You are on page 1of 10

Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228

DOI 10.1007/s10508-007-9208-x

ORIGINAL PAPER

Sex Differences in Visual Attention to Erotic and Non-Erotic


Stimuli
Amy D. Lykins Æ Marta Meana Æ Gregory P. Strauss

Received: 4 September 2006 / Revised: 29 March 2007 / Accepted: 29 March 2007 / Published online: 1 August 2007
 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Abstract It has been suggested that sex differences in the Introduction


processing of erotic material (e.g., memory, genital arousal,
brain activation patterns) may also be reflected by differ- Despite a recent meta-analysis indicating that men and
ential attention to visual cues in erotic material. To test this women may be more similar than different on a host of
hypothesis, we presented 20 heterosexual men and 20 het- psychological factors expected to show sex differentiation
erosexual women with erotic and non-erotic images of het- (Hyde, 2005), sexuality remains one of a number of areas in
erosexual couples and tracked their eye movements during which significant and persistent sex differences with large
scene presentation. Results supported previous findings that effect sizes abound (Lippa, 2005). In terms of cognition, men
erotic and non-erotic information was visually processed in a and women appear to process sexual information differently.
different manner by both men and women. Men looked at They selectively focus on different aspects of erotic stimuli,
opposite sex figures significantly longer than did women, and both implicitly and explicitly demonstrate different
and women looked at same sex figures significantly longer preferences and attitudes in regards to sexuality.
than did men. Within-sex analyses suggested that men had a The sexual content-induced delay, which refers to the
strong visual attention preference for opposite sex figures as interfering effect of erotic content on information processing
compared to same sex figures, whereas women appeared to time (i.e., decision times for word identification and dot
disperse their attention evenly between opposite and same probes, as well as overall reading times) (Geer & Manguno-
sex figures. These differences, however, were not limited to Mire, 1996), appears to be more pronounced in women (Geer
erotic images but evidenced in non-erotic images as well. No & Bellard, 1996; Geer & Manguno-Mire, 1996; Geer &
significant sex differences were found for attention to the Melton, 1997). This suggests that sexual information creates
contextual region of the scenes. Results were interpreted as more interference or contains more emotional content for
potentially supportive of recent studies showing a greater women than it does for men. Women have also consistently
non-specificity of sexual arousal in women. This interpre- demonstrated a preference and better memory for romantic
tation assumes there is an erotic valence to images of the sex words, images, and films than sexual ones, in contrast to
to which one orients, even when the image is not explicitly men, who show attentional and memory biases for the latter
erotic. It also assumes a relationship between visual attention (Geer & Bellard, 1996; Geer, Judice, & Jackson, 1994; Geer
and erotic valence. & McGlone, 1990; Laan, Everaerd, van Bellen, & Hane-
wald, 1994; Mosher & MacIan, 1994; Sigusch, Schmidt,
Keywords Sex differences  Visual attention  Reinfeld, & Wiedermann-Sutor, 1970; Wells, 1990).
Erotic stimuli Confirmatory evidence for these sex differences can be
also found in the largely self-report based sexual fantasy
literature. In their comprehensive review of this literature,
Leitenberg and Henning (1995) concluded that men’s sexual
A. D. Lykins (&)  M. Meana  G. P. Strauss fantasies focused more on explicit sexual acts, nude bodies,
Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Las Vegas,
Box 455030, Las Vegas, NV, 89154-5030, USA
and physical gratification, whereas emotional context and
e-mail: lykinsa@unlv.nevada.edu romance were more often found in women’s fantasies.

123
220 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228

Finally, women appear to have more negative implicit and In order for individuals to process visual sexual infor-
explicit attitudes toward sex as indicated by self-report and mation such as that utilized in the aforementioned studies
tests of associations (Geer & Robertson, 2005; Oliver & Hyde, (e.g., written passages, photos, videos), they must first
1993). Interestingly, despite the apparently more complex or visually attend to said stimuli. However, to our knowledge,
even conflicted cognitive processes associated with sexual there currently exist no controlled studies on sex differences
stimuli in women, the stimulus features required to evoke a in the visual processing of sexual material. Visual attention
genital response in women appear to be much less specific is arguably the most elementary step in the cognitive pro-
than in men (Chivers, 2005; Chivers, Rieger, Latty, & Bailey, cessing of information and clearly a primary component of
2004). Women are genitally aroused to a greater extent than most sexual experiences. Because of this lack of knowledge,
men to sexual stimuli that do not correspond to their sexual we are left to wonder: do men and women attend to different
orientation. scene regions when presented with the same erotic stimuli?
It is important to note that some studies have found no Eye-tracking methodology is uniquely suited to the
significant sex differences in the processing or arousability investigation of this question as it is a reliable and valid
of sexual information, with both men and women reporting measure of attention with a relatively long history of appli-
greater arousability to genital rather than romantic themes cation in the reading and scene perception literature (Rayner,
(Meuwissin & Over, 1991; Schmidt, Siguisch, & Schafer, 1995). In a first test to determine the validity of eye-tracking
1973; Smith & Over, 1991; Tokatlidis & Over, 1995). One methodology as a measure of differences in the visual pro-
study actually found men more susceptible than women to cessing of erotic and non-erotic material, Lykins, Meana,
cognitive interference from both sexual and romantic con- and Kambe (2006) demonstrated that both men and women
tent (Kirsch-Rosenkrantz & Geer, 1991). However, these visually attended differently to erotic and non-erotic images.
studies are outnumbered by those that find women to be more More specifically, their results strongly indicated that men
romantically focused and more susceptible to the interfering and women attended more to bodies than to faces or context
effect of sexual content than are men. in the erotic stimuli, in terms of total time spent looking at a
Geer and Manguno-Mire (1996) suggested that some of region as well as in terms of total number of fixations.
these sex differences may be explained by differential Although not surprising, the results of this study provided
attention to visual cues in erotic material. Recent investi- evidence that eye-tracking methodology could capture dif-
gations have revealed sex differences in both self-reported ferences in the processing of erotic and non-erotic material at
sexual arousal and brain activation patterns while viewing the level of visual attention.
erotic stimuli. Janssen, Carpenter, and Graham (2003) found Having established the validity of this methodology, we
significant differences in self-reported sexual arousal to then turned to the current investigation of potential sex dif-
different erotic film clips, such that men reported much ferences in the visual processing of erotic stimuli. To this
higher levels of arousal to male-selected film clips than did end, we presented the same erotic and non-erotic images to
women. Youn (2006) recently reported sex differences in men and women and tested for differences in total time spent
subjective sexual arousal to an erotic video, regardless of looking at same and opposite sex faces and bodies. We also
presence or absence of guided imagery, supporting the tested separately for time spent looking at contextual aspects
contention that men are more psychosexually responsive to (non-human setting) of the scenes. Drawing from the liter-
visual erotic material than are women. Hamann, Herman, ature here briefly reviewed, we expected that both men and
Nolan, and Wallen (2004) found that certain brain regions women would focus more on bodies than on faces in the
involved in the processing of appetitive, emotional, and erotic scenes, but that men would show a stronger pattern of
biologically salient stimuli (the amygdala and hypothala- visual attention to opposite sex bodies than would women,
mus) were more activated in men than in women when particularly in the erotic stimuli. We also hypothesized that
viewing visual erotic stimuli. A recent study investigating women would attend visually more so than men to the
event-related potentials (ERPs) in response to erotic pictures contextual aspects of both the erotic and non-erotic scenes.
suggested that men and women may have equal rates of
activation, although men and women were not directly
compared (Anokhin et al., 2006). If the latter two studies and Method
their conclusions are indeed confirmed by future research, it
will raise the possibility that different brain structures are Participants
being activated in women when they view erotic stimuli.
Regardless of the direction of differences in the subjective All participants were 21 years of age or older, and had
and physiological responses of men and women, we still do normal or corrected to normal vision. The sample consisted
not know what each sex is actually attending to within the of 20 men and 20 women. All participants were right-han-
erotic image. ded, and self-identified as heterosexual. Results indicated

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228 221

that sociodemographic variables (e.g., ethnic and religious include body regions, such as the background and all of the
distributions) were comparable for men and women. Par- objects included in it.
ticipants remained naı̈ve with respect to the purpose of the
study until debriefing.
Apparatus

The stimuli were displayed at a resolution of 1024 ·


Measures
786 pixels on a True Color monitor (256 colors) using a
Radon VE ATI Graphics card operating at a refresh rate of
The stimuli consisted of five erotic and five non-erotic digital
85 Hz. Eye movements were recorded by an SMI Eyelink
photographic color scenes. Erotic images were collected
headband-mounted eye-tracker, which was carefully bal-
from a book depicting various sexual foreplay scenarios for
anced to be comfortable even with extended use. The system
heterosexual couples. The scenes consisted of couples in
used infra-red (940 nm) video-based technology to simul-
various states of undress, interactively engaged in some
taneously track the eyes and head position composition. Eye
aspect of foreplay, with facial expressions communicating
positions were sampled at 250 Hz. Viewing was binocular,
high sexual receptivity. The matched non-erotic images
although only the position of the right eye was tracked as is
were photographs taken by us and their composition was
common in scene perception eye-tracking research (e.g.,
guided by an attempt to maximize the parallelism between
Henderson, Weeks, & Hollingworth, 1999).
erotic and non-erotic images. Models were positioned sim-
ilarly to those in the erotic images, similar zoom distances
were employed, and backgrounds approximating those of Procedure
the erotic images were chosen. The models, however, were
fully clothed, had neutral facial expressions, and their bodily All stimuli and procedures were approved by the University
positions were adjusted to divest the photo of erotic infer- of Nevada, Las Vegas Institutional Review Board, and par-
ence. All images were 800 · 600 pixels and were viewed at ticipants received course research credit for their participa-
a distance of 82 cm. tion. Participants were briefed about the procedure of the
In order to test whether the manipulation of eroticism in experiment before it began and were encouraged to ask
the images was successful, we asked participants to rate questions at any time. A male research assistant tested the
how arousing they found each set of images. Participants male participants and the primary investigator (who is
endorsed one of five response options, ranging from ‘‘very female) tested the female participants. Once the eye-tracker
unarousing’’ to ‘‘very arousing.’’ On average, women and was placed upon the participant’s head, the equipment was
men rated the erotic images as somewhat arousing (women: calibrated. Calibration consisted of having the participant
M = 3.8, SD = .70; men: M = 3.70, SD = .73), and the non- fixate, or focus upon, nine markers on the display area, and
erotic images as generally somewhat unarousing (women: the calibration was checked by having the participant per-
M = 2.65, SD = .93; men: M = 2.55, SD = .83). A depen- form the same task again. The Eyelink system was calibrated
dent samples t-test revealed that the erotic images were rated to each individual until the average error in gaze position was
as significantly more arousing than the non-erotic images by 0.5. The practice session began once the eye-tracker was
both men and women (women: t[19] = 5.21, p < .001; men: successfully calibrated.
t[19] = 5.21, p < .001), providing evidence for the suc- In the practice session, participants were presented with
cessful manipulation of eroticism. three images of people in non-erotic situations and were
Each participant underwent a practice and an experi- instructed to ‘‘look at the pictures as you normally would.’’
mental phase. In the practice phase, participants viewed Once the practice session was completed, the experimental
neutral images of people in various non-erotic situations in session began. In the experimental session, each participant
order to acclimate to the equipment and procedure. In the test was presented with ten images of couples (five erotic and five
phase, the participant was shown the erotic and non-erotic non-erotic). The presentation of erotic and non-erotic image
couples images. All participants were exposed to all images. sets was counterbalanced across all subjects, so that an equal
In order to analyze visual attention to the different aspects number of participants saw all erotic images first, followed
of the scene, each image was divided into five scene regions: by all non-erotic images, versus all non-erotic images first,
male face, female face, male body, female body, and context. followed by all erotic images.
The face scene regions included the face and the hair (head) Upon completion of the eye-tracking portion of the study,
of the individual in the image. The body scene regions participants completed a short questionnaire including demo-
consisted of all of the body save the face region (i.e., torso, graphic variables and potential prior exposure to images or
arms, breasts or chest area, genitals, and legs). The context of individuals in the images used in the study. The experiment
the scene was defined as everything in the image that did not lasted approximately 20 min.

123
222 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228

Data analyses shown in Table 1. Results of the omnibus ANOVA for total
number of fixations are presented in Table 2. Significant
Our two dependent measures of interest, total number of fix- main effects were found for Stimulus Type, Sex Congru-
ations and total gaze time, are two of the most commonly used ency, and Scene Region. Three significant two-way inter-
and reported variables in the scene perception literature and actions and two three-way interactions were found and
considered indicative of the amount of visual attention a given analyzed using simple main effects.
scene region draws and maintains. Total number of fixations Replicating previous results (Lykins et al., 2006), there
was a count of the number of times the participant’s eye landed was a significant Stimulus Type · Scene Region interaction.
on any given scene region across the entire stimulus presen- There was a simple main effect for Stimulus Type (erotic vs.
tation time; number of fixations is considered to be a measure non-erotic), whereby both men and women looked at the
of drawing attention. Total gaze time was a measure of the faces more times in the non-erotic than the erotic stimuli,
total number of milliseconds the individual attended to a p = .05, whereas bodies were looked at more times in the
particular scene region across the entire stimulus presentation erotic than the non-erotic stimuli, p < .001. There was also a
time (in this case, 15 s); total gaze time is thought to be an significant simple main effect for Scene Region (face vs.
indication of overall interest in a given scene region. body) in both the erotic and non-erotic stimuli: men and
For the purpose of our analyses, we considered 4 factors: women looked at the bodies more times than at the faces in
Sex (male, female), Stimulus Type (erotic, non-erotic), both the erotic (p < .001) and non-erotic (p < .001) stimuli.
Sex Congruency of Scene Region (same sex, opposite sex), We also found a significant Sex Congruency · Scene
and Scene Region (face, body). Although Sex Congruency of Region interaction. A significant simple main effect was
Scene Region could have been incorporated into a 4-level found for Sex Congruency, such that men and women looked
Scene Region factor (same sex face, opposite sex face, same at opposite sex faces more times than at same sex faces,
sex body, opposite sex body), its consideration as a separate p < .001; similar results were found for opposite vs. same
2-level factor facilitated interpretation of results. To further sex bodies, p < .001. There was also a significant simple
clarify, when we compared men and women’s gaze times on main effect for Scene Region, such that in both the erotic and
opposite sex bodies, for example, we compared how long non-erotic stimuli, participants looked at the bodies signifi-
men looked at women’s bodies to how long women looked at cantly more times than at the faces, p < .001 and p < .001,
men’s bodies. We also considered one other scene region respectively. Thus, both opposite sex faces and bodies drew
separately: context (non-human elements of the scene: set- more attention than same sex faces and bodies, across
tings such as a picnic, an office, a gym etc.). The reason for stimulus types. Bodies also drew more attention than faces in
not including context as another level of the Scene Region general, regardless of whether individuals were attending to
factor in the 4-factor analysis was that it would preclude the opposite or same sex individuals (Fig. 1).
consideration of a Sex Congruency factor. In the Sex · Sex Congruency interaction, a significant
Results were thus analyzed in two 2 (Sex of Participant: simple main effect was found for Sex, such that women
Men vs. Women) · 2 (Stimulus Type: Erotic vs. Non-Ero- looked at the same sex individuals more times than did men,
tic) · 2 (Sex Congruency of Scene Region: Opposite Sex vs. p = .001, and men looked at the opposite sex individuals
Same Sex) · 2 (Scene Region: Face vs. Body) mixed design more times than did women, p < .001. There was also a
ANOVAs (one each for number of fixations and total gaze significant Sex Congruency simple main effect for men, such
time). The very high correlations between number of fixa- that men looked significantly more times at opposite sex than
tions and total gaze time precluded the use of MANOVA. same sex individuals, p < .001. No such effect was found for
Pairwise dependent sample and independent t-tests were women. Thus, men attended to opposite sex individuals
conducted for simple effects analyses, as appropriate. There more times than did women, and women attended to same
were also separate analyses conducted to compare men and sex individuals more times than did men. Also, men looked
women on number of fixations and gaze times devoted to at opposite sex individuals significantly more times than they
contextual aspects of the erotic and non-erotic photographs. did same sex individuals, whereas there was no difference in
women’s attention to opposite and same sex individuals
(Fig. 2).
Results Two significant three-way interactions were observed.
First, there was a significant Sex · Sex Congruency · Scene
Number of fixations Region interaction. A significant Sex simple main effect was
found, such that men looked at the opposite sex bodies more
Means and SDs of total number of fixations men and women times than did women, p < .001, and women looked at the
had on each of the different scene regions (opposite or same same sex bodies more times than did men, p < .001. No such
sex faces or bodies, contextual aspects of the scenes), are effects were found for number of fixations on either the

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228 223

Table 1 Means and SDs for women and men: total number of fixations as a function of Stimulus Type, Sex Congruency, and Scene Region
Female participants (n = 20) Male participants (n = 20) Male and female participants combined
(n = 40)
Erotic Non-erotic Both stimulus Erotic Non-erotic Both stimulus Erotic Non-erotic Both stimulus
stimuli stimuli types stimuli stimuli types stimuli stimuli types
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Opposite sex
Face 5.58 2.05 6.13 1.78 5.86 1.92 5.77 1.88 6.24 2.53 6.01 2.21 5.68 1.94 6.19 2.16 5.93 2.06
Body 10.70 2.38 6.69 2.20 8.70 3.04 15.38 3.63 10.95 3.13 13.17 4.03 13.04 3.85 8.82 3.43 10.93 4.20
Both 8.14 3.40 6.41 2.00 7.28 2.90 10.58 5.64 8.60 3.69 9.59 4.84 9.36 4.79 7.50 3.14 8.43 4.14
Same sex
Face 4.42 1.40 5.17 2.18 4.80 1.84 3.84 2.16 4.55 2.06 4.20 2.11 4.13 1.82 4.86 2.11 4.50 4.99
Body 10.91 3.46 8.66 1.53 9.79 2.87 8.34 2.34 5.46 1.99 6.90 2.59 9.63 3.19 7.06 2.39 8.34 3.08
Both 7.67 4.14 6.92 2.56 7.29 3.47 6.09 3.18 5.01 2.05 5.55 2.71 6.88 3.78 5.96 2.50 6.42 3.23
Opposite and same sex
Face 5.00 1.80 5.65 2.02 5.33 1.94 4.81 2.23 5.40 2.43 5.10 2.32 4.90 2.02 5.52 2.23 5.21 2.14
Body 10.81 2.93 7.68 2.12 9.24 2.99 11.86 4.67 8.21 3.80 10.03 4.61 11.33 3.91 7.94 3.07 9.64 3.90
Both 7.90 3.80 6.66 2.30 7.28 3.19 8.33 5.08 6.80 3.45 7.57 4.40 8.12 4.48 6.73 2.93 7.42 3.84
Context 8.52 4.38 13.60 4.24 11.06 4.97 9.47 5.68 14.59 5.66 12.03 6.17 9.00 5.03 14.10 4.96 11.55 5.59

opposite or same sex faces. Women looked at both opposite 12


Face
and same sex bodies more times than faces, p < .001 and Body

10
p < .001, respectively. Women also looked at the opposite
sex faces more times than at the same sex faces, p = .028,
Total # Fixations

8
whereas they looked more times at the same sex bodies than
at opposite sex bodies, p = .018. Men also looked at opposite 6
and same sex bodies more times than at opposite and same
4

2
Table 2 Analysis of variance on total number of fixations for 4-way
interaction (Sex · Stimulus Type · Sex Congruency · Scene Region) 0
Opposite Sex Same Sex
Source df F g2 p Sex Congruency

Between subjects Fig. 1 Sex Congruency · Scene Region interaction for total number
Sex of participant (S) 1 <1 .02 ns of fixations
S within-group error 38 (8.06)
Within subjects
Stimulus Type (T) 1 62.24 .62 <.001 sex faces, p < .001 and p < .001, respectively. Men looked
Sex Congruency (SC) 1 35.93 .49 <.001
at opposite sex faces more times than same sex faces,
Scene Region (R) 1 124.95 .77 <.001
T·R 1 101.33 .73 <.001 p < .001, and also looked more times at opposite than same
SC · R 1 7.14 .16 .011 sex bodies, p < .001.
S · SC 1 36.47 .49 <.001 A significant Stimulus Type · Sex Congruency · Scene
T · SC 1 4.15 .10 .049
Region interaction was also observed. There was a signifi-
S·T 1 <1 .02 ns
S·R 1 1.65 .21 .042 cant simple main effect for Stimulus Type, such that par-
S · SC · R 1 58.60 .61 <.001 ticipants looked at the opposite sex body more times in the
T · SC · R 1 5.62 .13 .023 erotic than the non-erotic stimuli, p < .001, and also looked
S·T·R 1 <1 .01 ns
at the same sex bodies more times in the erotic than the non-
S · T · SC 1 <1 .00 ns
S · T · SC · R 1 <1 .00 ns erotic stimuli, p < .001. However, participants looked more
T within-group error 38 (2.47) times at the same sex face in the non-erotic stimuli than in
SC within-group error 38 (9.00) the erotic stimuli, p = .028. A significant Sex Congruency
R within-group error 38 (12.53)
simple main effect was also found, such that both the
T · R within-group error 38 (3.18)
SC · R within-group error 38 (3.72) opposite sex faces and bodies were looked at more times
T · SC within-group error 38 (4.23) than the same sex faces and bodies in the erotic stimuli,
T · SC · R within-group error 38 (1.83) p < .001 and p < .001, respectively. These results were

123
224 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228

12 Female was also a simple main effect for Scene Region in the erotic
Male
stimuli: both men and women looked at bodies for more time
10
than at faces in the erotic stimuli, p < .001. There was no
simple main effect for Scene Region in the non-erotic
Total # of Fixations

8
stimuli. Thus, visual attention to bodies was higher in the
6 erotic than in the non-erotic stimuli and visual attention to
bodies was higher than to faces in the erotic stimuli only.
4 A significant Stimulus Type · Sex Congruency interac-
tion was also observed. There was a simple main effect for
2
Stimulus Type (erotic vs. non erotic), whereby both men and
0
women looked longer at opposite sex individuals in the
Opposite Sex Same Sex erotic stimuli than in the non-erotic stimuli, p < .001. Men
Sex Congruency
and women also looked longer at same sex individuals in the
Fig. 2 Sex · Sex Congruency interaction for total number of erotic than in the non-erotic stimuli, p = .038. There was a
fixations simple main effect for Sex Congruency in the erotic stimuli
whereby all participants gazed longer at the opposite sex
found in the non-erotic stimuli as well, p < .001 (face) and individual than at the same sex individual, p < .001. There
p = .033 (body), respectively. Lastly, a significant simple was also a simple main effect for Sex Congruency in the non-
main effect was found for Scene Region. Opposite sex erotic stimuli whereby participants gazed longer at the
bodies were looked at more times than opposite sex faces in opposite sex individuals than at the same sex individuals,
both the erotic and non-erotic stimuli, p < .001 and p < .001, p < .001. Thus, there appeared to be a preference for
respectively. Participants also looked at the same sex bodies opposite sex individuals in both types of stimuli; however,
more times than at the same sex faces in both the erotic and that preference was accentuated in the erotic stimuli (Fig. 3).
non-erotic stimuli, p < .001 and p < .001, respectively. As with our results for total number of fixations, a sig-
A separate two-way ANOVA (Sex · Stimulus Type) was nificant Sex · Sex Congruency of Scene Region was
conducted to examine potential sex differences in number of observed. There was a simple main effect for sex whereby,
fixations to the contextual region of the scene. A significant regardless of stimulus type or scene region, men looked at
main effect was found for Stimulus Type only, F(1,38) = opposite sex figures significantly longer than did women,
56.71, p < .001, g2 = .60, whereby both men and women p = .004, and women looked at same sex figures signifi-
attended to the contextual region of the scene significantly cantly longer than did men, p < .001. There was a simple
more times in the non-erotic than in the erotic stimuli. There main effect for Sex Congruency in men whereby they looked
was no significant Sex · Stimulus Type interaction. at the opposite sex figures significantly longer than at same
sex figures, p < .001. There was no simple main effect for
Sex Congruency in women. Men thus conferred more visual
Total gaze time attention than women to opposite sex figures and women
conferred more visual attention to same sex figures than
Means and SDs of total gaze time male and female partici- men, regardless of stimulus type. Additionally, men
pants gazed at different components (face or bodies, oppo- appeared more focused on opposite sex figures than same sex
site sex or same sex figures) of erotic and non-erotic images figures while women appeared to divide their attention more
are shown in Table 3. Omnibus ANOVA results for total evenly (Fig. 4).
gaze time are presented in Table 4. Significant main effects Lastly, results indicated a significant Sex · Sex Con-
were found for Stimulus Type, Sex Congruency, and Scene gruency · Scene Region interaction. There was a simple
Region. Three significant two-way interactions and one main effect for Sex whereby, regardless of stimuli, men
three-way interaction were found and analyzed using simple looked longer at opposite sex bodies than did women,
main effects. p < .001 and women looked longer at same sex bodies than
Similar to our results for number of fixations, there was a did men, p < .001. There was a simple main effect for Sex
significant Stimulus Type · Scene Region interaction. Congruency in men in regards to both faces and bodies, as
There was a simple main effect for Stimulus Type, whereby they looked significantly longer at both the opposite sex
both men and women looked at the bodies of both opposite faces and bodies than the same sex faces and bodies, p < .001
sex and same sex figures in the erotic stimuli for longer and p < .001, respectively. There was a small simple main
periods of time than at the bodies in the non-erotic stimuli, effect for Sex Congruency in women with regard to attention
p < .001. There was no significant variation between Stim- to faces, such that women attended longer to opposite sex
ulus Type in the amount of time spent looking at faces. There than to same sex faces, p = .010. There was a simple main

123
Table 3 Means and SDs for women and men: total gaze time (in ms) as a function of Stimulus Type, Sex Congruency, and Scene Region
Female participants (n = 20) Male participants (n = 20) Male and female participants combined (n = 40)
Erotic stimuli Non-erotic stimuli Both stimulus types Erotic stimuli Non-erotic stimuli Both stimulus types Erotic stimuli Non-erotic stimuli Both stimulus types
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Opposite sex
Face 2429.00 1242.17 2360.80 1072.55 2409.90 1146.57 2346.88 761.32 2398.32 1025.92 2372.60 892.08 2402.94 1018.48 2379.56 1036.13 2391.25 1020.89
Body 3289.44 900.72 1942.94 793.51 2616.19 1080.23 4490.48 1281.99 3077.86 873.45 3784.17 1297.70 3889.96 1251.32 2510.40 1004.34 3200.18 1323.92
Both 2874.22 1150.56 2151.87 954.97 2513.05 1111.68 3418.68 503.75 2738.09 1001.42 3078.39 1314.78 3146.45 1358.26 2444.98 1016.01 2795.72 1246.32
Same sex
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228

Face 1748.64 514.27 1962.12 814.44 1855.38 680.94 1369.80 916.04 1709.72 795.01 1539.76 863.92 1559.22 757.93 1835.92 804.61 1697.57 789.04
Body 3234.36 1098.52 2597.44 757.96 2915.90 985.80 2253.88 695.58 1397.84 529.16 1825.86 748.35 2744.12 1034.47 1997.64 886.16 2370.88 1028.12
Both 2491.50 1118.34 2279.78 829.28 2385.64 996.69 1811.84 919.20 1553.78 685.03 1682.81 815.87 2151.67 1080.43 1916.78 844.92 2034.23 973.95
Opposite and same sex
Face 2103.82 1004.96 2161.46 961.43 2132.64 977.62 1858.34 967.46 2054.02 970.70 1956.18 967.95 1981.08 987.88 2107.74 961.46 2044.41 973.76
Body 3261.90 991.93 2270.19 834.56 2766.05 1038.54 3372.18 1522.84 2237.85 1109.87 2805.02 1441.77 3317.04 1278.15 2254.02 975.81 2785.53 1252.63
Both 2682.86 1150.59 2215.83 896.19 2449.34 1054.36 2615.26 1478.88 2145.94 1040.11 2380.60 1295.99 2649.06 1321.20 2180.88 968.39 2414.97 1180.02
Context 2424.12 1212.33 4117.28 1162.35 3270.70 1452.35 2536.16 1370.57 4436.64 1708.59 3486.40 1806.51 2480.14 1278.44 4276.96 1451.40 3378.55 1632.23

Total Gaze Time (ms)


S·T
S·R
T·R
Source

0
S · SC
T · SC

R · SC

500
3500

1000
1500
2000
2500
3000

time (ms)
S·T·R
S · T · SC
S · SC · R
T · SC · R
Within subjects

S · T · SC · R
Between subjects

Scene Region (R)


Stimulus Type (T)
S within-group error

T within-group error

R within-group error
Sex of participant (S)

Sex Congruency (SC)

SC within-group error

T · R within-group error
T · SC within-group error

Erotic
df

38
38
38
38
38
38

1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
F

<1
<1
<1
<1
<1
<1
<1

2.00
7.62

33.62
19.59
46.75
29.46
19.59
58.26

Stimulus Type
(571431)
(605453)
(300967)

(1491582)
(1642204)
(1086327)
g2

.00
.00
.02
.05
.47
.02
.00
.02
.34
.17
.61
.44
.43
.61
.01

preferentially to same sex bodies compared to men.

Non-Erotic
tion (Sex · Stimulus Type · Sex Congruency · Scene Region)
p

Same Sex
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns
ns

Opposite Sex
Table 4 Analysis of variance on total gaze time for 4-way interac-

Fig. 3 Stimulus Type · Sex Congruency interaction for total gaze


<.001
<.001
.009
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001

the bodies of these figures, and women appeared to attend


figures. In summary, men appeared to attend preferentially
a simple main effect for Scene Region in women in regards

to a greater extent than women to opposite sex figures and to


effect for Scene Region in women in regards to opposite sex
than at same sex faces, p < .001. There was no simple main
to same sex figures as they gazed longer at same sex bodies
effect for Scene Region in men in terms of opposite sex

Scene Region in men in terms of same sex figures. There was


the faces, p < .001. There was no simple main effect for
figures as they looked at opposite sex bodies longer than at
225

123
226 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228

A separate two-way ANOVA (Sex · Stimulus Type) was Similar to previous findings, the Stimulus Type · Scene
conducted to examine potential sex differences in attention Region interaction revealed a significant difference in the
to the contextual region of the scene. A significant main manner in which participants attended to erotic and non-
effect was found for Stimulus Type only, F(1, 38) = 60.34, erotic images. Not surprisingly, the bodies attracted more
p < .001, g2 = .61. There was no significant Sex · Stimulus attention in the erotic than in the non-erotic stimuli while
Type interaction. Thus, results indicate that both men and faces were attended to in relatively the same manner across
women attended to the contextual region of the scene sig- stimulus types. This was true for both men and women.
nificantly longer in the non-erotic than in the erotic stimuli. Results replicated the methodology study findings reported
by Lykins et al. (2006), and provide additional support for
the utility of eye-tracking methodology in human sexuality
Discussion research. An additional processing difference found in this
study was the preferential attention given to opposite sex
In summary, both men and women attended preferentially to figures in the erotic stimuli. Although there was a clear
the bodies as compared to the faces in both erotic and non- preference for opposite sex individuals across stimuli, this
erotic stimuli although, as hypothesized, this difference was preference was significantly amplified in the erotic stimuli.
much larger in the erotic stimuli than in the non-erotic Accordingly, these data further support the argument that
stimuli. These results provide additional support to the erotic information is processed differently than non-erotic
contention that erotic information is processed in an essen- information and add visual attention as another level of
tially different manner than non-erotic information, such that processing at which this difference might be manifested
attentional focus is differentially drawn toward individual (Anohkin et al., 2006; Geer & Bellard, 1996; Geer et al.,
scene regions depending on the level of eroticism in the 1994; Geer & Manguno-Mire, 1996; Geer & Melton, 1997).
image. Our second major hypothesis was partly confirmed: Perhaps more interestingly, the Sex · Sex Congruency
men visually attended more than did women to opposite sex interactions revealed that men showed a stronger and clearer
figures; however, they did so regardless of whether the visual attention preference for opposite sex figures than did
stimulus was patently erotic or not. Women divided their women. In fact, across stimulus types, women appeared to
visual attention more evenly than did men across male and disperse their attention between the male and female figures
female figures in both types of stimuli. This is an intriguing far more evenly than did men. The three-way interactions
finding that may be related to the non-specificity of sexual revealed that, when looking at opposite sex individuals, men
arousal in women or alternately harkens to a broader sex paid much more attention to the body than the face. Again,
difference unrelated to eroticism. Our third hypothesis was this difference was not seen in women. However, when
not confirmed as we found no significant sex differences in gazing at same sex individuals, women looked at women’s
the amount of time spent looking at the contextual features of bodies significantly longer than men looked at men’s bodies.
the stimuli. Though a wealth of data suggests that women are There are at least two potential explanations for this
relatively more focused on scenarios and the situations in result. Firstly, it could be that any photograph of a man and a
which sexual activity is taking place, this study did not find woman interacting positively has some erotic processing
concordant results at the level of visual attention. potential for heterosexual individuals. If this were the case,
then the sex differences we found in the dispersion of visual
attention across same sex and opposite sex figures might
indeed be indicative of a sex difference in the processing of
3500
Female
erotic material. An alternate possibility is that men and
Male
women differ fundamentally in the way they divide attention
3000
across a visual stimulus regardless of its content. If this were
Total Gaze Time (ms)

2500 the case, then we would not be able to reach any conclusions
2000
about sex differences in processing specific to sexual
material.
1500 Although our erotic stimuli were rated as significantly
1000
more arousing than our non-erotic stimuli, it seems reason-
able to posit that reasonably attractive figures of the opposite
500 sex are inherently erotic to some degree for heterosexual
0 individuals, even when they are fully dressed and not
Opposite Sex Same Sex engaged in sexual activity. After all, most people’s first
Sex Congruency
feelings of arousal in regards to a romantic partner occur
Fig. 4 Sex · Sex Congruency interaction for total gaze time (ms) under the latter conditions. If this is the case, then these

123
Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228 227

results may provide some evidence of the non-specificity of In addition, maybe men are subconsciously avoiding gazing
female sexual arousal at the level of visual attention. at the same-sex figure because it elicits homoerotic anxiety.
Women’s more equal distribution of attention across male Either of these factors could conceivably result in the
and female figures compared to the more narrow visual viewing patterns we found.
attentional focus of men on female figures seems to be in At the level of visual attention, the lack of a Sex ·
alignment with the sex differences in the specificity of sexual Stimulus Type interaction did not support the sex difference
arousal found by Chivers et al. (2004). Chivers et al. found hypothesis in the cognitive processing of sexual material
that women aroused more to stimuli that did not correspond (see Geer & Manguno-Mire, 1996). That is, of course, unless
with their self-reported sexual orientation than did men. our non-erotic stimuli were not as non-erotic as we had
Although more recent results suggest that lesbians demon- assumed. In their study of ERPs to erotic and non-erotic
strate a more category-specific pattern of arousal than het- images, Anohkin et al. (2006) argued that an ‘‘erotic bias’’
erosexual women (Chivers, 2006), the visual attention for information processing exists in both sexes, but that the
conferred on the female figures by the heterosexual women extent to which one may find sex differences expressed
in our study remains consistent with these findings. Clearly, likely depends on the chosen research methodology. It is also
we can only very cautiously infer arousal from visual possible that expected sex differences may have been found
attention as we did not measure arousal in this study. How- if we had examined higher-order cognitive processes (e.g.,
ever, it is not unreasonable to posit that visual attention and memory). Further investigation into this issue is warranted.
arousal may be related. One must attend to something before One final issue worth discussing is that of scene region
it can be appraised as arousing, and the scene perception size in within-participant analyses. It seems logical to posit
literature certainly supports the contention that visual that the body scene region was attended to the most simply
attention is drawn to regions containing informative, emo- because it was larger than the face scene region, and not
tional, appetitive, or aversive stimuli (Calvo & Lang, 2004; because of a greater level of interest in the body. Had this
Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999; Lundquist & Ohman, been a concern, we would have controlled for region size;
2005). however, we chose not to, based on a voluminous scene
Alternately, it could be that men and women have fun- perception literature indicating that complexity and infor-
damentally different visual scan patterns, such that men are mativeness of scene regions affects viewing to a greater
more narrowly focused on specific regions drawing their degree than their size (Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999).
attention, whereas women show a more general scan pattern Within our study, this argument is supported by the fact that
of the entire scene. Though we were unable to find any lit- our largest scene region (context), which comprised an
erature to either support or refute this contention in healthy average of 58% of the scenes, attracted far less than even
adults, we believe that our results for attention to context do 10% of the average total viewing time. It thus appears that, in
not support this explanation. We found no significant the case of visual attention to erotic images, size does not
Sex · Context interaction, which suggests that men and matter.
women attended to the contextual features of the scene to the Future research could target the limitations of the current
same extent. If women had a fundamentally more dispersive study. To tease apart the extent to which sex differences in
visual scan, we would have expected them to attend more the processing of erotic stimuli occur at a higher-order
than men to the background. Furthermore, the main effect for cognitive level versus at the basic level of visual attention,
Stimulus Type in this analysis revealed a significant differ- factors such as appraisal and interest could be assessed
ence in attention to context between erotic and non-erotic through concurrent self-report, and memory for scene
stimuli, further showing that men and women appeared to regions could be tested post-stimulus presentation. It would
vary their attention to the context in the same way. Our certainly be useful to test more directly the relationship
results thus seem to favor the explanation of there having between visual attention and subjective and physiological
been an erotic component in our supposedly non-erotic arousal, potentially through pupilometry in addition to
stimuli. genital arousal measures. As distraction has been linked to
One cannot, however, discount the possibility that the the development and maintenance of sexual dysfunction,
women in our study attended more to same-sex figures than utilizing paradigms examining this construct may prove
men for social comparison reasons. There is evidence to useful as both a basic research methodology and a potential
suggest that adolescent girls engage to a greater extent than treatment. Finally, it may be useful to use standardized
boys in bodily social comparison (e.g., Jones Carlson, 2001) computer-generated images that have been precisely
and that women appear to be significantly more plagued by designed to control for image features that are difficult to
body image concerns than men (Feingold & Mazella, 1998). standardize in photographic images.

123
228 Arch Sex Behav (2008) 37:219–228

References Jones Carlson, D. (2001). Social comparison and body image:


Attractiveness comparisons to models and peers among adoles-
Anokhin, A. P., Golosheykin, S., Sirevaag, E., Kristijansson, S., cent girls and boys. Sex Roles, 45, 645–664.
Rohrbaugh, J. W., & Heath, A. C. (2006). Rapid discrimination Kirsch-Rosenkrantz, J., & Geer, J. H. (1991). Gender differences in
of visual scene content in the human brain. Brain Research, memory for a sexual story. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20,
1093, 167–177. 295–305.
Calvo, M. G., & Lang, P. J. (2004). Gaze patterns when looking at Laan, E., Everaerd, W., van Bellen, G., & Hanewald, G. (1994).
emotional pictures: Motivationally biased attention. Motivation Women’s sexual and emotional responses to male- and female-
and Emotion, 28, 221–243. produced erotica. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 153–169.
Chivers, M. L. (2005). A brief review and discussion of sex Leitenberg, H., & Henning, K. (1995). Sexual fantasy. Psychological
differences in the specificity of sexual arousal. Sexual and Bulletin, 117, 469–496.
Relationship Therapy, 20, 377–390. Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, nature, and nurture (2nd ed.). Mahwah,
Chivers, M. L. (2006). Do women have a ‘‘sexual orientation?’’ NJ: Erlbaum.
Insights from female sexual psychophysiology. Paper presented Lundquist, D., & Ohman, A. (2005). Emotion regulates attention: The
at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, relation between facial configurations, facial emotion, and visual
Amsterdam. attention. Visual Cognition, 12, 51–84.
Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, J. M. (2004). A sex Lykins, A. D., Meana, M., & Kambe, G. (2006). Detection of
difference in the specificity of sexual arousal. Psychological differential viewing patterns to erotic and non-erotic stimuli
Science, 15, 736–744. using eye-tracking methodology. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
Feingold, A., & Mazzella, R. (1998). Gender differences in body 35, 569–575.
image are increasing. Psychological Science, 9, 190–195. Meuwissen, I., & Over, R. (1991). Multidimensionality of the content
Geer, J. H., & Bellard, H. S. (1996). Sexual content induced delays in of female sexual fantasy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29,
unprimed lexical decisions: Gender and context effects. Archives 179–189.
of Sexual Behavior, 25, 379–395. Mosher, D. L., & MacIan, P. (1994). College men and women
Geer, J. H., Judice, S., & Jackson, S. (1994). Reading times for erotic respond to X-rated videos intended for male or female
material: The pause to reflect. Journal of General Psychology, audiences: Gender and sexual scripts. Journal of Sex Research,
121, 345–352. 31, 99–113.
Geer, J. H., & Manguno-Mire, G. M. (1996). Gender differences in Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality:
cognitive processes in sexuality. Annual Review of Sex Research, A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51.
7, 90–124. Rayner, K. (1995). Eye movements and cognitive processes in
Geer, J. H., & McGlone, M. S. (1990). Sex differences in memory for reading, visual search, and scene perception. In J. M. Findley, R.
erotica. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 71–78. Walker, & R. W. Kentridge (Eds.), Eye movement research:
Geer, J. H., & Melton, J. S. (1997). Sexual content-induced delays Mechanisms, processes and applications (pp. 3–22). New York:
with double-entendre words. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26, Elsevier Science.
295–316. Schmidt, G., Sigusch, V., & Schafer, S. (1973). Responses to reading
Geer, J. H., & Robertson, G. G. (2005). Implicit attitudes in sexuality: erotic stories: Male-female differences. Archives of Sexual
Gender differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 671–677. Behavior, 2, 181–199.
Hamann, S., Herman, R., Nolan, C. L., & Wallen, K. (2004). Men and Sigusch, V., Schmidt, G., Reinfeld, A., & Wiedermann-Sutor, I. W.
women differ in amygdala response to visual sexual stimuli. (1970). Psychosexual stimulation: Sex differences. Journal of
Nature Neuroscience, 7, 411–416. Sex Research, 6, 10–24.
Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). High-level scene Smith, D., & Over, R. (1991). Male sexual fantasy: Multidimensi-
perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 243–271. onality in content. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 267–
Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The 275.
effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during Tokatlidis, O., & Over, R. (1995). Imagery, fantasy, and female
complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, sexual arousal. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 81–85.
25, 210–228. Wells, J. W. (1990). The sexual vocabularies of heterosexual and
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American homosexual males and females for communicating erotically
Psychologist, 60, 581–592. with a sexual partner. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 139–147.
Janssen, E., Carpenter, D., & Graham, C. (2003). Selecting film clips Youn, G. (2006). Subjective sexual arousal in response to erotica:
for sex research: Gender differences in erotic film preference. Effects of gender, guided fantasy, erotic stimulus, and duration
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 243–251. of exposure. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 87–97.

123

You might also like