You are on page 1of 13

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Classifying and Identifying Motor Learning Behaviors in


Voice-Therapy Clinician-Client Interactions: A Proposed Motor
Learning Classification Framework
*Catherine Madill, *Anna McIlwaine, *Rosanne Russell, †Nicola J. Hodges, and *Patricia McCabe, *Australia, and
yCanada

Summary: Purpose. We studied whether concepts in motor skill learning could be operationalized to identify
clinical interactions and behaviors in a voice therapy setting. Our aim was to test the feasibility of measuring
these behaviors in the prepractice phase so that we could eventually evaluate and apply principles of motor learn-
ing and skill acquisition to Speech-Language Pathology. Four general categories of behaviors that have been
identified in the client-clinician prepractice phase were identified: motivation, modeling, verbal information, and
feedback. All variables were extracted from a proposed Motor Learning Classification Framework.
Method. Nine participants categorized clinician behaviors in three voice therapy training videos into specific,
described, prepractice variables.
Results. Good intrarater reliability was shown across viewings. Inter-rater reliability was high for modeling and
verbal information, but raters were not consistent when identifying behaviors classified as motivation and feed-
back. Raters responded positively to the classification exercise and the categories encompassed nearly all noted
behaviors.
Conclusion. Behaviors described within the motor learning literature can be identified in the initial stages of
voice therapy, providing evidence that motor learning concepts can be used to identify interactions and behaviors
in clinical settings. Disagreement in classification among raters was influenced by differences in implicit and
explicit interpretations of verbal information. This suggests that greater clarity in specific concepts is needed to
support teaching of motor learning principles and implementation of these principles in clinical practice for the
treatment of speech-language pathology.
Key Words: Motor Learning−Voice−Therapy−Practice.

INTRODUCTION been operationalized, identified, and used in clinical settings


Motor learning theory in speech-language pathology and teaching of SLP students.
Motor learning theory is an area of expanding interest in There has been a specific interest in motor learning the-
Speech-Language Pathology (SLP) as researchers seek to ory as it applies to voice training.4−9 A number of research-
identify more effective treatments and processes for learning ers have investigated the effects of feedback on acquisition
the motor skills required for verbal communication. Investi- and learning in experimental studies with an SLP focus,
gations into the understanding and application of motor including focus of attention in feedback,4 modeling and
learning theory to voice, motor speech disorders, stuttering, instruction without feedback,5 self-controlled feedback,7
and swallowing have resulted in recommendations for goal knowledge of results feedback8 and biophysical feedback.9
setting (eg1), use of instructions and feedback (eg,2), and Practice organization has also been studied, but to a lesser
novel treatments (eg, ReST3). The majority of these studies degree (eg, massed vs spaced practice in vocology train-
have focused on specific motor learning variables, such as ing6). Voice therapy (the remediation of the disordered
motivation, verbal information, modeling, and feedback. voice) provides a well-defined context to investigate
To understand how the findings from this body of research whether interactions between, and behaviors of, clinicians
might apply to SLP, a broader, more comprehensive and and clients can be identified and analyzed against the back-
contextualized study of motor learning is needed to help drop of motor learning theory and pertinent concepts.
train therapists and improve treatment and training out- Such an analysis would contribute to the understanding of
comes. One step in this direction is to determine how con- what components of voice therapy involve motor skill
cepts described in motor learning research and theory have learning principles more generally, such as use of feedback
and demonstrations and whether these are provided in
Accepted for publication March 22, 2019. ways that are thought to be most optimal. This will also
From the *Voice Research Laboratory, Discipline of Speech Pathology, Faculty of help identify what aspects are not applied, not applied well
Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, Australia; and the ySchool of Kinesiol-
ogy, Faculty of Education, University of British Columbia, Canada. or are not associated with motor skill learning at all.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Catherine Madill, PO Box 170,
Lidcombe, NSW 1825 Australia. E-mail: cate.madill@sydney.usyd.edu.au
Journal of Voice, Vol. &&, No. &&, pp. &&−&&
0892-1997
Active ingredients in direct voice therapy
© 2019 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of The Voice Founda- In recent years, significant efforts have been made to investi-
tion. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license.
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)
gate the “active ingredients” of voice therapy.10 These ingre-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2019.03.014 dients have been identified as falling into two categories:
ARTICLE IN PRESS
2 Journal of Voice, Vol. &&, No. &&, 2019

pedagogic strategies (how we learn) and voice techniques or


exercises (what we learn).11 Identification of these categories
has been made difficult by the fact that even voice treatment
studies with the highest levels of evidence have not isolated
effects due to voice technique or pedagogic strategies.12 A
theoretical model of active ingredients in voice therapy
has been developed, where a wider range of classifications
has been proposed to explain efficacy of direct voice therapy
interventions.13 This includes differentiating so-called
extrinsic (clinician applied) versus intrinsic/patient-applied
delivery methods, counseling, and the analysis of direct
interventions into different subcategories.13 Many of these
elements include behaviors described in motor learning the-
ory, for example modeling, augmented feedback, and self-
evaluation. This is not unexpected given the need to learn,
relearn or in some other way alter the habitual movements of
the vocal system with the aim of producing an optimal voice.
A clearer overview of motor learning behaviors that are used
across different therapies and modes of delivery would facili-
tate further development of an active ingredient model of
voice therapy.

Motor learning theory


Two phases of motor learning interventions and activities
have been considered in research: prepractice and prac-
tice.14 Prepractice is the phase before the learner or client
begins to rehearse or change their behaviors. This is the cli-
nician-client period (or coach-athlete time), when there may
be time constraints and the primary aim is to convey to the
learner what needs to be practiced. The practice phase, by
contrast, may be unsupervised, at least in the rehabilitation
context, and this is the time when the learner is building rep-
etitions and putting into practice the knowledge acquired in
the prepractice phase. In the field of voice and motor
speech, there has been significant research into the later
practice phase, with a focus on improving skill retention
past the completion of intervention (see Maas et al, 2008 for
review). Significantly less research exists in SPL on the pre-
practice phase. The prepractice phase ensures that the client
understands the target movement (ie, builds a reference-of-
correctness15) and can produce it under optimal condi-
tions.16 Treatment effectiveness is the focus in practice.
However, treatment efficiency, that is the speed with which
the components required for practice are understood or
acquired, is a key concern in the prepractice phase. In pre- FIGURE 1. A proposed Motor Learning Classification Frame-
practice, the focus is on factors that allow a particular skill work. The main variables appear on the left of the model and are
to be accurately performed quickly, with the lowest degree ordered somewhat in relation to how they are delivered chronolog-
of effort and cost, and with minimized risk of injury.17 This ically. To the right of these variables are classifications of how
is especially salient in voice therapy as the target movement these variables can be considered along with more specific exam-
may be difficult to acquire and detect due to its transient ples. Note KR, Knowledge of results; KP, Knowledge of perfor-
and abstract nature, and the lack of visual, auditory and mance.
somatosensory feedback.
is motivated to change behavior is essential for learning.14
Motivation can be established by conveying the importance
Prepractice variables of the task to the client16 or by setting goals with the cli-
The prepractice variables include motivation, verbal infor- ent.18 Verbal information is a broad category of prepractice
mation, and modeling (Figure 1).14 Ensuring that the client behaviors within which instructions, explanations, and
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Catherine Madill, et al Classifying Motor Learning Behaviors in Voice Therapy 3

perceptual training can be considered subcategories.14 The amount of information, or difficulty, for each client. In pre-
client must possess an understanding of the target move- practice, the client's skill level is generally low and it is there-
ment in order to accurately produce the movement pat- fore appropriate for the clinician to reduce the difficulty of
tern.19 Thus, instructions and explanations about the the task by providing relevant cues, prompts, and supports.
movement and associated sensations may be necessary in If a learner appears to be struggling to achieve the target
the prepractice phase.14 For the acquisition of speech and action (eg, clear and effortless voicing), due to too little
voice tasks, auditory-perceptual and somatosensory train- information or decreased motivation, then increasing the
ing, especially discrimination training, is one way of alerting amount of guidance or information may be warranted.
to a to-be-acquired movement.20 This prepractice percep- The challenge for the SLP in prepractice is to identify
tual discrimination training has been shown to activate pre- suitable instructional and feedback behaviors to reduce
motor cortical areas21 and lead to motor learning in the the task demands and assist the client to achieve the task
absence of movement practice.22 Modeling involves physi- goal efficiently.31 While some proprietary programs such
cal demonstration of the target movement. This allows the as PROMPT32 or Voicecraft33 recommend specific instruc-
client to observe aspects of the skill that cannot be verbally tional strategies, overall there is relatively limited guidance
explained (eg,18,23,24). Modeling may be visual or auditory. in the SLP literature regarding whether these are appropri-
Feedback provided for initial movement attempts during ate instructional behaviors to expedite completion of the
prepractice is also essential to ensure that the client is aware necessary components of the prepractice phase. By investi-
of the target movement and can approximate this before gating the general motor learning literature it is possible to
beginning practice (eg,16). Of course, some attempt to try identify strategies which instructors can use to assist learners
the target movement is needed to check if the client is able to acquire any new motor task. For example, instructions
to make the correct movement,16 even though in the pre- that are short and simple, help cue individuals to key infor-
practice phase, attempts are typically limited. Attempts mation, and/or provide an external focus on the learning
may be made independently or with guidance from the outcome (ie, the sound or the feeling during sound produc-
clinician. Because in voice therapy, incorrect movement tion), rather than a detailed description of the technique,
attempts may cause phonotrauma,25 guidance in the initial have been shown to benefit learning.19,34,35
stages of intervention may be appropriate. Feedback on the Adoption of techniques which work to bring about a
accuracy of initial movement attempts is essential to change in behavior through more implicit means have also
encourage awareness and change.26 Clinicians provide been shown to be useful ways for promoting learning, whilst
information about the outcome of movement (knowledge of additionally keeping cognitive demands on the learner low
results) or about the nature of the movement pattern (eg,36−39). For example, use of analogies, or changes to
(knowledge of performance) (eg,27). In SLP contexts, feed- physical constraints (eg, posture) can bring about behaviors
back can relate to auditory, kinesthetic or visual aspects of as a consequence, rather than as an explicitly prescribed
the movement and may be provided during or after move- aim.40,41 Asking singers to focus on letting the breath flow
ment attempts.28 easily has been shown to be more effective in raising volume
Within these five broad categories are numerous subcate- than focusing on using breath support.42 Similarly, move-
gories which have been studied in the motor learning litera- ment demonstrations rather than explicit instructions can
ture and have shown to impact motor skill acquisition as help to bring about motor skills more implicitly.43,44 Skill
detailed in Figure 1. We focus primarily on the four preprac- acquisition and movement-form retention are most optimal
tice variables of motivation, verbal information, modeling, when learners are exposed to several episodes of modeling
and feedback in this study as they are most relevant in the ini- (or indeed multiple models)42 rather than just a single epi-
tial stages of intervention and therefore most likely to be iden- sode (or model) (Figure 2).45−48
tifiable in the clinician-client interaction. To simplify the
identification analysis for this first investigation, we only look
at subcategories associated with the types of verbal informa- Identifying activities and behaviors in training and
tion (ie, instructions, explanations or perceptual training). therapy settings
Content analysis is one method for analyzing written or ver-
bal texts such as clinical interactions. It provides a research
Why operationalize and apply motor learning technique which can be used for objective, systematic, and
concepts to therapy? quantitative description of communication and behavior.49
Generally, practice conditions that increase task demands To achieve objective analysis of instructional behavior, valid,
result in improved retention and generalization of motor and reliable classification systems must be developed.50,51 A
skills.29,30 However, clients can benefit from reduced task number of classification systems for sports-based instruc-
demands in the initial stages of intervention, that is, in the tional behaviors have been developed which meet these crite-
prepractice phase, when learning inherently requires effort ria. The Motor Teaching Principles Taxonomy, for example,
and has high information processing demands.17 According has been used to reliably analyze physical therapists’ instruc-
to the challenge point framework,29 the task difficulty and tional behaviors across multiple therapists and therapy
the client's skill level interact to determine the optimal sessions.52
ARTICLE IN PRESS
4 Journal of Voice, Vol. &&, No. &&, 2019

MATERIAL AND METHODS


Participants
Participants were nine female students with an age range of
21−36 years, in their final semester of a Master of Speech-
Language Pathology degree at the University of Sydney,
Australia. All were fluent speakers of English. All passed a
hearing screening of 20 dB at 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz.56 Partici-
pants will be referred to as “raters” throughout this study.
Ethical approval was obtained through Human Research
Ethics Committee of the University of Sydney (08-2009/
11958). Direct consent was obtained from all raters.

Procedure
Stimuli
Test stimuli were selected samples from three commercially
available voice therapy training videos: Lessac Madsen Res-
onant Voice Therapy (LMRVT),57 Yell Well,58 and Vocal
Function Exercises (VFE).59
FIGURE 2. Prepractice variables within the Motor Learning LMRVT57 is used for the treatment of hyper- and hypofunc-
Classification Framework. tional voice disorders. The aim of LMRVT is for the client to
produce a vocal tone in a resonant and easy manner such that
the vocal folds are barely adducted or barely abducted result-
Systematic observation of sports coaches during training ing in reduced impact pressure between the vocal folds.60
sessions has been undertaken to investigate the practice Yell Well58 is a treatment for vocal nodules in children.
activities and instructional behaviors used in an applied set- The program teaches children how to yell safely via a step
ting (eg,50,53). These studies have shown that coach behavior wise series of cues to induce release of laryngeal constric-
is often in conflict with principles of best practice based on tion, stabilize the laryngeal framework for optimal activa-
motor learning theory and hence promote better knowledge tion of intrinsic laryngeal musculature, and shape the
dissemination of evidence-based practice among practi- resonatory tract for amplification of the vocal signal.
tioners (see also54). Such observational research is also VFEs59 is a series of vocal exercises that are claimed to
essential in SLP to determine what clinicians actually do in promote physiologic balance across respiratory, phonatory,
clinical practice so that the match between theory and prac- and resonance subsystems. The goal of the exercises is to
tice can be made and appropriate interventions targeted strengthen the intrinsic laryngeal and auxiliary musculature,
based on this knowledge.55 and enhance the vocal tract resonance characteristics.61
For clinicians to apply motor learning principles and A pilot study to determine the most user-friendly classifi-
thereby improve the efficacy of the prepractice phase, as cation task and checklist format was run with four indepen-
well as the overall motor learning process, including prac- dent raters. None of the raters from the pilot study were
tice, there is a need for: (1) an overarching classification included in any further research. An additional prepractice
framework that incorporates all of the motor learning varia- behavior category, physical attempts, was included in the
bles demonstrated to facilitate learning and (2) a means by pilot and excluded from the checklist in the reported study
which clinicians can recognize and classify these elements in as it was a client behavior and reliability was very high.
real-life therapy contexts. The overall aim would be to char-
acterize, identify, evaluate, and test these various preprac- Classification protocol
tice and practice variables and then use this information to One therapy sample was selected from each video recording.
train speech-language clinicians. Such a framework would Each sample was selected on the basis that it represented a
include numerous variables referenced in the motor learning prepractice or acquisition sequence, that is, the clinician
literature. We have attempted to detail these motor learning was introducing a new vocal task to the client, the sample
variables in Figure 1. included one-on-one interaction between the presenter of
the video and a patient with a voice disorder, and the sam-
ple was considered by the clinician authors to be typical of a
Purpose of the study standard therapy session. In these interactions, the client's
We aim to study whether motor learning concepts can be voice disorder was addressed using the motor training tech-
operationalized and then reliably used to identify and niques recommended by the specific therapy approach.
classify prepractice variables (motivation, verbal informa- Each of the three samples was 3 minutes in duration. An
tion, modeling, and feedback) in clinical interactions in additional 2-minute sample from LMRVT57 was used as a
voice therapy. training stimulus. The video therapy samples were presented
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Catherine Madill, et al Classifying Motor Learning Behaviors in Voice Therapy 5

three times each. First the entire video was shown (ie, sample was chosen to avoid recency and primacy effects.62
3 minutes), then the video was shown in segments of 10 clini- The same procedure was used as in the first classification
cian behaviors interspersed with 20 second pauses between session. At the end of Session 2, raters were asked to evalu-
segments, and finally the entire video was repeated. This pro- ate the framework and classification task. Raters were
cedure was repeated for each video to give raters time to invited to give written comments on potential applications
classify each of the 10 behaviors. Raters were provided with of the framework and provide suggestions for improving the
a copy of the Motor Learning Classification Framework framework or classification task.
(MLCF) (Figure 1) and written definitions of the general
and specific prepractice variables (see Appendix A). An elec-
tronic version of the observational checklist (Appendix B) Data analysis
was provided to raters and included a written transcription The accuracy of data entry was maximized by having raters
of the video samples, with each spoken sentence and action enter classifications directly into the spreadsheets used for
in the therapy sample transcribed by the researcher (AM) on data analysis. A frequency count of the general behaviors as
a separate line of the spreadsheet. Raters were instructed to a function of the three different types of therapies was first
type a “1” in the column(s) of the identified prepractice vari- calculated. A frequency score for the general verbal informa-
able(s) identified in columns beside each transcribed behav- tion variable was calculated for each behavior by counting
ior. Raters could identify a single behavior as more than one each time at least one of the specific verbal information vari-
variable (eg, “feedback” and “explanation”), however, they ables was selected.
were advised to differentiate between the variables as much Intrarater reliability was measured by calculating percent
as possible. An additional category other was included for agreement between each participant's frequency score on
raters to identify clinically relevant behaviors that could not the first and second session for each behavior in each video
be classified according to the current framework. segment.63 Inter-rater reliability was measured by calculat-
The classifications were conducted over two classification ing percent agreement between all participants for each var-
sessions where all raters attended, and watched and scored iable and each behavior for each video segment.63 Only
the videos together. The first 2-hour classification session eight of the nine participants were included in the intra- and
included both a training component and the first classifica- inter-rater analyses; one rater was excluded as she did not
tion task. The second 1-hour session occurred 1 week after demonstrate clear understanding of the framework or learn-
the first session and included a reclassification and evalua- ing from the training provided. Percent agreement for pre-
tion task. The observational checklists and evaluations were practice variables was calculated by counting the number of
deidentified before data analysis. times one or more people classified a behavior in the same
way and dividing this number by the potential number of
Session 1-Training. The researcher verbally explained agreements and then multiplying by 100. If for example,
the classification task and answered any questions. Raters one person classified seven behaviors as motivation, there
then jointly watched the 2-minute training sample, which would be a potential total agreement of 56 (seven behaviors
illustrated a range of prepractice behaviors. Viewing the classified the same by eight people). If the actual agreements
training video segment conformed to the protocol used later for this behavior were 17, then the % agreement would be
in the classification sessions. That is, during the first view- 17/56 £ 100 = 30.4%.
ing, raters watched the whole sample without classifying the As there were eight response categories and eight partici-
behaviors, to orient them to the whole task, the types of pants, the chance of complete agreement on a category for a
behaviors and their rate of occurrence. Throughout the sec- single behavior was one in 64 (1.56%), that is, agreement by
ond and third viewings they had the opportunity to now chance was low. An estimate of guessing could not be calcu-
identify and classify the behaviors. For the training video lated as participants were able to provide more than one
only, raters compared their classifications to the researcher's classification for each behavior.
and had the opportunity to discuss and clarify any discrep-
ancies or difficulties with the framework and classification RESULTS
task. Frequency of prepractice variables
Table 1 shows the total number of times (#) each variable
Session 1-Classification. Following training, all raters was identified for each video segment across all eight partic-
viewed the three 3-minute therapy samples (one from each ipants included in the inter-rater analyses. The total n values
therapy program) and classified the clinician behaviors were different across the video segments. More variables
according to the prepractice variables. A 5-minute break were identified and classified in LMRVT than VFE and
was provided between each therapy sample. Yell Well.
Session 2-Reclassification. A second classification ses-
sion was conducted in which raters rerated the middle Intrarater reliability
1-minute segment from each 3-minute therapy sample to High intrarater reliability was demonstrated across all three
assess intrarater reliability. The middle segment of the video segments. Agreement ranged from 68.8% to 100%,
ARTICLE IN PRESS
6 Journal of Voice, Vol. &&, No. &&, 2019

TABLE 1.
Frequency Counts for Each Variable in Each Therapy Approach
LMRVT (n = 120) Yell Well (n = 67) VFE (n = 73)
# # #
General variables
Motivation 19 18 21
Modeling 411 59 148
Verbal Information 484 333 362
Feedback 73 164 56
Other 0 0 10
Specific variables (within the category verbal information)
Instructions 291 214 110
Explanations 88 49 198
Perceptual training 118 75 50
Other verbal information 5 44 18
Note: n, number of clinician behaviors within each therapy approach identified by the researchers. Multiplying this number by 8 would give the total number
of behaviors; #, number of clinician behaviors given by the raters for each prepractice variable summed across all 8 raters.

M = 88.7% (SD = 7.18). Intrarater percent agreements for VFE = 14.6%). The other category was not used by partici-
each rater as a function of therapy session are shown in pants in LMRVT or Yell Well but was used on 10 occasions
Figure 3. when classification VFE. Three participants consistently
rated the clinician asking “Ok?” after instructions or
explanations as other. They each described this behavior
Inter-rater reliability for general variables
as “checking/confirming the client understands.” One rater
Participants’ percent agreement for each general prepractice
also identified two additional behaviors as other and
variable within each video segment is represented in
described them as “building rapport.”
Figure 4.
The level of agreement for each therapy approach within
each prepractice variable was compared. Participants dem- Inter-rater reliability for specific prepractice variables
onstrated relatively high agreement across all video seg- Participants’ percent agreements for the specific verbal infor-
ments for modeling (LMRVT = 87.5%; Yell Well = 67.0%; mation variables within each therapy approach are repre-
VFE = 87.5%) and verbal information (LMRVT = 87.0%; sented in Figure 5 below.
Yell Well = 77.80%; VFE = 75.6%). Feedback was rated Participants demonstrated highest agreement across all
with high agreement in Yell Well (74%) but less so in VFE therapy approaches for instructions (LMRVT = 81.8%; Yell
(46.4%) and LMRVT (22.4%). Participants did not show Well = 69.0%; VFE = 49.5%). There was less agreement
good agreement on the motivation variable across all ther- when classification explanations (LMRVT = 41.1%; Yell
apy approaches (LMRVT = 35.4%; Yell Well = 10.2%; Well = 35.3%; VFE = 55.5%) and perceptual training was

FIGURE 4. Inter-rater reliability for each general prepractice


variable within each therapy approach. General variables are moti-
FIGURE 3. Percent agreement for each rater within each therapy vation (MOT), modeling (MOD), general verbal information (VI-
approach. SUM), feedback (FB), and other. Error bars denote Standard
R, rater. Deviation.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Catherine Madill, et al Classifying Motor Learning Behaviors in Voice Therapy 7

useful tool. Rater's evaluations of the framework and classi-


fication task are summarized in Table 4 below.

Relative frequency of prepractice variables across


different treatments
Only classifications that had complete agreement were
assessed to ascertain if there were differences in the ratio of
prepractice behaviors in each of the different video seg-
ments. There were observed differences in the amount of
modeling, instructions and feedback as shown in Figure 6.

FIGURE 5. Inter-rater reliability for each specific prepractice


variable within each therapy approach. Specific variables are DISCUSSION
instructions (IN), explanations (EX), perceptual training (PT) and We investigated whether motor learning concepts, specifi-
other verbal information (Other VI). cally prepractice variables, could be reliably identified and
thus operationalized in clinical interactions in voice therapy.
Evidence of good intrarater reliability was shown, but inter-
rater reliability was not as high. In general, there was rela-
rated relatively high for agreement in LMRVT (50.9%) but tively high inter-rater percent agreement for the identifica-
not in Yell Well (33.9%) and VFE (18.5%). The other verbal tion of modeling and verbal information, but lower
information category was not used by participants in either agreement for motivation and feedback. Within the category
in LMRVT or Yell Well but was used on 16 occasions for verbal information (instructions, explanations, and percep-
VFE with 14.8% agreement. tual training), inter-rater reliability was also typically low.
However, there were some consistent patterns in disagree-
ment, indicating that classification disagreement was mostly
Behavior analysis due to difficulties categorizing motivational and conversa-
Due to the relatively low inter-rater agreement for some tional statements. The participants were generally positive
prepractice variables, we conducted a further behavioral about the MLCF as a way of evaluating client-clinician
analysis to identify which behaviors had full agreement or interactions in voice therapy.
incomplete agreement. Complete agreement (100%) was
achieved when all participants categorized a behavior the
same (eg, all eight participants categorized the clinician Overall intrarater and inter-rater reliability
producing “m” as modeling). Incomplete agreement was The high intrarater agreement (M = 88.7%) suggests partici-
anything less than 100%. A summary of these analyses sub- pants developed internal standards for each prepractice var-
divided into general categories is given in Table 2. iable and used these standards to classify the clinician
For behaviors on which participants did not agree, we behaviors in each classification session. Inter-rater reliability
identified specific behaviors which are displayed in Table 3. across all general and specific variables was relatively low.
However, there was complete agreement between partici-
pants on the general variables for »77% of behaviors in
Rater evaluations LMRVT, »69% in Yell Well, and »66% in VFE. Given the
Analysis of written comments provided at the end of the ses- number of potential response categories for the eight partic-
sions revealed that participants thought the MLCF was a ipants, these numbers suggest that on a general level,

TABLE 2.
Summary Table Showing the Number and Percent Agreement for Behaviors Which had Complete Agreement or Incom-
plete Agreement in Each Therapy Approach
LMRVT (n = 120) Yell Well (n = 67) VFE (n = 73)
# % # % # %
Complete agreement
General variables only 91 76.7 46 68.7 48 65.8
General and specific variables 79 65.8 31 46.3 31 42.5
Incomplete agreement 28 23.3 16 23.9 17 23.3
Note: n, the total number of clinician behaviors within each therapy approach; #, the number of clinician behaviors for which there was complete agreement
or outlying ratings.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
8 Journal of Voice, Vol. &&, No. &&, 2019

TABLE 3.
Patterns of Disagreement for Clinician Behaviors With Low Inter-rater Reliability
Behavior Type Example Rating Pattern
Indirect feedback ie, instruction “I’m going to stop you”  All agree verbal information
provided immediately after  R3 & R8 also rated it as feedback
an attempt
Explanations to motivate client “You’re doing something very  Split between verbal information (explanations)
good for your vocal folds” and motivation
 Some rate as both
Hand and facial gestures Moves index finger back and  Most agree this is modeling
forth in front of body.  R4 & R8 consistently rated as feedback
 Occasionally no rating from one or more raters
Explanations “I’m going to tell you how to  Split between instructions and explanations
make the biggest shout.”  Raters also showed lower intrarater agreement
for these behaviors
Instructions with sensory “Now see if you can feel your  Split between instructions and explanations
terms giggle on your own hand.”
Knowledge of performance “Your neck’s disappeared.”  Split between feedback and verbal information
feedback (explanations or perceptual training).
Conversational behavior/ “Remember that?” “Ok?”  Split between other, other verbal information,
checking client’s knowledge explanations, feedback and motivation
or understanding
Instruction regarding sound to “With an ‘m’”  Most agree verbal information (instructions)
be produced  Some rated as modeling

participants were relatively consistent at discriminating doing (modeling) and talking (verbal information) behav-
between the behaviors. iors. The more reliably identified behaviors were also the
most frequently exhibited.
The low frequency use of the other variable was another
Inter-rater reliability for prepractice variables significant finding supporting the utility of the MLCF. This
Participants demonstrated relatively high agreement for category was never used in classifications of LMRVT and
modeling (67%−88%) and verbal information (76%−87%). Yell Well and only 10 times across all participants and
This suggests that participants found it easy to identify behaviors in VFE. This demonstrates that clinicians’

TABLE 4.
Evaluation of Framework and Rating Task
Evaluation Theme Example
The framework was initially over- “The framework is a bit overwhelming at first but once I became more familiar
whelming for some raters. with it, it was actually very clear to understand.”
The framework was comprehensive “The framework is an excellent way to conceptualize the components of ther-
and useful. apy. It’s comprehensive but not confusing.”
It was difficult to understand some “. . .some variables were harder than others.”
framework variables.
More training was required for the “I was confused about how to rate a few utterances and I think a little more
rating task. training and explanation time may have helped this.”
More time was required for rating “Also, perhaps by “pausing” a little more frequently... I found that I was not
task. keeping up with the audio.”
The observation checklist was well “The checklist is very useful, especially since all behaviors/utterances can be
formatted. coded under the headings included. It’s quick and easy to use.”
The framework could be used for “I really like it because it is quite a ‘meta’ way of looking at your own clinical
self-evaluation. behavior.”
The framework could be used as a “It would be useful to do a similar task, at uni, across the 5 areas (eg, speech/
training tool for student education voice/stuttering, etc) to help us realize the importance of giving motivation/
and evaluation. instructions/ explanations, etc., in therapy, and how to deliver it.”
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Catherine Madill, et al Classifying Motor Learning Behaviors in Voice Therapy 9

FIGURE 6. Average number of identified behaviors in 3 minutes.

behaviors can be operationalized and classified using the identified the explicit meaning (explanation) and a few iden-
MLCF, along with clarification of the framework categories tified the more implicit meaning (motivation).
through a pretraining session and an example look-up table.
It will be important to demonstrate that such observations
Mutual exclusivity and verbal information
improve with practice (and feedback).
subcategories
The lower reliability for the subcategories of verbal infor-
Disagreement analysis mation was most likely caused by a lack of clear definitions
Motivation and feedback and the specific prepractice varia- of the three subcategories. There was consistent confusion
bles instructions, explanations, and perceptual training had between explanations and instructions due to the subtle dif-
the lowest inter-rater agreements. The use of both general ference in definitions for these variables. Explanations typi-
categories (ie, verbal information) and specific variables cally involve a task description whereas instructions involve
(ie, instructions, explanations, and perceptual training) task description and an imperative to perform. A similar
within a single classification task, which is common to difficulty in distinguishing behaviors that included informa-
many similar checklists eg,64,65 may have improved speci- tion about the task but did not require the client to act was
ficity at the expense of accuracy. Distinguishing between noted in classification of motor learning behaviors in a reha-
different types of verbal information may be difficult due bilitation setting with children with coordination disor-
to the often implicit meaning of some statements and the ders.52 They called this variable “sharing knowledge” in
wider context in which they might be uttered. order to clearly distinguish it from both feedback and
instructions.
When operationalizing the variables for the classification
Multiple classifications of specific behaviors
task, there was difficulty in clearly defining each of the spe-
Analysis of the patterns of disagreement showed that the
cific variables. As these variables have typically been inves-
tendency to give multiple classifications to one specific
tigated independently, there is some overlap between their
behavior was unusual, and only shown by a few participants
definitions.67 For example, auditory perceptual training
(reducing overall reliability). Some participants picked up
may also be considered as auditory modeling. One way to
on more subtle meanings of phrases whilst other partici-
increase mutual exclusivity would be to pay careful consid-
pants only classified based on what was explicitly said. In
eration to the terminology. For example, renaming percep-
the VFE video segment, the client attempts the movement
tual training as “attention to sensory effects” may assist to
and then the clinician says, “I'm going to stop you.” Whilst
more clearly differentiate between modeling and perceptual
the explicit instruction is to stop, feedback that the move-
training.
ment was wrong is also implied by that instruction. All par-
ticipants identified the explicit meaning (instruction) and
two participants also identified the more implicit meaning Conversational behaviors
(feedback) in their classification. Thus, the reliability for Other behaviors which showed high disagreement were con-
feedback was reduced by “reinstruction” behaviors.66 Reli- versational-type behaviors. Classifications for behaviors
ability for motivation was similarly reduced for explanations such as the clinician asking, “OK?” were often split between
about the importance of the task where all participants multiple categories (eg, other, other verbal information,
ARTICLE IN PRESS
10 Journal of Voice, Vol. &&, No. &&, 2019

feedback). Excluding these conversational behaviors from Limitations and future directions
analysis would have increased the reliability in applying the The current study was undertaken as an initial exploration
MLCF. Conversational behavior may, however, be consid- of the use of the MLCF in helping to identify clinically rele-
ered a variable in and of itself. Whilst not specific to motor vant behaviors for effective learning. This study was, how-
learning,52 conversational behaviors are a clinical reality ever, limited by the small number of participants and the
and it is possible that these behaviors serve an important lack of diversity in the participants. Because we only col-
but as yet undescribed function in motor acquisition. For lected frequency data, we were also limited by the types of
example, they may establish and maintain motivation for analysis we could perform. In future, it will be important to
therapy, trust in the clinician through personal rapport and replicate and extend this work, comparing across more par-
/or provide necessary breaks from practice. These types of ticipants, and across participants of varying ability/experi-
conversational behaviors may be better understood using a ence. Online technologies can be used in future work to
systemic functional linguistics analysis61 rather than a facilitate ease and accuracy of classification (for example,
motor learning behavior analysis. Bridge2Practice68). Application of the MCLF to other areas
of voice training (eg, singing and spoken voice training) and
SLP practice (eg, motor speech, stuttering, dysphagia, etc)
Motor learning behaviors as differentiating
should be investigated to evaluate if there are specific motor
ingredients of different therapies
learning concepts that are operationalized differently
Analysis of the frequency of different prepractice variables in
depending on context. Similarly, the MLCF can be applied
the video segments of the three therapies showed high variabil-
to the teaching of any motor or perceptual skills in disci-
ity. This variability may be due to inherent differences in the
plines such as Medicine, Surgery, Physiotherapy, Occupa-
therapy approaches, individual clinician's differences, individ-
tional Therapy, Medical Radiation Sciences, and Exercise
ual patient differences or the production decisions made in the
and Sports Physiology.
video segments used in this study. All videos chosen were mar-
keted as demonstration videos in which the therapy is demon-
strated by the author of the therapy technique using a real CONCLUSIONS
patient. This suggests that these videos can be considered We provided some initial evidence supporting the use of a
exemplars of the specific therapy approach. An analysis of the proposed MLCF as a tool for analyzing the prepractice
therapy using the MLCF may provide insight in future phase of voice-therapy interventions. The general variables
research as to whether the motor learning behaviors used in a modeling and verbal information were identified reliably,
therapy might be considered as an “active ingredient” as much however, participants were less reliable in identifying moti-
as the underlying skill being taught. vation and feedback. Clearer instructions to participants
regarding the depth of analysis required for these behaviors,
along with a few more examples and further practice,
Clinical implications
should help increase reliability for these variables. The
The aim of developing the MLCF was to provide clinicians
reported difficulty in identifying specific prepractice varia-
with a means of identifying behaviors and interactions in ther-
bles such as explanations and perceptual training suggests
apeutic and training interventions. This would allow them to
the need for clearer definitions and distinctions between
consciously and more accurately follow explicit guidelines for
these variables and further research to determine if they are
the different stages of intervention. It appears that the MLCF
separable and involve different motor learning principles.
could be both refined and expanded. For example, it could be
There has been a recent move to explore motor learning
used to categorize and systematically analyze the instructional
principles in more applied settings. This initial analysis of
behaviors used by clinicians to prepare their client for prac-
the use and discrimination of motor learning variables in
tice. As such the MCLF could play an important role in edu-
voice therapy sessions paves the way for future research to
cating students and clinicians on the various behaviors that
better evaluate behaviors in SLP settings, with an eye to
can be utilized across the learning process.
analyzing whether principles of good motor learning are
Identification of different variables in the three voice ther-
being adhered in such settings and whether there are associ-
apy approaches might speak to inherent differences in the
ated consequences for treatment outcomes when they are
process of the specific voice therapy approach, or it may
not. Although we only provide a simple discrimination, fur-
just reflect differences in the style of the individual clinician
ther development of the MLCF would allow for a more in-
and/or client. The use of the MCLF presents an opportunity
depth analysis of SLP interventions, ultimately helping to
to further understand differences between voice therapy
identify key behaviors for success such that clinicians can
techniques and provides insight into which approach may
become more effective teachers of voice motor skills.
be more or less efficacious depending on the clinician's and
client's individual characteristics. This may ultimately help
us understand what the critical “active ingredients” are for Acknowledgments
successful therapy. Whether it will be possible to apply this Our sincere thanks to the participants, to Dr Robert Heard
classification analysis to real-life clinical interactions is yet for advice regarding research design and statistics, to San-
to be explored. tino Di Bartolo for assistance with video editing, and to Dr
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Catherine Madill, et al Classifying Motor Learning Behaviors in Voice Therapy 11

Duong Nguyen for manuscript preparation. This study was Examples:


supported by the Dr Liang Voice Program at The Univer-
sity of Sydney.  Asking the client to attend to a sensation
(eg, “I want you to notice if you feel more
vibrations.”).
 Asking the client to remember or self-evaluate a
APPENDIX A: WRITTEN DEFINITIONS OF sensation
PREPRACTICE VARIABLES (eg, “How did that feel/sound?”)
General prepractice variables: Definitions  Contrasting one sensation with another
(participants) (eg, “Which way felt easier?” using an old way/new
way).
1. Motivation
3. Modeling
The clinician engages in behavior which aims to increase
the client's motivation to participate in the therapy task. A physical demonstration of the movement is provided to
Examples: the client to assist them to develop an understanding of the
target movement. Demonstrations may be provided before
 Outlining the importance or benefits of the task.
or during the client's movement attempts.
 Setting goals for therapy.
The demonstration can be:
2. Verbal Information  Part of a movement.
 The whole movement.
The clinician verbally communicates what the target  Correct or incorrect movement (as in negative prac-
movement is and what they need to do to achieve the target. tice).
Common ways that clinicians provide this information is  A small hand gesture modeling some part of the
through instructions, explanations, and perceptual training. movement (eg, point to the upper lip to indicate
2 a. Instructions: The clinician provides information to that the client needs to project the sound forward).
the client about what they should do to produce the target
movement. Instructions occur before or during the client's 4. Feedback
movement attempt.
Instructions may be: The clinician makes a judgment about the client's move-
ment attempt. Feedback can occur during or after a move-
 Short and simple. ment.
 Long and complex. Examples:
 Refer to part of the movement.
 Refer to the movement holistically, eg, using meta-  Knowledge of Results (ie, correct/ incorrect or
phors (“Open your throat like when you yawn”). clear/husky).
 Knowledge of Performance (eg, “You brought the
Note: Instructions may include perceptual terms, how- vocal folds together too hard that time.”).
ever, the focus of the utterance is what the client should do
or the action to perform (eg, “I want you to slide into that Note: Feedback may include perceptual / sensory terms,
“m” so that it is a nice, smooth sound”). however the focus of the utterance is that the clinician is
2 b. Explanation: The clinician provides information and making a judgement (eg, “That sounded clearer.”).
descriptions to the client to help them understand some
aspect of the target movement or something related to the 5. Other-please explain
task. Explanations may occur before or after the movement.
Examples: Use this code if you cannot classify any behavior or phrase
according to the suggested variables. Please include a brief
 Information about the mechanical or biomechani- description of the clinical relevance of the behavior/phrase.
cal properties (eg, vocal fold vibration). Examples:
 Elaboration of an instruction.
 Acknowledgment of a client utterance (not an attempt)
2 c. Perceptual Training: Perceptual training involves rais-  Consolation of the client
ing the client's awareness of sensations to develop under-
standing of the target movement. The client notices and Multiple classifications: You are able to rate one utter-
reflects on what they see, feel, or hear during or after they ance or behavior as more than one variable. However, you
attempt a new movement. Perceptual training may occur are encouraged to differentiate between the variables as
before, during or after the client's movement attempts. much as possible.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
12 Journal of Voice, Vol. &&, No. &&, 2019

APPENDIX B: OBSERVATION CHECKLIST

REFERENCES
1. Langmore SE, Pisegna JM. Efficacy of exercises to rehabilitate dysphagia: 11. Mathieson L. Greene and Mathieson's the Voice and Its Disorders. (6th
a critique of the literature. Int J Speech Lang Pathol. 2015;17:222–229. ed.) Chichester, UK: Wiley and Sons Ltd; 2001.
2. Maas, Butalla, Farinella. Feedback frequency in treatment for child- 12. Eastwood C, Madill C, McCabe P. The behavioural treatment of mus-
hood apraxia of speech. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2012;21:239–257. cle tension voice disorders: a systematic review. Int J Speech Lang
3. Murray E, McCabe P, Ballard KJ. A randomized controlled trial for Pathol. 2015;17:287–303.
children with childhood apraxia of speech comparing rapid syllable 13. Van Stan JH, Roy N, Awan S, et al. A taxonomy of voice therapy. Am
transition treatment and the Nuffield Dyspraxia Programme−Third J Speech Lang Pathol. 2015;24:101–125.
Edition. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2015;58:669–686. 14. Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor Control And Learning: A Behavioural
4. Joscelyne-May C, Madill CJ, Thorpe W, et al. The effect of clinician Emphasis. (4th ed.) Champaign, Illinois: Human Kinetics; 2005.
feedback type on the acquisition of a vocal siren. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 15. Adams JA. A closed-loop theory of motor learning. J Motor Behav.
2015;67:57–67. 1971;3:111–150.
5. Look C, McCabe P, Heard R, et al. Show and tell: video modeling and 16. Maas E, Robin DA, Austermann Hula SN, et al. Principles of motor
instruction without feedback improves performance but is not sufficient learning in treatment of motor speech disorders. Am J Speech Lang
for retention of a complex voice motor skill. J Voice. 2019;33:239–249. Pathol. 2008;17:277–298.
6. Meerschman I, Van Lierde K, Van Puyvelde C, et al. Massed versus 17. Wulf G, Shea CH. Principles derived from the study of simple skills do
spaced practice in vocology: effect of a short-term intensive voice train- not generalize to complex skill learning. Psychonom Bullet Rev.
ing versus a longer-term traditional voice training. Int J Lang Commun 2002;9:185–211.
Disord. 2018;53:393–404. 18. McNeil MR. Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders.
7. Ma EPM, Yiu GKY, Yiu EML. The effects of self-controlled feedback 2nd ed Thieme New York; 2009.
on learning of a “relaxed phonation task”. J Voice. 2013;27:723–728. 19. Hodges NJ, Franks IM. Modelling coaching practice: the role of
8. Steinhauer K, Grayhack JP. The role of knowledge of results in perfor- instruction and demonstration. J Sports Sci. 2002;20:793–811.
mance and learning of a voice motor task. J Voice. 2000;14:137–145. 20. Schumann A, Liebscher T, Hoppe U. Phoneme discrimination training
9. Yiu EML, Verdolini K, Chow LPY. Electromyographic study of with experienced cochlear implant listeners. HNO. 2016;64:751–758.
motor learning for a voice production task. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 21. Brown S, Martinez MJ. Activation of premotor vocal areas during
2005;48:1254–1268. musical discrimination. Brain Cogn. 2007;63:59–69.
10. Roy N. Optimal dose-response relationships in voice therapy. Int J 22. Meegan DV, Aslin RN, Jacobs RA. Motor timing learned without
Speech Lang Pathol. 2012;14:419–423. motor training. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:860.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Catherine Madill, et al Classifying Motor Learning Behaviors in Voice Therapy 13

23. Granados C, Wulf G. Enhancing motor learning through dyad prac- 45. Hodges, Ste-Marie. Observation as an instructional method. Develop-
tice: contributions of observation and dialogue. Res Quart Exerc Sport. ing Sport Expertise: Researchers and Coaches Put Theory into Practice.
2007;78:197–203. (2nd ed) 2013. p. 115−128.
24. Ram N, Riggs S, Skaling S, et al. A comparison of modelling and 46. Andrieux M, Proteau L. Mixed observation favors motor learning
imagery in the acquisition and retention of motor skills. J Sports Sci. through better estimation of the model's performance. Exp Brain Res.
2007;25:587–597. 2014;232:3121–3132.
25. Behrman A, Rutledge J, Hembree A, et al. Vocal hygiene education, 47. Rohbanfard H, Proteau L. Learning through observation: a combina-
voice production therapy, and the role of patient adherence: a treat- tion of expert and novice models favors learning. Exp Brain Res.
ment effectiveness study in women with phonotrauma. J Speech Lang 2011;215:183–197.
Hear Res. 2008;51:350–366. 48. Weeks DL, Anderson LP. The interaction of observational learning
26. Hodges NJ, Chua R, Franks IM. The role of video in facilitating per- with overt practice: effects on motor skill learning. Acta Psychologica.
ception and action of a novel coordination movement. J Sport Exercise 2000;104:259–271.
Psychol. 2002;24:69–70. 49. Potter W.J., LevineDonnerstein D.. Rethinking validity and reliabil-
27. Steinhauer K, Grayhack JP. The role of knowledge of results in perfor- ity in content analysis. J Appl Commun Res 1999;27:258−284.
mance and learning of a voice motor task. J Voice. 2000;14:137–145. 50. Cushion CJ, Jones RL. A systematic observation of professional top-
28. Swinnen SP. Information feedback for motor skill learning: a review. level youth soccer coaches. J Sport Behav. 2001;24:354–376.
Adv Motor Learn Control. 1996:37–66. 51. Niemeijer AS, Schoemaker MM, Smits-Engelsman BC. Are teaching
29. Guadagnoli MA, Lee TD. Challenge point: a framework for conceptu- principles associated with improved motor performance in children
alizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. with developmental coordination disorder? A pilot study. Phys Ther.
J Motor Behav. 2004;36:212–224. 2006;86:1221–1230.
30. Lee TD, Swinnen SP, Serrien DJ. Cognitive effort and motor learning. 52. Niemeijer AS, Smits-Engelsman BC, Reynders K, et al. Verbal actions
Quest. 1994;46:328–344. of physiotherapists to enhance motor learning in children with DCD.
31. Gentile AM. Movement science: implicit and explicit processes during Hum Mov Sci. 2003;22:567–581.
acquisition of functional skills. Scand J Occup Ther. 1998;5:7–16. 53. Ford P, Yates I, Williams A. An analysis of activities and instruc-
32. Bose A, Square PA, Schlosser R, et al. Effects of PROMPT therapy on tional behaviours used by coaches during practice in English youth
speech motor function in a person with aphasia and apraxia of speech. soccer: exploring the link between theory and practice. J Sport Sci.
Aphasiology. 2001;15:767–785. 2010;28:483–495.
33. Bagnall A. Voicecraft workshop manual. Adelaide, SA: Voicecraft 54. Williams AM, Hodges NJ. Practice, instruction and skill acquisition in
International; 1997. soccer: Challenging tradition. J Sport Sci. 2005;23:637–650.
34. Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor Control And Learning: A Behavioral 55. Potrac P, Jones R, Cushion C. Understanding power and the coach's
Emphasis. 5th ed. Champaign, IL: Human kinetics; 2011. role in professional English soccer: a preliminary investigation of coach
35. Wulf G, Prinz W. Directing attention to movement effects enhances behaviour. Soccer Soc. 2007;8:33–49.
learning: a review. Psychonom Bullet Rev. 2001;8:648–660. 56. ASHA. Guidelines for manual pure-tone threshold audiometry. 2005.
36. Buszard T., Masters R.S. A culture of striving augments use of work- 57. Abbott KV. Lessac-Madsen Resonant Voice Therapy: Overview (Vid-
ing memory? Implications for attention control. Progress Brain Res. eorecording). San Diego: Plural Publishing; 2008.
2017;232:197−200. 58. Bagnall AD, Sprod R. Yell Well: AB Voice International; 1995.
37. Masters RS. Knowledge, knerves and knowhow: the role of explicit 59. Stemple J. Vocal Function Exercises (Videorecording). San Diego:
versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill Communicare Publishing; 2002.
under pressure. Br J Psychol. 1992;83:343–358. 60. Verdolini K, Druker DG, Palmer PM, et al. Laryngeal adduction in
38. Poolton JM, Zhu FF, Malhotra N, et al. Multitask training promotes resonant voice. J Voice. 1998;12:315–327.
automaticity of a fundamental laparoscopic skill without compromis- 61. Angadi V, Croake D, Stemple J. Effects of vocal function exercises: a
ing the rate of skill learning. Surg Endosc. 2016;30:4011–4018. systematic review. J Voice. 2019;33. 124.e13−124.e34.
39. Verdolini-Marston K, Balota DA. Role of elaborative and perceptual 62. Frensch PA. Composition during serial learning: a serial position
integrative processes in perceptual-motor performance. J Exp Psychol effect. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;20:423–442.
Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;20:739–749. 63. Portney LG, Watkins MP. Foundations of Clinical Research: Applica-
40. Davids KW. The constraints-based approach to motor learning: impli- tions to Practice. 3rd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Prentice
cations for a non-linear pedagogy in sport and physical education. In: Hall; 2009.
Renshaw I, Davids KW, Savelsbergh GJP, eds. Motor Learning in 64. Damico JS. Clinical discouse analysis: a functional approach to lan-
Practice: A Constraints-Led Approach. London: Routledge (Taylor & guage assessment. Communication Skills and Classroom Success. 1985
Francis Group); 2010:3–16. p 165−204.
41. Ara ujo D, Davids K, Bennett SJ, et al. 19. Emergence of sport skills 65. Prutting CA, Kittchner DM. A clinical appraisal of the pragmatic
under constraints. Skill Acquisition in Sport: Research, Theory and aspects of language. J Speech Hear Disord. 1987;52:105–119.
Practice. UK: Routledge; 2004:409–433. 66. Lacy AC, Darst PW. Evolution of a systematic observation system:
42. Kim S. The effect of two different singing instructions on subglottal the ASU coaching observation instrument. J Teach Phys Educ.
pressure, sound pressure level, and airflow rate. The Voice Foundation 1984;3:59–66.
46th Annual Symposium. 2017. Philadelphia, PA. 67. Verschuren PJ. Holism versus reductionism in modern social science
43. Hodges NJ, Franks IM. Modelling coaching practice: the role of research. Qual Quant. 2001;35:389–405.
instruction and demonstration. J Sports Sci. 2002;20:793–811. 68. Madill C, So T, Corcoran S. Bridge2practice: Translating theory into
44. Beek PJ. Toward a theory of implicit learning in the perceptual-motor practice 2019. Available at: https://bridge2practice.com/. Accessed
domain. Int J Sport Psychol. 2000;31:547–554. March 7, 2019.

You might also like