You are on page 1of 15

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier.

The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 –21 1

available at www.sciencedirect.com

www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres

Research Report

Neurodynamics of an election

Armando Freitas da Rocha a,b,⁎, Fábio Theoto Rocha a,b ,


Marcelo Nascimento Burattini b , Eduardo Massad b
a
RANI-Research on Natural and Artificial Intelligence, Rua Tenente Ary Aps, 172 Jundiaí, CEP 13207-110, Brazil
b
School of Medicine, University of São Paulo and LIM01-HCFMUSP, Rua Teodoro Sampaio 115, São Paulo, CEP 5405-000, SP, Brazil

A R T I C LE I N FO AB S T R A C T

Article history: Variables influencing decision-making in real settings, as in the case of voting decisions, are
Accepted 21 June 2010 uncontrollable and in many times even unknown to the experimenter. In this case, the
Available online 26 June 2010 experimenter has to study the intention to decide (vote) as close as possible in time to the
moment of the real decision (election day). Here, we investigated the brain activity
Keywords: associated with the voting intention declared 1 week before the election day of the Brazilian
EEG Firearms Control Referendum about prohibiting the commerce of firearms. Two alliances
Decision-making arose in the Congress to run the campaigns for YES (for the prohibition of firearm
Voting commerce) and NO (against the prohibition of firearm commerce) voting. Time constraints
Brain networks imposed by the necessity of studying a reasonable number (here, 32) of voters during a very
ERP short time (5 days) made the EEG the tool of choice for recording the brain activity associated
Distributed processing with voting decision. Recent fMRI and EEG studies have shown decision-making as a process
Benefits due to the enrollment of defined neuronal networks. In this work, a special EEG technique is
Risks applied to study the topology of the voting decision-making networks and is compared to
the results of standard ERP procedures. The results show that voting decision-making
enrolled networks in charge of calculating the benefits and risks of the decision of
prohibiting or allowing firearm commerce and that the topology of such networks was vote-
(i.e., YES/NO-) sensitive.
© 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction most real settings of decision-making, in which the influenc-


ing variables are uncontrollable and in many times even
New brain imaging techniques have opened the way for the unknown. As a matter of fact, we may consider that some
study of the processes of human decision-making. This kind decision-making processes are always hypothetical (e.g.,
of research is usually done under simulated conditions moral judgment) in the sense that there is no way to carry
that allows for substantial control, by the researcher, of the out experiments in real cases; some other kinds of decisions
most important variables influencing the decision-making (e.g., purchasing) allow simulated (and controlled) experimen-
(Knutson et al., 2007; Greene et al., 2004; McClure et al., 2004; tation, and, finally, some others (e.g., voting) are always done
Moll and Oliveira-Souza, 2007; Huettel et al., 2006). These in real and uncontrolled environments. In this latter case, the
controlled simulated conditions are in sharp contrast with best that can be done is to study the intention to decide as

⁎ Corresponding author. Rua Tenente Ary Aps, 172, Jundiaí, Brazil, CEP 13207-110. Fax: +55 11 3379 2010.
E-mail addresses: armando@enscer.com.br (A.F. da Rocha), fabio@enscer.com.br (F.T. Rocha), mnburatti@usp.br (M.N. Burattini),
edmassad@usp.br (E. Massad).
URL: http://www.eina.com.br (A.F. da Rocha).

0006-8993/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.046
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 – 2 11 199

close as possible in time to the moment of the real decision. decision-making stages, etc. Synchronous oscillations have
This is the approach used in this paper that investigates the been observed and investigated extensively in EEG, and
brain activity associated with the voting intention declared different coherence (de-coherence) indices were proposed to
1 week before the election day of the Brazilian Firearms quantify the synchronization between the EEG signals
Control Referendum. recorded by different electrodes. Most of the EEG literature
The Firearm Commerce Referendum called for a collective on decision-making is focused on the ERP technique (Cohen
decision about prohibiting the commerce of firearms with the et al., 2007; Esposito et al., 2009; Heldmann et al., 2005; Polezzi
purpose of reducing criminality. Voting is mandatory in Brazil, et al., 2008, 2010; Utku et al., 2002).
and political campaigns take advantage of free radio/TV EEG studies of decision-making (e.g., Chen et al., 2009;
propaganda during the 40 days before an election. Two Polezzi et al., 2008a,b, 2010; Utku et al., 2002) in human
political alliances arouse in the Brazilian Congress to run the subjects have mainly focused on the:
campaigns for the Yes (for the prohibition of firearm
commerce) and No (against the prohibition of firearm (a) feedback-related negativity (FRN), which reflects the
commerce) votes. The media campaign stressed the benefits medial frontal/anterior cingulate activity, and its am-
and costs of each decision, trying to oppose the benefits of one plitude codes the ongoing evaluation of events and the
decision against the risks of the opposite one. prediction of future events in terms of favorable or
The pro-YES media campaign stressed, for example, that unfavorable outcomes;
prohibiting fire arms commerce reduces passionate murders (b) P300, which has also been reported to reflect decision-
of beloved ones during family, neighbors, traffic, etc. quarrels, making, and its amplitude is thought to reflect the
that would result into a reduction of personal risks or costs. In outcome of stimulus evaluation and decision-making.
contrast, the pro-NO media campaign stressed that without The P300 amplitude varies with variables such as the
having a gun you are unable to protect yourself against event probability, the motivational significance of stimuli
criminals, which would result into a reduction of personal and the magnitude of feedback outcome, regardless of
benefits. Some others media advertisement stressed either whether the outcome is positive or negative; and
the possible social costs of facilitating gun acquisition by (c) N500, which is generally larger for unpredictable out-
criminals by means of a free fire arms commerce or the social comes, and this is thought to be generated by the posterior
benefits of an unarmed society. cingulate cortex and the visual association cortex.
Rocha et al. (2009) proposed a neuroeconomic decision-
making model, in which the adequacy or intention to Given the necessity of scanning a large number of
implement an action (e.g., voting) depends on the evaluation volunteers in a short period of time, we selected the EEG as
of the risks and benefits in two different decision spaces: the the most appropriate way to record the brain activity
Personal (PDS) and the Interpersonal (IDS) decision spaces. associated with voting decision-making. In addition, due to
PDS is in charge of calculating the personal risks and benefits the uncontrolled condition of our experiment, we had to focus
associated with the action, and IDS is in charge of calculating our analysis backwardly; that is, we focused on the brain
the social risks and benefits associates with a given action. In activity preceding the decision-making, whereas most, if not
the present case, the risks and benefits of the YES arguments all, of the experiments involving EEG and decision-making
may be supposed to be evaluated in the IDS space, whereas the focus on the analysis of brain activity following the decision-
risks and benefits of the NO arguments can be evaluated in the making or stimulus presentation (e.g., Chen et al., 2009; Polezzi
PDS space, and then these values are used for the calculations et al., 2008a,b, 2010; Utku et al., 2002). This motivated us to use
of the voting intention. the technique we developed (Rocha et al., 2004, 2005) before
The time constraints imposed by the necessity of studying investigating voting decision-making.
a reasonable number of voters (in the present case: 32) during The purpose of this paper is to investigate the brain activity
a very short time (in the present case: 5 days) render EEG as the associated with the voting intention declared 1 week before
only tool of choice for recording and analyzing the brain the election day of the Brazilian Firearms Control Referendum,
activity associated with the voting decision. Because we were supposing that that voting was influenced by media and
not allowed to control the most important variables influenc- that this influence can be deciphered by neural correlates
ing the voting decision, we have to hypothesize that this associated with different decision spaces (PDS and IDS), and
decision was dependent on the neural circuits involved assuming for this purpose the EEG as the tool of choice for
with self and social cognition, if the media was successful in recording and analyzing the brain activity associated with the
influencing voters. voting decision. The ERP technique is used to characterize the
In the analysis of event-related potentials (ERP), EEG power brain activity preceding the voting decision-making, and the
spectrum and coherence (or de-coherence) are the most brain imaging technique developed by Rocha et al. (2004, 2005)
frequent and widespread techniques used in EEG studies is applied to study the topology of the voting decision-making
(e.g., Alegre et al., 2006; Cohen et al., 2007; Hauk et al., 2006; brain network.
Jacobs et al., 2006; Olofsson et al., 2008; Polezzi et al., 2008).
Different ERP waveforms revealed specific amplitude and
latency characteristics related with stimulus perception: 2. Results
syntactic and semantic analysis, sleep and walking states,
decision-making, etc. Different band frequency patterns are From the total of 32 individuals studied, 10 (31%) declared the
associated with sleep and waking states, reasoning processes, intention to vote YES on Election Day, whereas 15 (47%)
Author's personal copy

200 BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 –21 1

individuals declared the intention to vote NO. The remaining Table 1 – F and p values for ANOVA mappings.
seven (22%) individuals were undecided at the time of the A ðei ; tÞ h(ei)
experiment. These voting percentages were similar to the
poll results published in the same week by Brazilian pollsters F p F p
(Ibope, 2005). Volunteers took an average of 12 s to reach a C3 1.20 0.27 37.00 0.00
decision. C4 6.05 0.01 121.00 0.00
Data analysis consisted of the following steps: CZ 95.00 0.00 121.00 0.00
F3 98.00 0.00 5.80 0.01
F4 4.30 0.02 12.11 0.00
(1) possible statistical differences for the average ampli-
F7 7.31 0.06 4.41 0.03
tude A ðei ; tÞ and the calculated entropyh(ei) concerning F8 0.00 0.82 1.51 0.02
Yes and No voters were first evaluated by means of FP1 0.00 0.82 1.93 0.21
ANOVA. The results of these analyses are shown by the FP2 97.00 0.00 2.54 0.12
ANOVA mappings in Figs. 1 and 3, plotting the statistical FZ 90.00 0.00 2.43 0.08
significance for the differences between A ðei ; tÞ of h(ei) O1 70.00 0.00 12.10 0.00
OZ 0.21 0.66 4.31 0.00
fro each electrode taking into account the type of vote as
O2 4.30 0.02 11.50 0.00
the grouping variables;
P3 0.65 0.43 11.50 0.00
(2) A ðei ; tÞ data were visually inspected (Fig. 1) for possible P4 0.32 0.52 62.00 0.00
easily identifiable components occurring at defined PZ 0.13 0.71 14.00 0.00
times before the voting decision; T3 1.36 0.25 62.00 0.00
(3) factor analysis was carried out for both A ðei ; tÞ and h(ei) T4 0.69 0.41 3.30 0.00
to disclose possible data covariation and to identify T5 29.90 0.00 15.00 0.00
T6 4.30 0.03 12.90 0.00
possible brain activity patterns associated with Yes or
No voting decisions. The results are presented in Table 1
for A ðei ; tÞ and Table 2 for h(ei). The loading factors were
used to generate the Factor Mappings shown in Fig. 2 for assumed to be different if R → 0 or inversely correlated if
A ðei ; tÞ and Fig. 3 forh(ei); R → − 1. The results showed that R → 1 or R → 0 for all
(4) linear regression analysis was used to study the possible compared mappings.
correlation between vote and h(ei), because for each
decision a unique value of h(ei) exists for each recording Fig. 1 shows the temporal evolution of A ðei ; tÞ during the
electrode. The results of this analysis are shown in two seconds that preceded the voting decision by the
Table 3, and the corresponding Regression Mapping is individuals of Class 1 or Yes voters and Class 2 or No voters.
shown in Fig. 3; and The visual inspection of Fig. 1 shows a difference for A ðei ; tÞ
(5) the Pearson's correlation coefficient Rwas calculated for evolution associated with the different voting decision,
comparing different brain mappings, such that two which is confirmed by the ANOVA shown in the inserted
mappings are expected to be similar if R → 1; they are brain mapping. This analysis showed statistically significant

Fig. 1 – Temporal evolution of A (ei ; t) during the 2 s that preceded the voting decision by the individuals of Class 1 or Yes voters
(n = 10) and Class 2 or No voters (n = 15). The inserted brain mapping color encodes the ANOVA analysis (Table 1), such that
statistically significant differences (p(ei) → 0) are depicted in bluish colors and non-significant differences (p(ei) → 1) are depicted
in reddish colors.
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 – 2 11 201


Table 2 – Factor Analysis for A(ei,t).
Vote NO (Class 2) Vote YES (Class 1)

Extraction: principal components Extraction: principal components

hna hsim

Factor Factor Factor Factor Factor Factor

1 2 3 1 2 3

C3 0.716393 −0.03283 0.383835 C3 0.54006 0.443392 0.296677


C4 0.176105 0.687686 0.026208 C4 − 0.38913 0.224257 0.755081
CZ 0.821405 0.363859 −0.04678 CZ 0.864032 0.15836 −0.04897
F3 0.246867 0.743744 0.345671 F3 0.495682 − 0.1313 0.542722
F4 0.933855 0.122746 0.141891 F4 − 0.0771 0.887777 0.092686
F7 0.074973 0.7737 0.078913 F7 0.291887 0.706532 0.124067
F8 0.183568 −0.20251 0.544943 F8 0.213645 0.855867 −0.04571
FP1 0.024346 0.414883 0.694659 FP1 − 0.13581 0.821801 0.122069
FP2 0.146386 0.014244 0.763065 FP2 0.22034 0.701512 −0.00923
FZ 0.314326 −0.11137 −0.22925 FZ − 0.07134 0.494585 0.469489
O1 0.526566 0.528918 −0.01984 O1 0.616206 0.249811 −0.00746
OZ 0.776535 0.373813 −0.05236 OZ 0.435752 0.16587 0.555836
O2 0.580343 0.553088 −0.26819 O2 0.790489 0.184794 −0.30451
P3 0.335255 0.480346 0.03468 P3 0.878269 − 0.0293 0.282258
P4 0.849659 0.090607 0.117452 P4 0.080344 0.520613 0.542198
PZ 0.48805 0.408624 −0.12976 PZ 0.511573 − 0.09745 0.379866
T3 0.705332 0.38674 0.317334 T3 − 0.13513 0.649345 0.144688
T4 0.936426 0.072963 0.156005 T4 0.727886 0.147857 0.032165
T5 0.469806 0.514611 −0.25512 T5 0.471078 − 0.18983 0.388343
T6 0.853142 0.074734 −0.26408 T6 0.845413 − 0.18492 −0.12509
Expl.Var 6.92465 3.555576 2.07715 Expl.Var 5.362906 4.640337 2.324696
Prp.Totl 0.346232 0.177779 0.103858 Prp.Totl 0.268145 0.232017 0.116235
0.936426 0.7737 0.763065 0.878269 0.887777 0.755081
0.024346 −0.20251 −0.26819 − 0.38913 − 0.18983 −0.30451

Extraction: principal components Extraction: principal components

% total Cumul. Cumul. % total Cumul. Cumul.

Eigenvalue Variance Eigenval. % Eigenvalue Variance Eigenval. %

1 8.44703 42.23515 8.44703 42.23515 1 6.064215 30.32108 6.064215 30.32108


2 2.107497 10.53748 10.55453 52.77264 2 4.348519 21.7426 10.41273 52.06367
3 2.002848 10.01424 12.55738 62.78688 3 1.915205 9.576024 12.32794 61.6397

differences for all electrodes except FP1, F8, C3, P3, P4, PZ and Three factors (Fi) explained around 60% of the total variance
O2. It may be considered that A ðei ; tÞ, calculated for the of A ðei ; tÞ in Class 1 and Class 2, respectively (Table 2). The
electrodes FP2, F3, CZ, C4 and T5 in Class 1 and for the eigenvalues for these factors varied from 1.91 for F3 of Class 1
electrodes F4, C3, CZ, FZ, F4, C3, CZ, T3 and OZ in the case of to 8.44 for F1 of Class 2, stressing the robustness of the
Class 2, exhibits a variation higher than for the remaining analysis.
electrodes during the 2-s study period. So, a distinction The Pearson's correlation coefficient between these
between the EEG activities recorded from the individuals in factors calculated for Classes 1 and 2 revealed the existence
Class 1 and Class 2 can be established based on A ðei ; tÞ of three patterns of A ðei ; tÞ covariation (Patterns Pi in Fig. 2).
calculated for the above-cited electrodes. Each pattern P i is defined by the pair of factors F i ,
In contrast to the usual outcome, the present A ðei ; tÞ analysis Fjhaving the highest Ri, j values (R = 0.63, 0.34 and 0.21 in
did not disclose any conspicuous negatively or positively isolated Fig. 2). Pattern P1 is composed by factors F3 from Class 1
components that could be easily identified with a defined latency (eigenvalue of 1.91) and F2 from Class 2 (eigenvalue of 2.10),
either for Class 1 (Yes vote) or Class 2 (No vote). Because of whose similarity is at the level of R = 0.63. Pattern P1 has C4
this lack of specific waveform with a distinctive (negative) as the common high-loading electrode (loading factor
latency identified for either the YES on the NO decision-making, greater than 0.6) for both Classes 1 and 2, with F3 and F7
we decided to use Principal Component Analysis to understand for Class 2 only. Pattern P2 is composed by factors F1 from
the possible differences of brain activity associated with these both Class 1 (eigenvalue of 6.06) and Class 2 (eigenvalue of
distinct decision-making directions (Fig. 2). 8.44), whose similarity is at the level of R = 0.34. Pattern P2
Fig. 2 shows the brain mappings depicting the results of has CZ, T4 and T6 as the common high-loading electrodes
A ðei ; tÞ Factorial Analysis for Classes 1 and 2, shown in Table 1. for both Classes 1 and 2, has T4, P3, O1 and O2 as high
Author's personal copy

202 BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 –21 1

Fig. 2 – A (ei ; t) Factorial brain mappings for Class 1 (Yes vote) and Class 2 (No vote). The normalized values f j (ei ) of the factor
loadings fj(ei) (Table 1) for each electrode (ei) of the three principal components Fj (eigenvalues greater than 1) are color-coded
such that f j (ei )→1 is colored blue and f j (ei )→0 is colored red. R-values represent the Pearson's correlation coefficient between
each pair of electrodes.

loading electrodes for Class 1 and has F4, C3, T3, P4 and OZ and T6 as the common high-loading electrodes (loading
for Class 2. Pattern P3 is composed by factors F2 from Class 1 factor greater than 0.6) for both Classes 1 and 2, and it was
(eigenvalue of 4.34) and F3 from Class 2 (eigenvalue of 2.00), P4 for Class 1 or F3, O1, O2 and OZ for Class 2. Pattern P2 is
whose similarity is at the level of R = 0.21. Pattern P3 has FP1 composed by factors F2 from Class 1 (eigenvalue of 3.4) and
and FP2 as the common high-loading electrodes for both F3 from Class 2 (eigenvalue of 3.2), whose similarity is at
Classes 1, and F4, F7 and F8 are the high-loading electrodes the level of R = 0.57. Pattern P2 has FP1 and FP2 as the
for Class 1. common high-loading electrodes for both Classes 1 and 2,
Fig. 3 shows the brain mappings depicting the results of and F4 and F8 are the high-loading electrodes for Class 1
h (ei) factorial and ANOVA analyses for Classes 1 and 2, only. Pattern P3 is composed by factors F1 from Class 1
shown in Table 2. The values of h(ei)calculated for Class 1 (eigenvalue of 12.0) and F2 from Class 2 (eigenvalue of 3.5),
and Class 2 were different for all electrodes except Fp1, Fp2 whose similarity is at the level of R = 0.03. Pattern P3 has C3,
and FZ. Three factors explained around 80% of the total h(ei) C4, Cz and P3 as the common high-loading electrodes for
variance in Class 1 and Class 2, respectively (Table 2). The both Classes 1 and 2, and F3, FZ, PZ, O1, O2 and OZ are the
eigenvalues for these factors varied from 1.30 for F3 of Class high-loading electrodes for Class 1 and F4, F8, T6 and P4 are
1 to 12.08 for F1 of Class 1, stressing the robustness of the for Class 2.
analysis. Fig. 3 also shows the brain mappings depicting the results
The Pearson's correlation coefficient between these of the h(ei)regression analysis for Classes 1 and 2, shown in
factors calculated for Classes 1 and 2 revealed the existence Table 3. The values of R2 for these regressions were 0.57 and
of three patterns of h(ei) covariation (Patterns Pi in Figure 3). 0.51, respectively. Their statistical significances were p = 0.09
Each pattern Pi is defined by the pair of factors Fi, Fj having and p = 0.01, respectively. The decision of voting Probably NO
the highest Ri, j values (R = 0.68, 0.57 and 0.03 in Fig. 3). (green) and Certainly NO (blue) increased as h(ei) augmented
Pattern P1 is composed by factorsF3 from Class 1 (eigenval- at the electrodes Fp2, F4 and F8. The decision of Certainly
ue of 1.3) and F1 from Class 2 (eigenvalue of 10.1), whose Not voting NO (red) increased as h(ei) augmented for
similarity is at the level of R = 0.68. Pattern P1 has T3, T4, T5 electrodes Fp1, F3, F7, T4, P3, P4, O2 and OZ. The decision
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 – 2 11 203

Fig. 3 – h(ei)regression brain mappings (Table 4) on the left, factorial brain mappings (Table 3) for Class 1 (Yes vote) and Class 2
(No vote) in the middle and ANOVA brain mapping on the right (Table 1). The normalized values β (ei ) of the angular coefficients,
BETA, for each electrode (ei) are color-coded such that β (ei )→1 is colored blue and β (ei )→0 is colored red. According to the
normalization rule, β(ei )>0:5 implies BETA > 0 and β (ei )<0:5 implies BETA < 0. The normalized values f j (ei ) of the factor loadings
fj(ei) (Table 2) for each electrode (ei) of the three principal components Fj (eigenvalues greater than 1) are color-coded such that
f j (ei )→1 is colored blue and f j (ei )→0 is colored red. The R-values represent the Pearson's correlation coefficient calculated
between each pair of electrodes. The p(ei) values of the ANOVA brain mapping are color-coded such that p(ei) → 0 is colored blue
while p(ei) → 1 is colored red. Brain contours are simply used as references for the topographical location of the 10/20 system
electrodes. CN in the middle bar means Certainly NO Vote, and NN means Certainly Not NO Vote. CY means Certainly YES
Vote, and NY means Certainly Not YES Vote.

of voting Probably YES (green) and Certainly YES (blue) tionship is found when we regress A ðei ; tÞPi as a function of
increased as h(ei) augmented at the electrodes Fp2, F3, F4, FZ, h(ei)Pi.
C3, CZ, PZ, T6 and OZ. The decision of Certainly Not voting Finally, logistic regression between YES(=1)/NO(=0) vote
YES (red) increased as h(ei) augmented for electrodes P3, T5 and h(ei)Pi and A ðei ; tÞPi explained 40% and 60% of their
and O1. variance, respectively.
The regression analysis for h(ei)Pi and A ðei ; tÞPi showed that:

hðei ÞP1 = 0:29 + 0:16A ðei ; tÞP1 + 0:34A ðei ; tÞP2 −0:07A ðei ; tÞP3 ;
r = 0:35 ðp < 0:17Þ; 3. Discussion

hðei ÞP2 = 0:87 + 0:05A ðei ; tÞP1 −0:57A ðei ; tÞP2 −0:17A ðei ; tÞP3 ;
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to
r = 0:55 ðp < 0:003Þ;
investigate the neural process supporting a real, complex
hðei ÞP3 = 0:18−0:15A ðei ; tÞP1 + 0:40A ðei ; tÞP2 −0:38A ðei ; tÞP3 ; and non-controlled decision-making situation such as voting
r = 0:44 ðp < 0:04Þ decision-making. Most of the recently published studies about
complex decision-making deal with experiments in well-
Note that there is a linear relationship between h(ei)Pi controlled laboratories (e.g., Esposito et al., 2009; Goldman
and A ðei ; tÞPi. In contrast, no reasonably good linear rela- et al., 2009; Ino et al., 2010) or simulated hypothetical conditions
Author's personal copy

204 BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 –21 1

Table 3 – Factor Analysis for h(ei).


Vote NO (Class 2) Vote YES (Class 1)

Factor loadings (Varimax raw) Factor loadings (Varimax raw)

Extraction: principal components Extraction: principal components

Factor Factor Factor Factor Factor Factor

1 2 3 1 2 3

C3 0.303021 0.756425 0.328989 C3 0.714671 0.05566 0.573831


C4 0.169343 0.953117 0.179718 C4 0.859526 0.017209 0.270431
CZ 0.394629 0.882466 0.030144 CZ 0.924638 0.08137 0.008911
F3 0.702341 0.531177 0.27285 F3 0.841299 0.267794 −0.14193
F4 0.365894 0.831675 0.237033 F4 0.254855 0.788703 0.092138
F7 0.395954 0.519182 0.430755 F7 0.024754 0.561752 0.41611
F8 −0.05468 0.778331 0.513248 F8 − 0.01893 0.867088 −0.04026
FP1 0.319527 0.354102 0.552115 FP1 − 0.09042 0.890918 0.108808
FP2 0.074725 0.450829 0.623862 FP2 0.061982 0.940355 −0.0032
FZ 0.113395 0.156293 0.83417 FZ 0.707317 0.419191 0.208262
O1 0.930001 0.231891 0.183899 O1 0.836588 −0.03039 0.438318
O2 0.870292 0.377368 0.253838 O2 0.901654 −0.03967 0.289379
OZ 0.790207 0.561794 0.071003 OZ 0.802363 0.109763 0.12503
P3 0.185154 0.901274 0.114804 P3 0.871922 −0.0704 0.3183
P4 0.583509 0.743501 −0.13627 P4 0.114162 0.153884 0.844156
PZ 0.859247 0.117221 −0.03408 PZ 0.734084 −0.08941 0.579657
T3 0.914068 0.265075 −0.05751 T3 0.08191 0.085199 0.931874
T4 0.976257 − 0.01251 0.054623 T4 0.56197 0.143208 0.73699
T5 0.92801 0.237388 0.019148 T5 0.509427 −0.02971 0.771146
T6 0.921141 0.217318 0.17597 T6 0.656472 −0.15516 0.604961
Expl.Var 8.041811 6.526223 2.284749 Expl.Var 7.872314 3.728809 4.47472
Prp.Totl 0.402091 0.326311 0.114237 Prp.Totl 0.393616 0.18644 0.223736
0.976257 0.953117 0.83417 0.924638 0.940355 0.931874
−0.05468 − 0.01251 −0.13627 − 0.09042 −0.15516 −0.14193

No Yes

Extraction: principal components Extraction: principal components

% total Cumul. Cumul. % total Cumul. Cumul.

Eigenvalue Variance Eigenval. % Eigenvalue Variance Eigenval. %

1 10.17027 50.85135 10.17027 50.85135 1 12.08706 60.43529 12.08706 60.43529


2 3.548731 17.74366 13.719 68.59501 2 3.456356 17.28178 15.54341 77.71707
3 2.224301 11.12151 15.9433 79.71651 3 1.30937 6.54685 16.85278 84.26392

(e.g., Robertson et al., 2007; Kliemann et al., 2008; Young and charge of calculating benefits and risks of the decision of
Saxe, 2008). prohibiting or allowing firearm commerce. The possible benefits
Given the necessity of scanning a large number of volunteers and risks of each decision were the focus of the media campaign
in a short period of time, we selected the EEG as the most in radio, TV and newspapers. As mentioned in Introduction, the
appropriate way to record the brain activity associated with the pro-YES media campaign stressed, for example, that prohibiting
voting decision-making. In addition, due to the uncontrolled fire arms commerce reduces passionate murders of beloved
condition of our experiment, we had to focus our analysis ones during family, neighbors, traffic, etc. quarrels, that would
backwardly; that is, we focused on the brain activity preceding result into a reduction of personal risks or costs. In contrast, the
the decision-making, whereas most, if not all, of the experi- pro-NO media campaign stressed that without having a gun you
ments involving EEG and decision-making focus on the analysis are unable to protect yourself against criminals, which would
of the brain activity following the decision-making or stimulus result into a reduction of personal benefits. Some others media
presentation (e.g., Utku et al., 2002; Cohen et al., 2007; Polezzi et advertisement stressed either the possible social costs of
al., 2008a,b). This motivated us to look for new methods for EEG facilitating gun acquisition by criminals by means of a free fire
analysis that could be informative without being dependent on arms commerce or the social benefits of an unarmed society.
the recognition of defined wave forms with characteristic The voters were then asked to consider the benefits for one (YES
latency, as usually done in ERP analysis. or NO) decision and the risk of the opposite (NO or YES) decision
It is possible to conclude, from the new ERP analysis in order to decide their vote. So, it may be hypothesized here that
proposed here, that the vote decision involved a set of widely our volunteers were influenced by the media campaign, and
distributed neurons as a part of a complex processing network in their voting decision was based upon the comparison of the
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 – 2 11 205

20
Table 4 – Regression analysis v=τ+∑ βihv(Rj).
i=1

Regression summary for dependent variable: v

R = 0.47401843, R2 = 0.22469347, Adjusted R2 = 0.21720620

F(20.2071) = 30.010, p < 0.0000, Std. Error of estimate: 1.6052

St. Err. St. Err.

BETA of BETA B of B t(2071) p-level h ðei Þ h ðei Þbi

Intercept 73.4995 0.092447 795.0415 0

C3 0.213719 0.044681 0.268551 0.056144 4.783253 1.85E-06 2.048764 0.550198


C4 − 0.18256 0.040078 − 0.21511 0.047223 −4.55522 5.54E-06 2.320727 -0.49921
CZ − 0.1403 0.044575 − 0.17471 0.055509 −3.14745 0.00167 2.217982 -0.3875
F3 − 0.35163 0.034037 − 0.45556 0.044098 − 10.3307 1.98E-24 2.3037 -1.04947
F4 0.070889 0.035143 0.100321 0.049734 2.017142 0.043809 2.344191 0.235173
F7 − 0.06023 0.02748 − 0.10254 0.046781 −2.19192 0.028496 1.698536 -0.17417
F8 0.128032 0.031324 0.20291 0.049643 4.08735 4.53E-05 1.832645 0.371862
FP1 0.33265 0.034048 0.479484 0.049077 9.769934 4.51E-22 2.092309 1.003228
FP2 − 0.31124 0.03965 − 0.40437 0.051514 −7.84967 6.63E-15 2.250064 -0.90985
FZ 0.034281 0.032449 0.038471 0.036415 1.056467 0.290878 2.498155 0.096107
O1 − 0.72118 0.172827 − 0.22476 0.053864 −4.17281 3.13E-05 2.143427 -0.48176
O2 1.063042 0.175132 0.331745 0.054654 6.069933 1.52E-09 2.197273 0.728935
OZ − 0.25695 0.147649 − 0.07995 0.045941 −1.74026 0.081962 1.963082 -0.15695
P3 − 0.0072 0.036996 − 0.01012 0.052025 −0.1946 0.845726 1.876827 -0.019
P4 − 0.07842 0.033514 − 0.11801 0.050434 −2.33995 0.019381 1.742227 -0.2056
PZ − 0.01659 0.03997 − 0.02012 0.048492 −0.41496 0.678213 1.819464 -0.03661
T3 0.138843 0.033612 0.225105 0.054496 4.130688 3.76E-05 1.671445 0.376251
T4 0.156891 0.03378 0.227492 0.048981 4.644515 3.62E-06 1.570627 0.357305
T5 0.037126 0.031826 0.065031 0.055748 1.166522 0.243538 1.802555 0.117223
T6 − 0.173 0.053258 − 0.05039 0.015512 −3.24834 0.001179 1.772145 -0.08929

calculated benefits and risks to calculate the adequacy of each At this point, it would be convenient to propose a model
voting alternative. If this is true, then the differences of A ðei ; tÞ for the voting decision-making in the case of a referendum.
covariance observed between YES and NO Voting for patterns P2 First, however, it should be mentioned that a referendum is
and P3 could reflect the differences in the corresponding a particular case of election in which only two alternatives
calculated benefits and risks of each decision. In addition, it about a single problem are presented to potential voters.
may be also hypothesized that voting decision-making required Therefore, this is a rather simple type of voting decision, and it
recursive calculations of the benefits and risks for different can be done by contrasting two sets of arguments supporting
arguments in favor of both voting alternatives, such that no or rejecting each voting option. This was the case in the
specific waveform with a distinctive (negative) latency is easily Brazilian firearm referendum. Two political coalitions were
identified either for the YES on the NO decision-making. formed, and two sets of arguments were elaborated and
The Principal Component Analysis also revealed the exis- discussed. The media campaign worked out this process
tence of three different patterns (Pi) (Fig. 3) of brain activity intensely asking the voters to evaluate the personal/social
concerning h(ei) that explain 80% of the data covariance costs/benefits of each possible voting decision to calculate the
associated with the vote decision. If it is considered that the adequacy of each of these decisions.
different patterns Pi reflect the structure of the brain networks In line with these assumptions, the decision model intro-
involved in the reasoning supporting the voting decision, then it duced by Rocha et al. (2009) proposes the adequacy or intention
may be concluded from the Principal Component Analysis that to implement an action (e.g. voting) depends on the evaluation
the YES and NO voting reasoning differences are mostly of risks and benefits in two different decision spaces: the
supported by the differences in network organization charac- Personal (PDS) and the Interpersonal (IDS) decision spaces. The
terized by patternP3. This hypothesis seems to be further adequacy (or fairness) of the action (voting YES or NO) was
substantiated by the linear multiple regression analysis corre- proposed to be dependent on the balance between the ratios
lating the vote decision v and h(ei), showing that voting YES or personal risk/social benefit and the social risk/personal benefits.
NO was associated with high values of h(ei) calculated for distinct The adequate (fair) action is supposed to provide high social and
set of electrodes. Therefore, it may be proposed here that: (a) the personal benefits at low personal and social risks. Whenever the
distinct networks disclosed by the distinct NO and YES voting h ratio risk/benefit approaches one, conflict about acting or not
(ei) pattern P3 would support the distinct arguments for and arises and makes the decision-making difficult. Conflict is a
against each voting decision; and (b) the similar networks source for procrastination, that is, in the case of voting, is the
disclosed by similar h(ei) patterns P1 and P2 would be enrolled in source for undecided and certainly not votings. Therefore, the
the calculations of the risks and benefits associated with the willingness to act (intention to vote) depends on adequacy and
selected arguments and the adequacy of each voting decision. conflict. In the present case, the risks and benefits of the YES
Author's personal copy

206 BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 –21 1

arguments are evaluated in the IDS space, whereas the risks and the relations between the EEG activity and the brain network
benefits of the NO arguments are evaluated in the PDS space, organization. However, further experiments involving, for
and then these values are used for the calculations of the voting instance, simultaneous EEG and fMRI recordings are necessary
intention. to provide a better support for this hypothesis.
In this context, we may propose that the networks If our hypothesis holds, then h(ei)P3 differences for Yes and
identified by the different h(ei)P3 patterns are those supporting No voters shall reflect the choices of the different arguments
the risk and benefits evaluation in PDS and IDS, whereas the for supporting their distinct voting decisions. Taking into
similar networks disclosed by h(ei)P1 and h(ei)P2 patterns are in consideration that the media campaign proposed YES argu-
charge of calculating the adequacy or intention of each voting ments to be evaluated in the IDS space, whereas the risks and
decision. Being more specific, we may propose that the pattern benefits of the NO arguments should be evaluated in the PDS,
h(ei)P2 is associated with the executive network in charge of then the Yes h(ei)P3 is associated with the neural circuits
controlling the calculations, whereas pattern h(ei)P1 is associ- composing the IDS space and the No h(ei)P3 is associated to
ated with this calculation. those composing the PDS space.
Dosenbach et al. (2006) and Dosenbach (2007) proposed that Kennan (2000) reviewed the literature on fMRI, ERPs,
mixed blocked/event-related fMRI allow the direct and separate transcranial stimulation, split-brain and focal lesions and
extraction of three types of task set-related signals: (1) a signal proposed that self-recognition, a PDS-specific task, shows that
tied to the start of a specific task condition, which should be, at the right prefrontal cortex may be an important component of a
least in part, related to the instantiation of task parameters; neural circuit for self-evaluation. Udin (2004) showed that
(2) activities sustained at a constant level across the task period, images containing more “self” information increased activity
some of which likely reflect task set maintenance; and (3) an in the right hemispheres, including the inferior parietal lobe,
error-related feedback, to monitor processing and encourage inferior frontal gyrus and inferior occipital gyrus, whereas
adjustments of top-down signals. Using data from ten different images containing more “other” information increased signals
experiments and 183 subjects, these authors (Dosenbach et al., in the medial prefrontal cortex and precuneus. Iacoboni (2004)
2008) confirmed their propositions and identified, besides a set studied people watching realistic movie clips depicting everyday
of task-related signals, the presence of two different networks social interactions, and they observed that the movie clips'
related with task control and control maintenance: the fronto- epochs showing people interaction, as compared to those
parietal and the cingulo-opercular networks. The fronto-parietal depicting single individuals involved in daily activities, yielded
network includes the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and intra- increased activity in the medial parietal (precuneus) and
parietal sulcus, and it seems to initiate and adjust the control of dorsomedial prefrontal cortices. Those authors proposed that
the task-solving cognitive mechanism. The cingulo-opercular default networks would participate in social cognition. A similar
net includes the dorsal anterior cingulated/medial superior proposition was also made by Schilbahc (2008). The ability to
frontal cortex, anterior insula/frontal operculum and anterior differentiate kin from non-kin members of one's social group is
prefrontal cortex, and it provides the stability for this mecha- paramount to survival when living in social groups. Platek and
nism for the duration of the task. In addition, another network Kemp (2009) showed that kin recognition is associated with the
would provide the instantiation of task parameters used by the posterior cingulate cortex and cuneus. All of these data are in
fronto-parietal network. In addition, Fair et al. (2009) showed accordance with our results showing that No h(ei)P3 is charac-
that brain processing changes from a more local to a more terized mostly by high h(ei)values for the right anterior central
distributed one from early school-age children to adults, while and frontal electrodes, whereas Yes h(ei)P3 depicts high h(ei)
maintaining “small-world”-like properties. Here, we propose values mostly for the medial electrodes.
that h(ei)P1 and h(ei)P2 are the EEG signatures of the fronto- From what was discussed so far, we may assume that:
parietal and cingulo-opercular networks, and h(ei)P3 is associat-
ed with the instantiating network setting the initial parameters (1) h(ei)P3 is associated with the networks evaluating
for calculating task-solving. benefits (b(vi)) and risks (r(vi)) at both PDS and IDS
Recent studies involving EEG spectra analysis has provided networks;
support for identifying different networks associated with the (2) h(ei)P2 is associated with the Voting Processing Network
most common band frequencies (Chen et al., 2008; Ferri et al., calculating the adequacy (ψ(vi)) of voting YES or NO
2008; Mantini et al., 2007) and have shown that each fMRI- taking into account the calculations done by the PDS
identified network is characterized by a specific electrophys- and IDS networks; ; and
iological signature that involves the combination of different (3) h(ei)P2 is associated with the Voting Decision Network
brain rhythms. Scheering et al. (2008) correlated frontal theta determining the intention of voting YES or NO taking
activity with default network activity. Chen et al. (2008) into account the adequacy and conflict estimated by the
showed that the spectral EEG default network is characterized Voting Processing Network.
by a complex EEG spectral field dominance, with delta, theta,
beta-2 and gamma activities predominating at the frontal
electrodes, while alpha1, alpha2 and beta-1 activities pre- 4. Conclusion
dominated at the posterior electrodes. A complex but specific
correlation between resting state networks and the EEG We claimed at the Introduction that the study of real and
default network is reported by Mantini et al. (2007). The natural decision-making imposes some constraints over the
relationships between h(ei)Pi and A ðei ; tÞPi disclosed by regres- experimental paradigms and technologies used to collect the
sion analysis adds new information to the understanding of required data for analysis and decision-making modeling. We
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 – 2 11 207

have stressed that such kind of study does not allow the reporting their intended votes in the real election to be held
control of the most important variables by the researcher, that one week later. For the purpose of calculations, the possible
in the majority of the cases it requires a portable recording answers were numerically coded as:
technique and that the experiments have to be done as close
to the actual moment of the decision-making as possible, if
CY = 1; PY = 2; NN = 3; UN = 4; NY = 5; PN = 6 and CN = 7;
not at the very moment. If the last is not the case, we have to
study the intention or willingness of deciding instead of the
real decision-making. and they were classified as: Class 1 or Yes vote (comprising
Here, we studied the voting intention (or willingness) in the codes 1, 2 and 3) and Class 2 or No vote (comprising codes 5,6
Brazilian Firearm Commerce Referendum during the week and 7).
preceding the election, using two different EEG methodologies Two networked personal computers were used, one for the
(Event Related Activity and Event Related Entropy), and we EEG recording and the other for presenting the voting options.
correlated the results with the media campaign, which linked The volunteers were allowed to take as much time as needed
voting decision with the evaluation of the cost and benefits in to vote. The timing of voting options' presentation (to) and
both personal and social contexts. voting decision (tv) were tagged as corresponding marks
Because we were not allowed to control the most important (mo, mv) in the file of the recorded EEG. The EEG was visually
variables influencing the voting decision, we had to hypoth- inspected for artifacts before being processed, and the events
esize that this decision was to be dependent on the neural associated with a bad EEG (e.g., when eye movements could
circuits involved with self and social cognition, if the media compromise the regression analysis) were discarded. The
was successful in influencing voters. voting decision time was calculated as tv − to and averaged 32 s.
Our results show that voting decision-making involves a The EEG recorded during the two seconds preceding the voting
very complex network of widely distributed neurons in charge decision was used for both the ERP analysis and the brain
of: (1) evaluating the risk and benefits in both Personal and mapping (Foz et al., 2001; Rocha et al., 2004, 2005).
Interpersonal Decision Spaces; (2) calculating the fairness The ERP was calculated as the average A ðei ; tÞ of the
of each voting decision; and (3) determining the intention amplitudeA(ei, t) of the EEG activity recorded for each electrode
(or willingness) to vote Yes or No or not to vote. The results averaged over the 2-s epoch preceding the voting decision
validate, therefore, the EEG technologies used here as the tool (therefore, t ∈ ½−2; 0Š). The correlation coefficient ri, jcomputed
of choice for such a study type, and they show that if the for the EEG amplitude recorded by the electrodes ei and ej was
important variables influencing the decision-making cannot used as a measure of the connectivity w(ni, nj) to compute the
be controlled by the researcher, they can be learned from the net entropy h(ei) according to Rocha et al. (Foz et al., 2001;
environmental conditions under which the decision is sup- Rocha et al., 2004, 2005). One-way Analysis of Variance
posed to be made. This kind of approach paves the way for (ANOVA) was used to compare A ðei ; tÞ and h(ei), with classifica-
future studies of human decision-making for the complexes tions variables of the above-mentioned Classes 1 and 2.
issues of a modern society. Factorial analysis was used to study the covariation of
A ðei ; tÞ and h(ei) for the groups of volunteers that voted Yes
(Class 1) and No (Class 2). This analysis (Tables 2 and 3)
5. Experimental procedures revealed that three factors accounted for more than 60% of
covariation of A ðei ; tÞ or h(ei) for both Class 1 and Class 2. The
Thirty-two subjects, 16 female and 16 male adults (Table 5), resulting eigenvalues for all factors were greater than 1.9.
had their EEG registered with 20 electrodes placed according to Three factorial brain mappings (Figs. 2 and 3) for the Yes and
the 10/20 system; impedance was smaller than 10 Kohm; the No voters were constructed to describe the results of these
low band pass filter was at 50 Hz; the sampling rate was factorial analyses. These mappings were constructed taking
256 Hz; and there were 10 bits of resolution while selecting one into account the loading values fj(ei) of A ðei ; tÞ or h(ei) for factors
of the possible options, Fj( j = 1, 2, 3). To estimate possible similarities between the
loadings fj(ei)of Fj( j = 1, 2, 3) in Class 1 and Class 2, we used
1) I will certainly vote YES (CY) Pearson's correlation coefficients.
2) I will probably vote YES (PY) Linear regression analysis was used to estimate the
20
3) I will certainly not vote YES (NY) function v = τ + ∑ βi hðei Þ, correlating the vote v and the net
i=1
4) I am undecided (UN)
entropy h(ei) (see Table 5). A regression brain mapping (Fig. 3)
5) I will certainly not vote NO (NN)
was constructed taking into account the normalized values of
6) I will probably vote NO (PN)
βh ðei Þ, where h ðei Þ is the average of the net entropy h(ei)
7) I will certainly vote NO (CN),
calculated for all subjects.

Table 5 – Characteristics of the studied population.


Age Gender Instruction Income Appendix 1. Graph theory
Mean 35 Female 0.42 Primary School 0.17 Low 0.41
A graph G = ðN; A; WÞ is a mathematical description of a
Max 63 Male 0.58 High School 0.30 Medium 0.44
network, consisting of a collection N of nodes n, A of arcs a
Min 18 College 0.53 High 0.27
(n i , n j )connecting the nodes n i , n j and W of weights of
Author's personal copy

208 BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 –21 1

connections w(ni, nj)of a(ni, nj). The average weighting connec- nodes that may connect to hub nodes (Iturria-Medina et al.,
tion wðni Þ of the node ni is calculated as: 2008). For example p(k) = k − α log(k / kc)is a broad-range network,
where kc is the limiting degree. The high connectivity of hubs
c−1
∑ wðni ; nj Þ reduces the average path length of a network.
j=1 According to the cluster definition w(ni, nj) = w(nj, ni) → 1 for
wðni Þ =
c−1 all ni, nj ∈ σ. Therefore h(ni, nj) → 0. Now, if it is required that
wðni Þ≅0:5, then h ðni Þ≅1. In such a condition, h(ni) → k.
where c is the cardinality of G.
The following are important definitions in graph theory.
A pathway p is an ordered set of nodes ni, nm for which Appendix 2. Brain networks
w(nj, nj + 1) > 0, i ≤ j < m. A closed pathway is defined for ni = nm. The
length l(p)of a pathway p is equal to the number k = m − i of arcs Many functional neuroimaging studies, have frequently
a (ni, nj)connecting the nodes ni, nj. The power π(p)of a pathway p shown task-induced signal attenuation compared to a control
−1
is defined by its weakest link, that is π(p) = min m j = i w(nj, nj + 1). A state (e.g., Raichle et al., 2001). Among the brain areas with
pathway p is called preferential over p' if π(p) > π(p ' ). decreased brain activity, the anterior (ACC) and posterior
A graph is called direct if w(ni, nj) ≠ w(nj, ni) for at least a pair cingulate cortex (PCC), precuneus (PCu), medial prefrontal
of nodes ni, nj ∈ N. A graph is called strictly direct if w(ni, nj) ≠ w cortex (MPFC), left inferior temporal cortex (ITC) and bilateral
(nj, ni) for all pairs of nodes ni, nj ∈ N. inferior parietal cortex (IPC) often exhibit decreased brain
Information is allowed to flow from ni to nj if w(ni, nj) > 0. As a activity from a baseline state other than from an activated
consequence, a pathway is a channel for information distri- state. During rest but awake state, the areas within this default
bution. Here it is proposed that the maximum amount of mode network have cerebral blood and cerebral metabolism
information h(ni, nj) that may be transmitted from ni to nj is rate for oxygen levels significantly above the global mean, but
constrained by w(ni, nj) and it is calculated as: have oxygen extraction fraction level similar to the global
hðni ; nj Þ = −wðni ; nj Þlog2 wðni ; nj Þ−ð1−wðni ; nj ÞÞlog2 ð1−wðni ; nj ÞÞ mean (Raichle et al., 2001). This set of areas are proposed to
compose the so called default network. PCC/PCu and ACC/MPFC
The average amount h ðni Þ of information handled by ni is constitute the main elements of this default mode network
proposed to be calculated as: (Greicius et al., 2003; Raichle et al., 2001).
Subsequently, a large set of studies has showed the existence
h ðni Þ = −w ðni Þlog2 w ðni Þ−ð1−wðni ÞÞlog2 ð1−wðni ÞÞ
of a default mode network and its anti-correlation network
The maximum amount of information h(p) that may be which showed negative linear correlation with the default mode
transmitted through pathway p is given by: network (Fransson, 2005; Fox et al., 2005) during a resting-state
using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Kelly et al.
hðpÞ = −πðpÞlog2 πðpÞ−ð1−πðpÞÞlog2 ð1−πðpÞÞ (2008) quantified the negative correlation between these two
networks in 26 subjects, during active (Eriksen flanker task) and
and the total amount h(ni) of information handled by node ni is
resting state scans, and found that the strength of the
given by:
correlation between the two networks varies across individuals.
c−1 These individual differences appear to be behaviorally relevant,
hðni Þ = ∑ h ðni Þ−hðni ; nj Þ
j=1 as interindividual variation in the strength of the correlation
was significantly related to individual differences in response
The network efficiency has been expressed in terms of time variability: the stronger the negative correlation (i.e., the
information flow (Marchiori and Latora, 2000) and it depends closer to 180° antiphase), the less variable the behavioral
on the structure of the network. performance. This relationship was moderately consistent
The concept of “small-world” is strongly related to the across resting and task conditions, suggesting that the measure
average shortest path length (L) and clustering coefficient (C) indexes moderately stable Individual differences in the integrity
concepts (Iturria-Medina et al., 2008). A cluster σ is a subset of of functional brain networks. Fransson (2006) compared intrin-
the network in which vertices adjacents to any vertex are sic activity during rest and the two-back task to the signal
adjacent to each other (Wats and Strogatz, 1998). The small- increases and decreases observed in an epoch-related version of
world network is characterized by a high clustering coefficient the working memory task. His results show that spontaneous
and a small average path length. The scale free network intrinsic activity in the default-mode network is not extin-
studied by Barabasi and Albert (1999) was the second major guished but rather attenuated during performance of the
step for the understanding of the growth of the real networks. working memory task. Moreover, he showed that the intrinsic
The authors proposed a model for the growth of a network activity in the task-positive network is reorganized in response
where p for newly added edges to a node (or vertex) depends to the working memory task. He concluded that his results
upon the degree k of this vertex, such that vertices with a large indicates that task-induced signal deactivations in the default-
number of edges (high k), called hub nodes, are more likely to mode regions is modulated by cognitive load and that intrinsic,
get even more nodes, that isp(k) = k − α. The exponent is spontaneous signal fluctuations in the default-mode regions
expected to vary in the range2 < α < 3. Broad-scale networks persist and reorganize in response to changes in external work
are characterized by a degree distribution that has a power law load.
regime followed by a sharp cutoff that restricts the increase of Graph theory allows the definition of what should be
p(k). The cutoff function constrains the maximum number of considered an optimal network. The notion of an optimal
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 – 2 11 209

network is closely associated with the “small-world” phenom- organization of the functionally connected brain, in which
enon (Reijneveld et al, 2007; Wats and Strogatz, 1998). The so- voxels are mostly connected with their direct neighbors forming
called “small-world” network architecture is distinguished clustered sub-networks, which are held together by a small
from either ordered or random networks. On average, a number of highly connected hub-voxels that ensure a high level
sparsely connected graph is expected to have a lower of overall connectivity.
clustering coefficient and longer path length than a densely
connected one with the same topology. Networks with ‘small-
world’ architecture are characterized by a combination of Appendix 3. The entropy as a measure of graph
strong local clustering and a short characteristic path length connectivity
(an index of global integration). This means that although most
of the connectivity is local, the network remains highly A graph G = ðN; A; WÞ is a mathematical description of a
integrated due to a small number of long distance connections. network, consisting of a collection N of nodes n, A of arcs
Networks with “scale-free” architecture (Barabasi and Albert, a(ni, nj) connecting the nodes ni, nj and W of weights of
1999) are characterized by the presence of nodes with very connections w(ni, nj)of a(ni, nj). The weight w(ni, nj) is a linear
large number of long distance connections (the hub nodes). function of the probability of the success of correctly transmit-
The likelihood p(k) of a node to have k connections is given by p ting a signal from nitonj through the arc (channel) a(ni, nj).
(k) = k − α,2 < α < 5. Broad-scale networks are characterized by a Therefore w(ni, nj) → 1 when that probability tends to 1 and
degree distribution that has a power law regime followed by a conversely w(ni, nj) → 0 when the probability tends to 0. If signal
sharp cutoff that restricts the increase of p(k). The cutoff identity condition is relaxed by requiring the existence of
function constrains the maximum number of nodes that may defined associations between the signals identified at ni and nj,
connect to hub nodes (Iturria et al, 2008). For example p(k) = k1 then the values of w(ni, nj) may be estimated by the correlation
−α
log(k / kc) is a broad-range network, where kc is the limiting between the observed signals at ni and nj, respectively. It may be
degree. From equations 4, broad-range and scale-free networks assumed that the correlation between the signals identified at
have clusters of well-connected nodes ei and ejfor which pi, niandnjincreases as the probability of the success of correctly
j → 0.5 because the characteristic pathway length li, j tends to 2,
transmitting a signal from nitonj increases. The average weight-
and a small number of hub nodes eh for which ph, j → 1 if k < kc. ing connection w ðni Þ of the node ni is calculated as:
Micheloyannis et al. (2006) recorded EEG signals to study c−1
neuronal interactions during working memory tests in indivi- ∑ wðni ; nj Þ
j=1
duals who had few years of formal education (LE) as compared w ðni Þ = ð1Þ
c−1
to individuals with university degrees (UE). They quantified the
synchronization between EEG channels in several frequency where c is the cardinality of G.
bands, and then converted EEG signal correlations into graphs to Appendix 1 provides a more detailed formal description of
estimate the clustering and distance characteristics of the the graph structure supporting small-world, scale-free and
underlying processing networks. According to the authors, broad-range networks. Its reading may be useful for the full
their finding supports the neural efficiency hypothesis and understanding of what follows.
suggests that the connections between brain areas of well- Information is efficiently transmitted through small-world,
educated subjects engaged in working memory tasks have less scale-free and broad range networks because the mean path
small-world characteristics than those of less-educated volun- lengths of those network are small (ref). Following (Rocha
teers. Iturria et al. (2008) estimated the anatomical connection et al., 2004, 2005):
probabilities (ACP) between 90 cortical and subcortical brain
gray matter areas from diffusion-weighted Magnetic Resonance (a) the capacity of the channel supported by the arc a(ni, nj)
Imaging (DW-MRI) techniques and concluded that all the is given by the entropy h(ni, nj) defined as:
studied networks have small-world and broad-scale character-
hðni ; nj Þ = −wðni ; nj Þlog2 wðni ; nj Þ−ð1−wðni ; nj ÞÞlog2 ð1−wðni ; nj ÞÞ;
istics. Van den Heuvel et al. (2008) used a voxel-wise approach
ð2Þ
for a model-free examination of both inter-regional as well as
intra-regional in the human brain. Resting-state 3 Tesla fMRI (b) the average amount of information handled by the node
recordings of 28 healthy subjects were acquired and individual ni if given by the entropy h ðni Þ calculated as:
connectivity graphs were formed out of all cortical and sub-
cortical voxels with connections reflecting inter-voxel function- h ðni Þ = −w ðni Þlog2 w ðni Þ−ð1−w ðni ÞÞlog2 ð1−w ðni ÞÞ; and ð3Þ
al connectivity. Graph characteristics from these connectivity
networks were computed. The clustering-coefficient of these c) the net amount of information handled by the node ni if
networks turned out to be much higher than the clustering- given by the entropy h(ni) of the node ni, calculated as:
coefficient of comparable random graphs, together with a short
c−1
average path length, indicating a small-world organization. hðni Þ = ∑ ðh ðni Þ−hðni ; nj ÞÞ: ð4Þ
j=1
Furthermore, the connectivity distribution of the number of
inter-voxel connections followed a power-law scaling with an In this context, the actual value of h(ni) is:
exponent close to 2, suggesting a scale-free network topology.
Their findings suggested a combined small-world and scale-free (a) zero for any node of a totally connected graph because in
organization of the functionally connected human brain. The this case w(ni, nj) = 1 for all nj, and consequently h(ni, nj) = 0,
results were interpreted as evidence for a highly efficient w ðni Þ = 1 and h ðni Þ = 0;
Author's personal copy

210 BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 –21 1

(b) zero for any node of a totally disconnected graph Goldman, R.I., Wei, C.Y., Philiastides, M.G., Gerson, A.D., Friedman,
because in this case w(ni, nj) = 0 for all nj, and conse- D., Brown, T.R., Sajda, P., 2009. Single-trial discrimination for
integrating simultaneous EEG and fMRI: identifying cortical
quently h(ni, nj) = 0, w ðni Þ = 0 and h ðni Þ = 0;
areas contributing to trial-to-trial variability in the auditory
(c) zero for any node of a random graph because in this case oddball task. Neuroimage 47, 136–147.
w(ni, nj) = 0.5 for all nj, and consequently h(ni, nj) = 1, Greene, J.D., Nystrom, L.E., Engell, A.D., Darley, J.M., Cohen, J.D.,
w ðni Þ = 0:5 and h ðni Þ = 1. 2004. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in
moral judgment. Neuron 44, 389–400.
In addition, in the case of the nodes ni ni highly connected Greicius, M.D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A.L., Menon, V., 2003. Functional
to c other nodes np, the actual value of h(ni): connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the
default mode hypothesis. Proc. Nat. Acad. Scie. 100, 253–258.
c Hauk, O., Davis, M.H., Ford, M., Pulvermüller, F., Marslen-Wilson,
(d) tends to a value dependent on the ratio because
n−1 W.D., 2006. The time course of visual word recognition as
w(ni, np) → 1 for all c highly connected nodes np and revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. Neuroimage
w(ni, nq) → 0 for all the other remaining nodes nq = n − c, 30, 1383–1400.
such that n−1 c
≤w ðni Þ≤ 0:5nn−1
+ 0:5c
, 0 < h ðni Þ≤1, h(ni, np) → 0 and Heldmann, M., Russeler, J., Munte, T.F., 2005. Event-related
0 ≤ h(ni, nq) ≤ 1. potentials in a decision-making task with delayed and
immediate reward conditions. J. Psychophysiol. 19, 270–274.
Huettel, S.A., Stowe, C.J., Gordon, E.M., Warner, B.T., Platt, M.L.,
REFERENCES 2006. Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and
ambiguity. Neuron 49, 766–775.
Iacoboni, M., 2004. Watching social interactions produces
Alegre, M., Imirizaldu, L., Valencia, M., Iriarte, J., Arcocha, J., dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal
Artieda, J., 2006. Alpha and beta changes in cortical oscillatory increases compared to a resting baseline. Neuroimage 21,
activity in a go/no go randomly-delayed-response choice 1167–1173.
reaction time paradigm. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 16–25. Ibope (2005) (www.ibope.com.br) is the most antique and famous
Barabasi, A.L., Albert, R., 1999. Emergence of caling in radom institute of public opinion research in Brazil, with branches in
networks. Science 286, 509–512. other 15 countries.
Chen, A.C.N., Feng, W., Zaho, H., Yin, Y., Wang, P., 2008. EEG Ino, T., Nakai, R., Azuma, T., Kimura, T., Fukuyama, H., 2010.
default mode network in the human brain: spectral regional Differential activation of the striatum for decision making and
field powers. Neuroimage 41, 561–574. outcomes in a monetary task with gain and loss. Cortex 46,
Chen, P., Qiu, J., Li, H., Zang, Q., 2009. Spatiotemporal cortical 2–14.
activation underlying dilemma decision-making: an Iturria-Medina, Y., Sotero, R.C., Canales-Rodríguez, E.J., Alemán-
event-related potential study. Biol. Psychol. 82, 111–115. Gómez, Y.R., Melie-García, L., 2008. Studying the human brain
Cohen, M.X., Elger, C.E., Ranganath, C., 2007. Reward expectation anatomical network via diffusion-weighted MRI and Graph
modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. Theory. NeuroImage 40, 1064–1076.
Neuroimage 35, 968–978. Jacobs, J., Hwang, G., Curran, T., Kahana, M.J., 2006. EEG oscillations
Dosenbach, N.U.F., 2007. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and and recognition memory: Theta correlates of memory retrieval
stable task control in humans. PNNAS 104, 11703–???. and decision making. Neuroimage 32, I978–I987.
Dosenbach, N.U.F., Visscher, E.D., Palmer, F.M., Miezin, K.K., Kelly, A.M., Uddin, L.Q., Biswal, B.B., Xavier Castellanos, F.,
Wenger, H.C., Kang, E.D., Burgund, A., Grimes, L., Schlaggar, B.L., Milham, M.P., 2008. Competition between functional brain
Petersen, S.E., 2006. A core system for the implementation of networks mediates behavioral variability. NeuroImage 39,
task sets. Neuron 50, 799–812. 527–537.
Dosenbach, N.U.F., Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Schlaggar, B.L., Petersen, Kennan, J.P., 2000. Self- recognition and the right prefrontal cortex.
S.E.A., 2008. Dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 338–344.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 99–105. Kliemann, D., Young, L., Scholz, J., Saxe, R., 2008. The influence of
Esposito, F., Mulert, C., Goebel, R., 2009. Combined distributed prior record on moral judgment. Neuropsychologia 46,
source and single-trial EEG–fMRI modeling: application to 2949–2957.
effortful decision making processes. Neuroimage 47, 112–121. Knutson, B., Rick, S., Wimmer, G.E., Prelec, D., Loewenstein, G.,
Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Power, J.D., Dosenbach, N.U.F., Chruch, J.A., 2007. Neural predictors of purchases. Neuron 53, 147–156.
Miezin, F.M., Shclaggar, B.L., Petern, S.E., 2009. Functional brain Mantini, D., Perrucci, M.G., Del Gratta, C., Romani, G.L., Corbetta, M.,
networks develolp from a “local to distributed” organization. 2007. Electrophysiological signatures of retings state networks
PLoS Comput. Biol. 5, e1000381. in the human brain. PNAS 104 (32), 13170–13175.
Ferri, R., Rundo, F., Bruni, O., Terzano, M.G., Stam, C.J., 2008. The Marchiori, M., Latora, V., 2000. Harmony in the small-world.
functional connectivity of different EEG bands moves towards Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 285,
small-world network organization during sleep. Clin. 539–546.
Neurophysiol. 119, 2026–2036. McClure, S.M., Laibson, D.I., Loweestein, G., Cohen, J.D., 2004.
Fransson, P., 2005. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed
fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default monetary rewards. Science 306, 503–507.
mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, Micheloyannis, S., Pachou, E., Stam, C.J., Vourkas, M.l., Erimaki, S.,
15–29. Tsirka, V., 2006. Using graph theoretical analysis of multi
Fransson, P., 2006. How default is the default mode of brain channel EEG to evaluate the neural efficiency hypothesis.
function?: Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal Neurosci. Lett. 402, 273–277.
fluctuations. Neuropsychologia 44, 836–2845. Moll, J., Oliveira-Souza, R., 2007. Moral judgments. emotions and
Foz, F.B., Luchini, Palmeri, S., Rocha, A.F., Rodela, E.C., Rondó, A.G., the utilitarian brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 319–321.
Cardoso, M.B., Ramazzini, P.B., Leite, C.C., 2001. Language Olofsson, J.K., Nordin, S., Sequeira, H., Polich, J., 2008. Affective
plasticity revealed by EEG mapping. Pediatr. Neurol. 26, picture processing: an integrative review of ERP findings. Biol.
106–115. Psychol. 77, 247–265.
Author's personal copy

BR A I N R ES E A RC H 1 3 5 1 ( 2 01 0 ) 1 9 8 – 2 11 211

Platek, S.M., Kemp, S.M., 2009. Is family special to the brain? An Rocha, F.T., Rocha, A.F., Massad, E., Menezes, R.X., 2005. Brain
event-related fMRI study of familiar. familial and self-face mappings of the arithmetic processing in children and adults.
recognition. Neuropychologia 47, 849–858. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 359–372.
Polezzi, D., Daum, I., Rubaltelli, E., Lotto, L., Civai, C., Sartori, G., Rocha, A.F., Burattini, M.N., Rocha, F.T., Massad, E., 2009. A
Rumiati, R., 2008a. Mentalizing in economic decision-making. neuroeconomic modeling of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Behav. Brain Res. 190, 218–223. disorder (ADHD). J. Biol. Syst. 17, 10–19.
Polezzi, D., Lotto, L., Daum, I., Sartori, G., Rumiati, R., 2008b. Schilbahc, L., 2008. Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default
Predicting outcomes of decisions in the brain. Behav. Brain Res. mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the default
187, 116–122. system of the brain. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 457–467.
Polezzi, D., Sartori, G., Rumiati, R., Vidotto, G., Daum, I., 2010. Brain Udin, L.Q., 2004. Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal
correlates of risky decision-making. Neuroimage 49, 1886–1894. “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: and event-related
Raichle, M., MacLeod, A.M., Snyder, A.Z., Powers, W.J., Gusnard, D. fMRI study. Neuroimage 25, 926–935.
A., Shulman, G.L., 2001. A default mode of brain functioning. Utku, H., Erzengin, Ö.U., Cakmak, E.D., Karaka, S., 2002.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 98, 676–682. Discrimination of brain's neuroelectric responses by a
Reijneveld, J.C., Ponten, S.C., Berendse, H.W., Stam, C.J., 2007. The decision-making function. J. Neurosci.
application of graph theoretical analysis to complex networks Meth. 114, 25–31.
in the brain. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2317–2331. van den Heuvel, M.P., Stam, C.J., Boersma, M., Hulshoff Po, H.E.,
Robertson, D., Snarey, J., Ousley, O., Harenski, K., Bowman, F.D., 2008. Small-world and scale-free organization of voxel-based
Gilkey, R., Kilt, C., 2007. The neural processing of moral resting-state functional connectivity in the human brain.
sensitivity to issues of justice and care. Neuropsychologia 45, NeuroImage 43, 528–539.
755–766. Wats, D.J., Strogatz, S.H., 1998. Collective dynamics of small-words
Rocha, A.F., Massad, E., Pereira Jr., A., 2004. The brain: from fuzzy networks. Nature 393, 440–442.
arithmetic to quantum computing. Springer Verlag, Young, L., Saxe, R., 2008. The neural basis of belief encoding and
Heildelberg. integration in moral judgment. Neuroimage 40, 1912–1920.

You might also like