You are on page 1of 17

InterLibrary Loan Briscoe Library

Article Delivery Service 7703 Floyd Curl Drive


http://www.library.uthscsa.edu/illiad San Antonio, TX 78229
ariel-ill@uthscsa.edu (210) 567-2460
(210) 567-2463 (fax)

Desktop Delivery Cover Sheet


If you receive a copy that is missing pages, smudged, or unreadable, please contact the UT HSC
Libraries at (210) 567-2460 so we may obtain a clean copy for you as quickly as possible.

Notice: Warning concerning copyright restrictions

The copyright law of the United States [Title 17, United States Code] governs the making of
photocopies or other reproductions of copyrighted materials.

Under certain conditions specified in the law, libraries and archives are authorized to furnish a
photocopy or other reproduction. One of these specified conditions is that the reproduction is not to
be used for any purpose other than private study, scholarship, or research. If a user makes a request
for, or later uses, a photocopy or reproduction for purposes in excess of "fair use," that use may be
liable for copyright infringement.

This institution reserves the right to refuse to accept a copying order if, in its judgment, fulfillment of
the order would involve violation of copyright law.

This notice is posted in compliance with


Title 37 C.F.R., Chapter II, Part 201.14
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 1 2000

ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION


ABILITIES OF ADULTS WITH
NEUROGENIC COMMUNICATION
DISORDERS
Laura L. Murray, Ph.D.* and Amy E. Ramage, M.S.†

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
ABSTRACT—Whereas it is known that executive function abilities are often
impaired in clients having neurogenic communication disorders, few assess-
ments of this cognitive domain are available that consider the speech and lan-
guage deficits of this population. This article provides an overview of current
procedures for assessing executive functions including a discussion of team
approaches to assessment, a review of currently available neuropsychological
and functional tests of executive function abilities, as well as a critique of those
assessment procedures. In addition, suggestions are provided for how best to
use or modify appropriately current tests of executive functioning for clients
having acquired speech and language disorders as a result of brain damage.

KEY WORDS: Executive functions, assessment, neurogenic communication


disorders

Executive function abilities refer to a quently, upon clients’ social, vocational,


collection of high-level, interconnected, educational, and rehabilitative outcome
control processes that allow us to generate, (Chen et al., 19984). As Filley5 noted, adults
choose, organize, and regulate our goal- with neurogenic communication disorders
directed, adaptive, and nonautomatic be- such as dementia, traumatic brain injury
haviors (Phillips, 1997;1 Ylvisaker & Feeney, (TBI), right hemisphere damage (RHD),
19982) or, as Lezak (19953) noted, are skills and aphasia may present with varying de-
that “enable a person to engage successfully grees of impairment in a number of execu-
in independent, purposive, self-serving tive functions. Therefore, it is important
behavior” (p. 42). Consequently, executive that the speech-language clinicians who
function impairments can have dire effects work with these individuals be able to iden-
upon the rehabilitation process and, subse- tify the presence, nature, and severity of ex-

*Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, and
†Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona

Reprint requests: Dr. L. L. Murray, Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana
University, Bloomington, IN 47405.

Copyright © 2000 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001,
USA. Tel.: +1(212) 584-4663. 0734-0478,p;2000,21,02,0153,0168,ftx,en;ssl00042x
153
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

ecutive function deficits. To be able to utive functions of our clients is to determine


quantify and qualify executive function who is responsible for this assessment so as to
deficits in adults with neurogenic communi- avoid redundant testing procedures. Ideally,
cation disorders, clinicians must be familiar all team members should directly or indi-
with the variety of formal and informal tests rectly participate in the assessment. Mem-
of executive functions that are currently bers who may be best trained for and most
available. Furthermore, clinicians must be frequently involved in directly assessing exec-
able to interpret the results of these tests in utive function abilities include the speech-
terms of reliability and validity. This is par- language clinician, the neuropsychologist
ticularly true given that theoretical models (or psychologist, school psychologist, etc.),
of executive functions have yet to be explic- and the occupational therapist.

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
itly delineated and that few tests of execu- Prior to administering tests of executive
tive functions can be administered without functioning, speech-language clinicians should
accommodating for the neurogenic com- check with other team members to ensure
munication disorders with which our clients that they are not overstepping professional
present. boundaries. For example, in some states in
The purpose of this article is to intro- the United States and provinces in Canada,
duce clinicians to the variety of procedures only psychologists are licensed to purchase
that may be used to assess the executive and/or administer certain tests of executive
function abilities of adults with neurogenic functioning (as well as tests of other cogni-
communication disorders. First we address tive abilities such as memory or attention).
different degrees of involvement speech- Even under these circumstances, however,
language clinicians might have in examin- speech-language clinicians should be in-
ing executive functions. Next we review volved in the executive function assessment
general types of assessment procedures in- of clients with neurogenic communication
cluding collection of case history informa- disorders because of our unique expertise in
tion and observational and questionnaire evaluating and interacting with this popula-
data, administration of commonly used for- tion of individuals. Whereas other profes-
mal tests of executive functioning, and func- sionals may have had more training and ex-
tional assessment of executive functioning. perience in administering tests of executive
Lastly, we discuss limitations associated with functioning, speech-language clinicians have
the currently available assessment proce- had more training and experience in accom-
dures, as well as provide suggestions for im- modating test procedures for and interpret-
proving tests of executive function abilities ing the test results of adults with speech and
for adults with neurogenic communication language impairments.
disorders.

PROCEDURES FOR ASSESSING


THE SPEECH-LANGUAGE EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
CLINICIAN’S ROLE IN ASSESSING
EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS Generally, the purposes of an executive
function assessment are to identify not only
In most clinical settings, whether they impairments but also strengths and to de-
are educational or medical, a team approach termine whether and how these impair-
to assessment and treatment has been ments and strengths contribute to clients’
adopted. Depending on the team and the daily functioning. Ylvisaker et al. (19986)
clinical setting, a number of team members recommended that a thorough assessment
may be capable of and interested in assessing of executive functioning should “attempt to
the executive function abilities of adults with understand how effectively an individual
neurogenic communication disorders. Con- can size up a possibly confusing situation,
154 sequently, the first step in assessing the exec- decide what to do to achieve meaningful
ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ABILITIES OF ADULTS—MURRAY, RAMAGE

goals, plan how to do it, do it and stick to it perceptual, motoric, or emotional problems
until it is done, pay attention to how effec- can be taken into consideration not only
tively it was done, and identify a clever way when choosing and administering tests of
to do it better if necessary” (p. 223). To executive functions but also when interpret-
achieve these purposes, a number and vari- ing test results (Elias & Treland, 199911).
ety of assessment procedures must be com- The assessment of executive function abili-
pleted including acquiring a case history, ties will also be guided by information per-
conducting observational and interviewing taining to clients’ pre- and postmorbid occu-
procedures, and administering formal tests pation, residential setting (e.g., community
of executive function abilities. dwelling, skilled nursing facility), and sup-
port systems (e.g., family, co-workers, home

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
health care services). Such information can
Case History help clinicians determine the type and ex-
tent of demands that currently are, or will
An important initial step to the evalua- be, placed upon their clients’ executive
tion of executive function abilities is to re- function abilities (Sbordone, 199612).
view the client’s medical history. First, infor-
mation pertaining to the etiology and
location of brain damage should be ob- Observations and Interviews
tained because it may provide insight into
whether assessment of executive function In addition to completing formal test-
abilities will be necessary. For example, ing, it is extremely important for clinicians
frontal lobe damage is highly associated to observe and interact with clients and to
with executive function deficits (Levine et talk with caregivers and other team mem-
al., 1998;7 Shallice & Burgess, 19918). Con- bers to acquire information regarding how
sequently, clients who suffer from traumatic clients interact and function in a variety of
brain injuries, strokes, tumors, or progres- formal and informal social contexts and in
sive diseases (e.g., Parkinson’s disease, the presence of a variety of daily stressors.
Huntington’s disease) that directly or indi- Observational data are critical to evaluating
rectly compromise frontal lobe functioning executive functions because the external
might be expected to present with executive structure that typifies formal testing proce-
function deficits. A negative history of dures may mask the executive function
frontal lobe involvement, however, does not deficits of many clients (Cripe, 1996;13 Dug-
dismiss the possibility of executive function bartey et al., 199914). That is, structured
deficits as other neuroanatomical regions tasks and the controlled environment asso-
(e.g., limbic system, cerebellum) are also as- ciated with formal testing procedures may
sociated with executive function abilities actually provide the type of support that
(Cummings, 1993;9 Schatz, 199810). Addi- many clients need to compensate for their
tional suggestions regarding the identifica- executive function deficits. Furthermore,
tion of pertinent medical history informa- the emotional or personality changes of
tion are provided by Filley.5 clients with executive function deficits are
As part of the case history, clinicians usually not revealed or quantified by their
must also collect information concerning formal test performances (Chen et al.,
the status of their clients’ speech, language, 1998;4 Burgess et al., 199815). As Dugbartey
attention, memory, sensory (e.g., vision, and colleagues (199914) warned, “to rely ex-
hearing), motoric, and mental health func- clusively on summary psychometric scores
tioning (e.g., depression). The results of a in the assessment of executive dysfunctions
recent, formal assessment of these areas of is a fundamental mistake” (pp. 9–10).
functioning should be available and re- To help organize observational sessions
viewed prior to evaluating executive func- and to assist in summarizing observational
tion abilities so that any existing cognitive, data, clinicians can use the commercially 155
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

available Profile of Executive Control Sys- tive or frontal lobe syndrome including
tem (PRO-EX) (Braswell et al., 199216). The changes in emotion or personality (e.g., ag-
PRO-EX is an observational protocol that gressiveness), motivation (e.g., apathy or
involves observing clients perform daily lack of concern), behavior (e.g., impulsive,
tasks and some “set-up” tasks, as well as in- distractible), and cognition (e.g., planning
terviewing clients about their executive difficulties). Sample questions include
function abilities. Included in the PRO-EX “She/he loses his/her temper at the slight-
manual are suggestions regarding unstruc- est thing,” “She/he seems lethargic, or un-
tured settings in which to complete observa- enthusiastic about things,” and “She/he has
tions (e.g., clients’ home, hospital room, difficulty thinking ahead or planning for
hospital cafeteria), tasks to elicit executive the future.” Two versions of the question-

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
function behaviors (e.g., telephone tasks in naire are used, one to be completed by the
which clients are asked to obtain informa- client and one to be completed by a close
tion such as how to get a building permit), caregiver or other team member who has
and general and specific questions to ask frequent daily contact with the client. Par-
when interviewing clients, their caregivers, ticipants are asked to rate each of the behav-
or both. While collecting the observational iors on a scale from 0 (i.e., “never”) to 4
data, clinicians rate their clients’ behaviors (i.e., “very often”) in terms of frequency of
according to seven areas of executive func- occurrence. It is imperative that clinicians
tioning: goal selection, planning/sequenc- not rely solely upon the clients’ self-ratings
ing, initiation, execution, time sense, aware- because individuals with executive function
ness of deficits, and self-monitoring. After deficits commonly underestimate the sever-
all the data have been amassed, clinicians ity of their deficits (Cripe, 1996;13 Burgess et
identify their clients’ highest level of func- al., 1998;15 Wilson et al., 199618). Instead,
tioning in each area according to the levels clinicians can obtain data concerning their
of functioning listed on the PRO-EX Rating clients’ insight by comparing self-ratings
Protocol. For example, in terms of initia- with those of caregivers. Importantly, the
tion, ratings can vary from the lowest level Dysexecutive Questionnaire ratings of care-
in which clients are “Able to initiate only givers have been found to correlate highly
with physical prompting” to the highest with the performances of their brain-dam-
level in which clients “Independently initi- aged relatives on tests of executive function
ate a variety of complex tasks even if they do (Burgess et al., 199815).
not feel motivated” (Braswell et al., 199216, Observations and interviews completed
p. 2). Braswell et al. reported acceptable in- prior to formal assessment procedures can
terrater reliability when the PRO-EX was provide valuable information regarding
used to observe and rate the executive func- which components of executive functioning
tion abilities of 32 clients who had suffered may be problematic for clients and, conse-
either a TBI or a stroke. Tools such as this quently, which formal testing procedures
can help verify the reliability of data ob- clinicians should select. Observational and
tained from fully unstructured observations interviewing methods can also be incorpo-
of executive function abilities. rated into formal testing sessions (Ylvisaker
To help structure interviews with et al., 1998;6 Levine et al., 1998;7 Dughartey
clients, caregivers, or other team members, et al., 199914). For example, prior to admin-
clinicians might use the Executive Interview istering a test clinicians might ask clients to
(Royall et al., 199217) or the Dysexecutive judge whether the test will be easy or diffi-
Questionnaire part of the Behavioral Assess- cult and why and to predict how well they
ment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome test will perform. Similarly, after the test, clients
battery (BADS) (Wilson et al., 199618). The might be asked how well they performed
Dysexecutive Questionnaire documents oc- and what type of strategies they used to
currences of 20 of the most frequently re- complete the test. By including these types
156 ported symptoms associated with dysexecu- of questions, clinicians will obtain not only
ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ABILITIES OF ADULTS—MURRAY, RAMAGE

formal test results but also qualitative infor- tests that evaluate a number of these
mation concerning their clients’ self-aware- processes should be administered to delin-
ness and self-monitoring abilities. eate which executive functions are problem-
atic and will require remediation (Dug-
bartey et al., 1999;14 Burgess et al., 199815).
Formal Tests of Executive Practically speaking, given the current limi-
Function Abilities tations in the amount of time and funding
available for assessment, it is best that for-
A variety of commercially available tests mal testing methods should be preceded by
(Table 1) and experimental procedures observation and interviewing procedures to
(Table 2) may be used to assess the execu- help identify which specific executive func-

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
tive function abilities of adults with neuro- tion behaviors require further, in-depth
genic communication disorders. This as- evaluation.
sortment of assessment tools can be
categorized into tests or batteries that exam- Executive Function Test Batteries
ine various executive functions, tests that
aim to evaluate only one or two executive As of the writing of this article, only
functions, and tests that yield a so-called one test battery, the Behavioral Assessment
functional appraisal of executive function of Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) (Wilson
abilities. Because executive functioning is et al., 199618), is commercially available for
viewed as a corpus of complex, controlling evaluating executive function abilities. An-
processes, whenever possible, several formal other test battery, the Delis-Kaplan Execu-

TABLE 1. Some Commercially-Available Tests of Executive Function Abilities


Tests Source
Observational/interview tool
Profile of Executive Control Systema Braswell et al. (1992)16
Test battery
Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndromea Wilson et al. (1996)18
Selected tests targeting specific abilities
Booklet Category Testb DeFilippis and McCampbell (1997)19
Color Trails Testb D’Elia et al. (1996)20
Colored Progressive Matricesc Raven et al. (1984)21
Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligenced Hammill et al. (1996)22
Controlled Oral Word Association Testc Benton et al. (1994)23
Picture Arrangement subtest of the WAIS-3c Wechsler (1997)24
Porteus Maze Testc Porteus (1965)25
Ruff Figural Fluency Testb Ruff (1996)26
Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Testb Trenerry et al. (1989)27
The Category Teste Williams (1994)28
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence—3d Brown et al. (1997)29
Tower of LondonDX: Research Versionf Cullbertson and Zillmer (1999)30
Williams Inhibition Teste Williams (1994)31
Wisconsin Card Sorting Testb Grant & Berg (1993)32
aAvailable from Northern Rehabilitation Services, 117 North Elm Street, P.O. Box 1247, Gaylord, MI 49735. Telephone:
517–732–3866. Internet: www.nss-nrs.com
bAvailable from Psychological Assessment Resources, P.O. Box 998, Odessa, FL 33556. Telephone: 1–800–331–8378. Internet:

www.parinc.com
cAvailable from The Psychological Corporation, 555 Academic Court, San Antonio, TX 78204–2498. Telephone: 1–800–211–8378.

Internet: www.PsychCorp.com
dAvailable from Pro-Ed, 8700 Shoal Creek Boulevard, Austin, TX 78757–6897. Telephone: 1–800–897–3202. Internet: www.

proedinc.com
eAvailable from CoolSpring Software, P.O. Box 130, Woodsboro, MD 21798. Telephone: 301–845–8719. Internet:

users.aol.com/CoolSpring/Cspring.html
fAvailable from Multi-Health Systems, 908 Niagara Falls Blvd., North Tonawanda, NY 14120–2060. Telephone: 1–800–456–3003.

Internet: www.mhs.com
157
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

TABLE 2. Some Experimental Procedures for Assessing


Executive Function Abilities
Test Source
Observational/interview tools
Executive Function Self-Rating Scale Coolidge and Griego (1995)31
Executive Interview (EXIT25) Royall et al. (1992)32
Functional tests
American Multiple Errands Test Aitken et al. (1993)33
Photocopy Tasks Crepeau et al. (1997)34
Route-Finding Test Boyd and Sautter (1993)35
Wheelbarrow Test Butler et al. (1989)36
Selected tests targeting specific abilities
Antisaccade Task Guitton et al. (1985)37

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale Grigsby et al. (1992)38
CLOX: An Executive Clock Drawing Task Royall et al. (1998)32
Everyday Problem Solving Inventory Cornelius and Caspi (1987)33
Mental Dice Task Brugger et al. (1996)34
Six Elements Test Shallice and Burges (1991)7
Strategy Application Test Levine et al (1998)6
Stuck-in-Set Test Sandson and Albert (1987)35
Tinkertoy Test Lezak (1995)3
Tower of Toronto Saint Cyr and Taylor (1992)36
Twenty Questions Test Laine and Butters (1982)37

tive Function System (Delis et al.38) is under search for a lost set of keys in a large
development and will be ready for retail in field (represented by a square on a
the latter part of 2000. piece of paper).
The BADS (Wilson et al., 199618) was 4. Temporal Judgment Test: assesses prob-
designed to determine whether clients have lem solving or reasoning by asking
impairments of one or several executive clients four questions about the length
functions and whether these impairments of time required for commonplace oc-
compromise their daily functioning. This currences for which there is no exact
test battery consists of the following seven answer (e.g., “How long do most dogs
subtests which were devised to assess a spec- live for?”).
trum of executive function abilities: 5. Zoo Map Test: evaluates planning and
cognitive flexibility by having clients
1. Rule Shift Cards Test: assesses cognitive draw out the route they would follow to
flexibility and inhibition by requiring visit a number of prescribed locations
clients to respond correctly to one rule on a map of a zoo while also adhering to
for a period of time (i.e., say “yes” when a number of prescribed rules.
the card is red and “no” when the card 6. Modified Six Elements Test: examines
is black) and then to a new rule (i.e., say planning, organizing, and self-monitor-
“yes” if the previous card was red and ing by requiring clients to complete
“no” if the previous card was black) in three tasks, each of which has two parts,
the second part of the task. and by imposing the rule that clients
2. Action Program Test: examines planning must perform these tasks without com-
and problem solving by having clients pleting the two parts of the same task
figure out how to get a cork out of a tube consecutively.
using a variety of objects and materials 7. The Dysexecutive Questionnaire (as
and following a small set of rules. previously described), which identifies
3. Key Search Test: evaluates planning and the presence and frequency of a variety
self-monitoring by requiring clients to of symptoms associated with impaired
158 draw the route they would take to executive functioning.
ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ABILITIES OF ADULTS—MURRAY, RAMAGE

Several of these subtests represent adapta- Tests of Specific Executive Function Abilities
tions of previously published tests. Gener-
ally, these adaptations were made to bring As previously discussed, executive func-
the tests more in line with real-life activities tioning has been hypothesized by some to
and to increase the variety of clients for be a multidimensional construct consisting
which the tests might be appropriate. of a number of separate, controlling func-
The BADS was normed for 216 non- tions or domains (Burgess, 199740). Present-
brain-damaged, healthy control individuals ly, however, there is no consensus on the
who ranged in age from 16 to 87 years vernacular or composition of these execu-
(mean = 47 years) and 78 individuals with tive domains (Phillips, 19971). Therefore, in
brain damage related to various etiologies an attempt to provide some organization to

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
(i.e., TBI, stroke, dementia, infection, and the numerous tests of executive functioning
anoxia) who ranged in age from 19 to 76 available, we chose to arrange tests into the
years (mean = 39 years) (Wilson et al., following executive domains most fre-
199618). Specific information pertaining to quently encountered in the literature: (1)
the speech and language abilities of the planning or the ability to formulate a strat-
brain-damaged individuals was not pro- egy and sequence the steps of that strategy
vided. It might be anticipated, however, that in order to meet intended goals; (2) organi-
clients with moderate to severe language zation or the ability to structure or catego-
comprehension deficits would have diffi- rize incoming information as well as one’s
culty understanding the directions to many own responses; (3) inhibition or the ability
of the subtests (e.g., Action Program, Zoo to suppress automatic, routine, or irrelevant
Map) and that clients with moderate to se- processing or responding; (4) cognitive
vere language production deficits would flexibility or the ability to change or adapt
have difficulty completing the subtests re- one’s response set in the event of failure;
quiring verbal responses (e.g., Rule Shift, (5) problem solving or the ability to iden-
Modified Six Elements). In terms of psycho- tify the problem and the ability to generate
metric properties, the BADS accurately dif- and select solutions to the problem; and
ferentiated the performances of brain- (6) self-monitoring or the ability to evalu-
damaged and non-brain-damaged individu- ate and regulate one’s own performance
als; more recently, it was successful in dis- and behavior on the basis of environmental
criminating between the performances of feedback, including the ability to identify
schizophrenic and non-brain-damaged indi- one’s own mistakes and deficits. We ac-
viduals (Evans et al., 199739). In addition, knowledge that other domains of executive
caregivers’ ratings of executive problems functioning may exist, that cognitive
were predicted by the BADS performances processes within these domains may over-
of their brain-damaged relatives (i.e., brain- lap, and that most tests can plausibly fit into
damaged individuals with executive func- more than one domain. Still, we hope that
tion problems were distinguished from our categorization of tests provides clini-
brain-damaged individuals without execu- cians with a framework for selecting tests to
tive function problems) (Wilson et al., ensure that a spectrum of their clients’ ex-
199618). High interrater reliability has been ecutive function abilities are evaluated
reported, but test-retest reliability was only (Tables 1 and 2).
moderate with a trend toward improvement “Planning” Tests. Tower tests, clock
on subsequent testing. Because a crucial as- drawing, and maze completion are often de-
pect of testing executive function abilities is scribed as planning tests because they re-
novelty, (Phillips, 1997;1 Ylvisaker & Feeney, quire clients to sequence and plan ahead,
19986), Wilson et al.18 conjectured that this taking into consideration the consequences
test-retest performance pattern could be re- of certain responses. Tower tests such as the
lated to the reduction of novelty that occurs Tower of London DX: Research Version
on repeated administrations. (TOL:RV) (Cullbertson & Zillmer, 199930), 159
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

the original Tower of London (Shallice, denced by overwriting or latencies. In addi-


198241), and the Tower of Toronto (Saint tion, clinicians should note when clients ask
Cyr & Taylor, 199236) involve deriving the for repetition of instructions, possibly indi-
most efficient solution (i.e., use the fewest cating a confounding memory deficit. Not
moves) to a particular spatial arrangement. only does clock drawing provide useful
For example, the TOL:RV, which has been qualitative and quantitative information
normed for children and adults, requires about clients’ executive function abilities, it
that individuals plan ahead to determine is also a quick and easy task that can be ad-
the order of moves necessary to arrange col- ministered in the acute as well as other ther-
ored wooden beads from their starting posi- apeutic settings.
tion on two of three towers to a target con- “Organization” Tests. The Wisconsin

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
figuration on another tower. Configuration Card Sorting Test (WCST) is the most
difficulty depends on the number and com- widely used and readily accepted test of ex-
plexity of steps necessary to rearrange the ecutive functioning (Grant & Berg, 199332).
beads. Tower tests can be scored in terms of It requires clients to develop a strategy to
number of moves as well as number of cor- sort or categorize a set of cards, to maintain
rect solutions and have been used with a that strategy, to utilize feedback to change
variety of neurogenic communication- the strategy, and to inhibit impulsive re-
disordered populations (Glosser & Good- sponses to achieve accurate responses
glass, 1990;42 Lawrence et al., 199643). (Phillips, 19971). Consequently, in addition
Clock drawing tasks have been used to organizational skills, the WCST assesses a
since Luria44 to assess executive functions. number of other executive functions such
In addition to planning skills, clock drawing as cognitive flexibility and inhibition (Pen-
or setting tasks have been hypothesized to nington et al., 199646). For the test, clients
tap the executive functions of organization, are given a deck of 128 cards; on each card
inhibition, and self-monitoring (Freedman is printed one to four symbols (circles, trian-
et al., 1994;45 Royall et al., 199832). These gles, stars, and crosses) in red, green, yel-
tasks can be administered in one or both of low, or blue, and therefore the cards can be
the following conditions: (a) a free-drawn sorted by form, number, or color. Clients
condition in which clients are given a blank are then asked to place the cards, one by
piece of paper and a pen and asked to one, under four stimulus (key) cards ac-
“Draw a clock, put in all the numbers, and cording to the category that they deduced
set the hands to . . .” or (b) a pre-drawn con- from the pattern of the clinicians’ feedback
dition in which clients are given a piece of (i.e., “correct” or “incorrect”). After 10 con-
paper with a circle representing a clock and secutive correct sorts, the category by which
asked to put in the numbers and set the clients can sort and receive credit is
hands to a specified time (usually “ten past changed; clients are not blatantly told that it
eleven”). Scoring for clock drawing is not has changed but rather have to deduce
standardized and many different scoring again from the pattern of the clinicians’
systems have been devised. For example, feedback that it has changed. The WCST
Freedman et al.45 created a comprehensive takes approximately 20–30 minutes to ad-
scoring system in which they defined each minister to healthy adults but can be ex-
element needed to construct a well-formed tremely frustrating and time consuming for
clock including specifics about the clock brain-damaged as well as non-brain-dam-
contour, numbers, and hands. Rather than aged clients (Parker & Crawford, 199247).
solely relying on these scoring systems, clini- The Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
cians should also observe clients while draw- (MWCST) (Nelson, 197648) was devised to
ing the clock to make note of planning cut time by using only 48 cards, reducing
strategies (e.g., placing anchor numbers— the requirements for task discontinuation,
12, 3, 6, and 9—before placing other num- and eliminating cards that shared more
160 bers) or areas of particular difficulty as evi- than one attribute with the key cards to
ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ABILITIES OF ADULTS—MURRAY, RAMAGE

minimize confusion and make the sorting “Cognitive Flexibility” Tests. Cognitive
principle more obvious. The MWCST has flexibility is necessary when performing
been used in research studies and, like the tasks that require a shift from one thing to
original WCST, has been found to be sensi- another when feedback (either internal or
tive to problems with organization and cog- external) indicates the need to change.
nitive flexibility (Lezak, 1995;3 Glosser & Cognitive flexibility may subsume or be de-
Goodglass, 199042). pendent upon attentional skills and also is
“Inhibition” Tests. These tests require closely related to inhibition (Sohlberg &
clients to ignore extraneous, irrelevant in- Mateer, 198953). One popular test of cogni-
formation that may conflict with a goal. In- tive flexibility is the Trail Making Test, (Rei-
hibition can play a role at multiple levels of tan & Wolfson, 198554) which requires

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
cognitive processing such as inhibiting a clients to connect digits (Part A) or to con-
response to allow for response formula- nect digits and letters in an alternating se-
tion, inhibiting attention to irrelevant por- quential order (Part B). This test is easily
tions of a task, and inhibiting previous re- administered, as long as clients understand
sponses or task requirements that are no the instructions, has published normative
longer appropriate (i.e., stuck-in-set perse- data for elderly adults (Spreen & Strauss,
veration) (Barkley, 1996;49 Sandson & Al- 199155), and has been found to be sensitive
bert, 198735). One well-known test of inhi- to even the subtle deficits observed in mild
bition is the Stroop Test (Trenerry et al., TBI (Brooks et al., 199956). A variation of
198927) for which clients either read color this test is the Color Trails Test (D’Elia et al.,
names or name the color in which the 199620) in which colors (i.e., bright pink
word is written (e.g., the word “blue” writ- and yellow) are substituted for letters, mak-
ten in red ink). The greatest interference ing it more appropriate for clients with neu-
(i.e., difficulty inhibiting irrelevant infor- rogenic communication disorders, particu-
mation) typically occurs when the color larly those who are aphasic. A number of
and written word are conflicting (Mac- other cognitive flexibility tasks that are de-
Leod, 199150); this interference is particu- scribed in the research literature and are
larly problematic for clients with left hemi- quick and simple to administer include
sphere damage, RHD, or closed head repetitive graphomotor tasks such as copy-
injury (Nehemkis & Lewisohn, 1972;51 ing alternating figures or letters and repeti-
Stuss et al., 198552). Obviously, the Stroop tive sequential hand movements (e.g.,
Test would be difficult for clients with spo- rapidly alternating hand pronation and
ken language or reading deficits. For these supination; alternating fist-edge-palm)
individuals, a more appropriate, nonverbal (Luria, 1973;44 Grigsby et al., 1992;38 Shatz,
test of inhibition is the Antisaccade Task, 199857). Clinicians also may refer to
(Guitton et al., 198537) which requires Rende58 for a more detailed description of
them to focus on a center fixation point on cognitive flexibility and its assessment.
a computer monitor. Cues are randomly “Problem Solving” Tests. The Raven’s
flashed on the right or left side of the Progressive Matrices (RPM) (Raven et al.,
screen and clients are instructed to make 197659) and the Colored Progressive Matri-
an eye movement in the direction opposite ces (Raven et al., 198421) are described as
to the cue; that is, clients must inhibit the measures of nonverbal problem solving and
natural response of directing their eyes in thus are useful when assessing clients with
the same direction as the abrupt visual cue. neurogenic communication disorders.
This task is difficult and even non-brain- These tests also tap visuoperceptual skills
damaged adults make incorrect saccades and cognitive flexibility as clients search for
around 30% of the time, suggesting that a solution. Verbal demands are minimal as
there is a continuum of performance the instructions can be pantomimed by clin-
decrement from normal to disordered icians and clients indicate their response by
functioning (Parker & Crawford, 199247). pointing. In brief, clients are shown a de- 161
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

sign with a piece missing from it and are re- Functional Tests of Executive Function Abilities
quired to choose from an array of possibili-
ties the piece that correctly completes the With respect to the World Health Orga-
design. Kertesz and McCabe60 found that nization63 (WHO) model of health condi-
RPM scores were not correlated with apha- tions and disablement, an executive func-
sia severity scores, indicating that this test tion assessment should quantify and qualify
measures problem solving independent of impairments of structure or function (e.g., poor
the language impairment observed in that self-awareness, decreased inhibition), limi-
population. In contrast, others have found a tations of personal activities (e.g., problems
relation between test performance and cooking, difficulty using public transporta-
aphasia severity (Grigoroiu & Mihailescu, tion), and restrictions in participation in society

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
197961), suggesting that some individuals (e.g., social isolation, loss of one’s job). Cur-
may use verbal mediation to arrive at solu- rently, the vast majority of formal tests of ex-
tions (Lezak, 19953). ecutive function assess for problems at only
“Self-Monitoring” Tests. Fluency tasks the impairment level of the WHO model.
are often used as executive function mea- This inordinate focus on impairments is
sures as they require clients to monitor the problematic because there is no apparent
exemplars they have already produced one-to-one correspondence among the
while continuing to generate new exem- WHO concepts of impairment, activity, and
plars. Many fluency tasks are verbal in na- participation; that is, it is difficult to antici-
ture, requiring the generation of words pate whether and how executive function
beginning with a certain letter or belong- impairments might be manifest as personal
ing to a particular semantic category (e.g., activity or social participation limitations
animals, grocery store items). The most (Burgess, 199740).
widely used and standardized letter flu- Consequently, tests such as the Ameri-
ency task is the FAS test (also referred to as can Multiple Errands Test (AMET) (Aitken
Controlled Oral Word Association Test) et al., 199333) and the BADS (Wilson et al.,
(Benton et al., 199423) for which clients 199618) were developed to assess executive
are given 1 minute to produce as many function abilities in daily activities and set-
words as they can beginning with the letter tings. These test procedures are described
F, then a minute for A, and a minute for S. as ecologically valid in that most are simply
Clients are instructed to avoid giving formalized variants of daily activities. For ex-
proper names or numbers. Normative data ample, the AMET consists of a set of 16 tasks
for other letter fluency tasks are provided that revolve around buying six items as
in Lezak.3 Another option for clients with promptly and cheaply as possible in a real
speech and language deficits is the Ruff shopping environment while adhering to
Figural Fluency Test (Ruff, 199626), a stan- six prescribed rules. Clients must also mail a
dardized, nonverbal fluency task. During postcard with certain information on it to
this test, clients are given a piece of paper clinicians as well as meet the clinician at a
that has 40 squares each filled with a set of certain time and place. Prior to arriving at
five dots. Clients are asked to generate as the shopping area, clients receive instruc-
many unique designs as possible within 1 tions pertaining to the tasks and the rules.
minute by connecting each set of dots with Clients are allowed to use only the small
straight lines. This test can be scored by amount of money provided and their
calculating the number of unique draw- watches; all other personal belongings and
ings and perseverative errors to examine money are temporarily taken away until
clients’ ability to initiate, plan, and inhibit client completion of the AMET. At the shop-
their drawing responses (see also ping area, task instructions are repeated
Sohlberg62 for an in-depth discussion of and then clinicians observe and record
the concept and assessment of self-moni- their clients’ performance (e.g., successful
162 toring abilities). task completion, rule breaks, inefficient
ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ABILITIES OF ADULTS—MURRAY, RAMAGE

strategies). In terms of executive function tional definition of what executive function


abilities, the AMET provides information or functions are being assessed and without
concerning daily problem-solving and plan- stipulating the specific model of executive
ning abilities. Although further research functioning upon which test construction
into the psychometric properties is needed, was based (Phillips, 1997;1 Pennington et
initial findings indicated that the AMET al., 199646). For example, although Tower
performances of adults with TBI were signif- tests (e.g., Tower of London) are commonly
icantly worse than those of non-brain- proposed to assess planning ability, statisti-
damaged adults and were related to care- cal examination of Tower test performances
givers’ ratings of the TBI adults’ functional indicates that many additional cognitive
abilities (McCue, 199564). Interestingly, no skills such as spatial working memory are in-

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
strong associations were found between the volved and, consequently, assessed (Robbins
TBI group’s AMET performance and their et al., 199866). Therefore, without addi-
performance of more traditional, planning tional testing, clinicians cannot be sure that
and problem-solving tests (e.g., Trail Mak- their clients’ poor performance on a Tower
ing Test, The Category Test), indicating that test represents planning difficulties, spatial
the AMET identified executive function working memory deficits, or both.
deficits missed by standard psychometric In addition, few executive function
evaluation. This result underscores that re- tests have been shown to possess acceptable
liance on more formal and structured tests ecological validity, a type of predictive valid-
of executive functioning may lead to under- ity that examines the extent to which indi-
estimation of the degree of difficulty clients viduals’ test performances predict their be-
will have in real-world contexts (Ylvisaker et havior in real-world settings (Sbordone,
al., 1998;6 Braswell et al., 199216). 199612). That is, with respect to the WHO
model of health conditions, few executive
tests will help clinicians reliably predict
CURRENT LIMITATIONS IN which of their clients are at risk for daily ac-
EXECUTIVE FUNCTION PROCEDURES tivity limitations, social participation restric-
tions, or both (Cripe, 1996;13 Dugbartey et
Although a variety of assessment tools al., 1999;14 Braswell et al., 199216). Collec-
and methods have been developed to exam- tively, these validity problems indicate that
ine executive function abilities, there are future research is necessary to specify which
psychometric weaknesses and procedural processes are tapped by each existing test of
limitations associated with many of these executive functioning and to determine
tests. A primary weakness of current execu- whether there are meaningful relations be-
tive function tests relates to the psychomet- tween performances on these tests and our
ric property of validity. Generally, validity clients’ daily functional outcome and status
refers to a test’s ability to measure what it (see Miyake et al.67 for a more detailed dis-
contends to measure (Carmines & Zeller, cussion of the validity of executive function
197965). There are several types of validity tests).
including content validity, the extent to Another psychometric weakness of
which a test measures all of the behaviors it many tests of executive functioning is their
should be measuring, and predictive valid- low level of test-retest reliability (Phillips,
ity, the accuracy with which a test predicts 1997;1 Pennington et al., 199646). Test-retest
whether individuals have the target deficit. reliability refers to the extent to which a test
Unfortunately, both of these types of validity yields similar results across repeated admin-
are inadequate in most presently available istrations under similar testing conditions:
tests of executive functioning. For example, the more consistent the repeated measure-
the content validity of most executive func- ments are, the more reliable the test
tion tests is suspect because they have been (Carmines & Zeller, 197965). As noted previ-
developed without providing a clear opera- ously, at least part of the poor test-retest reli- 163
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

ability of executive tests may relate to the re- Therefore, when assessing executive
duction of novelty that occurs on repeated functioning in clients with neurogenic com-
administrations (Wilson et al., 199618). It munication disorders, it is important to se-
has been argued that executive tests must lect tests that are or can be adapted to be
include some type of novelty (i.e., novel compatible with their cognitive, sensory,
content or task) so that when completing and motoric abilities as well as their demo-
these tests, participants use nonroutine, ef- graphic characteristics. For example, to
fortful processes that are representative of identify executive function impairments in
executive functioning abilities (Phillips, clients with aphasia, clinicians should
1997;1 Ylvisaker & Feeney, 19986). When ex- choose tests that have minimal language de-
isting executive tests are readministered, mands (e.g., clock drawing, Tower tests).

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
part of the novelty is lost, and subsequent Clinicians also could use nonstandardized
test completion places fewer demands upon administration of verbal, executive tests in
executive function abilities. Consequently, an attempt to accommodate for their apha-
there is a need to develop tests that can be sic clients’ language deficits (e.g., present-
repeatedly administered without significant ing verbal, written, and gestural test instruc-
practice effects or that have alternate forms tions; allowing written rather than, or in
so that clinicians can quantify the effects of addition to, verbal responses; substituting
rehabilitation programs aimed at remediat- test stimuli with items that the client can
ing or accommodating for executive func- name or understand). Of course, these
tion deficits (Elias & Treland, 1999;11 Dug- types of accommodations might negate the
bartey et al., 199914). use of test norms but may still provide use-
Another problem with most tests of ex- ful information about the aphasic client’s
ecutive functioning is that they were not executive function abilities. To improve the
specifically developed for clients with neu- accuracy and reliability with which clini-
rogenic communication disorders. Conse- cians can assess the executive function abili-
quently, there are various cognitive, sensory, ties of clients with neurogenic communica-
motoric, and demographic variables that tion disorders, future research must focus
may affect this population’s performance on (1) developing novel tests of executive
and thus obscure assessment of their execu- functioning that minimize linguistic, vis-
tive function abilities. Possible confounding uoperceptual, or motoric demands; (2)
variables include (1) aphasia that may inter- standardizing adaptations of existing tests
fere with understanding task instructions or to accommodate for linguistic, visuopercep-
with completing tasks that have specific ver- tual or motoric deficits; and (3) extending
bal output or input demands; (2) visuoper- the normative data of existing tests to repre-
ceptual problems such as hemianopsia that sent the increasing age and ethnocultural
may interfere with perceiving visual stimuli diversity of our client population (Sbor-
or with completing tasks that require done, 199612).
drawn, written, or visuoconstruction re-
sponses; (3) motoric impairments such as
hemiparesis, tremor, and rigidity that may CONCLUSIONS
interfere with completing tasks that involve
hand or limb movements (e.g., object ma- Currently, the numerous procedures
nipulation, pointing, drawing, writing); (4) available to examine executive functioning
motor speech impairments such as dysar- abilities of clients with neurogenic communi-
thria and apraxia of speech that may inter- cation disorders require further refinement
fere with completing tasks that require spo- and development. Nonetheless, by looking
ken responses; and (5) the client’s age for converging evidence across the data ac-
(particularly 70 years or older) and ethno- quired from both qualitative (e.g., observa-
cultural background, which may be incom- tions, interviews) and quantitative (e.g., for-
164 patible with test norms. mal tests) assessment methods, clinicians
ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ABILITIES OF ADULTS—MURRAY, RAMAGE

should be able to obtain an accurate deter- dures for assessing executive functioning so
mination of the presence, nature, and sever- that clinicians working with neurogenic
ity of executive function deficits of their client populations may begin to incorporate
clients (Cripe, 199613). Unfortunately, with the examination of executive function abili-
the possible exception of clients with TBI, ties into their daily clinical practice.
clients with neurogenic communication dis-
orders (e.g., dementia, aphasia, cognitive-
communicative disorders associated with
RHD, Parkinson’s disease) probably rarely ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
receive assessment for executive function
deficits despite evidence that the extent of Preparation of this article was sup-

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
executive deficits can predict their func- ported, in part, by grant DC03886 and by
tional outcome and abilities (Chen et al., the National Multipurpose Research and
1998;4 Grigsby et al., 199238). We have tried Training Center grant CD-01409 from the
to provide a sufficient review and description National Institute on Deafness and Other
of the variety of formal tests of and proce- Communication Disorders.

REFERENCES

Ed: We did 1. Phillips, L.H. (1997). Do “frontal tests” 8. Shallice, T., & Burgess, P.W. (1991). Deficits
measure executive function? Issues of as- in strategy application following frontal
not balance
sessment and evidence from fluency tests. lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727–741.
columns for In Rabbit, P. (Ed.). Methodology of frontal and 9. Cummings, J.L. (1993). Frontal-subcortical
Reference executive function (pp. 191–213). Hove, UK: circuits and human behavior. Archives of
section be- Psychology Press. Neurology, 50, 873–880.
cause there 2. Ylvisaker, M., & Feeney, T.J. (1998). Collabo- 9. Schatz, J., Hale, S., & Myerson, J. (1998).
are many rative brain injury intervention: Positive every- Cerebellar contribution to linguistic pro-
repeat day routines. San Diego: Singular. cessing efficiency revealed by focal damage.
numbers 3. Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological assess- Journal of the International Neuropsychological
for refer- ment (3rd Ed.). New York: Oxford Univer- Society, 4, 491–501.
ences. sity Press. 10. Shatz, R.S. (1998). Behavioral neurology. In
4. Chen, S.T., Sultzer, D.L., Hinkin, C.H., Johnson, A.F., & Jacobson, B.H. (Eds.).
Please ad-
Mahler, M.E., & Cummings, J.L. (1998). Ex- Medical speech-language pathology: A practi-
vise. ecutive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: tioner’s guide (pp. 243–284). New York:
—Comp Association with neuropsychiatric symp- Thieme.
toms and functional impairment. Journal of 11. Elias, J.W., & Treland, J.E. (1999). Executive
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 10, function in Parkinson’s disease and subcor-
426–432. tical disorders. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsy-
5. Filley, C. (2000). Clinical neurology and ex- chiatry, 4, 34–40.
ecutive dysfunction. Seminars in Speech and 12. Sbordone, R.J. (1996). Ecological validity:
Language, 21, 95–108. Some critical issues for the neuropsycholo-
6. Ylvisaker, M., Szekeres, S.F., & Feeney, T.J. gist. In Sbordone, R.J., & Long, C.J. (Ed.).
(1998). Cognitive rehabilitation: Executive Ecological validity of neuropsychological testing
functions. In Ylvisaker, M. (Ed.), Traumatic (pp. 15–41). Delray Beach, FL: St. Lucie
brain injury rehabilitation: Children and adoles- Press.
cents (2nd Ed., pp. 221–269). Boston: But- 13. Cripe, L.I. (1996). The ecological validity of
terworth-Heinemann. executive function testing. In Sbordone,
7. Levine, B., Stuss, D., Milberg, W., Alexan- R.J., & Long, C.J. (Eds.). Ecological validity of
der, M., Schwartz, M., & MacDonald, R. neuropsychological testing (pp. 171–202). Del-
(1998). The effects of focal and diffuse ray Beach, FL: St. Lucie Press.
brain damage on strategy application: Evi- 14. Dugbartey, A., Rosenbaum, J.G., Sanchez,
dence from focal lesions, traumatic brain P., & Townes, B.D. (1999). Neuropsycholog-
injury, and aging. Journal of International ical assessment of executive functions. Semi-
Neuropsychological Society, 4, 247–264. nars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 4, 5–12.
15. Burgess, P., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie,
H., & Wilson, B. (1998). The ecological va- 165
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

lidity of tests of executive function. Journal task. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
of the International Neuropsychological Society, Psychiatry, 64, 588–594.
4, 547–558. 32. Grant, D.A., & Berg, E.A. (1993).Wisconsin
16. Braswell, D., Hartry, A., Hoornbeek, S., Jo- Card Sorting Test. Tampa, FL: Psychological
hansen, A., Johnson, L., Schultz, J., & Assessment Resources.
Sohlberg, M. (1992). The profile of executive 33. Aitken, S., Chase, S., McCue, M., & Ratcliff,
control system. Gaylord, MI: Northern Reha- G. (1993). An American adaptation of the
bilitation Services. Multiple Errands test: Assessment of execu-
17. Royall, D.R., Mahurin, R., & Gray, K.F. tive abilities in everyday life (Abstract). Ar-
(1992). Bedside assessment of executive chives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, 212.
cognitive impairment: The executive inter- 33. Cornelius, S.W., & Caspi, A. (1987). Every-
view. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, day problem solving in adulthood and old
40, 1221–1226. age. Psychology of Aging, 2, 144–152.

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
18. Wilson, B.A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P.W., 34. Brugger, P., Monsch, A.U., Salmon, D.P., &
Emslie, H., & Evans, J.J. (1996). Behavioral Butters, N. (1996). Random number gener-
assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Gay- ation in dementia of the Alzheimer type: A
lord, MI: Northern Rehabilitation Services. test of frontal executive functions. Neuropsy-
19. DeFilippis, N.A., & McCampbell, E. (1997). chologia, 34, 97–103.
Booklet Category Test (2nd Ed.). Odessa, FL: 34. Crepeau, F., Scherzer, B., Belleville, S., &
Psychological Assessment Resources. Desmarais, G. (1997). A qualitative analysis
20. D’Elia, L.F., Satz, P., Uchiyama, C.L., & of central executive disorders in a real-life
White, T. (1996). Color Trails Test. Odessa, work situation. Neuropsychological Rehabilita-
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. tion, 7, 147–165.
21. Raven, J.C., Court, J., & Raven, J. (1984). 35. Sandson, J., & Albert, M.L. (1987). Perse-
Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K veration in behavioral neurology. Neurology,
Lewis. 37, 1736–1741.
22. Hammill, D.D., Pearson, N.A., & Widerholt, 35. Boyd, T.M., & Sautter, S.W. (1993). Route-
J.L. (1996). Comprehensive test of nonverbal in- finding: A measure of everyday executive
telligence. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. functioning in the head-injured adult. Ap-
23. Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K., Rey, G.J., & plied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 171–181.
Sivan, A.B. (1994). Multilingual Aphasia Ex- 36. Butler, O.T., Anderson, L., Furst, C.J., &
amination (3rd Ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Namerow, N.S. (1989). Behavioral assess-
Psychological Corporation. ment in neuropsychological rehabilitation:
24. Wechsler, D. (1997).Wechsler Adult Intelli- A method for measuring vocational related
gence Scale (3rd Ed). San Antonio, TX: The skills. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 3, 235–243.
Psychological Corporation. 36. Saint Cyr, J.A., & Taylor, A.E. (1992). The
25. Porteus, S.D. (1965). Porteus Maze. San An- mobilization of procedural learning: The
tonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. “key signature” of the basal ganglia. In
26. Ruff, R. (1996). Ruff Figural Fluency Test. Squire, L.R., & Butters, N. (Eds.). Neuropsy-
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Re- chology of memory (2nd Ed.). New York: Guil-
sources. ford Press.
27. Trenerry, M.R., Crosson, B., DeBoe, J., & 37. Laine, M., & Butters, N. (1982). A prelimi-
Leber, W.R. (1989). Stroop Neuropsychological nary study of the problem-solving strategies
Screening Test. Odessa, FL: Psychological As- of detoxified long-term alcoholics. Drug
sessment Resources. and Alcohol Dependence, 10, 235–242.
28. Williams, M. (1994). The Category Test. 37. Guitton, D., Buchtel, H.A., & Douglas, R.M.
Woodsboro, MD: Cool Spring Software. (1985). Frontal lobe lesions in man cause
29. Brown, L., Sherbenou, R.J., & Johnsen, S.K. difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances
(1997). Test of nonverbal intelligence (3rd and in generating goal-directed saccades.
Ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Experimental Brain Research, 58, 455–472.
30. Cullbertson, W.C., & Zillmer, E.A. (1999). 38. Grigsby, J., Kaye, K., & Robbins, L.J. (1992).
Tower of LondonDX: Research version. North Reliabilities, norms and factor structure of
Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems. the Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale. Perceptual
31. Williams, M. (1994). Williams Inhibition Test. and Motor Skills, 74, 883–892.
Woodsboro, MD: Cool Spring Software. 38. Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J.H. (In
31. Coolidge, F.L., & Griego, J.A. (1995). Exec- press). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System.
utive functions of the frontal lobes: Psycho- San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corpo-
metric properties of a self-rating scale. Psy- ration.
chological Reports, 77, 24–26. 39. Evans, J.J., Chua, W.E., McKenna, P.J., &
32. Royall, D.R., Cordes, J.A., & Polk, M. Wilson, B.A. (1997). Assessment of dysexec-
166 (1998). CLOX: An executive clock drawing
ASSESSING THE EXECUTIVE FUNCTION ABILITIES OF ADULTS—MURRAY, RAMAGE

utive syndrome in schizophrenia. Psychologi- in patients with good recovery after closed
cal Medicine, 27, 635–646. head injury. Neurosurgery, 17, 41–47.
40. Burgess, P.W. (1997). Theory and method- 53. Sohlberg, M.M., & Mateer, C.A. (1989). In-
ology in executive function and research. In troduction to cognitive rehabilitation: Theory
Rabbit, P. (Ed.). Methodology of frontal and ex- and practice. New York: Guilford Press.
ecutive function (pp. 81–116). Hove, UK: Psy- 54. Reitan, R.M., & Wolfson, D. (1985).The Hal-
chology Press. stead-Reitan Neuropsychology Test Battery. Tuc-
41. Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of son, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.
planning. Philosophical Transactions of the 55. Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1991). A com-
Royal Society of London, 298, 199–209. pendium of neuropsychological tests. New York:
42. Glosser, G., & Goodglass, H. (1990). Disor- Oxford University Press.
ders in executive control functions among 56. Brooks, J., Fos, L.A., Greve, K.W., & Ham-
aphasic and other brain-damaged patients. mond, J.S. (1999). Assessment of executive

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
Neuropsychology, 12, 485–501. function in patients with mild traumatic
43. Lawrence, A.D., Sahakian, B., Hodges, J.R., brain injury. Journal of Trauma: Injury, Infec-
Rosser, A.E., Lange, K., & Robbins, T.W. tion, and Critical Care, 46, 159–163.
(1996). Executive and mnemonic functions 58. Rende, B. (2000). Cognitive flexibility: The-
in early Huntington’s disease. Brain, 119, ory, assessment, and treatment.Seminars in
1633–1645. Speech and Language, 21, 121–134.
44. Luria, A.R. (1973). The working brain. New 59. Raven, J.C., Court, J., & Raven, J. (1976).
York: Basic Books. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Lon-
45. Freedman, M., Leach, L., Kaplan, E., don: H. K. Lewis.
Winocur, G., Shulman, K.I., & Delis, D.C. 60. Kertesz, A., & McCabe, P. (1975). Intelli-
(1994). Clock drawing: A neuropsychological gence and aphasia: Performance of apha-
analysis. New York: Oxford Press. sics on Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matri-
46. Pennington, B.F., Bennetto, L., McAleer, ces. Brain and Language, 2, 387–395.
O., & Roberts, R. (1996). Executive func- 61. Grigoroiu, M., & Mihailescu, L. (1979). Op-
tions and working memory: Theoretical erational strategies in the thinking of apha-
and measurement issues. In Lyon, G.R., & sics and patients with right hemisphere le-
Krasnegor, N.A. (Eds.). Attention, memory, sion. Romanian Journal of Neurology and
and executive function (pp. 327–348). Psychiatry, 17, 293–302.
Toronto: Paul H. Brookes Publishing. 62. Sohlberg, M.M. (2000). Assessing and man-
47. Parker, D.M., & Crawford, J.R. (1992). As- aging unawareness of self. Seminars in Speech
sessment of frontal lobe dysfunction. In and Language, 21, 135–152.
Crawford, J.R., Parker, D.M., & Mckinlay, 63. World Health Organization (1998). Towards
W.M. (Eds.). The handbook of neuropsychologi- a common language for functioning and disable-
cal assessment (pp. 267–291). Hove, UK: ment: ICIDH-2 the international classification
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. of impairments, activities, and participation.
48. Nelson, H.E. (1976). A modified Wisconsin Geneva, Switzerland: Author.
Card Sorting Test sensitive to frontal lobe 64. McCue, M. (1995). Ecologically valid assess-
defects. Cortex, 12, 313–324. ment of problem-solving abilities: The
49. Barkley, R.A. (1996). Linkages between at- American Multiple Errands Test (Abstract).
tention and executive function: A behav- Journal of the International Neuropsychology So-
ioral approach. In Lyon, G.R., & Krasnegor, ciety, 1, 149.
N.A. (Eds.). Attention, memory, and executive 65. Carmines, E.G., & Zeller, R.A. (1979). Relia-
function (pp. 307–326). Baltimore: Paul H. bility and validity assessment. Newbury Park,
Brooks. CA: Sage Publications.
50. MacLeod, C.M. (1991). Half a century of 66. Robbins, T.W., James, M., Owen, A., Sa-
research on the Stroop effect: An integra- hakian, B., Lawrence, A., McInnes, L., &
tive review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163– Rabbitt, P. (1998). A study of performance
203. on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive
51. Nehemkis, A.M., & Lewinsohn, P.M. (1972). to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sam-
Effects of left and right cerebral lesions on ple of normal volunteers: Implications for
the memory process. Perceptual Motor Skills, theories of executive functioning and cog-
35, 787–798. nitive aging. Journal of the International Neu-
52. Stuss, D.T., Ely, P., Hugenholtz, H., Richard, ropsychological Society, 4, 474–490.
M.T., LaRochelle, S., Poirer, C.A., & Bell, I. 67. Miyake et al. (2000). Seminars in Speech and
(1985). Subtle neuropsychological deficits Language, 21, 169–184.

167
SEMINARS IN SPEECH AND LANGUAGE—VOLUME 21, NUMBER 2 2000

ARTICLE FIVE

SELF-ASSESSMENT QUESTIONS

1. Executive functions are difficult to as- (c) the testing environment may actu-
sess because: ally facilitate better performance.
(a) they are a composite of several dif- (d) none of the above
ferent cognitive domains. (e) all of the above
(b) many current tests measure more 4. Caregivers may be valuable in an execu-
than one executive function, mak- tive function assessment by providing
ing it difficult to delineate exactly information about:

Downloaded by: IP-Proxy Texas A&M University, Texas A&M University. Copyrighted material.
what is being measured. (a) premorbid functional abilities
(c) many current tests were standard- (b) the home environment
ized on populations that are not (c) expectations for treatment outcomes
representative of the current neuro- (d) personality changes since event
genic communication population. (e) all of the above
(d) valid assessment procedures have 5. Retesting executive functions in clients
not been developed. is particularly difficult because:
(e) all of the above (a) clients get too tired after an entire
2. Executive functions can be assessed by: evaluation session.
(a) neuropsychologists (b) changes in test scores are probably
(b) occupational therapists secondary to spontaneous recovery.
(c) speech-language pathologists (c) tests are no longer novel after be-
(d) all of the above ing administered once.
(e) a and c only (d) the tests are too time consuming
3. Executive function ability may be over- and insurance will not pay for the
estimated with classic, standardized outcome information.
tests because: (e) all of the above
(a) they do not measure these functions.
(b) clients may have poor insight to
their disorders.

168

You might also like