You are on page 1of 16

Accelerat ing t he world's research.

Do rats have a prefrontal cortex?


Harry B Uylings, H. Uylings, Henk Groenewegen

Behavioural Brain Research

Cite this paper Downloaded from Academia.edu 

Get the citation in MLA, APA, or Chicago styles

Related papers Download a PDF Pack of t he best relat ed papers 

T he medial prefront al cort ex in t he rat : evidence for a dorso-vent ral dist inct ion based upon f…
Henk Groenewegen

Do Rat s Have Prefront al Cort ex? T he Rose-Woolsey-Akert Program Reconsidered


Norbert Hajnal

T he orbit al cort ex in rat s t opographically project s t o cent ral part s of t he caudat e–put amen complex
Henk Groenewegen
Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

Review
Do rats have a prefrontal cortex?
Harry B.M. Uylings a,b,∗ , Henk J. Groenewegen b , Bryan Kolb c
a Netherlands Institute for Brain Research, KNAW, Graduate School Neurosciences, Meibergdreef 33, 1105 AZ Amsterdam, The Netherlands
b Department of Anatomy, Graduate School Neurosciences, VU University Medical Center, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
c Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, AB, Canada T1K 3M4

Abstract

The lack of a single anatomical or functional definition of ‘prefrontal cortex’ has led to different and, in some respects, controversial
views on the existence of a prefrontal cortex in non-primate mammals, in particular in rats. Until the classic paper by Rose and Woolsey
[Res. Publ. Assoc. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 27 (1948) 210], the general idea was that a prefrontal cortex is unique to primate species. Rose and
Woolsey’s ‘prefrontal cortex’ definition was based upon a single anatomical criterion, i.e. the cortical projection area of the mediodorsal
thalamic nucleus. Single criteria, however, do not appear to be sufficient for defining the prefrontal cortex. Therefore, other anatomical
and functional characteristics are currently used to identify the prefrontal cortex in different species. Yet, recently the debate about the
nature of the prefrontal cortex in non-primate species has been resumed. In the present paper we will compare the structural and functional
characteristics of the prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates and rats. We will argue that rats have a functionally divided prefrontal cortex
that includes not only features of the medial and orbital areas in primates, but also some features of the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
© 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Prefrontal cortex; Rat; Primates; Parallel circuits; Basal ganglia; Mesocortical dopaminergic system; Monoamines; Acetylcholine

1. Introduction

Abbreviations: ac, anterior commissure; ACd, dorsal anterior cingulate The volume of the cerebral cortex of a rat is about a
area; ACv, ventral anterior cingulate area; AId, dorsal agranular insular hundred times smaller than that of the cerebral cortex of
area; AIp, posterior agranular insular area; AIv, ventral agranular insu- macaques, and about a thousand times smaller than that
lar area; AM, anterior medial thalamic nucleus; AO, anterior olfactory of humans [138]. This increase in cortical volume in pri-
nucleus; BAC, basal amygdaloid complex; cc, corpus callosum; CPm,
mates is paralleled by an evolutionary differentiation of
caudate–putamen complex, medial part; DStr, dorsal striatum; Ent, en-
torhinal area; FL, forelimb area, according to Zilles [157]; Fr1/3, frontal cortical areas and by the development of more complex,
cortical areas 1 and 3, according to Zilles [157]; Fr2, frontal cortical area cognitive cerebral functions [114]. In this light it is not
2, rostral to about −1 mm from bregma; GPe, globus pallidus, external surprising that discussions are ongoing about whether or
segment; GPi, globus pallidus, internal segment; Hip, hippocampus; HL, not particular cortical areas in the rat brain are comparable
hindlimb area, according to Zilles [157]; IL, infralimbic cortical area;
with specific cortical areas in primates. Recently this issue
IMD, intermediodorsal thalamic nucleus; LO, lateral orbital cortical area;
MC, motor cortex; MDl, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus, lateral segment, has been raised about the prefrontal cortex, in particular
includes here MDpl; MDm, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus, medial seg- whether or not rats possess a prefrontal region that is com-
ment; MDm(a), anterior part of MDm; MDm(p), posterior part of MDm; parable with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in primates
MDpl, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus, paralamellar segment; MO, medial [111,114]. Such a question is complicated, since the rat
orbital cortical area; OB, olfactory bulb; Oc1, primary occipital (visual)
cortical fields are generally less evoluted, less differenti-
cortex [157]; Oc2L, lateral part of occipital cortex, area 2 [157]; Oc2M,
medial part of occipital cortex area 2 [157]; Par1(dysgr), dysgranular part ated and less segregated than those in the primate cerebral
of parietal cortex area 1 [157]; Par2, parietal cortex area 2 (supplemen- cortex. The primate prefrontal cortex consists of various
tary somatosensory cortex) [157]; PC, posterior cingulate area; PC/CL,
paracentral and central lateral thalamic nuclei; PF, parafascicular thala-
mic nucleus; Pir, (pre)piriform cortex; PL, prelimbic cortical area; PRh, taenia tecta; VA, ventral anterior thalamic nucleus; VL, ventral lateral
perirhinal cortical area; PV, paraventricular thalamic nucleus; rs, rhinal thalamic nucleus; VLO, ventrolateral orbital cortical area; VM, ventral
sulcus; RSA, agranular retrosplenial cortex; RSG, granular retrosplenial medial thalamic nucleus; VMm, medial part of VM; VO, ventral orbital
cortex; SMA, supplementary motor area; SNc, substantia nigra pars com- cortical area; VP, ventral pallidum; VStr, ventral striatum
pacta; SNr, substantia nigra pars reticulata; SNrdm, dorsomedial part of ∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +31-20-5665500; fax: +31-20-6961006.

SNr; STh, subthalamic nucleus; Te2, area 2 of temporal cortex [157]; TT, E-mail address: h.uylings@nih.knaw.nl (H.B.M. Uylings).

0166-4328/$ – see front matter © 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.028
4 H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

anatomically different subfields [10,23,138], roughly di- the first three criteria for comparing prefrontal cortical areas
vided in a dorsolateral, a medial and an orbital region [44]. in primates and rats. These three criteria also were applied
These different subdivisions of the primate prefrontal cor- by Preuss [111]. From this perspective we will consider the
tex are thought to be involved in different cognitive and connections of the rat and primate prefrontal cortices with
emotion functions [8,12,31,44,99]. It is generally accepted thalamic, basal ganglia, cortical, limbic and monoaminergic
that the prefrontal cortex is involved in different aspects structures, with respect to both pattern and density. Subse-
of executive control and that the neuronal basis for these quently, we will review comparatively the functional prop-
functions is formed by the extensive neuronal networks in erties of prefrontal areas in rats and primates.
which the prefrontal cortex is intricately involved. Although
Preuss [111] did not question the existence of a rat pre-
frontal cortex in general, this was the way it was perceived 3. Neuronal networks involving the prefrontal cortex
by many primate researchers. However, Preuss [111] and
Preuss and Kaas [114] questioned explicitly the existence of 3.1. Connections between prefrontal cortex and thalamus
an equivalent of the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
in non-primate species. To answer the question whether rats Thalamocortical connections are important for cortical
have a dorsolateral-like prefrontal cortex we must consider differentiation and specialization (e.g. [90]). The reciprocal
various anatomical and functional criteria that define the connections of the major thalamic nuclei are therefore used
different prefrontal regions. On the basis of the structural to define cerebral cortical areas. At the time of Rose and
and functional data reviewed below, we conclude that rats Woolsey [126], the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus
have a prefrontal cortex, part of which (in particular the dor- was assumed to be the only nucleus with thalamocorti-
somedial shoulder region) displays features that resemble cal projections to the prefrontal cortex, and was therefore
characteristics of the primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. viewed as the ‘defining’ nucleus. However, with the ad-
vent of more refined anterograde and retrograde tracing
techniques, it became apparent that the (prefrontal) cor-
2. Criteria for the definition of a prefrontal cortex tical areas that receive mediodorsal thalamic input also
are connected with other thalamic nuclei. Thalamic nuclei
For a long time after Brodmann’s studies [17] the pre- other than the mediodorsal nucleus that reach the prefrontal
frontal cortex was considered unique to the primate species cortex include the intralaminar and midline nuclei, the
and called the ‘frontal granular cortex’ [14]. The definition anterior medial nucleus and the rostral parts of the ven-
of prefrontal cortex at that time was based upon the cy- tral complex [11,12,15,35,50,55,56,67,72,133]. In addition,
toarchitectonic criterion of having a granular layer IV and thalamic mediodorsal nucleus projections appear to reach
a location rostral to the agranular (pre)motor areas. How- some cortical areas outside the prefrontal cortex, such as
ever, comparing different cortical areas in more distantly re- the premotor, motor, temporal and parietal cortices, as has
lated species solely on the basis of cytoarchitectonic criteria been demonstrated in, for example, macaque monkeys, cats,
appeared to be untenable. For example, the primary motor sheep, and dogs [1,37,50,56,72,100,136]. Among others,
cortex in rats is considered to be homologous to the one in Nauta [103] and Leonard [88] regarded the reciprocity of
monkeys [105], but this cortical area is agranular in mature the cortical projections of the thalamic mediodorsal nu-
primates and granular in rats. Likewise, Barbas and Pandya cleus as an important criterion for defining the prefrontal
[10] consider limbic cortices in primate brains, which are cortex. However, this definition also does not lead to an un-
agranular and dysgranular (i.e. layer IV is not and is not eas- ambiguous delineation of the prefrontal cortex. Therefore,
ily discernible, respectively) as part of the prefrontal cortex. Uylings and Van Eden [138] suggested inclusion of only
These are just two examples to emphasize why cytoarchitec- those cortical areas in the prefrontal cortex for which the
tonic criteria have been replaced by other criteria in seeking reciprocal connections with the mediodorsal nucleus (MD)
homologies between different brain areas in more distantly are stronger (i.e. in terms of a higher number of projecting
related species. It is now generally accepted that the follow- neurons and a higher density of terminals) than the recip-
ing criteria have to be taken into account when discussing rocal connections with other thalamic nuclei. This feature,
homologies between cortical areas in different species: (1) together with the pattern of cortico-cortical connections, has
the pattern of specific connections and the relative density led us to include the primate and rat anterior cingulate cor-
of these connections; (2) the functional (i.e. electrophys- tex in the prefrontal cortex [138]. This approach in defining
iological and behavioral) properties; (3) the presence and the prefrontal cortex is strengthened by the recent analysis
specific distribution of different neuroactive substances and of Kötter, who demonstrated the special, predominant posi-
neurotransmitter receptors; (4) the embryological develop- tion of the mediodorsal nucleus for the macaque prefrontal
ment; and (5) only for closely related species, the cytoarchi- cortex on basis of multidimensional scaling analysis of
tectonic characteristics. The greater the similarities between the thalamoprefrontal cortical projections (Kötter, personal
the characteristics, the more likely it is that brain regions communication, 2003). In addition, Barbas et al. [11], Der-
are homologous. In the following account we will employ mon and Barbas [35] and Ray and Price [122] showed in
H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17 5

Fig. 1. The extent of rat’s prefrontal cortex. (A, B). The increase in what is considered to be prefrontal cortex due to improvements in anatomical
techniques. Fine dots indicate the area described by Leonard [88], large dots the extension described by Krettek and Price [87], oblique lines the extension
proposed by Groenewegen [54] and vertical lines the extension proposed by Verwer in [138]. The nomenclature of Krettek and Price has been followed
with the exception of the neutral term frontal area 2 (Fr2), see Section 6 in text. MO and VO are the medial and ventral orbital areas and AI the rat
anterior insular area. (C) The view illustrated by Preuss and Kaas [114], modified from Preuss [111], in which ACd is now incorporated in the anterior
cingulate cortex. In the view of Preuss and Kaas shown in (C) the anterior cingulate (AC), the prelimbic (PL) and the infralimbic (IL) areas of (A) form
the prefrontal cortex, while the orbital and lateral prefrontal cortex are conspicuously lacking. For abbreviations see the Abbreviations section.

rhesus monkey that the majority of the thalamic neurons from the thalamic ventral anterior nucleus than from
projecting to the prefrontal cortex, as it has been defined be- the mediodorsal nucleus. When this is also true for the
fore, are located in the mediodorsal nucleus. This also goes efferent corticothalamic projections from area 25, then
for the rat prefrontal cortex (see Fig. 1) [54,87,123,140]. macaque area 25 should probably not be included in the
Dermon and Barbas [35] reported that area 25 in prefrontal cortex [12]. A similar situation holds likely
macaques (which is considered to be a prefrontal corti- for the ventrolateral orbital cortical area (VLO) in the rat
cal area, e.g. [10,106]) receives more thalamic afferents [54,123,155].
6 H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

For a further comparison of monkey and rat thalamic data Van Eden [138], and Van Eden et al. [142], because it is
it is important to know that in non-human primates thalamic caudal to about −1 mm from bregma (see also note below).
ventral nuclei have a differential cortical projection pattern In addition, Vertes [143] showed that both ACd and Fr2
for the prefrontal, premotor and motor cortex, respectively have strong projections particularly to the MD. Therefore,
[66]. The thalamic ventral anterior (VA) and posterior ven- we conclude that ACd and Fr2 have a more prefrontal than
tromedial (VMp) nuclei project quite diffusely and spread premotor type of thalamic connections. On basis of both
out over an extensive region of neocortex (i.e. premotor, sup- thalamic and subcortical and cortical connections we sup-
plementary motor area (SMA), supplementary eye field, an- pose also that the caudal Fr2 and (supragenual) parts of ACd
terior cingulate (AC) and posterior parietal cortex), but these incorporates a zone homologous to the macaque frontal eye
projections have a relatively higher concentration in the pre- field (FEF) [138,142]. This is corroborated by electrophys-
frontal cortex. In contrast, the anterior ventrolateral (VLa) iological studies [59,104] and by unilateral lesion studies
cortical projections are more concise and concentrated in causing multimodal neglect [73,95].
premotor and SMA areas, with additional lesser projections We realize the pitfalls of the quantitative definition of our
also to the motor area. In addition, the posterior ventrolat- anatomical ‘prefrontal cortex’ definition. Even with the use
eral nucleus (VLp) is the principal source of projections to of modern neuroanatomical tracing techniques, it is rather
primary motor area 4, but projects also to premotor, SMA, difficult to determine unequivocally for those cortical re-
and some to posterior parietal cortex [66]. In rats the topog- gions showing considerable overlap of connections from
raphy and the pattern of connectivity of the mediodorsal nu- various thalamic nuclei, which of these thalamic nuclei pro-
cleus is rather comparable with the one in monkeys [54,56]. vides the strongest connections. For example, comparison
The VA in rats, however, is usually included in the VL due of numbers [139] of different types of neurons projecting to
to its difficult cytoarchitectonic delineation [56,65,138]. a particular cortical area with retrograde tracing implies the
In the last decade the inclusion of the rat frontal area assumption that the extent of these axonal terminal fields is
2 (Fr2) and the dorsal anterior cingulate area (ACd) (see similar in the different regions that have been injected with
Fig. 1) in the prefrontal cortex has been disputed by Preuss a particular tracer. This is not always the case, however. It
[111] and Condé et al. [28,29]. The results of Condé et al.’s is therefore important to consider in addition the quality of
retrograde tracer studies on thalamic afferents [28] and connections (e.g. ‘driver’ and ‘modulator’ type of thalamic
cortico-cortical afferents [29] to rat medial frontal cortex connections [56,132]), the cortico-cortical and subcortical
were decisive for their and Preuss’ thesis, namely that the neuronal networks and functional properties in which dif-
rat Fr2 (also called the precentral medial area (PrCm) or ferent prefrontal cortical areas have a particular, different
agranular medial cortex (Agm), see below) and the rat ACd position.
are premotor areas and do not belong to the prefrontal
cortex. As a consequence they denied the presence of 3.2. Prefrontal cortex–basal ganglia relationships
dorsolateral-like prefrontal cortical features in rats. Condé
et al. [28] described in their retrograde tracer study that The frontal lobe as a whole has a special relationship
only a few neurons from the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus with the basal ganglia in that it is the part of the cerebral
project to Fr2 and ACd and that a higher number of thalamic cortex that receives the main input from the basal ganglia,
projecting neurons are positioned in the intralaminar, the through a relay in the thalamus [97]. This basal ganglia
ventrolateral (VL) and ventromedial (VM) nuclei. In this influence has long been known for the motor and premotor
respect, their Fig. 12 [28] is conspicuous for illustrating a cortices, but the frontal areas anterior to the (pre)motor
case with a relatively high number of MD neurons project- cortices (i.e. mostly the prefrontal cortical areas) are not
ing to ACd. Furthermore, several groups [54,87,121] have excepted from this particular subcortical influence. Like the
observed in extensive anterograde and retrograde tracing frontal cortex, the parietal, occipital and temporal cortices
studies that the rat Fr2 and ACd (Fig. 1) have a very dense all project to the basal ganglia, in particular the striatum, but
reciprocal connection with the paralamellar or ventrolateral these posteriorly located cortices do not receive information
segment of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus. This can also back from the basal ganglia, with the exception of area TE
be concluded from the anterograde and retrograde tracer in the inferotemporal cortex [96]. A particular subset of
studies of Reep and Corwin [124], which were directed thalamic nuclei (i.e. the mediodorsal, ventromedial, ventral
especially to the Fr2 (their AGm). Reep and Corwin dis- anterior and, to a lesser degree, the ventral lateral nuclei)
tinguished caudal AGm from mid and rostral AGm, and form the essential link between the pallidum and substantia
showed that rostral and mid AGm receives thalamic affer- nigra pars reticulata, as the output structures of the basal
ents from a higher number of neurons in the mediodorsal ganglia [2,58,94,98] and the (pre)frontal cortex. It is also
nucleus. It appeared that only the caudal AGm has afferents relevant to emphasize the high degree of topographical or-
from cells mainly located in VL and only a few neurons in ganization in the frontal cortical–basal ganglia connections
the mediodorsal nucleus. However, this part of AGm or Fr2 and in the basal ganglia–thalamic projections. This topo-
[156] is not included in the rat prefrontal cortex as defined graphical organization forms the basis for the existence of
by Krettek and Price [87], Groenewegen [54], Uylings and a number of parallel, largely functionally segregated basal
H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17 7

Fig. 2. Schematic diagrams to illustrate, for the rat brain, the involvement of different parts of the (pre)frontal cortex in various basal ganglia–thalamocortical
circuits and amygdaloid and midline/intralaminar thalamic circuits. Four basal ganglia–thalamocortical circuits are depicted with similar organizational
characteristics. Compare also the organization of similar circuits in primates [2]. All four circuits involve different parts of the (pre)frontal cortex, the dorsal
or ventral striatum, the pallidal and nigral complex and the ventral and medial thalamic nuclei. The present diagram emphasizes the ‘closed’ character
of these circuits, but they may in various specific ways be interconnected, and the (pre)frontal cortical and basal ganglia way stations have extensive
projections outside the depicted circuits. (A) ‘Motor circuit’: note the projections from the parafascicular thalamic nucleus to the motor cortex and the
dorsal striatum, together with the absence of the amygdala connections. (B) ‘Dorsal shoulder prefrontal’ circuit (Fr2 and ACd): the cortical and striatal way
stations of this circuit receive specific projections from the intralaminar thalamic complex (PC/CL) and the anterior part of the basal amygdaloid complex.
(C) ‘Medial prefrontal/anterior cingulate’ circuit (PL/IL/MO/ACv): the paraventricular thalamic nucleus and the posterior part of the basal amygdaloid
complex are strongly involved in this particular circuit. (D) ‘Lateral prefrontal/agranular insular’ circuit (AIv/AId): the intermediodorsal thalamic nucleus
and the anterior part of the basal amygdaloid complex project to the cortical and striatal way stations of this circuit. For a comparison with the circuits
described in primates, it is important to note that the topography of subcortical afferents and thalamocortical efferents of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus
in rats and primates is largely similar [56]. The medial (and central) segments of the rat mediodorsal nucleus resemble the magnocellular part of the
primate MD. The lateral (and paralamellar) segments of the rat MD are comparable with the multiform and densocellular parts of the primate MD.

ganglia–thalamocortical circuits that have been identified the dorsal anterior cingulate (ACd) and Fr2 areas receive the
both in primates [2,3,58,98]. Alexander et al. [2,3] identi- least amygdaloid fibers of the prefrontal areas [57], while
fied five circuits in primates: a ‘motor’, an ‘oculomotor’, also in nonhuman primates the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
an ‘anterior cingulate/medial orbitofrontal’, a ‘lateral has the weakest amygdaloid input in the prefrontal cor-
orbitofrontal’, and a ‘dorsolateral’ prefrontal circuit, which tex [5]. All these connectional aspects point to similarities
are further subdivided by Middleton and Strick [98]. In rats, between prefrontal cortex–basal ganglia–thalamocortical
similar circuits have been described, as illustrated in Fig. 2. circuits of primates and rats. In particular, they show a num-
Further important aspects of the circuitry in which the ber of comparable features for both the primate dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia are involved concern prefrontal cortex and the rat ACd and Fr2 areas.
the specific relationships of the projections from the mid-
line/intralaminar thalamic complex as well as the amygdala 3.3. Cortico-cortical networks
and, to a lesser extent the hippocampus, to the cortical and
striatal relay stations in the basal ganglia–thalamocortical Both the thalamocortical and the cortico-cortical con-
circuits (Fig. 2) [9,55,57,58,93,144]. In both rats and pri- nections with the rat and primate prefrontal cortex are
mates, the circuits that involve the prefrontal cortical areas predominantly ipsilateral [1,54,108,142]. The majority of
are characterized by amygdaloid inputs. However, in rats terminal axons in the prefrontal cortex are cortical afferents
8 H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

[108,142]. In both rats and primates the prefrontal cortex is and paralimbic cortical regions (see Fig. 3). The reciprocal
extensively connected with different cortical areas such as cortico-cortical connections reveal at least three subfields in
premotor, somatosensory, auditory, visual, olfactory, gus- the rat medial prefrontal cortex, i.e. a ‘dorsal shoulder’ re-
tatory and limbic cortical areas [7,24,26,142]. Pandya and gion (Fr2 and ACd); a rostral part of the medial prefrontal
Yeterian [108] showed that in macaques the connections cortex; and the prelimbic and infralimbic cortices.
between the prefrontal and other cortical areas are recipro- The architectonically less differentiated areas in the rat,
cal and that such connections exist preferentially between the infralimbic and prelimbic cortices and the lateral pre-
cortical areas with a similar level of cytoarchitectonic differ- frontal cortex (i.e. agranular insular cortices), have recip-
entiation. However, connections between distinct prefrontal rocal connections with the perirhinal and entorhinal cortex,
cortical areas exist both between areas with similar levels and with the CA1 field and subiculum of the hippocampal
of cytoarchitectonic differentiation as well as between areas formation [57,62,145]. The rostral part of the rat medial pre-
with different levels of differentiation. In addition, Barbas frontal cortex has reciprocal connections with motor, mixed
et al. [12] showed that the cytoarchitectonically less differen- somatosensory-motor, and somatosensory association cor-
tiated prefrontal regions have fewer specific cortico-cortical tices. Thus, premotor characteristics appear to coincide here
connections. They receive projections from two or more with prefrontal characteristics [138]. The ‘dorsal shoulder’
cortical areas representing different sensory modalities to- region has reciprocal connections mainly with visual cor-
gether with a substantial input from limbic cortices. The tices and the retrosplenial cortex. As mentioned above the
cytoarchitectonically more highly differentiated (thus eu- caudal part of the dorsal shoulder region appears to incor-
laminate or strongly granular) prefrontal areas receive more porate features of the frontal eye field.
specific projections representing only one or two different It is of interest to note that Fuster [44,45] views the in-
modalities, and relatively few projections from limbic cor- teractions of prefrontal with other cortices in the context
tices. In primates, in particular the macaque, the dorsolateral of ‘perception–action cycle’. This is a hierarchical concept
prefrontal cortex has extensive cortico-cortical connections. too. The prefrontal cortex and other cortices are functionally
The rat data currently available do not contradict this connected for as long as the behavior contains novelty, un-
“hierarchical” theory of Pandya and Barbas, but more in- certainty or ambiguity, and has to span time intervals with
formation is required before definitive conclusions can be short-term or working memory. These functional connec-
drawn. As mentioned above the retrograde tracer study on tions disappear or weaken when the action becomes auto-
cortico-cortical afferents of Condé et al. [29] has strength- matic. The action is then integrated in lower brain structures
ened the view of Condé et al. and Preuss [111] that the rat [45].
Fr2 and ACd is largely a premotor cortex and do not belong
to the prefrontal cortex. By excluding this ‘dorsal shoulder’ 3.4. The prefrontal gating position in cholinergic and
region from the rat prefrontal cortex, they arrived to their monoaminergic systems
opinion, that the prefrontal cortex in rats lack features of the
primate dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [29,111]. Condé et al. Data on the cholinergic and monoaminergic transmitter
[29] concluded that Fr2 and ACd have the main features systems together with prefrontal cortico-cortical connections
of a premotor cortex and “none of the features of any area in primates and rats also show the unique gating position of
of the macaque prefrontal cortex”. A typical feature of the the prefrontal cortex.
macaque prefrontal cortical areas is, as noted above, how- In both rats and primates the entire neocortex receives
ever, the property of a multimodal association area in a hi- cholinergic innervation from the basal forebrain nuclei. The
erarchical organized cortex [108] and the feature of a nodal prefrontal cortex, in addition, gets cholinergic fibers from
station in several distributed parallel networks [26,52,113]. the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus. Likewise in both species,
Like the macaque prefrontal cortex, the rat prefrontal cortex the prefrontal cortex (in primates mainly the orbital and
receives multimodal cortico-cortical projections from motor, medial PFC and in rats mainly the medial PFC) is the only
somatosensory, visual, auditory, gustatory, and limbic cor- cortical region that has direct projections back to these basal
tices in such a way that the rat prefrontal cortex appears to be forebrain and brainstem nuclei [48,49,57,127,138,156].
a nodal station embedded in several parallel networks [142]. The noradrenergic fibers from the locus coeruleus and the
Moreover, Van Eden et al. [142] have shown (with antero- serotonergic fibers from the dorsal and median raphe nuclei
grade labeling) that Fr2 and ACd receive more projections are widely distributed over almost the entire neocortex. Also
from somatosensory and associational visual cortices than with respect to these transmitter systems in both rats and
from the primary motor cortex. The macaque prefrontal cor- primates certain prefrontal areas are the only cortical areas
tex has the strongest connections with posterior parietal and that project back to the locus coeruleus and to the dorsal and
temporal association areas [26,27]. Rat equivalents are not median raphe nuclei. In primates the cortical projections to
well described for these parietal and temporal association the noradrenergic locus coeruleus and the serotonergic raphe
areas [142], which makes a good comparison not yet possi- nuclei derive from the dorsolateral PFC [6]. In rats the corti-
ble. The data available indicate extensive, reciprocal connec- cal projections to the locus coeruleus derive from the medial
tions with premotor, motor, somatosensory, visual, auditory PFC and the agranular insular PFC areas, while the cortical
H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17 9

Fig. 3. A diagram of cortico-cortical connections of the rat prefrontal cortex (modified from [138]). For abbreviations see the Abbreviations section.

projections to the dorsal and median raphe nuclei are from across species must be function. One of the major obsta-
the medial PFC, especially its ventral part [60,63,64,138]. cles in comparing the behavior of different species of mam-
In rats, the cortical terminal fields of dopaminergic fibers mals is that each species has a unique behavioral repertoire
are mainly restricted to the prefrontal areas and the entorhi- that permits the animal to survive in its particular environ-
nal cortex, whereas only the prefrontal cortex (both medial mental niche. There is therefore the danger that neocortical
and agranular insular PFC) projects back to the dopamin- organization is uniquely patterned in different species in a
ergic neurons in ventral tegmental area (VTA or A10) [25] way that reflects the unique behavioral adaptation of differ-
and the dopaminergic pars compacta of the substantia ni- ent species. One way to address this problem is to recognize
gra (A9). The rat prefrontal cortex receives distinct paral- that although the details of behavior may differ somewhat,
lel dopaminergic inputs. The supragenual anterior cingulate mammals share many behavioral traits and capacities (e.g.
cortex receives dopaminergic fibers in the superficial layers [81]). For example, all mammals must detect and interpret
from the pars compacta of the substantia nigra and in the sensory stimuli, relate this information to past experience,
deeper layers from the lateral part of the ventral tegmen- and act appropriately. Similarly, all mammals appear to be
tal area [89,91]. The ventral tegmental area is the origin capable of learning complex tasks under various schedules
of the major dopaminergic input in the rat prefrontal cor- of reinforcement (e.g. [146]). The details and complexity of
tex and different parts of the ventral tegmental area appear these behaviors clearly vary, but the general capacities are
to project to different prefrontal areas [69]. Particularly in common to all mammals. Warren and Kolb [146] proposed
primates, cortical dopaminergic fibers appear not to be re- that behaviors and behavioral capacities demonstrable in
stricted to the prefrontal cortex. As suggested [47,138] this all mammals could be designated as class-common behav-
appears to be caused mainly by extension of dopaminergic iors. In contrast, behaviors that are unique to a species and
midbrain cellular groups such as the retrorubral areas A8 that have presumably been selected to promote survival in a
and A9 [152]. Direct recurrent projections from the primate particular niche are designated as species-typical behaviors.
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to the medial substantia nigra This distinction is important because it has implications for
pars compacta (A9) have been reported [89], but they can the organization of the cerebral cortex.
also be expected to project to the ventral tegmental area on Kaas [68] has argued, for example, that all mammalian
the basis of the above. species have similar regions devoted to the analysis of ba-
In rats the histaminergic neurons in the tuberomammillary sic sensory information (e.g. primary visual (V1), primary
hypothalamic region have reciprocal connections with ven- auditory (A1), and primary somatosensory (S1) areas), the
tromedial prefrontal cortex [154]. These connections have control of movement (primary motor, M1), and a frontal re-
not yet been described for primates. gion involved in the integration of sensory and motor infor-
mation. We can extend Kaas’s idea by suggesting that these
regions have class-common functions. To be sure, there are
4. Functional characteristics of prefrontal areas large species differences in the details of the class-common
behaviors. Monkeys (and humans) have chromatic vision
The function of any brain region is to produce behavior. compared to the largely achromatic vision of cats or rats.
Thus, a key issue in identifying similarities in cortical areas Nevertheless, in all mammalian species studied, removal of
10 H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

visual cortex severely disrupts object recognition. Indeed, information or to internal states. Thus, there must be some
although the visual cortex of the rat has often been portrayed type of master (sometimes referred to as executive) control
as primitive in organization, the visual acuity of rats is sur- system that selects behavior. It is our contention that the
prisingly good and the tuning characteristics of visual neu- class-common function of the prefrontal cortex is to select
rons is strikingly similar to that of larger-brained mammals. and generate behavior patterns. In addition, it is proposed
Similarly, rats and cats have a large somatosensory represen- that this system has a working memory subsystem but that
tation of the whiskers, whereas monkeys and humans have it uses a long-term memory store that is largely a func-
no such representation but in all species the somatosensory tion of the medial temporal regions. Although this general
cortex functions to represent skin-related receptors for tac- view of prefrontal functioning is hardly novel (see reviews
tile sensations. Thus, both the visual and tactile recognition [44,52,78,109]), it is the idea that a prefrontal system with
of objects are class-common functions, even though the de- such functions will be found in all mammals that is the key
tails of this recognition may vary in a species-typical man- concept in the current discussion.
ner. A similar argument can be made for motor functions One prediction from our general hypothesis is that all
[147]. Intracortical stimulation studies have shown that all mammals with damage to the prefrontal homologue should
mammals have a motor map (e.g. [153]) in which the relative show functional disruptions of the same general sort. The
motor facility of different body regions is reflected by the prefrontal cortex is extensive in most mammals, however, so
size of the motor representation. Curiously, although there we might also anticipate that there will be some subdivision
are clear interspecies differences in the capacity to use the of functions across the prefrontal regions in different species
forelimbs for object manipulation [61], it has become appar- [138]. The organization of the subdivisions is likely to have
ent from the work of Whishaw et al. [148,149] that the ca- some species-typical characteristics, however. Consider, for
pacity for independent digit manipulation, and the cerebral example, the relative importance of vision to the control of
organization of this control, is strikingly similar between forelimb movement in primates versus a parallel olfactory
rodents and primates. Indeed, recent taxonomic studies of control of forelimb movement in the rat (e.g. [148]). Such
mammalian evolution have noted that rodents and primates differences would be expected to be reflected in differences
are more closely related than had been realized previously. in the subtle organization of the prefrontal subregions. We
For example, they are more related than cats and primates shall review first the evidence for general symptoms of
are [112,138]. This similarity is especially germane to com- prefrontal injury in the major subdivisions of primates and
parisons of frontal lobe function between primates and ro- rodents, and then consider the issue of how to compare
dents, given that one primary function of the frontal lobe is functions of the prefrontal subregions across the two orders.
to control the initiation and organization of movement.
As we look for general class-common functions of the 4.1. Effects of frontal lesions in primates
frontal cortex we might anticipate that if the frontal cortex
of mammals developed because all mammals face common As noted earlier, it is possible to distinguish function-
functional problems related to the organization of behav- ally three distinctly different frontal lobe regions in humans.
ior, then we should be able to identify these functions. One Thus, in addition to the motor and oculomotor areas, there
place to begin searching for class-common frontal functions are the dorsolateral frontal region, the lateral orbitofrontal
is to consider what animals use sensory inputs for. The most region, and an anterior cingulate/medial orbitofrontal region.
obvious function is to guide behavior on line, such as in Damage to the dorsolateral frontal region is characterized
the visuomotor control of movements in space or the iden- especially by deficits in working memory, particularly as it
tification of food items using visual, tactile, and olfactory relates to certain executive processes such as the monitor-
information. But the sensory world has far more informa- ing and planning of behaviors (e.g. [44,129]). Damage to
tion available than the brain can handle at one time so there the orbitofrontal frontal region is characterized by altered
must be some system to select information as well as to socio-emotional behaviors, hyperkinesis, deficits in the pro-
focus and maintain attention. Similarly, although behavior cessing of olfactory and gustatory information, and in spon-
can be directed to sensory stimuli on-line, it can also be taneity (e.g. [22,83,84]). Damage to the anterior cingulate
related to information that is stored or expected. Stored in- region is not as well characterized, but can include reduced
formation may be in a type of scratch-pad memory system, response to pain, akinetic mutism, and impaired motor ini-
which is often referred to as working memory and implies tiation (e.g. [36]). In addition, all three areas probably pro-
a short-term erasable storage of information, or by a type of duce some form of attentional deficit but distinctions in the
long-term memory system in which information is stored for attentional domain remain unclear and are likely related to
an extended time. In both instances the stored information the type of sensory information being processed. Thus, at-
is used to select and generate behavior that is appropriate tentional processes related to olfactory and gustatory inputs
for the particular context. The behaviors that are generated are likely related to orbitofrontal regions, those related to
may be novel, and directly related to the sensory events, or visual inputs are likely related to dorsolateral regions, and
they may be preprogrammed in chains that are innate but those related to internal states (such as pain) are related to
still must be selected with respect either to ongoing sensory anterior cingulate regions.
H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17 11

In monkeys the symptoms of lesions to the dorsolateral recently, deficits have been shown in various types of atten-
prefrontal, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate regions re- tional tasks [102] and in a task requiring a reversal or a shift
spectively produce analogous behavioral deficits to those of attention from one set of cues to another [16,33,41,82].
observed in humans. In particular, dorsolateral lesions espe- Medial frontal lesions also produce disruptions to the pro-
cially produce deficits in working memory, whereas lesions duction of various motor and species-typical behaviors that
that include the frontal eye fields (area 8) produce attentional require the ordering of motor sequences, such as in nest
deficits (e.g. [44]). Orbitofrontal lesions produce a disinhi- building, food hoarding, or latch opening [74,75,80,134].
bition of motor behaviors, abnormalities in social-affective Although these types of experiments were viewed by
behaviors, deficits in certain types of olfactory and gusta- many as convincing evidence of parallel (and perhaps ho-
tory processing, and in the association between reward and mologous) functions in rodents and primates, Preuss [111]
external cues (e.g. [125]). Cingulate lesions lead to deficits remained unconvinced. Indeed, he has argued that in view
in the encoding of movements (including eye movements), of what he believed to be significant anatomical differences,
and reduced response to pain. Although an early study and the failure to find prolonged or long-lasting deficits after
suggested that cingulate lesions produce deficits in delayed mPFC lesions in rodents that are equivalent to those observed
alternation, this is only seen in acquisition and not in reten- in primates with dorsolateral lesions, that the research on the
tion, which is rather different than the effect of dorsolateral mPFC of the rat has little to offer to those interested in un-
lesions (e.g. [115]). derstanding frontal lobe functioning in primates. Preuss was
wrong on his conclusion that rats with medial frontal do not
4.2. Effects of frontal lesions in rats have significant memory deficits (e.g. [85,86]), but the fact
that most studies of medial frontal function had made lesions
It was demonstrated in the early 1970s that lesions to the including all of the medial prefrontal subregions did pro-
medial and lateral orbital regions in rats produced very dif- vide grist for his skepticism. Accordingly, in the past decade
ferent behavioral syndromes, and that these changes were there has been considerable interest in dissociating the dif-
strikingly similar to those observed in primates with lesions ferent medial prefrontal subregions in the rat. It has now
to the dorsolateral and orbitofrontal regions, respectively become clear that the dorsal anterior cingulate region, and
(Table 1; for reviews see [78,79]). For example, damage to prelimbic/infralimbic region can be functionally dissociated
the medial prefrontal (mPFC) area produces severe deficits [51,60a]. In general, it appears that the PL region is involved
in acquisition and retention of working memory tasks such in attentional and response selection functions as well as vi-
as delayed response [86], delayed alternation [38,86,151], sual working memory (e.g. [53]) whereas the more dorsal
different types of delayed nonmatching-to-sample tasks regions (ACd) are involved with generating rules associated
(e.g. [19,40,85,107]) and related tasks (e.g. [71,118]). More with temporal ordering and motor sequencing of behavior
(see reviews [51,70]. Indeed, on the basis of such studies,
Table 1 Kesner [70] has gone so far as to suggest that the anterior
Summary of the effects of mPFC and OFC lesions in rats cingulate region is homologous to Brodmann’s areas 6/46
whereas the PL/IL regions are homologous with Brodmann’s
Behavioral impairment References
areas 45 and 47. Additionally, although less is known about
mPFC its precise role in behavior, it appears that the IL region
Visual working memory [86]
plays a special role in autonomic control, and especially in
Strategy formation [85]
Spatial reversal [38,86] the modulation of fear-related behaviors (e.g. [101,116]).
Habituation [74,76] Kesner’s hypothesis will be a difficult one to unequivo-
Skilled reaching [149] cally prove or disprove, but it is not necessary for the current
Motor sequencing [80] argument, which is simply that the medial frontal regions
Attention [18,102]
have class-common functions that are similar to those of the
Attention set shift [16]
Food hoarding [75] dorsolateral and possibly cingulate regions in the monkey
Fear extinction [116] frontal lobe. We suggest that these class-common functions
Conditioned emotional responses [42,43] include functions that are often referred to as executive func-
Operant reversal learning [33,41] tions in primates. These functions would include working
Conflict behavior [20]
memory, the selection of information (often referred to as
Operant working memory [19]
attention), and the shifting of attention from one stimulus
OFC attribute to another (e.g. [21]).
Hyperactivity [74]
Social behavior [32,77]
There is much more parsimony in reviews comparing the
Incentive association [46] effects of lateral orbitofrontal lesions in rodents and pri-
Odor working memory [107] mates [130]. The lateral orbital region receives significant
Configural odor learning [150] olfactory and taste input and although lateral orbital lesions
Odor reversal learning [128,131] do not produce deficits in olfactory or taste discriminations,
Feeding [75]
they do produce deficits in tasks requiring working memory
12 H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

for odor or taste information (e.g. [4,34,107,117,120]). Fur- reviewed above, it is likely that these latter areas are also
thermore, lesions to the lateral orbital region disrupt the subdivided. If this is so, then it is likely that the primi-
learning of cross-modal associations that involve odor or tive mammals that gave rise to extant mammals had a more
taste cues (e.g. [150]). More recently, studies by Schoen- or less subdivided prefrontal cortex that evolved to solve
baum and his colleagues (e.g. [46,131]) have emphasized class-common behavioral problems.
a role of the orbital cortex in the encoding of the acquired
incentive value of cues. For example, both rats and primates
can show intact performance on discriminations that require 5. Conclusions
responding to neutral cues (such as a light) that predicts re-
ward, while at the same time showing marked deficits when The present anatomical and functional data indicate that
the incentive value of the stimulus is reduced. Such deficits rats have a prefrontal cortex, in which Fr2 and ACd are in-
can be seen during extinction when the incentive value of corporated. Preuss and Kaas [114] have stated that evidence
a stimulus is reduced to zero, yet animals continue to re- available to them at that time “is consistent with the pos-
spond to the cue as though reward is expected (e.g. [13,46]). sibility that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is a primate
The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in stimulus-reward as- specialization”. Certainly, the rat prefrontal cortex is not as
sociations is further seen in studies measuring the tuning differentiated as it is in primates and evolutionary later spe-
characteristics of neurons in the orbital region of both rats cializations are likely, but dorsolateral-like features, includ-
and monkeys [130]. Finally, damage to the orbitofrontal ing both anatomical and functional ones, are shown in rats.
cortex produces deficits in social and play behavior in rats They are present in areas Fr2 and ACd, and functional data
(e.g. [32,77]). indicate that PL is implicated in some dorsolateral-like fea-
The overall pattern of deficits related to orbitofrontal tures. These areas have also features of other type of cortices,
(OFC) lesions leads to a general conclusion that there is a e.g. premotor and anterior cingulate cortex [138]. However,
class-common function related to making higher order use it holds also for primates that the closer prefrontal cortex
of olfactory and taste information. This can be seen easily areas are to the premotor cortex the more premotor features
in tests that require the association of such information with they have.
events in the world, whether they are learned associations We focus in this review on rats, but Kosmal’s group from
such as neutral cues and reward or natural stimuli (such as Warsaw has indicated, that anatomical dorsolateral-like pre-
conspecific odors) or rewards that may be more abstract frontal characteristics are present in dogs as well [92,119,
(such as social bonding). Although odors obviously play a 135,136]. This too would be consistent with the conclusion
reduced role in the control of social behaviors in humans, that the dorsolateral prefrontal regions of primates are not a
the neural networks underlying many social functions re- unique specialization.
mains related to the orbitofrontal cortex.
One region of the rat frontal cortex that has not been
subject to many lesion studies is the more medial and ventral 6. Note on prefrontal nomenclature
orbital area (but see [30]). We noted earlier that this region
has ambiguous anatomical characteristics so at present we A stable and unequivocal nomenclature is desired [110],
must see this region as a somewhat of an enigma. although neuroanatomical nomenclature should remain
Taken together, the lesion studies of the prefrontal re- flexible in order to incorporate new insights [137]. Unfortu-
gions in primates and rodents lead us to conclude that there nately, a generally accepted nomenclature for the prefrontal
is a strong convergence of class-common symptoms across cortical areas in rats is still lacking. When a literature search
mammals. Furthermore, it seems likely that most mam- based on key words is executed, this has to be taken into
mals have a gross subdivision of the frontal areas into an account. For the rat prefrontal cortex we prefer the most
“orbital-like” region involved especially with the control commonly used nomenclature of Krettek and Price [87]. In
of socioaffective behaviors, a “dorsolateral-like” area in- a developmental study, Van Eden and Uylings [141] adopted
volved especially in working memory, and an “anterior their nomenclature with the exception of the term medial
cingulate-like” area concerned primarily with visceromotor precentral area (PrCm) [138]. We use the neutral term Fr2
behaviors and some forms of motor sequencing. To be sure, introduced by Zilles [157] instead. We prefer to use the term
there are likely to be significant cross species differences Fr2 rather than PrCm [87], since rats do not have a central
in details of the anatomical and functional organization of sulcus. Likewise, the term medial agranular area (AGm;
these areas. The critical point for this paper is that both pri- [39]) is not satisfactory, because more cortical areas in the
mates and rodents, and probably all mammals, have a region medial frontal cortex are agranular and the area lateral to
of frontal cortex that can be defined both anatomically and AGm, called lateral agranular area (AGl) is not agranular.
functionally as prefrontal cortex. Further, it is likely that the Unfortunately, the nomenclature of Krettek and Price [87]
prefrontal cortex is subdivided into at least an “orbital-like” for the rat prefrontal areas is not applied by Paxinos and Wat-
and another region that may include both “dorsolateral- and son [110]. In this atlas no distinction has been made between
anterior cingulate-like” features. Finally, given the anatomy the infralimbic (IL) and the medial orbital (MO) areas: both
H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17 13

areas are included in MO of Paxinos and Watson [110]. In its structure, function and pathology. Progress in brain research,
addition, they have called the dorsal and ventral anterior cin- vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1990. p. 119–46.
[3] Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick P. Parallel organization of func-
gulate areas (ACd and ACv) the cingulate cortices 1 (CG1), tionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Ann
and 2 (CG2), respectively. We prefer the AC areas, because Rev Neurosci 1986;9:357–81.
these areas have “anterior cingulate” features, which are dif- [4] Alvarez P, Eichenbaum H. Representations of odors in the rat or-
ferent from other cingulate areas, see our review above. The bitofrontal cortex change during and after learning. Behav Neurosci
Fr2 area is indicated by Paxinos and Watson [110] as the 2002;116:421–33.
[5] Amaral DG, Price JL. Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey
secondary motor area (M2). Interestingly, with the sole ex- (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Neurol 1984;230:465–96.
ception of the Fr2 area, Swanson [137] in his atlas adopted [6] Arnsten AFT. Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortex. J
the nomenclature of Krettek and Price [87]. In this atlas Psychopharmacol 1997;11:151–62.
Fr2 is indicated instead with the term secondary motor area [7] Barbas H. Architecture and cortical connections of the prefrontal
(“MOs”). From our review above, it is evident why we sug- cortex in the rhesus monkey. Adv Neurol 1992;57:91–115.
[8] Barbas H. Anatomic basis of cognitive–emotional interactions in the
gest to avoid the term secondary motor area (MOs and M2,
primate prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehavior Rev 1995;19:499–
respectively). 510.
Please note that besides differences in nomenclature there [9] Barbas H, Blatt GJ. Topographically specific hippocampal projec-
are also discrepancies in the delineations of the pertinent tions target functionally distinct prefrontal areas in the rhesus mon-
areas: identical names not necessarily imply a similar delin- key. Hippocampus 1995;5:511–33.
[10] Barbas H, Pandya DN. Architecture and intrinsic connections
eation by different authors. Our cytoarchitectonic definition
of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol
of borders of prefrontal areas [138,141] was described in 1989;286:353–75.
such a way that they appear reproducible for other PFC re- [11] Barbas H, Henion TH, Dermon CR. Diverse thalamic projections
searchers. This delineation of the prefrontal cortex is more to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol
or less comparable with the one of Price and coworkers 1991;313:65–94.
[12] Barbas H, Ghasghaei HT, Rempel-Clower NL, Xiao D. Anatomic
[87,121]. More specifically, our definition of Fr2 [138,141]
basis of functional specialization in prefrontal cortices in primates.
is comparable with the delineation of PrCm by Price and In: Handbook of neuropsychology, vol. 7. Amsterdam: Elsevier;
colleagues. However, the caudal border of ‘our’ Fr2 or 2002. p. 1–27.
PrCm differs strongly from the Fr2 defined in Zilles [157], [13] Baxter MG, Parker A, Lindner CCC, Izquierdo AD, Murray EA.
M2 in Paxinos and Watson [110] and MOs in Swanson Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction
of amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2000;20:4311–9.
[137]. The caudal border of this area defined by Krettek and
[14] Benton AL. The prefrontal region: its early history. In: Levin HS,
Price [87] and Van Eden and Uylings [141] is much more Eisenberg HM, Benton AL, editors. Frontal lobe function and dys-
rostral (i.e. at about −1 mm from bregma) than that of the function. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. p. 3–32.
comparable areas in the atlases of Zilles [157], Swanson [15] Berendse HW, Groenewegen HJ. Restricted cortical terminal fields
[137] and Paxinos and Watson [110] at about −3.7 mm of the midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei in the rat. Neuro-
science 1991;42:73–102.
from bregma). Our caudal border is not only based on cy-
[16] Birrel JM, Brown VJ. Medial frontal cortex mediates perceputal
toarchitectonics, but also on the patterns of thalamic and attentional set shifting in the rat. J Neurosci 2000;20:4320–4.
other connections mentioned above. There are further dif- [17] Brodmann K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnhinde.
ferences in the delineation of the prefrontal cortical areas in Barth: Leipzig. Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde. Leipzig:
the atlases of Swanson [137] and Paxinos and Watson [110], Barth-Verlag; 1909. p. 324.
[18] Broersen LM, Uylings HBM. Visual attention task performance
but it is outside the scope of this paper to discuss these in
in Wistar and Lister hooded rats: response inhibition deficits after
detail. medial prefrontal cortex lesions. Neuroscience 1999;94:47–57.
[19] Broersen LM, Heinsbroek RPW, de Bruin JPC, Uylings HBM,
Olivier B. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex of rats in
Acknowledgements short-term memory functioning: further support for involvement
of cholinergic, rather than dopaminergic mechanisms. Brain Res
1995a;674:221–9.
We thank Ms. W.T.P. Verweij for her secretarial assistance [20] Broersen LM, Heinsbroek RPW, de Bruin JPC, Laan JB, Joosten
and Mr. H. Stoffels for drawing Figs. 1 and 3. RNJMA, Olivier B. Local pharmacological manipulations of pre-
frontal dopamine affect conflict behaviour in rats. Behav Pharmacol
1995b;6:395–404.
[21] Brown VJ, Bowman EM. Rodent models of prefrontal cortical
References function. Trends Neurosci 2002;25:340–3.
[22] Butter CM, Snyder DR. Alterations in aversive and aggressive
[1] Akert K, Hartmann-Von Monakov K. Relationships of precentral, behaviors following orbital frontal lesions in rhesus monkeys. Acta
premotor, and prefrontal cortex to the mediodorsal and intralami- Neurobiol Exp 1972;32:115–56.
nar nuclei of the monkey thalamus. Acta Neurol Exp 1980;40:7– [23] Carmichael ST, Price JL. Architectonic subdivision of the orbital
25. and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp
[2] Alexander GE, Crutcher MD, DeLong MR. Basal ganglia– Neurol 1994;346:366–402.
thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, [24] Carmichael ST, Price JL. Limbic connections of the orbital and
‘prefrontal’ and ‘limbic’ functions. In: Uylings HBM, Van Eden medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol
CG, De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: 1995;363:615–41.
14 H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

[25] Carr DB, Sesack SR. Projections from the rat prefrontal cortex to [44] Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex: anatomy, physiology, and neu-
the ventral tegmental area: target specificity in the synaptic associ- ropsychology of the frontal lobe, 3rd ed. New York: Raven Press;
ations with mesoaccumbens and mesocortical neurons. J Neurosci 1997. p. 333.
2000;20:3864–73. [45] Fuster JM. Linkage at the top. Neuron 1998;21:1223–4.
[26] Cavada C, Goldman-Rakic PS. Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus [46] Gallagher M, McMahan RW, Schoenbaum G. Orbitofrontal cortex
monkey. II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking and representations of incentive value in associative learning. J
sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe. J Comp Neurol Neurosci 1999;19:6610–4.
1989;287:422–45. [47] Gaspar P, Stepniewska I, Kaas JH. Topography and collateralization
[27] Cavada C, Company T, Tejedor J, Cruz-Rizzolo RJ, Reinoso-Suárez of the dopaminergic projections to motor and lateral prefrontal
F. The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex in owl monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1992;325:1–21.
cortex. A review. Cereb Cortex 2000;10:220–42. [48] Gaykema RPA, Van Weeghel R, Hersh LB, Luiten PGM. Prefrontal
[28] Condé F, Audinat E, Maire-Lepoivre E, Crépel F. Afferent con- cortical projections to the cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain.
nections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. A study using ret- J Comp Neurol 1991;303:563–83.
rograde transport of fluorescent dyes. I. Thalamic Afferents. Brain [49] Ghashghaei HT, Barbas H. Neural interaction between the basal
Res Bull 1990;24:341–54. forebrain and functionally distinct prefrontal cortices in the rhesus
[29] Condé F, Maire-Lepoivre E, Audinat E, Crépel F. Afferent con- monkey. Neuroscience 2001;103:593–614.
nections of the medial frontal cortex of the rat. II. Cortical and [50] Giguere M, Goldman-Rakic PS. Mediodorsal nucleus areal, lam-
subcortical afferent. J Comp Neurol 1995;352:567–93. inar, and tangential distribution of afferents and efferents in the
[30] Corwin JV, Fussinger M, Meyer RC, King VR, Reep RL. Bilat- frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1988;277:195–
eral destruction of the ventrolateral orbital cortex produces allo- 213.
centric but not egocentric spatial deficits in rats. Behav Brain Res [51] Gisquet-Verrier P, Winocur G, Delatour B. Functional dissociation
1994;61:79–86. between dorsal and ventral regions of the medial prefrontal cortex
[31] Damasio AR. Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human in rats. Psychobiology 2000;28:248–60.
brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam; 1994. p. 312. [52] Goldman-Rakic PS. Circuitry of the primate prefrontal cortex and
[32] De Bruin JPC. Social behavior and the prefrontal cortex. In: Uylings regulation of behavior by representational memory. In: Plum F, ed-
HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MPG, editors. The itor. Handbook of physiology: the nervous system. Vol. 5. Higher
prefrontal cortex: its structure, function and pathology. Progress functions of the brain, Part 1. Bethesda, MD: American Physiolog-
in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1990. p. 485– ical Society; 1987. p. 373–417.
96. [53] Granon S, Poucet B. Involvement of the rat prefrontal cortex in
[33] De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MPG, Broersen LM, Van Leeuwen M, cognitive functions: a central role for the prelimbic area. Psychobi-
Arens C, De Vries S, et al. Role of the prefrontal cortex of the rat in ology 2000;28:229–37.
learning and decision making: effects of transient inactivation. In: [54] Groenewegen HJ. Organization of the afferent connections of the
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Feenstra MGP, Pen- mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to mediodorsal–
nartz CMA, editors. Cognition, emotion and autonomic responses: prefrontal topography. Neuroscience 1988;24:379–431.
the integrative role of the prefrontal cortex and limbic structures. [55] Groenewegen HJ, Berendse HW. The specificity of the non-specific
Progress in brain research, vol. 126. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2000. midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Trends Neurosci
p. 103–13. 1994;17:52–7.
[34] DeCoteau WE, Kesner RP, Williams JM. Short-term memory for [56] Groenewegen HJ, Witter MP. Thalamus. In: Paxinos G, editor. The
food reward magnitude: the role of the prefrontal cortex. Behav rat nervous system, 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2004.
Brain Res 1997;88:239–49. p. 407–453.
[35] Dermon CR, Barbas H. Contralateral thalamic projections predom- [57] Groenewegen HJ, Wright CI, Uylings HBM. The anatomical re-
inantly reach transitional cortices in the rhesus monkey. J Comp lationships of the prefrontal cortex with limbic structures and the
Neurol 1994;344:508–31. basal ganglia. J Psychopharmacol 1997;11:99–106.
[36] Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cin- [58] Groenewegen HJ, Berendse HW, Wolters JG, Lohman AHM. The
gulate cortex to behavior. Brain 1995;118:279–306. anatomical relationship of the prefrontal cortex with the striatopal-
[37] Dinopoulos A, Karamanlidis AN, Papadopoulos G, Antonopoulos lidal system, the thalamus and the amygdala: evidence for a paral-
J, Michaloudi H. Thalamic projections to motor, prefrontal and so- lel organization. In: Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC,
matosensory cortex in the sheep studied by means of the horseradish Feenstra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure, func-
peroxidase transport method. J Comp Neurol 1985;241:63–81. tion and pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam:
[38] Divac I. Frontal lobe system and spatial reversal in the rat. Neu- Elsevier; 1990. p. 95–118.
ropsychologia 1971;9:171–83. [59] Guandalini P. The efferent connections to the thalamus and brain-
[39] Donoghue JP, Wise SP. The motor cortex of the rat: cytoarchitec- stem of the physiologically defined eye field in the rat medial frontal
ture and microstimulation mapping. J Comp Neurol 1982;212:76– cortex. Brain Res Bull 2001;54:175–86.
88. [60] Hajos M, Richards CD, Szekely AD, Sharp T. An electrophysio-
[40] Dunnett SB. Role of the prefrontal cortex and striatal output logical and neuroanatomical study of the medial prefrontal cortical
systems in short-term memory deficits associated with ageing, projection to the midbrain raphe nuclei in the rat. Neuroscience
basal forebrain lesions, and cholinergic-rich grafts. Can J Psychol 1998;87:95–108.
1990;44:210–32. [60a] Heidbreder CA, Groenewegen HJ. The medical prefrontal cortex
[41] Feenstra MGP, De Bruin JPC. Strategy switching and the rat pre- in the rat: evidence for a dorso-ventral distinction based upon
frontal cortex. In: Otani S, editor. Prefrontal cortex: from synaptic functional and anatomical characteristics. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
plasticity to cognition. Dordrecht: Wolters-Kluwer; 2003, in press. 2003, in press.
[42] Frysztak RJ, Neafsey EJ. The effect of medial frontal cortex lesions [61] Iwaniuk A, Whishaw IQ. On the origin of skilled forelimb move-
on cardiovascular conditioned emotional responses in the rat. Brain ments. Trends Neurosci 2000;23:372–6.
Res 1994;643:181–93. [62] Jay T, Witter MP. Distribution of hippocampal CA1 and subicular
[43] Frysztak RJ, Neafsey EJ. The effect of medial frontal cortex lesions efferents in the prefrontal cortex of the rat studied by means of
on respiration, ‘freezing’, and ultrasonic vocalizations during con- anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J Comp
ditioned emotional responses in rat. Cereb Cortex 1991;1:418–25. Neurol 1991;313:574–86.
H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17 15

[63] Jodo E, Aston-Jones G. Activation of locus coeruleus by prefrontal [86] Kolb B, Nonneman AJ, Singh R. Double dissociation of spatial
cortex is mediated by excitatory amino acid inputs. Brain Res impairment and perseveration following selective prefrontal lesions
1997;768:327–32. in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974;87:772–80.
[64] Jodo E, Chiang C, Aston-Jones G. Potent excitatory influence of [87] Krettek JE, Price JL. The cortical projections of the mediodorsal
prefrontal cortex on noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons. Neuro- nucleus and adjacent thalamic nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol
science 1998;83:63–79. 1977;171:157–91.
[65] Jones EG. Thalamus. New York: Plenum Press; 1985. p. 935. [88] Leonard CM. The prefrontal cortex of the rat. I. Cortical projection
[66] Jones EG. A description of the human thalamus. In: Steriade M, of the mediodorsal nucleus. II. Efferent connections. Brain Res
Jones EG, McCormick DA, editors. Thalamus, vol. 2. Amsterdam: 1969;12:321–43.
Elsevier; 1997. p. 425–99. [89] Lewis DA, Sesack SR. Dopamine systems in the primate brain. In:
[67] Jones EG, Leavitt RY. Retrograde axonal transport and the demon- Björklund A, Hökfelt T, Bloom FE, editors. Handbook of chem-
stration of non-specific projections to the cerebral cortex and stria- ical neuroanatomy, Vol. 13. The primate nervous system, Part I.
tum from thalamic intralaminar nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1997. p. 363–75.
1974;154:349–78. [90] Lopez-Bendito G, Molnár Z. Thalamocortical development: how
[68] Kaas JH. The organization of neocortex in mammals: implications are we going to get there? Nat Rev Neurosci 2003;4:276–89.
for theories of brain function. Ann Rev Psychol 1987;38:129–51. [91] Loughlin SE, Fallon JH. Substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area
[69] Kalsbeek A, Buijs RM, Hofman MA, Matthijssen MAH, Pool CW, projection to cortex: topography and collateralization. Neuroscience
Uylings HBM. Effects of neonatal thermal lesioning of mesocortical 1984;11:425–35.
dopaminergic projection on the development of the rat prefrontal [92] Markow-Rajkowska G, Kosmal A. Organization of cortical affer-
cortex. Dev Brain Res 1987;32:123–32. ents to the frontal association cortex in dogs. Acta Neurobiol Exp
[70] Kesner RP. Subregional analysis of mnemonic functions of the 1987;47:137–61.
prefrontal cortex in the rat. Psychobiology 2000;28:219–28. [93] McDonald AJ. Organization of amygdaloid projections to the pre-
[71] Kesner RP, Holbrook T. Dissociation of item and order spatial frontal cortex and associated striatum in the rat. Neuroscience
memory in rats following medial prefrontal cortex lesions. Neu- 1991;44:1–14.
ropsychologia 1987;25:653–64. [94] McFarland NR, Haber SN. Thalamic relay nuclei of the basal
[72] Kievit J, Kuypers HGJM. Organization of the thalamo-cortical con- ganglia form both reciprocal and nonreciprocal cortical connections,
nexions to the frontal lobe in the rhesus monkey. Exp Brain Res linking multiple frontal cortical areas. J Neurosci 2002;22:8117–32.
1977;29:299–322. [95] Mesulam M-M. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and
[73] King V, Corwin JV. Neglect following unilateral ablation of the cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional
caudal but not the rostral portion of medial agranular cortex of the targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond
rat and the therapeutic effect of apomorphine. Behav Brain Res B: Biol Sci 1999;354:1325–46.
1990;37:169–84. [96] Middleton FA, Strick PL. The temporal lobe is a target of output
[74] Kolb B. Dissociation of the effects of lesions of the orbital or from the basal ganglia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996;93:8683–7.
medial aspect of the prefrontal cortex of the rat with respect to [97] Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal-ganglia ‘projections’ to the pre-
activity. Behav Biol 1974a;10:329–43. frontal cortex of the primate. Cereb Cortex 2000;12:926–35.
[75] Kolb B. Prefrontal lesions alter eating and hoarding behavior in [98] Middleton FA, Strick PL. A revised neuroanatomy of frontal–
rats. Physiol Behav 1974b;12:507–11. subcortical circuits. In: Lichter DG, Cummings JL, editors. Frontal–
[76] Kolb B. Some tests of response habituation in rats with prefrontal subcortical circuits in psychiatric and neurological disorders. New
lesions. Can J Psychol 1974c;28:260–7. York: Guildford Press; 2001. p. 44–58.
[77] Kolb B. The social behavior of rats with chronic prefrontal lesions. [99] Miller EK, Wallis JD. The prefrontal cortex and executive brain
J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974d;87:466–74. functions. In: Squire LR, Bloom FE, Landis SC, Roberts JL, Zig-
[78] Kolb B. Functions of the frontal cortex of the rat: a comparative mond MJ, editors. Fundamental of neuroscience. San Diego, CA:
review. Brain Res Rev 1984;8:65–98. Academic Press, Elsevier; 2003. p. 1353–76.
[79] Kolb B. Animal models for human PFC-related disorders. In: [100] Morán AM, Reinoso-Suárez F. Topographical organization of the
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Corner MA, Feen- thalamic afferent connections to the motor cortex in the cat. J Comp
stra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure, function and Neurol 1988;270:64–85.
pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier; [101] Morgan MA, Schulkin J, LeDoux JE. Ventral medial prefrontal
1990. p. 501–19. cortex and emotional perseveration: the memory for prior extinction
[80] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Dissociation of the contributions of the training. Behav Brain Res 2003, in press.
prefrontal, motor and parietal cortex to the control of movement in [102] Muir JL, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. The cerebral cortex of the rat
the rat. Can J Psychol 1983a;37:211–32. and visual attentional function: dissociable effects of mediofrontal,
[81] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Generalizing in neuropsychology: problems cingulate, anterior dorsolateral and parietal cortex lesions on a
and principles underlying cross-species comparisons. In: Robinson five-choice serial reaction time task. Cereb Cortex 1996;6:470–81.
TE, editor. Behavioral contributions to brain research. New York: [103] Nauta WJH. Neural associations of the amygdaloid complex in the
Oxford University Press; 1983b. p. 237–64. monkey. Brain 1962;85:505–20.
[82] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Neonatal frontal lesions in the rat: sparing [104] Neafsey EJ, Hurley-Gius KM, Arvanitis D. The topographical orga-
of learned but not species-typical behavior in the presence of re- nization of neurons in the rat medial frontal, insular and olfactory
duced brain weight and cortical thickness. J Comp Physiol Psychol cortex to the solitary tract nucleus, olfactory bulb, periaqueductal
1981;95:863–79. gray and superior colliculus. Brain Res 1986;377:261–70.
[83] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Brain plasticity and behavior. Ann Rev [105] Northcutt RG, Kaas JH. The emergence and evolution of mammalian
Psychol 1998;49:43–64. neocortex. Trends Neurosci 1995;18:373–9.
[84] Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Fundamentals of human neuropsychology, [106] Öngür D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital
5th ed. New York: Worth; 2003. and medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex 2000;10:206–19.
[85] Kolb B, Buhrmann K, MacDonald R, Sutherland RJ. Dissociation [107] Otto T, Eichenbaum H. Complementary roles of the orbital
of the medial prefrontal, posterior parietal, and posterior temporal prefrontal cortex and the perirhinal–entorhinal cortices in an
cortex for spatial navigation and recognition memory in the rat. odor-guided delayed-nonmatching-to-sample task. Behav Neurosci
Cereb Cortex 1994;4:15–34. 1992;106:762–75.
16 H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17

[108] Pandya DN, Yeterian EH. Prefrontal cortex in relation to other acquisition and reversal of odor-guided go, no go discrimination
cortical areas in rhesus monkey: architecture and connections. In: task. Soc Neurosci Abstr 2000;23.
Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Corner MA, Feenstra [129] Sawaguchi T, Goldman-Rakic PS. The role of D1-dopamine receptor
MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure, function and in working memory: local injections of dopamine antagonists into
pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Amsterdam: Elsevier; the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys performing an oculomotor
1990. p. 63–94. delayed-response task. J Neurophysiol 1994;71:515–28.
[109] Passingham R. The frontal lobes and voluntary action. Oxford [130] Schoenbaum G, Setlow B. Integrating orbitofrontal cortex into pre-
psychology series, vol. 21. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1993. frontal theory: common processing themes across species and sub-
p. 299. divisions. Learn Mem 2002;8:134–47.
[110] Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San [131] Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Changes in functional
Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1998. connectivity in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during
[111] Preuss TM. Do rats have a prefrontal cortex? The Rose–Woolsey– learning and reversal training. J Neurosci 2000;20:5179–89.
Akert program reconsidered. J Cogn Neurosci 1995;7:1–24. [132] Sherman SM, Guillery RW. Exploring the thalamus. San Diego,
[112] Preuss TM. Taking the measure of diversity: comparative alterna- CA: Academic Press; 2001. p. 312.
tives to the model–animal paradigm in cortical neuroscience. Brain [133] Shibata H. Topographic organization of subcortical projections to the
Behav Evol 2000;55:287–99. anterior thalamic nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol 1992;323:117–
[113] Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Connections of the ventral gran- 27.
ular frontal cortex of macaques with perisylvian premotor and so- [134] Shipley JE, Kolb B. Neural correlates of species typical behavior in
matosensory areas: anatomical evidence for somatic representation the Syrian Golden hamster. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1977;91:1056–
in primate frontal association cortex. J Comp Neurol 1989;282:293– 73.
316. [135] Stepniewska I, Kosmal A. Distribution of mediodorsal thalamic
[114] Preuss TM, Kaas JH. Human brain evolution. In: Zigmond MJ, nucleus afferents originating in the prefrontal association cortex of
Bloom FE, Landis SC, Roberts JL, Squire LR, editors. Funda- the dog. Acta Neurobiol Exp 1986;46:311–22.
mental of neuroscience. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1999. [136] Stepniewska I, Kosmal A. Differential projection from the motor
p. 1283–311. and limbic cortical regions to the mediodorsal thalmic nucleus in
[115] Pribram KH, Wilson WA, Connors J. Effects of lesions of the the dog. Acta Neurobiol Exp 1989;49:23–37.
medial forebrain on alternation behavior of rhesus monkeys. Exp [137] Swanson LW. Brain maps: structure of the rat brain. Amsterdam:
Neurol 1962;6:36–47. Elsevier; 1992. p. 240.
[116] Quirk GJ, Russo GK, Barron JL, Lebron K. The role of ventromedial [138] Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG. Qualitative and quantitative com-
prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear. J Neurosci parison of the prefrontal cortex in rat and in primates, including
2000;20:6225–31. humans. In: Uylings HBM, Van Eden CG, De Bruin JPC, Corner
[117] Ragozzino ME, Kesner RP. The role of the agranular insular cortex MA, Feenstra MPG, editors. The prefrontal cortex: its structure,
in working memory for food reward value and allocentric space in function and pathology. Progress in brain research, vol. 85. Ams-
rats. Behav Brain Res 1999;98:103–12. terdam: Elsevier; 1990. p. 31–62.
[118] Ragozzino ME. The contribution of cholinergic and dopaminergic [139] Uylings HBM. About assumptions in estimation of density of neu-
afferents in the rat prefrontal cortex to learning, memory, and rons and glial cells. Biol Psychiat 2002;51:840–2.
attention. Psychobiology 2000;28:238–47. [140] Van Eden CG. Development of connections between the mediodor-
[119] Rajkowska G, Kosmal A. Intrinsic connections and cytoarchitectonic sal nucleus of the thalamus and the prefrontal cortex in the rat. J
data of the frontal association cortex in the dog. Acta Neurobiol Comp Neurol 1986;244:349–59.
Exp 48:169–92. [141] Van Eden CG, Uylings HBM. Ctyoarchitectonic development of
[120] Ramus SJ, Eichenbaum H. Neural correlates of olfactory recognition the prefronal cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol 1885;241:253–67.
memory in the rat orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2000;20:8199– [142] Van Eden CG, Lamme VAF, Uylings HBM. Heterotopic cortical
208. afferents to the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat. A combined
[121] Ray JP, Price JL. The organization of the thalamocortical connec- retrograde and anterograde tracer study. Eur J Neurosci 1992;4:77–
tions of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to 97.
the ventral forebrain–prefrontal cortex topography. J Comp Neurol [143] Vertes RP. Analysis of projections from the medial prefrontal cortex
1992;323:167–97. to the thalamus in the rat, with emphasis on nucleus reuniens. J
[122] Ray JP, Price JL. The organization of projections from the mediodor- Comp Neurol 2002;442:163–87.
sal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex [144] Verwer RW, Van Vulpen EH, Van Uum JF. Postnatal develop-
in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 1993;337:1–31. ment of amygdaloid projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rat
[123] Reep RL, Corwin JV, King VR. Neural connections of orbital cortex studied with retrograde and anterograde tracers. J Comp Neurol
in rats: topography of cortical and thalamic afferents. Exp Brain 1996;376:75–96.
Res 1996;111:215–32. [145] Verwer RWH, Meijer RJ, Van Uum HFM, Witter MP. Collateral
[124] Reep RL, Corwin JV. Topographic organization of the striatal and projections from the rat hippocampal formation to the lateral and
thalamic connections of rat medial agranular cortex. Brain Res medial prefrontal cortex. Hippocampus 1997;7:397–402.
1999;841:43–52. [146] Warren JM, Kolb B. Generalizations in neuropsychology. In: Finger
[125] Rolls ET. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. In: Stuss DT, S, editor. Brain damage, behavior and the concept of recovery of
Knight RT, editors. Principles of frontal lobe function. New York: function. New York: Plenum Press; 1978.
Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 354–75. [147] Whishaw IQ. Change in sensory control related to behavioural and
[126] Rose JE, Woolsey CN. The orbitofrontal cortex and its connections anatomical evolution of primate skilled reaching. Behav Brain Res,
with the mediodorsal nucleus in rabbit, sheep and cat. Res Publ in press.
Assoc Nerv Ment Dis 1948;27:210–32. [148] Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Gorny BP. Skilled reaching in rats and
[127] Russchen FT, Amaral DG, Price JL. The afferent connections of the humans: evidence of parallel development or homology. Behav
substantia innominata in the monkey. J Comp Neurol 1985;242:1– Brain Res 1992;47:59–70.
27. [149] Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Gorny BP. Medial frontal cortex lesions
[128] Saddoris MP, Setlow B, Nugent S, Schoenbaum G. A reexamination impair the aiming component of rat reaching. Behav Brain Res
of the role of orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in 1992;50:93–104.
H.B.M. Uylings et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 146 (2003) 3–17 17

[150] Whishaw IQ, Tomie J, Kolb B. Ventrolateral frontal cortex lesions [154] Wouterlood FG, Steinbusch HW, Luiten PG, Bol JG. Projec-
in rats impair the acquisition and retention of a tactile–olfactory tion from the prefrontal cortex to histaminergic cell groups in
configural task. Behav Neurosci 1992;106:597–603. the posterior hypothalamic region of the rat. Anterograde trac-
[151] Wikmark RGE, Divac I, Weiss R. Retention of spatial delayed ing with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin ombined with im-
alternation in rats with lesions of the frontal lobes: implications munocytochemistry of histidine decarboxylase. Brain Res 1987;406:
for a comparative neuropsychology of the prefrontal system. Brain 330–6.
Behav Evol 1973;8:329–39. [155] Yoshida A, Dostrovsky JO, Chiang CY. The afferent and efferent
[152] Williams SM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Widespread origin of the primate connections of the nucleus submedius in the rat. J Comp Neurol
mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb Cortex 1998;8:321–45. 1992;324:115–33.
[153] Woolsey CN. Organization of somatic sensory and motor areas of [156] Zaborsky L, Gaykema RPA, Swanson DJ, Culliman WE. Cortical
the cerebral cortex. In: Harlow HF, Woolsey CN, editors. Biological input to the basal forebrain. Neuroscience 1997;79:1051–78.
and biochemical bases of behavior. Madison, WI: University of [157] Zilles K. The cortex of the rat. A stereotaxic atlas. Berlin:
Wisconsin Press; 1957. Springer-Verlag; 1985. p. 121.

You might also like