system are mentioned here.
The sensory side of the picture. The role of auditory In normal speakers, the left planum temporale, part of
function has long been discussed in the field of stuttering. the auditory association cortex, is larger in the left than the
Stuttering tends to be reduced in noisy environments (as right hemisphere. In some individuals who stutter, however,
Demosthenes knew), and it may be reduced or even this structural difference is reduced (Foundas, Bollich,
eliminated with delayed auditory feedback or white noise in Corey, Hurley, & Heilman, 2001). Functional studies have
one or both ears. This effect is even more curious in that also found differences in auditory association cortex (in the
when normally fluent speakers have delayed auditory temporal lobe, which includes Wernicke’s area) in adults
feedback played, their speech tends to become disrupted. In who stutter as compared to those who are normally fluent.
1946, Shane (1955) demonstrated that stuttering was Both Braun et al. (1997) and Fox, Ingham, and colleagues
reduced with high levels of binaurally applied noise. She (Fox et al., 1996; Ingham, Fox, Ingham, & Zamarripa,
concluded that when PWS cannot hear themselves, speech 2000) reported deactivation in this area, especially in the
anxiety is reduced and so is the stuttering. A decade later, right hemisphere, in stuttering speakers.
Cherry and Sayers (1956) conducted a series of experi- One relatively recent study of particular interest used
ments using both air- and bone-conducted noise of different MEG to compare and contrast left versus right auditory
frequency ranges to identify which one of many possible association areas of the brain (Salmelin et al., 1998).
combinations is the most effective in ameliorating stutter- Participants were asked to read silently, read while mouth-
ing. Based on their results, Cherry and Sayers theorized ing but silent, read aloud individually, and read in chorus.
that PWS have abnormal auditory feedback. They thought The stuttering speakers were disfluent during reading aloud
that the noise works because it neutralizes the defective alone but fluent during choral reading. Brain activation
auditory feedback. Therefore, they concluded, stuttering is a levels (and latencies) in the right and left hemispheres in
perceptual problem. On the other hand, Wingate (1969) response to tones delivered to each ear during these tasks
suggested that noise reduces stuttering because it causes were monitored. Higher activation typically occurs opposite
motoric alterations, such as changes in vocalization. Yairi to the ear being stimulated, providing a contralateral
(1976) compared monaural and binaural noise and con- response. Both groups performed similarly in terms of
cluded that the psychologic explanation does not hold, that timing of activation (latency). But, the pattern of activation
the motor explanation receives some support, and that of left and right auditory association areas differed between
perceptual difficulties are possible factors. Hall and Jerger the stuttering and the normally fluent speakers. As ex-
(1978) administered a number of auditory function tests to pected, all speakers showed higher contralateral responses

Ambrose: Theoretical Perspectives on the Cause of Stuttering 83

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


in general, and during the speaking tasks, there was there seems to be a higher incidence of the co-occurrence
reduced activation. For all of the tasks, however, there was of stuttering and phonological disorders, which led to
higher activation in the stuttering speakers, with the excep- speculations about a mechanism explaining both. Postma
tion of right hemisphere activation to right ear stimuli. The and Kolk (1993) introduced the covert repair hypothesis,
second notable difference occurred in the reading aloud which explains not just stuttering, but all types of dis-
condition, the only task where stuttering speakers were fluencies, as self-repairs. Monitoring and error detection
disfluent. During this task, the balance of left hemisphere can occur at the prearticulatory level. Disfluencies are seen
activation by the left stimulus (left ipsilateral) was de- as by-products of covert repairs of internal speech errors, at
creased, and approached the level of the control group. The the level of phonological encoding. When correction is
authors concluded that interhemispheric balance in the successful, no error appears, but it may impede progress of
auditory cortex appears to be unstable in PWS. an utterance, thereby leading to disfluency. Normal speakers
As studies have become more and more specific and perform covert repairs of phonological encoding. Therefore,
sophisticated, differences in the auditory systems of PWS PWS may have a deficit in phonological encoding, leading
have continued to be evident. All of the studies to date to more frequent phonological encoding errors, which must
have been conducted with adults, and so it is not possible frequently be repaired, which leads to stuttering. More recent
to separate what may be effects due to having stuttered for work has argued this theory (Yaruss & Conture, 1996). Other
years from brain function as it was at the onset of stutter- work has been directed toward linguistic complexity. As
ing. In addition, it is always important to remember that mean length of unit (MLU) and syntactic complexity
there is no evidence that stutterers form a homogeneous increase, stuttering is more frequent (Logan & Conture,
group, and perhaps future studies may reduce or eliminate 1995; Yaruss, 1999). It has also been shown that adults
some of the contradictions found in studies by identifying who stutter may have more difficulty producing unfamiliar
subtypes of stuttering. Any theory of stuttering, then, must vocabulary fluently (Hubbard & Prins, 1994).
take the auditory system into account. Bosshardt (2002) designed a study to measure the effects
Central processing and sensorimotor integration: of cognitive interference on the speech of PWS. Two
A look inside the brain. groups, normally fluent and stuttering participants, were
Differential hemispheric activation. In the 1930s, Travis asked to repeat sequences of three words as fast as possible
(1931/1978) expounded on a theory of the involvement of (primary task) while simultaneously reading or memorizing
the brain in stuttering, claiming that stuttering resulted two additional words presented visually (secondary task).
from incomplete cerebral dominance or, more precisely, The additional words were either phonologically similar or
from “general reduction in cortical lead control” (p. 276). dissimilar to those of the primary task. They were then
The brains of PWS have been under scrutiny continuously asked to recall those words. Accuracy was measured for
from that time. As technology has improved, documentation recollection of the similar and dissimilar words that had
of functional differences in brain activity in normally fluent been presented visually. For repetition of the sequence of
speakers versus those who stutter has become available, three words, accuracy and frequency of disfluency were
although results were often conflicting and thus inconclu- measured. For the control group, the concurrent reading
sive. For example, some EEG studies showed abnormalities task had no significant effect on the fluency of their
in hemispheric processing in stutterers in that they used the repetitions, but the stuttering group had significantly more
right hemisphere more than normally fluent speakers did disfluencies when reading the list of similar words. In other
(Fitch & Batson, 1989; Moore & Haynes, 1980); other words, their fluency-generating system was more easily
studies had negative results (Pinsky & McAdam, 1980). perturbed by, or subject to interference from, the concurrent
Age of participants and stimuli varied from study to study, cognitive task. The elegant design of the study provided a
which may explain the conflicting findings. A better way to isolate the effect of a very specific element—that of
explanation may be that PWS are a heterogeneous group, phonological similarity. This does not rule out, however,
and not knowing how to subgroup them leads to very susceptibility to interference of a host of other cognitive,
inconclusive findings. auditory, motor, or affective processes. Some of the brain
Differences in hemispheric activation have been docu- imaging studies lend support to the hypothesis that phono-
mented more recently with imaging techniques. Several logical planning is aberrant in PWS, or at least in a sub-
independent research groups have reported greater use of group of them. DeNil, Kroll, Kapur, and Houle (1998), and
the right hemisphere in speech and language in PWS as Ingham et al. (2000) reported clear differences in activation
compared to normally fluent speakers (see Ingham, 2001 of the anterior insula in the frontal lobe, which is presumed
for a review of findings); that is to say, language is more to involve phonological planning. Another study, however,
bilaterally represented. Although an early study (Pool, did not find such differences (Braun et al., 1997).
Devous, Freeman, Watson, & Finitzo, 1991) reported that Sensorimotor integration. Brain activation levels can be
left blood flow in the left hemisphere was overall reduced studied using functional imaging techniques such as PET
compared to the right, areas of greatest difference were and functional MRI. These types of studies require a
those of auditory association and motor planning. complex design and can be difficult to interpret. Tasks must
Language. Another area within the purview of central be clearly differentiated theoretically and physiologically—a
processing is the involvement of language in stuttering. resting state, or baseline, is needed from which to measure
There have been investigations of phonology, syntax, activation or deactivation. It is not easy to determine
semantics, and cognitive processing. It has been noted that exactly what strategies are used or what functions are

84 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 80–91 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


accessed or taxed when someone is instructed to view a were better able to discern the complexities of response
shape versus read a word silently versus read a word aloud differences. Their results indicated that reaction times for
versus generate a synonym and so on. A newer technique both stuttering and control groups increased with either
yields indirect information on function through visualization longer utterances or minimal preparation time, but in-
of structure. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) allows creased more for the stuttering group. They concluded that
measurement of the degree of organization of white matter, a difficulty in motor programming for speech might
or myelin tracts, in the brain. The myelin sheaths are what underlie stuttering.
neural impulses are conducted through, and the more white Other recent studies have begun to examine aspects of
matter and higher organization, the better the signal. A reaction time. Van Lieshout, Hulstijn, and Peters (1996)
strong signal indicates a high degree of connectivity. Areas tested whether or not PWS have difficulty assembling an
used for comprehension, planning, and production of speech abstract motor plan by having speakers name pictures
typically show a high degree of connectivity in the left representing one-, two-, and three-syllable words. They
hemisphere for normal speakers. Sommer, Koch, Paulus, measured reaction time as well as duration of the spoken
Weiller, and Buchel (2002) studied the brains of 15 adults word. If PWS have difficulty in building a motor plan, then
who stutter and 15 control speakers. They reported that the they should take more time for words that are longer, as
stuttering group showed significantly less fiber coherence, compared to normally fluent speakers. They found that the
or myelin organization, in the area of the left rolandic stuttering speakers had slower reaction times for all of the
operculum, which contains connections from the temporal words, but the gap between their reaction times and those
to frontal lobes in areas subserving speech. The arcuate of the fluent speakers remained static—that is, in both
fasciculus, simplistically identified decades ago as the groups, performance did not appreciably change as word
bridge between Wernicke’s and Broca’s area, is included in length grew. This evidence refutes the motor plan assembly
this area. If indeed it were such a connection that fails to hypothesis, and so contrasts with studies described in the
perform smoothly and automatically in PWS, one would previous section that reported that central processing load
expect that the areas that it connects might show lower interferes with speech output. Overall, most studies point to
activation than normal. The authors further speculated that generally slower laryngeal, oral, and manual reaction times
overactivation of the right hemisphere in PWS may for stutterers, although there are inconsistencies.
represent a compensatory strategy. Execution of speech, as opposed to reaction times, has
Motor planning and execution. Could stuttering be due also been examined in PWS. Many methods have been
to a strictly motoric deficit? Zimmermann (1980) conducted used. Muscle activity and movement can be examined via
articulatory dynamics experiments to explore this question. electromyography (EMG), or second formant (F2) transi-
In the speech of adults who stutter, he described slower tion, where articulator movement is indirectly represented
and asynchronous movements in their fluent speech, and in the acoustic signal. Voice onset time (VOT) can be used
aberrant positioning of articulators during stuttered speech. to measure laryngeal movement. In studies of speech
Zimmermann concluded that stuttering was a disorder of execution, investigators can choose to examine either
movement, and explained that as we speak, there is a stuttered or fluent speech of PWS and compare it to the
temporal and/or spatial range of movement that is tolerable speech of people who are normally fluent. Freeman and
to the system. As long as the speaker remains within that Ushijima (1978) and Shapiro (1980) found evidence of
range, there is no fluency breakdown. If, however, the muscle activity of antagonistic pairs of laryngeal muscles
range is exceeded, due to emotional, perceptual, and/or during stuttering. This was thought to be at the root of
physiological events, the system is thrown off balance with moments of stuttering involving laryngeal closure. Smith,
conflicting signals to and from the brain at a reflex level, Denny, and Wood (1991), however, looked at the speech of
leading to oscillations (repetitions) and fixations (blocks normally fluent individuals and, surprisingly, found that they
and prolongations). too showed activation of both adductor and abductor
Additional studies examined the motor system as laryngeal muscles, negating the findings of the earlier
reflected in the planning, initiation, and execution stages of studies. Conture, Schwartz, and Brewer (1985) also exam-
speech. A broad range of studies generally found slower ined laryngeal behavior during stuttering using fiber optics,
reaction times for individuals who stutter as compared to finding inappropriate opening and closing of vocal folds
normally fluent speakers for at least some tasks. Prosek, during repetitions and prolongations, indicating difficulty in
Montgomery, Walden, and Schwartz (1979) found differ- motor control of the larynx during stuttering. Another
ences in acoustic, but not manual or laryngeal, reaction interesting set of studies (Alfonso, 1991) focused on the
times. Similarly, Reich, Till, and Goldsmith (1981) con- spatial and temporal dynamics of articulator (lip, tongue,
cluded that PWS had longer initiation times for speech and jaw) movements in the fluent speech of PWS. Results
vocalization, but not for nonspeech vocalization, or manual indicated that articulatory targets were accurately reached,
tasks. Borden (1983) had participants count out loud and but articulators attained targets in a less than efficient
count silently using their fingers and also found those who manner in terms of the relative timing of their movements.
stutter to be slower, but in the execution stage, rather than Webster (1988, 1989) looked at manual motor perfor-
the initiation stage. In Peters, Hulstijn, and Starkweather’s mance of PWS, their ability both to write with both hands
(1989) study of acoustic and physiological reaction times, simultaneously and to perform finger-tapping tasks. In
the conditions under which normally fluent and stuttering general, he found that the participants who stutter made
people spoke were varied. By changing conditions, they more errors and were slower in their performance than

Ambrose: Theoretical Perspectives on the Cause of Stuttering 85

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


normally fluent participants, and in addition they were group, do not exhibit pervasive deficits in speech motor
more susceptible to interference when they were asked to function, but instead may exhibit different thresholds for
perform a second manual motor task simultaneous with perturbation.
finger tapping. Thus, these conclusions, similar to motor Ideally, we would like to follow children from birth
speech studies, give an indication that more than just through their stuttering development, but this is extremely
speech is involved. difficult. Another method of getting at such information is
Most recently, Max, Caruso, and Gracco (2003) contin- to compare traits of people who stuttered only as children
ued to examine neuromotor skills in speech and across with those of individuals who continue to stutter in
other motor systems. This study combined features of a adulthood. Traits that are the same would likely have been
number of earlier studies. Data were obtained for three present close to onset of stuttering, and those that are
motor systems—speech, orofacial nonspeech, and finger absent in the recovered people but present in the persistent
movement—and in each system, four movement sequences people may represent traits that normalized during recovery
varying in length and complexity were tested. The use of from stuttering. Forster and Webster (2001) used this logic
nonspeech domains allows examination of skill levels that to explore the idea that those who recovered from stutter-
would not have been affected by either treatment for ing outgrew a deficit in speech motor control. (They also
stuttering or years of adaptation to the disorder. If the addressed interhemispheric anomalies, as discussed in the
deficit underlying stuttering extends to more than speech, previous section on central processing.) The authors
then PWS would be expected to perform more poorly designed tasks to test the function of the supplementary
across the motor systems as compared to normally fluent motor area (SMA), which has been implicated as being
speakers, and, as the task conditions become more difficult, involved in stuttering. Three groups of 24 adults partici-
their performance might be expected to deteriorate more pated—PWS, people who stuttered only in childhood, and
quickly. The movements were motorically comparable and people who never stuttered. There were two procedures
involved both flexion (closing) and extension (opening). designed to test the integrity of the SMA—a finger-tapping
Kinematic analyses revealed sharp contrasts in the closing task and a bimanual crank turning task. The recovered
versus opening movements for the stuttering versus the group performed similarly to the control group, but the
fluent participants. For closing movements, in the speech persistent group had poorer skills. The authors interpreted
and finger movement tasks, the group who stutter had this to mean that for those who recovered, the SMA had
statistically significantly longer duration and longer peak matured, but the SMAs of the persistent individuals
velocity latency than the fluent group. Opening movements continued to function less than optimally. This type of
were not significantly different between the groups. In information leads us much further into the realms of
orofacial nonspeech movements, the same trend appeared possible mechanisms not only at the root of stuttering, but
but was not statistically significantly different. The analysis also those prompting recovery.
of subcomponents of the movements allowed the distinct The Fox et al. (1996) study, mentioned earlier in the
differences to be observable without being watered down section concerning central processing, found overactivation
by components that were not different. across wide areas in the right hemisphere of PWS during
When adults who stutter are examined, it is not possible speaking, notably in the SMA, premotor areas, and cerebel-
to tell if any differences found represent a deficit that lum. When the individuals who stutter read in chorus and
caused stuttering or are due to having stuttered for years, were fluent, many of the differences in their brain activa-
or even are unrelated to stuttering at all. Most past tion patterns were reduced or even disappeared. Other
investigations of young children’s speech production imaging studies have confirmed activation abnormalities in
concluded that if differences exist between children who motor planning areas (see Ingham, 2001).
stutter and nonstuttering children in motor speech produc- The motor component of stuttering clearly must be
tion, they must be subtle (see review by Conture, 1991). central to any theory of stuttering, as the cardinal symptom
Some differences, however, were observed in comparisons of stuttering is motoric. It would appear that PWS have a
of temporal relationships between and among related events generalized motor deficit, extending to more than speech,
rather than for singular speech variables (Zebrowski, but not perceptible in any behavior other than speech.
Conture, & Cudahy, 1985). Recent research among very Studies have documented slower reaction times, longer
young stuttering children has provided additional indica- durations of motor behaviors, and effects becoming more
tions of possible speech motor difficulties present close to apparent in more difficult behaviors, such as closing versus
stuttering onset. Hall and Yairi (1992) reported that opening movements.
stuttering children differed from normally fluent peers in Genetics. Woven through some of the literature on
the shimmer and jitter parameters of vocal fold vibration. physiologic and biologic aspects of PWS and stuttering are
Also, Hall, Amir, and Yairi (1999) found that preschool references to genetics (see a brief review by Yairi &
children near onset of stuttering had slower speaking rates Ambrose, 2002). Published research first appeared in the
in comparison with normally fluent controls. In fact, other 1930s, when Bryngelson and Rutherford (1937) reported
investigators reported that even 1 year before stuttering that the incidence of stuttering among relatives of stuttering
onset, children’s speech contained imperceptible speech probands is higher than in the population at large. Early
aberrations such as faster speaking rate than children who studies focused on family incidence and personal character-
did not develop stuttering (Kloth, Janssen, Kraaimaat, & istics, such as handedness, of PWS (Bryngelson & Ruther-
Brutten, 1995). Perhaps young children who stutter, as a ford, 1937). From the 1930s to 1990s, a number of

86 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 80–91 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


investigations (e.g., Andrews, Morris-Yates, Howie, & stuttering. Ambrose et al. (1997) focused on possible
Martin, 1991; Felsenfeld et al., 2000; Godai, Tatarelli, & subtypes of stuttering and performed separate segregation
Bonanni, 1976; Howie, 1981) explored twinning and analyses for the families of children who recovered from
stuttering. In all, the rate of concordance for stuttering stuttering (excluding persistent stuttering relatives) and the
(both stuttered) was higher for monozygotic (identical) than families of children who persisted in stuttering (excluding
dizygotic (fraternal) twins, indicating a strong genetic recovered stuttering relatives). Both analyses yielded the
factor. But, if stuttering was completely governed by single highest likelihood for the presence of a major gene
genetics, then if one identical twin stuttered, his or her component (SML) in addition to MFP contributions.
twin would also stutter, and that is not the case—the rate is Two additional findings were remarkable. First, the
considerably less than 100, revealing the existence of parameters for the SML were almost identical in each
strong environmental factors. analysis, indicating that this component involves (at least)
Andrews and Harris (1964) published the first statistical one major gene that is present in both forms of stuttering,
analysis of the pattern of expression of stuttering in persistent and recovered. But, for the MFP parameters, the
families. They found a higher risk of stuttering for relatives heritability component (phenotypic variation attributed to a
of females who stutter, and documented that more males polygenic, additive effect) differed. Thus, the Ambrose et al.
than females stutter. Kidd (1984) confirmed these findings, 1997 study advanced knowledge about the role of genetics a
determining that susceptibility to stuttering was transmitted step further. The results showed that not only does the initial
vertically, that is, generation to generation, with sex- expression of stuttering have strong genetic components,
modified expression, consistent with genetically controlled but also that its developmental course is influenced by
traits. They concluded that males are more susceptible to genetics. Specifically, the authors showed that children who
stuttering, with females having a higher threshold requiring stutter and who have a familial history of chronic stuttering
more factors for stuttering to be expressed. Ambrose, Yairi, would tend to follow that same pattern. And vice versa,
and Cox (1993) reported different conclusions, finding that children who stutter but have a familial tendency of
the risk to relatives of males and females who stutter is recovered stuttering would tend to follow that pattern.
equal. Their sample, however, was very different from that In summary, it is very likely that both forms of stuttering
of the Yale study (Kidd, 1984) in that it consisted of very share a common major gene, contributions of additional
young children who stutter who were followed for a period genes, and environmental factors that are unique to the
of years. Thus, the sample was representative of popula- individual. It may be that persistent stuttering arises more
tions of both those who recovered and those who persisted from additional genetic components, whereas recovered
in stuttering. stuttering may be less genetically controlled, with evident
Several studies have used statistical analyses (segregation but less prominent family history. In addition, it is important
analyses) to predict what genetic model of transmission fits to remember that the division of stuttering into persistent
the observed data best. There are several models to be and recovered subtypes could be a false dichotomy, and that
tested: there might be a better way to classify types of stuttering.
• nongenetic, random, or only environmental Currently, however, this appears to be the best lead.
• major contribution of one gene: single major locus The statistical evidence described above has laid the
(SML) groundwork for linkage analysis. Linkage analysis works by
identifying alleles for certain known marker genes on each
• polygenic (many genes)
chromosome that are co-inherited with the disorder in
• multifactorial–polygenic (MFP): several or many genes question. When a marker gene is co-inherited with stutter-
AND environmental factors ing, it means that the gene contributing to stuttering is on
Andrews and Harris (1964) found that stuttering was the same chromosome as the marker gene; in fact, very
most likely accounted for either by polygenic inheritance or close to it. These regions can then be studied further to
by a dominant gene in conjunction with MFP components. identify specific genes involved in the transmission of
The Yale studies by Kidd and colleagues (Cox, Kramer, & stuttering. Cox (2000) reported the results of the first
Kidd, 1984; Kidd, 1984) reported that the data were most complete standard genome-wide screen of DNA markers for
consistent with either an MFP model or an MFP plus major analysis of stuttering performed on members of the
locus model. More recently, Andrews et al. (1991) and Hutterite population in North Dakota, all descendants of a
Felsenfeld et al. (2000), using the Australian Twin Registry, single genealogy. In this groundbreaking study, Cox
determined that 70% of the heritability was due to additive mentioned sites on chromosomes 1, 13, and 17 as possible
genetic effect and 30% to nonshared environmental effect. locations of genes underlying stuttering. One other linkage
Contributing environmental factors are those that are study has been published with weak evidence for linkage to
nonshared, or unique to the individual, rather than those several chromosomes (Shugart et al., 2003), and a third one
that are shared by the family. by the Illinois group is currently close to completion.
All of the studies discussed above were based almost
exclusively on adults with persistent stuttering. The genetic
studies conducted by the Illinois group (Ambrose, Cox, &
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS
Yairi, 1997; Ambrose, Yairi, & Cox, 1993) used a unique
sample in that it is truly representative of stuttering in The question used to be, Is stuttering a motor disorder? A
general by including both persistent and recovered motor speech disorder? A language disorder? A genetic

Ambrose: Theoretical Perspectives on the Cause of Stuttering 87

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


disorder? A psychologic disorder? A learned behavior? becomes central processing-language-cognitive processes
Well, the answer is probably yes, to all. It all depends on and where motor planning begins. All of these areas are
what you are trying to explain. More simply put, What is interconnected, and a problem at one site will create a
stuttering a symptom OF? There has been great progress in cascade effect prompting aberrancies downstream. It is not
many areas, and soon researchers will begin to link up as simple as finding an electrical problem in a motor,
their findings. The multidisciplinary approach is absolutely where one can systematically check the battery, the
necessary, although contributions can be made within any generator, the distributor, the spark plugs, and the wiring
specific field. How can we put all of this information that connects them.
together to devise a well-formed theory? We strive to Although there may be, and probably are, subtypes of
delineate the necessary and sufficient factors, differentiating stuttering, there does appear to be commonality in the
cause versus contributing factor versus result. It has become underlying genetically orchestrated deficit. In PWS, perhaps
clear that we cannot account for the existing data using one neural development, under genetic influence, is uneven, and
schema over another, but we must integrate them. Previous systems subserving speech and language do not mature in
comprehensive frameworks, such as Smith and Kelly’s the right order at the right time. It is known that the two
(1997) multifactorial dynamic model, the demands and hemispheres of the brain proceed differently in their
capacities model (Starkweather & Gottwald, 1990), or maturation (Kent, 1999). The best bet may be that there is
Bernstein Ratner’s (1997) trade-off hypothesis, have much to a deficit in the left hemisphere affecting both auditory and
offer but do not incorporate specifics as described in many motor functions, and that the right hemisphere perhaps
of the studies discussed here. The Smith and Kelly model attempts to compensate, and one or more parts (subsystems)
posits that it is the interaction of a number of components of the complex multilevel sensory and motor system
that underlies fluency breakdown, and that the contribution responsible for the planning and orderly execution of fluent
of each component will vary from individual to individual speech are fragile and easily perturbed. In persistent
and over time within an individual. The demands and stuttering, the left hemisphere system may be wired
capacities model merely states that when demands exceed differently and less efficiently, but may attempt to develop
capacities, breakdown occurs, whereas the trade-off coping strategies, compensating with the right hemisphere.
hypothesis is based on uneven resource allocation, so that Those who stutter mildly and/or occasionally have success-
if resources are diverted for a challenging task, other fully developed organized wiring mechanisms to circumvent
functions may suffer. Because these models are general, the problem areas. Those who stutter consistently and/ or
they lack explanatory relevance and are difficult to test. severely manage to use available pathways but cannot
They were not devised to answer specifics but to provide a maintain and/or develop consistent new efficient wiring.
framework for more intricate and testable hypotheses. (See discussion in Yairi and Ambrose, in press). The neural
We are not yet at a point to propose an encompassing system of a child who fully and naturally recovers from
detailed model of the cause of stuttering. It is safe to say stuttering may develop unevenly but become indistinguish-
that stuttering involves, at its ultimate origin, a complex able from that of a child who is normally fluent. The
but strong genetic component affecting the fluency- challenge now is to create testable hypotheses to continue
generating system of the brain, but this does not narrow the to narrow down specifics. It is an exciting time, and it is
possibilities as much as one might think. Unfortunately, we not unreasonable to expect to see exciting results around
do not yet know what such genes actually do, how they the corner.
interact with each other, or how they interact with the Science has come full circle, but the spiral has tightened.
environment. The ultimate question is, What are the types In his discussion of the cerebral dominance theory, Travis
of genes that could cause the types of effects in the brain (1931/1978) wrote that stuttering “reflects a certain lack of
that could cause the types of aberrant brain activity that maturation of the central nervous system which either does
could cause the types of stuttering symptoms? Researchers not afford integration of the highest neurophysiologic levels
in fluency are trying determine how we go from a major involved in speech or predisposes these levels to disintegra-
gene or two, some additional genes, and nonshared environ- tion by various types of exogenous or endogenous stimuli”
mental factors to a fragile fluency-generating system that is (p. 277). This sounds very much like the current position
susceptible to breakdowns related to/from factors in the on the etiology of stuttering—but now we are accumulating
spheres of linguistic complexity, time pressure, excitement the concrete evidence to support it.
or anxiety, among a host of other specifics, susceptible to
interference. This mechanism must also explain how so Relevance to Evidence-Based Practice
many children stutter even severely and yet recover
completely without formal intervention, whereas others One’s perspective on the cause of stuttering perforce drives
stutter lifelong in spite of treatment. one’s therapeutic approach. The current thrust toward
Is there a specific location, or specific locations, where the evidence-based practice demands that clinicians take a
process goes awry? Are all of the components in good position and provide an objective rationale for their
working order, but their intercommunication does not treatment regimens. This does not mean that the art of
proceed smoothly enough to allow fluent speech? Does the clinical judgment or client desires must be replaced by
most primal deficit lie in auditory processing, or central scientific findings, as Bothe (2003) pointed out. Art and
processing, or speech planning? It is not easy, and probably science are not in opposition; rather, they should be
not overly important, to determine where auditory processing incorporated into a unity, or consilience, of knowledge (cf.

88 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 80–91 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Starkweather, 2002). Not only is the nature–nurture debate Blanton, S. (1931). Stuttering. Mental Hygiene, 15, 271–282.
pointless and artificial, the division between researchers and Blood, G., & Blood, I. (1984). Central auditory function in young
clinicians is equally fruitless. As Wilson (1998) stated, in stutterers. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 59, 699–705.
reference to science and literature, “the way to unite…is to Blood, G., & Blood, I. (1989). Multiple data analysis of dichotic
view the boundary…not as a territorial line but as a broad listening advantages of stutterers. Journal of Fluency Disorders,
and mostly unexplored terrain awaiting cooperative entry 14, 97–107.
from both sides” (p. 137). Demosthenes was a philosopher Bloodstein, O. (1958). Stuttering as anticipatory struggle reaction.
and did not write a treatise on the amelioration of stutter- In J. Eisenson (Ed.), Stuttering: A symposium (pp. 1–70). New
ing in the presence of noise, nor did he conduct a scientific York: Harper & Row.
experiment in this regard. But, his self-therapy could still Bloodstein, O. (1995). A handbook on stuttering (5th ed.). San
be considered evidence based, using the best quality Diego, CA: Singular.
information available. Borden, G. (1983). Initiation vs. execution time during manual
Understanding the present state of affairs of stuttering and oral counting by stutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing
etiology theories is not a dry mental exercise, nor a mandate Research, 26, 389–396.
from the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. As Bosshardt, H.-G. (2002). Effects of concurrent cognitive process-
a scientist, it is a privilege and a duty to provide objective ing on the fluency of word repetition: Comparison between
insight that might drive treatment strategies, and as a persons who do and do not stutter. Journal of Fluency Disor-
clinician, it is a privilege and a duty to artfully incorporate ders, 27, 93–114.
scientific findings for the benefit of PWS. Bothe, A. (2003). Evidence-based treatment of stuttering: V. The
art of clinical practice and the future of clinical research.
Journal of Fluency Disorders, 28, 247–258.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT Braun, A. R., Varga, M., Stager, S., Schulz, G., Selbie, S.,
Maisog, J. M., et al. (1997). Altered patterns of cerebral
A portion of the research described here was supported by the activity during speech and language production in developmental
National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders stuttering: An H215O positron emission tomography study. Brain,
Grant R01 DC05210, Principal Investigator: Ehud Yairi. 120, 761–784.
Brill, A. (1923). Speech disturbances in nervous and mental
diseases. Quarterly Journal of Speech Education, 9, 129–135.
REFERENCES Brutten, G. J., & Shoemaker, D. (1967). The modification of
stuttering. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Alfonso, P. (1991). Implications of the concepts underlying task-
dynamic modeling on kinematic studies of stuttering. In H. Bryngelson, B., & Rutherford, B. (1937). A comparative study of
Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor laterality of stutterers and non-stutterers. Journal of Speech
control and stuttering (pp. 79–100). Amsterdam: Elsevier Disorders, 2, 15–16.
Science. Cherry, C., & Sayers, B. (1956). Experiments upon the total
Ambrose, N., Cox, N., & Yairi, E. (1997). The genetic basis of inhibition of stammering by external control and some clinical
persistence and recovery in stuttering. Journal of Speech, results. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 1, 233–246.
Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 567–580. Conture, E., Schwartz, H., & Brewer, D. (1985). Laryngeal
Ambrose, N., & Yairi, E. (1999). Normative disfluency data for behavior during stuttering: A further study. Journal of Speech
early childhood stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and and Hearing Research, 28, 233–240.
Hearing Research, 42, 895–909. Conture, E. G. (1991). Young stutterers’ speech production: A
Ambrose, N. G., Yairi, E., & Cox, N. (1993). Genetic aspects of critical review. In H. F. M. Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C. W.
early childhood stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering:
Research, 36, 701–706. Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Speech Motor
Anderson, J., Pellowski, M., Conture, E., & Kelly, E. (2003). Control and Stuttering (pp. 365–384). New York: Elsevier.
Temperamental characteristics of young children who stutter. Coriat, I. (1928). Stammering: A psychoanalytic interpretation.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, Nervous & Mental Disorders Monographs, Series #47, 1–68.
1221–1233. Cox, N. (2000, November). Genetics of stuttering: Insights and
Andrews, G., & Harris, M. (1964). The syndrome of stuttering: recent advances. Paper presented at the annual convention of
Clinics in developmental medicine, No. 17. London: Spastics the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Washing-
Society Medical Education and Information Unit in association ton, DC.
with Wm. Heinemann Medical Books. Cox, N., Kramer, P., & Kidd, K. (1984). Segregation analyses of
Andrews, G., Morris-Yates, A., Howie, P., & Martin, N. G. stuttering. Genetic Epidemiology, 1, 245–253.
(1991). Genetic factors in stuttering confirmed. Archives of Craig, A., Hancock, K., Tran, Y., & Craig, M. (2003). Anxiety
General Psychiatry, 48, 1034–1035. levels in people who stutter: A randomized population study.
Bernstein Ratner, N. (1997). Stuttering: A psycholinguistic Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46,
perspective. In R. F. Curlee & G. M. Siegel (Eds.), Nature and 1197–1206.
treatment of stuttering: New directions (2nd ed., pp. 99–127). DeNil, L., Kroll, R., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). A positron
Boston: Allyn & Bacon. emission tomography study of silent and oral single word
Berry, M. F. (1938). A common denominator in twinning and reading in stuttering and nonstuttering adults. Journal of Speech,
stuttering. Journal of Speech Disorders, 3, 51–57. Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 1038–1053.

Ambrose: Theoretical Perspectives on the Cause of Stuttering 89

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Felsenfeld, S., Kirk, K. M., Zhu, G., Statham, D. J., Neale, M. functional development. In A. Caruso & E. Strand (Eds.),
C., & Martin, N. G. (2000). A study of the genetic and Clinical management of motor speech disorders in children (pp.
environmental etiology of stuttering in a selected twin sample. 46–62). New York: Thieme.
Behavior Genetics, 30, 359–366. Kidd, K. K. (1984). Stuttering as a genetic disorder. In R. F.
Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New Curlee & W. H. Perkins (Eds.), Nature and treatment of
York: W. W. Norton. stuttering: New directions (pp. 149–169). San Diego, CA:
Fitch, J., & Batson, E. (1989). Hemispheric asymmetry of alpha College-Hill.
wave suppression in stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Kloth, S. A., Janssen, P., Kraaimaat, F. W., & Brutten, G. J.
Fluency Disorders, 14, 47–55. (1995). Speech-motor and linguistic skills of young stutterers
Flanagan, B., Goldiamond, I., & Azrin, N. (1958). Operant prior to onset. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 20, 157–170.
stuttering: The control of stuttering behavior through response- Logan, K., & Conture, E. (1995). Length, grammatical complex-
contingent consequences. Journal of Experimental Analyses of ity, and rate differences in stuttered and fluent conversational
Behavior, 1, 173–177. utterances of children who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders,
Forster, D., & Webster, W. (2001). Speech-motor control and 20, 35–61.
interhemispheric relations in recovered and persistent stuttering. Max, L., Caruso, A., & Gracco, V. (2003). Kinematic analyses of
Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 125–145. speech, orofacial nonspeech, and finger movements in stuttering
Foundas, A. L., Bollich, A. M., Corey, D. M., Hurley, M., & and nonstuttering adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Heilman, K. M. (2001). Anomalous anatomy of speech-language Hearing Research, 46, 215–232.
areas in adults with persistent developmental stuttering. Moore, W. (1938). A conditioned reflex study of stuttering.
Neurology, 57, 207–215. Journal of Speech Disorders, 3, 163–183.
Fox, P. T., Ingham, R. J., Ingham, J. C., Hirsch, T. B., Downs, Moore, W., & Haynes, W. (1980). Alpha hemispheric asymmetry
J. H., Martin, C., et al. (1996). A PET study of the neural and stuttering: Some support for a segmentation dysfunction
systems of stuttering. Nature, 382, 158–162. hypothesis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23,
Freeman, F., & Ushijima, T. (1978). Laryngeal muscle activity 229–247.
during stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 21, Onslow, M., Packman, A., & Harrison, E. (2001). The Lidcombe
538–562. Program of Early Stuttering Intervention: A clinician’s guide.
Glauber, P. J. (1958). Stuttering and personality dynamics. In J. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Eisenson (Ed.), Stuttering: A symposium (pp. 71–120). New Palmer, M., & Gillette, A. (1938). Sex differences in the cardiac
York: Harper & Row. rhythms of stutterers. Journal of Speech Disorders, 3, 3–12.
Godai, U., Tatarelli, R., & Bonanni, G. (1976). Stuttering and Perkins, W. H. (1990). What is stuttering? Journal of Speech and
tics in twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 25, Hearing Disorders, 55(3), 370–382.
369–375.
Peters, H., Hulstijn, W., & Starkweather, C. (1989). Acoustic
Goodstein, L. (1958). Functional speech disorders and personality: and physiological reaction times of stutterers and nonstutterers.
A survey of the research. Journal of Speech and Hearing Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 668–680.
Research, 1, 359–376.
Pinsky, S., & McAdam, D. (1980). Electroencephalographic and
Hall, J., & Jerger, J. (1978). Central auditory function in stutterers. dichotic indices of cerebral laterality in stutterers. Brain &
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 21, 324–337. Language, 11, 374–397.
Hall, K. D., Amir, O., & Yairi, E. (1999). A longitudinal Pool, K., Devous, M., Freeman, F., Watson, B., & Finitzo, T.
investigation of speaking rate in preschool children who stutter. (1991). Regional cerebral blood flow in developmental stutterers.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, Archives of Neurology, 48, 509–512.
1367–1377.
Postma, A., & Kolk, H. (1993). The covert repair hypothesis:
Hall, K. D., & Yairi, E. (1992). Fundamental frequency, jitter, and Prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered
shimmer in preschoolers who stutter. Journal of Speech and disfluencies. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36,
Hearing Research, 35, 1002–1008. 472–487.
Howie, P. M. (1981). Concordance for stuttering in monozygotic Prosek, R., Montgomery, A., Walden, B., & Schwartz, D.
and dizygotic twin pairs. Journal of Speech and Hearing (1979). Reaction-time measures of stutterers and nonstutterers.
Research, 24, 317–321. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 4, 269–278.
Hubbard, C., & Prins, D. (1994). Word familiarity, syllabic stress Reich, A., Till, J., & Goldsmith, H. (1981). Laryngeal and
pattern and stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, manual reaction times of stuttering and nonstuttering adults.
37, 564–571. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 192–196.
Ingham, R. (2001). Brain imaging studies of developmental Rosenfield, D., & Goodglass, H. (1980). Dichotic testing of
stuttering. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 493–516. cerebral dominance in stutterers. Brain & Language, 11,
Ingham, R., Fox, P., Ingham, J., & Zamarripa, F. (2000). Is 179–180.
overt stuttered speech a prerequisite for the neural activations Ryan, B. P . (2001). Programmed therapy for stuttering in
associated with chronic developmental stuttering? Brain and children and adults (2nd ed.). Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Language, 75, 163–194. Thomas.
Johnson, W., & Associates. (1959). The onset of stuttering: Salmelin, R., Schnitzler, A., Schmitz, F., Jancke, L., Witte, O.
Research findings and implications. Minneapolis: University of W., & Freund, H-J. (1998). Functional organization of the
Minnesota. auditory cortex is different in stutterers and fluent speakers.
Kent, R. D. (1999). Motor control: Neurophysiology and NeuroReport, 9, 2225–2229.

90 CONTEMPORARY ISSUES IN COMMUNICATION SCIENCE AND DISORDERS • Volume 31 • 80–91 • Spring 2004

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions


Schuell, H. (1946). Sex differences in relation to stuttering: Part I. assembly hypothesis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
Journal of Speech Disorders, 11, 277–298. 39, 76–92.
Shames, G. H., & Sherrick, C. E. (1963). A discussion of Webster, W. (1988). Neural mechanisms underlying stuttering:
nonfluency and stuttering as operant behavior. Journal of Speech Evidence from bimanual handwriting performance. Brain &
and Hearing Disorders, 28, 3–18. Language, 33, 226–244.
Shane, M. (1955). Effect on stuttering of alteration in auditory Webster, G. (1989). Sequence initiation performance by stutterers
feedback. In W. Johnson & R. Leutenegger (Eds.), Stuttering in under conditions of response competition. Brain & Language,
children and adults (pp. 286–297). Minneapolis: University of 36, 286–300.
Minnesota Press. Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New
Shapiro, A. (1980). An electromyographic analysis of the fluent York: Vintage Books.
and dysfluent utterances of several types of stutterers. Journal of Wingate, M. (1964). A standard definition of stuttering. Journal of
Fluency Disorders, 5, 203–231. Speech and Hearing Disorders, 29, 484–489.
Sheehan, J. (1953). Theory and treatment of stuttering as an Wingate, M. (1969). Sound and pattern in “artificial” fluency.
approach-avoidance conflict. Journal of Psychology, 36, 27–49. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 12, 677–686.
Sheehan, J. G. (1958). Projective studies of stuttering. Journal of Wingate, M. (1997). Stuttering: A short history of a curious
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 23, 18–25. disorder. Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey.
Shugart, Y., Mundorff, J., Kilshaw, J., Doheny, K., Doan, B., Wischner, G. J. (1950). Stuttering behavior and learning: A
Wanyee, J., et al. (2003). Results of a genome-wide linkage preliminary theoretical formulation. Journal of Speech and
scan for stuttering. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 124A, Hearing Disorders, 15, 324–335.
133–135.
Yairi, E. (1976). Effects of binaural and monaural noise on
Smith, A., Denny, M., & Wood, J. (1991). Instability in speech stuttering. The Journal of Auditory Research, 16, 114–119.
muscle systems in stuttering. In H. Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C. W.
Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering: Yairi, E., & Ambrose, N. (1999). Early childhood stuttering I:
Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Speech Motor Persistency and recovery rates. Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research 42, 1097–1112.
Control and Stuttering (pp. 231–242). New York: Elsevier.
Smith, A., & Kelly, E. M. (1997). Stuttering: A dynamic, Yairi, E., & Ambrose, N. (2002). Evidence for genetic etiology in
multifactorial model. In R. F. Curlee & G. M. Siegel (Eds.), stuttering. Perspectives, 12(2), 10–15.
Nature and treatment of stuttering: New directions. (2nd ed., pp. Yairi, E., & Ambrose, N. (in press). Early childhood stuttering.
204–217). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Sommer, M., Koch, M. A., Paulus, W., Weiller, C., & Buchel, C. Yaruss, J. S. (1999). Utterance length, syntactic complexity, and
(2002). Disconnection of speech-relevant brain areas in childhood stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
persistent developmental stuttering. Lancet, 360, 380–383. Research, 42, 329–344.
Starkweather, C. (2002). The epigenesis of stuttering. Journal of Yaruss, J. S., & Conture, E. G. (1996). Stuttering and phonologi-
Fluency Disorders, 27, 269–288. cal disorders in children: Examination of the covert repair
Starkweather, C. W., & Gottwald, S. R. (1990). The demands hypothesis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39,
and capacities model: II. Clinical applications. Journal of 349–364.
Fluency Disorders, 15(3), 143–157. Zebrowski, P. M., Conture, E. G., & Cudahy, E. A. (1985).
Travis, L. (1957). The unspeakable feelings of people, with special Acoustic analysis of young stutterers’ fluency: Preliminary
reference to stuttering. In L. Travis (Ed.), Handbook of speech observations. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 10, 173–192.
pathology (pp. 1009–1033). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Zimmermann, G. (1980). Stuttering: A disorder of movement.
Travis, L. (1978). Neurophysiological dominance. Journal of Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23, 122–126.
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 43, 275–277. (Original work
published 1931).
Travis, L., Tuttle, W., & Cowan, D. (1936). A study of the heart
rate during stuttering. Journal of Speech Disorders, 1, 21–26. Contact author: Nicoline Grinager Ambrose, University of
Van Lieshout, P., Hulstijn, W., & Peters, H. (1996). Speech Illinois, Speech and Hearing Science, 901 S. Sixth St., Champaign,
production in people who stutter: Testing the motor plan IL 61820. E-mail: nambrose@uiuc.edu

Ambrose: Theoretical Perspectives on the Cause of Stuttering 91

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 85.153.205.87 on 12/10/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions

You might also like