You are on page 1of 4

Psychotherapy Research

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tpsr20

The implications of the Dodo bird verdict for


training in psychotherapy: prioritizing process
observation

Henny A. Westra

To cite this article: Henny A. Westra (2023) The implications of the Dodo bird verdict for
training in psychotherapy: prioritizing process observation, Psychotherapy Research, 33:4,
527-529, DOI: 10.1080/10503307.2022.2141588

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2022.2141588

Published online: 16 Dec 2022.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 2700

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tpsr20
Psychotherapy Research, 2023
Vol. 33, No. 4, 527–529, https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2022.2141588

ARTICLE COMMENTARY

The implications of the Dodo bird verdict for training in psychotherapy:


prioritizing process observation

HENNY A. WESTRA

Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada


(Received 24 October 2022; accepted 25 October 2022)

Wampold et al.’s 1997 meta-analysis found that the with experience and in fact, the evidence suggests
true differences between bona fide psychotherapies that skills may decline slightly over time (Goldberg
is zero, supporting the Dodo bird conjecture that et al., 2016). Not surprisingly then, while the
“All have won and must have prizes”. Two and half number of model-based treatments has proliferated,
decades later, the field continues to be slow to the rate of client improvement has not followed suit
absorb this and similar uncomfortable discoveries. (Miller et al., 2013). Could stagnant training
For example, entirely commensurate with Wam- methods may be related to stagnant patient
pold’s conclusion is the meta-analytic finding that outcomes?
adherence to a given model of psychotherapy is unre- We need innovations in training that better align
lated to therapy outcomes (Webb et al., 2010). our training foci and methods with factors empiri-
Despite the clear implication that theoretical cally supported as influencing client outcomes.
models should not be the main lens through which Process researchers have long observed that trained
psychotherapy is viewed if we are aiming to process coders (typically for research purposes)
improve outcomes, therapists continue to identify make better therapists due to their enhanced attune-
themselves primarily by their theoretical orientation. ment (e.g., Binder & Strupp, 1997). While such
And a major corollary of Wampold’s conclusions is training is not yet available in training programs, it
that despite the evidence for non superiority of a arguably should be based on emerging developments
given model, our focus in training continues to be in the science of expertise (Ericsson & Pool, 2016)
model-driven. This article seeks to elaborate the and the urgent need to bring outcome information
training implications of Wampold et al.’s conclusion, forward in real time so that it can be used to make
with a rationale and appeal to incorporate process- responsive adjustments to the process of therapy. In
centered training. fact, such information could be considered “routine
Consider these similarly uncomfortable findings outcome monitoring in real time” (Westra & Di Bar-
regarding the state of training. We assume, rather tolomeo, 2022).
than verify the efficacy of our training programs. To elaborate, Tracey et al. (2014) provocatively
Yet, there is no evidence that continuing education argued that acquiring expertise in psychotherapy
workshops for example, produce sustained skill may not even be possible. This is because the
development—quite the opposite. Large effects on ability to predict outcomes is crucial to shaping effec-
self-report are found but any modest improvement tive performance. Yet there is a lack of feedback
in performance erodes over time without further available to therapists regarding the outcomes of
coaching (Madson et al., 2019). Perhaps most con- their interventions and such information, if it
cerning, psychotherapists do not appear to improve comes at all, comes too late to make a difference in

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Henny A. Westra, Department of Psychology, York University, 127 Behav-
ioural Sciences Bldg., Toronto, ON, Canada M3J 1P3. Email: hwestra@yorku.ca

© 2022 Society for Psychotherapy Research


528 H. A. Westra

the moment. Therapists are essentially like blind It seemed that we had incidentally trained ourselves
archers attempting to shoot at a target. The develop- to detect and use these process signals. Our training
ment of Routine Outcome Monitoring (ROM) was different and very unique; it was more akin to
measures capable of forecasting likely outcomes is a deliberate practice focused on discrimination train-
major advance in correcting this blindness and ing for detecting empirically supported processes.
improving predictive capacity. However, in order to In short, information capable of diagnosing the
be effective for skill development, feedback needs health of the process and critically, of forecasting
to occur more immediately so that the relationship eventual outcomes is arguably hiding in plain sight if
between the therapist action and the client response one can acquire the requisite observational capacity
(or nonresponse) can be quickly ascertained and to harvest it. And transforming an unpredictable
adjustments made in real time. Interestingly, while environment into a predictable one makes expertise
ROM has been helpful in improving failing cases, it possible to acquire (Kahneman, 2011). Importantly,
has not been effective in enhancing clinical skills extracting such vital information relies on observa-
more generally (Miller et al., 2013). tional skill, rather than patient report, end of session
Learning to preferentially attend to, extract and measures, or longer-term outcome; thus, such real
continuously integrate empirically supported time data extraction is immediately accessible and
process data may prove to be the elusive immediate can complement existing outcome monitoring
feedback that has been lacking in psychotherapy (Westra & Di Bartolomeo, 2022). Moreover, process
training but that is crucial to developing expertise. markers are often opaque; requiring systematic obser-
Observable process data that has been validated vational training for successful detection. Without
through process science as differentiating good proper discrimination and perceptual acuity training,
from poor patient outcomes, could be considered this gilded information remains obscured. Thus,
“little outcomes”; which in turn are related to heeding Wampold et al.’s call to refocus our efforts
session outcomes and ultimately treatment outcome must include innovations in training; innovations
(Greenberg, 1986). Moreover, thin-slicing research that harness outcome information. We need more
supports that it is possible to make judgements process research to further uncover the immediately
about important outcomes from even tiny slices of observable factors capable of differentiating poor
expressive behavior (Ambady & Rosenthal, 1992). and good outcomes, but existing process science
If one considers real time process information as gives us a good start. And since process-centered
micro-outcomes, properly trained clinicians, just training is transtheoretical, it can exist alongside
like expert-trained process coders, may no longer models of therapy—learning to see while doing
have to be blind. For example, a therapist trained (Binder & Strupp, 1997). Training in psychotherapy
to identify and monitor resistance and signals of alli- has primarily prioritized intervention (models) and
ance ruptures, can be continuously tracking these now it may be time to emphasize observation.
important phenomena and responsively adjusting to
safeguard the alliance. Or a therapist who is sensitive
to markers of low and high levels of experiencing Disclosure Statement
(Pascual-Leone & Yeryomenko, 2017) and client
ambivalence (Westra & Norouzian, 2018) can not No potential conflict of interest was reported by the
only optimize the timing of their interventions but author(s).
also continuously watch the client for feedback on
the success of their ongoing efforts.
Being steeped in process research gives one a References
unique perspective on the promise of process obser- Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive be-
vation to advance clinical training. Our lab recently havior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-
took our first foray into studying practicing commu- analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111(2), 256–274. https://doi.
nity therapists. As we coded the session videotapes, org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.256
Binder, J. L., & Strupp, H. H. (1997). “Negative process”: A
we became aware that we possessed a unique skill
recurrently discovered and underestimated facet of therapeutic
set that was absent in therapist’s test interviews. process and outcome in the individual psychotherapy of adults.
Therapists seemed to be guided solely by some Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 4(2), 121–139. https://
model of how to bring about change but failed to sim- doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1997.tb00105.x
ultaneously appreciate the ebb and flow of the rela- Ericsson, A., & Pool, R. (2016). Peak: Secrets from the new science of
tional context of the work. They seemed absorbed expertise. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Goldberg, S. B., Rousmaniere, T., Miller, S. D., Whipple, J.,
in their own moves (their model) but not aware Nielsen, S. L., Hoyt, W. T., & Wampold, B. E. (2016). Do psy-
that they were in a dance and must continually chotherapists improve with time and experience? A longitudi-
track and coordinate the process with their partner. nal analysis of outcomes in a clinical setting. Journal of
Psychotherapy Research 529
Counseling Psychology, 63(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/ (6), 653–665. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2016.
cou0000131 1152409
Greenberg, L. S. (1986). Change process research. Journal of Tracey, T. J. G., Wampold, B. E., Lichtenberg, J. W., &
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(1), 4–9. https://doi.org/ Goodyear, R. K. (2014). Expertise in psychotherapy: An
10.1037/0022-006X.54.1.4 elusive goal? American Psychologist, 69(3), 218–229. https://
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and doi.org/10.1037/a0035099
Giroux. Webb, C. A., DeRubeis, R. J., & Barber, J. P. (2010). Therapist
Madson, M. B., Villarosa-Hurlocker, M. C., Schumacher, J. A., adherence/competence and treatment outcome: A meta-ana-
Williams, D. C., & Gauthier, J. M. (2019). Motivational inter- lytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78
viewing training of substance use treatment professionals: A (2), 200–211. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018912
systematic review. Substance Abuse, 40(1), 43–51. https://doi. Westra, H. A., & Di Bartolomeo, A. (2022). Developing expertise
org/10.1080/08897077.2018.1475319 in psychotherapy. The case for process coding as clinical train-
Miller, S. D., Hubble, M. A., Chow, D. L., & Seidel, J. A. (2013). ing [Manuscript submitted for publication], Department of
The outcome of psychotherapy: Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Psychology, York University.
Psychotherapy, 50(1), 88–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031097 Westra, H. A., & Norouzian, N. (2018). Using motivational inter-
Pascual-Leone, A., & Yeryomenko, N. (2017). The client “experi- viewing to manage process markers of ambivalence and resist-
encing” scale as a predictor of treatment outcomes: A meta- ance in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy. Cognitive Therapy and
analysis on psychotherapy process. Psychotherapy Research, 27 Research, 42, 193–200.

You might also like