You are on page 1of 16

The Behavior Analyst 1996, 19, 257-272 No.

2 (Fall)

Envisioning Cultural Practices


Mark A. Mattaini
Columbia University School of Social Work
Graphic visualization has demonstrated its value for organizing transactional data and modeling
complex phenomena in a wide variety of fields, from theoretical physics to medicine. Behavior
analysts have historically used a variety of graphic tools not only for presentation but also for
analysis and teaching. As they turn increasingly to the analysis and design of cultural practices, the
phenomena behavior analysts study are becoming increasingly complicated. Many cultural practices
of interest are embedded in extensive webs of interlocking practices and contingencies that can be
difficult to grasp comprehensively. Building on contingency diagrams, which have proven to be
useful for the analysis of operant behavior, and graphic tools developed for object-oriented systems
analysis, this paper suggests graphic tools for capturing the interlocking contingencies that constitute
cultures. These diagrams offer a broad-bandwidth technology for analyzing and designing cultural
practices.
Key words: cultural practices, interlocking contingencies, graphic visualization

Graphic visualization is useful, and elements combine to produce complex


even essential, for organizing and un- repertoires (Malott, 1 992b; Malott,
derstanding complexity in many fields Whaley, & Malott, 1993; Mattaini,
(Bertin, 1983; Tufte, 1983, 1990). De- 1995a).
veloping iconographies that allow one As behavior analysts turn to the
to see previously hidden relationships analysis and design of cultural practic-
and comprehensively capture concep- es, the level of complexity becomes an
tual advances is often important for order of magnitude greater, and the
scientific progress: need for ways to capture, coherently
Intellectual innovation often requires, above all
and contextually, the conditions and
else, a new image to embody a novel theory. events relevant to such analyses cor-
Primates are visual animals, and we think best respondingly increases (Biglan, 1995).
in pictorial or geometric terms. Words are an Systems analysts have struggled with
evolutionary afterthought.... Icons are primary similar issues and have come to rely
molders of our thoughts, and the search for fun- heavily on graphic tools for identifying
damentally new representation is one of the
most important efforts a scholar can make. and tracing multiple objects and events
(Gould, 1995, pp. 10, 12) in their transactional associations (e.g.,
Coad & Yourdon, 1991; Yourdon,
Behavior analysts rely heavily on the 1989). Related strategies have potential
visual analysis of data, and have de- for cultural analysis and design as well.
veloped multiple graphic approaches This paper traces initial efforts to de-
for representing behavioral events velop a graphic language for analyzing
(e.g., Chisholm & Cook, 1995; Mi- cultural practices and the interlocking
chael & Shafer, 1995; see also Lee, contingencies within which they are
1994), including contingency diagrams embedded.
(Malott, 1992a). Contingency diagrams
are particularly valuable for teaching
the concepts of the science of behavior UNITS OF
by helping students to trace how basic CULTURAL ANALYSIS
Skinner (1981) argued that the caus-
I thank Janet S. Twyman for helpful com- es of human behavior lie in selection
ments on earlier drafts. by consequences at three levels: natu-
Address correspondence to the author at the ral selection at the biological level, op-
Columbia University School of Social Work,
622 West 113th Street, New York, New York erant conditioning at the behavioral
10025 (E-mail: mattaini @columbia.edu). level, and the evolution of social en-
257
258 MARK A. MATTAINI

vironments at the cultural level. Be- 47), but is clearly more inclusive than
havior analysts have only begun to ex- common anthropological definitions
plore cultural selection (see, e.g., sev- that focus on the practices of distinc-
eral contributions in Lamal, 1991). tive societies or ethnic groups.
Biglan (1995) recently outlined basic The natural line of fracture between
principles for cultural analysis and for the second (operant behavioral) and
changing cultural practices; that is, cul- third (cultural) levels of selection ap-
tural design. Cultural design, the ap- pears to occur at the point at which op-
plied side of cultural analysis, seeks to erants are transmitted among persons
increase the incidence and prevalence and shaped and maintained by orga-
of beneficial cultural practices and de- nized networks of social reinforce-
crease the extent of detrimental ones ment. Although this assertion may be
by making changes in the interlocking controversial, there may be no essen-
contingencies that instantiate cultural tially different processes at work in
entities. cultural entities of varying sizes (al-
As described by Glenn, cultural though of course larger groups involve
practices "involve repetition of analo- substantially more complexity). If all
gous operant behavior across individ- involve similar core processes, it may
uals of a single generation and across be possible to study those processes
generations of individuals" (Glenn, first in simpler cultures and move later
1991, p. 60). Most important issues of to those that are more complex, an ap-
our time involve such cultural practic- proach that may be particularly useful
es, from those supported by the micro- for advancing a science in its early
culture of the family (which may be stages.
targeted in family therapy) to those Others have suggested ways to es-
supported by peer, ethnic, religious, or- tablish the "bright lines" (Ainslie,
ganizational, and national cultures. 1993) among levels of selection. Var-
Cultural practices include patterns of gas (1993) distinguishes between nat-
social exchange (kindness, some forms ural selection and cultural selection as
of violence), language, political action, follows:
and many other extraordinarily com-
plicated social conditions and events. By "natural selection" I mean those effects that
There is as yet only limited agree- occur when the organism's behavior directly
contacts the physical, biological, and behavioral
ment on the essential units of cultural properties of contingencies relevant to selection
analysis, an important consideration in consequences. ... The 3-term contingency re-
the development of any science. One lationship of much of infrahuman organism be-
issue is what behavior analysts mean havior exemplifies natural selection. By "cultur-
al selection" I mean those effects that occur
by culture. Skinner defined culture as through the contact of the organism's behavior
"the contingencies of social reinforce- with another organism's behavior that mediates
ment maintained by a group" (1984/ its contact with physical, biological, and behav-
1987, p. 74). Note that this is an ex- ioral contingencies relevant to selection conse-
tremely inclusive definition, and it was quences. The 4-term contingency relationship of
verbal behavior thus requires a prior verbal com-
probably meant to be. From this per- munity; a culture. (p. 20)
spective, much of social behavior, in-
cluding most verbal behavior, falls For Vargas, the essential distinction be-
within the purview of cultural analysis; tween the behavioral and cultural lev-
and a broad range of organized groups, els of selection is mediation by the be-
from the smallest (e.g., a family) to the havior of another, but this does not yet
largest (e.g., a nation), appear to fit clearly establish the basic unit. He also
Skinner's definition of cultures. This notes that "nonverbal behavior is the
definition is similar to but not identical target for natural selection, and verbal
with Harris's ("the learned repertory of behavior the target for cultural selec-
thoughts and actions exhibited by the tion" (p. 20), which suggests that cul-
members of social groups," 1979, p. tural analysis primarily examines con-
ENVISIONING CULTURAL PRACTICES 259

tingent relations between verbal behav- cies, whereas others (e.g.; doing home-
ior and other events. work) are maintained largely by so-
In answer to the question, "What cially mediated contingencies. Both
exactly is being selected?" in cultural seem to fit the basic definitions of cul-
evolution, Skinner suggests, "within a tural practices, however; cooking and
given group, the answer seems to be homework are done differently in dif-
practices-better ways of hunting, ferent cultures. Where the active con-
gathering, growing, making tools, and tingencies are socially mediated in the
so on" (in Catania & Harnad, 1988, p. material that follows, I will attempt to
36). Although understanding cultural clarify that in the diagrams.
phenomena requires analysis of contin- Glenn and Malagodi (1991) suggest
gency interlocks (and that is the pur- that the essential unit of cultural evo-
pose of the tools suggested in this pa- lution is the permaclone, a term bor-
per), for purposes of cultural design, rowed from Harris (1979). The basic
the accessible targets of change are the elements of the permaclone are de-
cultural practices that are involved in scribed as "1) repeated enactments of
the interlocking contingencies. Cer- a scene, 2) by a group of individuals,
tainly for applied work, utility is an im- 3) the personnel of which group
portant consideration, and a focus on changes gradually over time" (p. 8);
the practice seems valuable. "enactments" therefore are seen as ba-
One reviewer of this paper suggest- sic. Practices are components of inter-
ed that, in a scene involving a teacher locking contingencies. Those inter-
reading to students, the interlocking locking contingencies (the content of
behaviors of teacher and students need the permaclone) lead to cultural out-
to be accounted for simultaneously in comes. Glenn and Malagodi, however,
defining the practice of interest; this is suggest that the practices themselves
a compelling point. It is analogous to (e.g., an action on the part of a teacher)
Lee's (1994) description of the units of are not the essential units; rather, the
behavior analysis as "things done"- set of interlocking contingencies (in-
not just acts, but the act and its con- stantiated by the scene) that support
sequences, that is, the two-term contin- those practices are. That set, then,
gency. In this case there are interlock- leads to an outcome-a certain level of
ing two-term (or more) contingencies. educational achievement. This view,
Recognition of interlocking contingen- although it is somewhat different than
cies predates discussions of cultural the one presented in the present paper,
analysis and design; Skinner discussed has the advantage of clearly focusing
transactions between speaker and lis- attention on the interlocks.
tener, for example (1957, 1989). The definition of cultural practice,
McGinnies (1970), who attempted to however, is still problematic. Some
construct a coherent social psychology suggest that a practice be defined as the
in reinforcement terms, stated that "so- multiple behaviors involved in inter-
cial behavior is evidenced whenever locking social contingencies, but this
two or more organisms, either directly appears to differ from Skinner's use of
or indirectly, serve both to prompt and the term, or Glenn's description ("rep-
to reinforce one another's perfor- etition of analogous operant behavior
mances" (p. 1). The level of complex- across individuals," 1991, p. 60). Prac-
ity involved at the cultural level, how- tices clearly involve multiple instances,
ever, is often substantially greater. as do operants, but I believe that it is
Many of the actions of any social- reasonable to use the term cultural
ized person have been learned from practice to refer to an operant trans-
other members of the cultures within mitted (and often maintained) by a cul-
which he or she is embedded. Some ture; this definition is consistent with
(e.g., cooking, driving a car) are main- both Skinner's and Glenn's usages. I
tained largely by nonsocial contingen- may be wrong here; it may ultimately
260 MARK A. MATTAINI

Cultural practices are selected, ac-


°Nss zl
,, HS Graduation
Rate of
cording to Skinner (Catania & Hamad,
1988), on two levels. Selection can oc-
\\9 14 cur through differential survival of en-
~b~' 0) 90% tire cultural entities in which the prac-
tices are embedded (e.g., celibacy
Figure 1. Interlocking cultural practices asso-
ciated with an aggregate outcome (adapted from among the Shakers probably contrib-
Glenn, 1988). uted to the almost total extinction of
that culture). Most cultural selection,
however, occurs at the level of the
prove more helpful to use the term in- practice. Practices that work better-
stead to refer to entire webs of inter- produce better outcomes or aggregate
locking behaviors; but I believe that outcomes-for the group, its members,
the approach taken here is a useful be- or those who have power in the group
ginning for applied work. (Biglan, 1995) are therefore reinforced
A key to understanding cultural by members of the group. Much,
practices is that the practices them- though not all, selection of this type
selves consist of behavior; Skinner probably involves rule governance and
stated that they involve "no new be- verbally mediated behavior.
havioral process" (in Catania & Har- Operants consist of functionally
nad, 1988, p. 38). This observation equivalent classes of acts, not of single
provides a valuable access point for events. The frequency with which an
analyses of sociocultural phenomena. organism emits instances of such a
Practices are often reinforced by mem- class is a central datum in the science
bers of a cultural entity because of ag- of behavior (Michael, 1995). With re-
gregate advantages for the group gard to cultural practices, which are
(Glenn, 1991) and is a process that special cases of operant behavior, the
may require a heavy overlay of rule frequency (incidence, prevalence) with
governance (cf. Malott, 1988). Glenn which the practices occur is also cen-
(1988) offered the diagram shown in tral. There is an additional level of
Figure 1 (slightly adapted here) to de- complexity, however, present at the
pict how interlocking practices are as- cultural level-practices are not emit-
sociated with an outcome. This sche- ted only by a single organism, but by
matic is essentially conceptual; it sim- a class of persons, whom I refer to here
ply indicates that interlocking practices as actors. The datum of interest is of-
supported by cultures produce, and are ten the frequency with which any
selected by, the aggregate outcome. member of the class (say, high school
This paper might be seen as an effort students in a community) emits the
to identify tools to further specify and practice of interest (attends school).
detail the left side of Glenn's figure and
to suggest some tools that may help GRAPHIC TOOLS
cultural analysts move beyond the gen-
eral and abstract to the identification of The processes by which cultural
specific data items that can then be ex-practices are established and main-
plored empirically. Approaches for ex- tained, according to Skinner, consist of
the basic behavioral relations identified
plicating the links between the left side
of Glenn's diagram (interlocking con- by the science of behavior (e.g., rein-
tingencies) and the right (aggregate forcement, extinction, discrimination,
outcomes) are also needed. Many be- establishing and abolishing opera-
havior analysts have relied on the work tions). A graphic language for captur-
of anthropologist Marvin Harris (1979, ing cultural-level events therefore can
1989) for understanding some of those build on what is already known, but
connections (e.g., Biglan, 1995; Glenn, needs to be able to organize more data
1991). elements at a higher conceptual level,
ENVISIONING CULTURAL PRACTICES 261

SD A

BEFORE BEHAVIOR AFTER

have no food ask for food have food

Figure 2. A standard contingency diagram (Malott, 1992a).

allowing the transactional structure to volved are substantial, and it is no sur-


emerge. The objective, no doubt only prise, therefore, that achieving specific-
partially achieved here, is to develop ity and comprehensiveness in this work
new ways to specify contingency in- is difficult. It is certainly possible to
terlocks more clearly and precisely. As analyze verbally the contingencies that
Tufte indicates (Tufte, 1983, 1990), the affect the behavior of each class of ac-
purpose of graphic visualization is not tor (see, e.g., Newman, 1992, who dis-
to simplify, but rather to make the cusses contingencies associated with
complex comprehensible. the actions of drug users, drug dealers,
Analyzing, designing, and changing and policy makers). But to be able to
interlocking cultural practices are at truly capture the transactional unity of
least as complex as understanding and these variables, it may be most helpful
affecting processes in other fields in to find ways to view them all as an
which visualization has proven to be organic, interrelated whole.
important, and graphics can be similar-
ly important for this work, not only for Contingency Diagrams
presentation and teaching, but perhaps
most crucially for analytic purposes. Although many behavior analysts
Major advantages of graphic depictions have diagrammed contingent relation-
include their increased bandwidth, ships among behaviors, antecedents,
which enables the observer to see mul- and consequences in various ways, one
tiple interacting phenomena "all at current standard is Malott's contingen-
once"; and their utility for differential- cy diagram (Malott, 1992a, 1992b;
ly viewing the phenomena from a va- Malott et al., 1993; Mattaini, 1995a).
riety of perspectives, each emphasizing At its most basic, a contingency dia-
one or more dimensions and deem- gram portrays the behavior of interest
phasizing others that are less relevant and at least one consequence that af-
to the viewer's purpose. fects its frequency; Malott usually in-
Analyzing events at the cultural lev- cludes the state of affairs prior to the
el requires tools for identifying and behavior (the "motivating condition")
specifying not only the practices of in- and often the occasion (the discrimi-
terest but also the way these practices native stimulus) as well. See, for ex-
interlock with those of other classes of ample, Figure 2.
actors (e.g., the connections among the Mattaini (1993, 1995a) suggested
acts of drug dealers, drug users, law several refinements to contingency di-
enforcement personnel, peers of the agrams that may capture certain prop-
dealer, neighborhood residents, and erties of operant behavior with addi-
policy makers). The complexities in- tional precision. For example, the sim-
262 MARK A. MATTAINI

Rules Occasion/SD Consequence


pi7t i fV1
awfu wgV
8Jression
od
anyone*drespectsIf n & yowwI reinforcers
anein ay wSYw t 2tt_ t

W stRbegIaI Slv

Establishing Motivatin g "' ConsequeNce


OeratIns Condition [
*w
flaPoemebsh t
generand
mwy averuses
* verbal
fi EOs
4 rbk of
*face ted respect
nI I

Structuralgr rf Imodelsifo
Antecdent
IModtb for Consequence
Pow
EwAwmaiekof Ioenc fv#l not resnt,
nwdab
of
vbbwe
~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~~cMtyI we

Figure 3. Violent acts directed against a rival group (see Mattaini et al., in press, for further detail).

ple rounded rectangle in Figure 2 can stances of such behaviors may be in


be replaced with the class-and-object part rule governed ("I lose face if I let
symbol used in object-oriented systems that guy from outside the neighbor-
analysis (Coad & Yourdon, 1991), hood disrespect me"), in part learned
which consists of a black rounded rect- through imitation (observing other
angle (representing the class) surround- gang members gain respect through vi-
ed by a gray (representing a "stack" olent acts), and may be evoked in part
of underlying instances), when one by multiple establishing operations
wishes to indicate multiple members of (e.g., high levels of overall deprivation,
the class of interest, for example, mul- increasing sensitivity to whatever
tiple employees in an organizational sources of reinforcement are available)
database, or multiple functionally consistent with the matching law (Mc-
equivalent instances of an operant. (All Dowell, 1988). Multiple concurrent
subsequent figures in this paper use the consequences may also be active (re-
class-and-object notation.) In addition, spect from peers, attention from young
the arrows on the diagrams might be women, relief from the learned aver-
drawn as dashed rather than solid, to sive condition generated by the unhar-
suggest that operant behavior is evoked med presence of a member of another
probabilistically by conditions and gang).
events that are contingently related to These concurrent and coactive con-
it, rather than being elicited mechanis- ditions and events can be portrayed in
tically. a more complex contingency diagram
Cultural practices (say, acts of gang such as that shown in Figure 3. Note
violence) are often embedded in highly that this image offers multiple points
complex webs of antecedents and con- toward which preventive interventions
sequences (Canada, 1995; Mattaini, might be directed, and also demon-
Twyman, Chin, & Nam, 1996). In- strates why unidimensional strategies
ENVISIONING CULTURAL PRACTICES 263

Practice:
car pooling to work

Class of Actor:
worker Possible Aggregate
Outcomes:
Antecedents: i Production
_*
* time of day, etc.(Si ) _o* Fuel savings
* rules regarding potential r * Increased worker
deprivation if fail to icommunity"
drive to work (EO)
Consequences:
. opportunity to earn $
* pleasant conversation with
car pool partners
* financial incentives for
car pooling

Figure 4. A practice diagram depicting the practice, class of actor participating in it, antecedents,
consequences, and possible aggregate outcomes.

may be ineffective, given the multiple come increasingly cumbersome, and


active factors involved. the purpose of the figures (to clarify
Up to this level of complexity, con- complexity) can be lost. Cultural prac-
tingency diagrams are robust, captur- tice diagrams, as presented below, are
ing substantial details that can be ag- an approach for organizing such data
gregated and disaggregated for analytic that may have utility for more complex
purposes. The situation becomes more cultural analysis and design purposes.
challenging when the interlocking acts
of multiple persons, or especially mul- Diagramming Cultural Practices
tiple classes of actors, are of interest.
Up to a point, one can overlay trans- In dealing with cultural practices,
parencies that depict how the behavior scientists are interested in similar con-
of a member of one class of actors (say tingencies across a class of actors, rath-
peers) may be an important conse- er than in the behavior of a single in-
quence for another (a young person dividual. In seeking ways to capture
perpetrating an act of violence), and these events graphically, it is essential
the behavior of yet another person (the to maintain that focus. A practice dia-
youth from outside the neighborhood) gram like that shown in Figure 4 is one
may be a necessary antecedent for the way to do so. The core element is the
attack. The relevant contingencies as- practice itself, noted in the top cell.
sociated with all three acts can be di- The class of actors who emit the prac-
agrammed separately, and overlaid on tice is shown next, followed by ante-
each other with careful planning of cedents and consequences that are in-
graphic layout; as classes of cultural volved in contingent relationships with
practices and classes of actors accu- the practice. The central object in Fig-
mulate, however, such overlays be- ure 3 is therefore the practice; the ac-
264 MARK A. MATTAINI
tors who emit them, although impor- stract, generalized class of practices,
tant, are analytically secondary. The for example, acts of street violence.
single practice depicted in Figure 4 This practice may interlock with cer-
alone, of course, does not result in the tain law enforcement practices, by-
aggregate outcomes shown. Multiple stander-emitted actions, and so on, and
practices and the interlocking contin- "act of street violence" may be spe-
gencies within which they are embed- cific enough for some analytic purpos-
ded are required to do so. The real es. At other times, however, finer
power of practice diagrams emerges grained analyses may be required, and
when such interlocking webs are de- the generalization-specialization (gen-
picted, as in the more complex figures spec) notation shown in the upper pan-
that follow. el of Figure 5 may be useful for that
Practice diagrams draw heavily on purpose. Such diagrams allow the an-
tools and concepts from object-orient- alyst to identify and work with rela-
ed analysis (OOA) (Coad & Yourdon, tively discrete subclasses when this is
1991), incorporating such OOA prin- useful.
ciples as abstraction, encapsulation, in- At other times, what object-oriented
heritance, and association. As noted by analysts call whole-part structure may
Coad and Yourdon, tools with these be important to portray. The most ob-
functions allow the analyst to "tackle vious example in behavioral work is
more challenging problem domains when a behavior of interest consists of
explicitly represent commonality, a chain of simple acts; one may not
[and] provide a consistent underlying always need this level of detail, but in
representation for analysis ... and de- some cases it may be valuable. A sim-
sign" (pp. 35-36)-all crucial objec- ple example, using the standard OOA
tives in the analysis and design of cul- notation, is shown in the lower panel
tural practices. Note that "objects" in of Figure 5.
OOA need not be "things"; they may Portraying contingency interlocks.
be events, as in the analyses presented The central purpose of practice dia-
here. grams is to depict the interlocks among
The practice diagram can be viewed multiple practices and the contingent
as a contingency diagram that has been relations within which they are embed-
further collapsed into an iconic repre- ded. The sequence of events within a
scene is also often important. An ex-
sentation. It is possible, and often pref-
erable, to code items listed in terms of ample-a sort of behavior-analytic
their function (e.g., positive reinforcer"classic"-is shown in Figure 6, in
[R+ or SR+], establishing operation which escalation of coercive exchanges
[EO]). For practical purposes it is not (as described by Patterson, 1976) oc-
always necessary to note all the rele- curs between a parent and an aggres-
vant antecedents and consequences sive child in one of the simplest of cul-
that are involved in less central parts tures-a family. Although later ele-
of the network; at minimum, the prac- ments in the sequence may result from
tice and class of actors performing it naturally occurring consequences that
should consistently be noted. My col- do not require cultural mediation, the
leagues and I often begin with contin- initial steps (a coercive demand and
gency diagrams that trace the practices noncompliance) are often elements of
of interest to ensure precision, then family cultures that are, indeed, passed
collapse those diagrams into practice on across persons and generations
diagrams to explore the interlocks within the family. In fact, the incidence
among practices and their associated of such behaviors varies dramatically
contingencies. between larger cultural units as well;
Aggregating and disaggregating Biglan (1995) suggests that the core of
practices. In some cases, cultural ana- the analysis of cultural practices is the
lysts are interested in a relatively ab- examination of incidence and preva-
ENVISIONING CULTURAL PRACTICES 265

act of street
Gen-Spec violence
diagram L

§[violent act related 79 f{violen t response X frustration-relatedX


8<to drug turffwar J ll to "disrespectN " random" attack j

Whole-Part speaking to
Diagram:_

orient body vocalize

Figure 5. A gen-spec diagram and a whole-part diagram. Note that only the practices are shown
here; the class of actor, antecedents, and consequences related to each practice are omitted for
simplicity.

lence rates of desirable and undesirable alization and demystification that can
practices within cultural groups. Al- be important to intervening, to design-
though I am focusing in this example ing new family cultures, which is the
on practices within the family as a cul- core of family intervention. This is an-
tural entity, a related analysis could, other way of understanding what Min-
and probably should, examine this uchin (1974; Minuchin & Nichols,
larger picture. 1993) terms restructuring-not just a
Family therapists (e.g., Friedman, change, but a substantive change that
1991) recognize that such patterns can is supported by corresponding changes
be passed on from generation to gen- elsewhere in the system. Practice dia-
eration, a process that occurs in non- grams can be useful for elaborating and
clinical family systems as well (Walsh, specifying both current contingency in-
1993). What the science of behavior terlocks and those that may be more
can bring to bear here is an operation- desirable. Patterson and his colleagues
266 MARK A. MATTAINI

ractice: Practice:
makes coercive demand makes coercive counter-response
toward other family member toward initiator
Actor: Actor:
initiator responder

Antecedents: _ m_4 Antecedents:


aversive situation under aversive situation created
control of other family by demand of initiator
member
Consequences: Consequences:
sometimes reinforced, sometimes escapes demand,
sometimes not sometimes does not

2 -

Practice: Practice:
escalates (makes demand escalates (makes increasingly
more coercively) coercive counter-response)
Actor: Actor:
initiator A
responder

Antecedents: 1 3 Antecedents:
o aversive
. aversive situation dernand
l: versive non-cornpliance ave|rseb
: situation

Consequences: Consequences:
eventually, compliance eventually, demand may
with demand may occur be withdrawn

Boundary of Scene
Figure 6. The development of Patterson's (1976) coercive spiral within a family with an aggressive
child.

found that in families with aggressive fore, is shaped by negative reinforce-


children, coercive exchanges were fre- ment (and perhaps sometimes by pos-
quent (and might be initiated by either itive reinforcement as well) on an in-
the child or the parent; it is precisely termittent schedule for each party.
because the roles in the scene may be Designing a new family culture, a
taken by any of a class of family mem- new set of interlocking practices and
bers that this is a cultural practice). A contingencies, is crucial in such fami-
coercive behavior was usually fol- lies, because there is no way out of this
lowed by a coercive counterresponse, pattern, once it is established, within
and the aversiveness of the exchanges the existing family culture. Although
tended to escalate until one party broke such ineffective patterns of family ex-
it off. More severe aggression, there- changes were clearly described over
ENVISIONING CULTURAL PRACTICES 267

two decades ago by Patterson (1976) The particular power of visual tools
and others (e.g., Sloane, 1988), con- becomes most evident in analyses of
ceptualizing the issues this way has not practices that are supported by multiple
yet become mainstream. Effective be- interlocking contingencies both within
havior-analytic intervention involves and outside the scene, many of which
not just teaching individuals new be- may involve verbal mediation. In this
haviors but embedding new sets of section, two examples of relatively
practices within the family system. A complex interlocking practices and the
good example is presented by Serna, extended webs of contingencies within
Schumaker, Sherman, and Sheldon which they are embedded will be brief-
(1991), in which they taught very dis- ly elaborated. Similar analyses using
turbed families to participate in family practice diagrams can be conducted for
conferences in the home. These con- any practice or set of practices of so-
ferences involved sets of practices in ciocultural importance, from encour-
which roles were interchangeable aging condom use to demonstrating re-
among members and interlocking net- spect and appreciation for cultural di-
works of social reinforcement were es- versity.
tablished; ideally, these practices As one example, Jesse Jackson and
would be passed along to younger his Rainbow Coalition have targeted
members of the family as they grow the reduction of violence as a core pri-
up. Practice diagrams could be useful ority (Citizen Education Fund, no
for clarifying the specifics that need to date). Rather than simply exhorting
be addressed in helping families (or or- young people not to be violent, how-
ganizations, or other cultural entities) ever, Jackson has targeted success in
change in these ways. school as a key variable (this is con-
sistent with the analysis of our group;
Beyond the Boundaries of the Scene refer to Mattaini et al., 1996). As a part
of this strategy, he has asked churches
Not all of the relevant contingencies to ask parents and children to make an
involved in many cultural practices oc- annual pledge to take specific actions.
cur within the boundaries of the im- These actions include parents taking
mediate scene. There are several ways children to school, meeting with teach-
in which conditions and events that are ers, and turning off the television for
not present at the moment may affect three hours each evening while chil-
behavior. For example, the history of dren do homework; and youth attend-
delivery of consequences under similar ing school, not carrying weapons, and
stimulus conditions is of course cru- doing homework instead of watching
cial, because this is how, ultimately, television. A partial depiction of this
behavior has been selected. Particular strategy, including several probably
behaviors are also often segments of necessary but in some cases unspeci-
behavior chains; one performs an ex- fied contingencies (identified by ques-
tensive series of behaviors in commut- tion marks), is shown in Figure 7. Only
ing, for example, that eventually result the parent and child may be present
in access to the reinforcers available at during the actual occurrence of the
home. Perhaps even more important scene of interest, but many extrascene
for our purposes, the occurrence of practices interlock with their behav-
many, perhaps most, cultural practices iors. Not all of the relevant contingen-
is heavily rule governed (see Malott, cies are noted on the diagram; readers
1988, and Malott & Garcia, 1991, for can identify additional practices and
further discussion). Many cultural resources that may be necessary or
practices involve verbal mediation and helpful at various points in the inter-
rule governance through which tem- locking cultural system. Identifying
porally distant antecedents and conse- such missing elements is important to
quences come to affect behavior. effective introduction and stabilization
268 MARK A. MATTAINI

Practice: Practice: < Practices: tuns off Practice: prOides


assigns homevwu* does haomer* TV, einforces socia reinforcement
Actor: Actor: Actor: Actor:
teacher child parent parent peer
Antecedents: Antecedents: Antecedents: Antecedents:
?qn honxvsiWO*k
aCo assignsed
quiet settjng (few rCo es fromn
cWn'ieking reinforcers) mi7lster, peer
Consequencea: Consequences: Consequences: Consequences:
itipv aswinged ,fresfromn rea,forcers from
by adnihstraotors parents,teachrs peers, nwnisters

BoT -ey of th scn

Practice: Practice: aorgnize Practice: Practice:


ang incentives pkdge meeting proide mterials ?
Actor: Actor: Actor: officsl of Actor:
school niistrator miister Jacksonor n|ization
Antecedents: Antecedents: Antecedents: Antecedents:
nAes providd by - EOs & SOs from ides
pants, mWtste parents, Jackson by organation
laders
coanunity oaaniation
Consequences: Consequences: Consequences: Consequences:
? social reinforcement orntional
f perents4 peers k,iantlis

Figure 7. Practices and interlocks involved in Jesse Jackson's "Back to School" pledge campaign.

of new cultural practices, and the dia- zation dedicated to universal voter reg-
gram makes it easy to do so, prompting istration) is to embed this practice,
the analyst to consider potential ways which would substantially reduce the
to fill in the gaps. response cost for clients, in the routine
In another example, voting by mem- organizational cultures of a wide vari-
bers of traditionally disempowered ety of agencies.
groups is a potentially significant prac- As can be seen in Figure 8, the key
tice. Voting in this country is actually scene involves an agency staff member
a two-step process: registration and providing the voter registration form,
voting itself. Current data suggest that the potential voter completing the
persons who are registered usually vote form, and the staff member putting the
in national elections, including those form in the outgoing mail. Although
who register to vote in social agencies this appears to be a fairly straightfor-
(Fawcett, Seekins, & Silber, 1988). ward process, it is not easy to institu-
This process is depicted in the whole- tionalize in practice (Conley & Freed-
part practice diagram in Figure 8. The man, 1995). Some of the problems re-
first step in the process (registration) is late to misunderstandings on the part
highlighted in the diagram; note, how- of clients, but the major issue appears
ever, that a similar analysis may be im- to be the contingencies involved in the
portant in developing strategies to sup- behavior of agency staff members.
port the second step (voting) as well. Given many competing demands, these
With the recent passage of the national often overworked individuals com-
Motor Voter Bill, the availability of monly fail to take the desired steps;
voter registration is mandated in cer- agency administrators, in turn, fail to
tain governmental offices, including consistently take steps that encourage
motor vehicle and welfare depart- staff to do so.
ments. The bill also encourages non- The practice diagram in Figure 8
profit social agencies to make registra- makes clear where the gaps are. Al-
tion available on-site, and a major goal though many national organizations
of Human Serve (a nonprofit organi- have taken strong positions in favor of
ENVISIONING CULTURAL PRACTICES 269

Practice: Voting
Practice: Provides form and IActor: Low,income citizen
instructions (rules)
Actor: Agency staff nember
Antecedents:
Rules provided by
agency administrators
Consequences:
Incentives provided by Practice: Casts vote
agency adminstrators
Actor. Low-income citizen

*1~~~~~~~~~~N**
Practice: Completes form Practice: Pndrvidcenles
Actor: Low-income citizen Actor: Agency Administrator
Antecedents: Antecedents: Rules from
Form provided (SD), national organizations, advocates
instructions from agency staff Consequences: Social incentives
from organizations, advocates
Consequences:
Relief from self-generated aversives,
social R+ frorn agency staff

Practice: Provide social incentives


Actor: Representative of
2 ** onational organization
Antecedents: Rules
from advocates
Consequences: Social R+ from
Practice: Mails form advocates, agency adminstrators
Actor: Agency staff member
Antecedents:
Rules provided by Practice: Provide strategic
agency administrators rules, social R+
Consequences: Actor: Registration advocates
Incentives provided by Antecedents: Rulesextractedby
agency adminstrators observation, empirical research
Consequences: Social R+ from
members for successes
Boundary of the Scene
Figure 8. Interlocking practices involved in facilitating voter registration in social agencies.

agency-based registration, few of their may be many creative ways to do this;


local affiliates have yet taken consis- some agencies are discussing experi-
tent action. What is required is provid- menting with charting progress, con-
ing the necessary antecedents, and es- tests, routine inclusion of these behav-
pecially incentives, for line staff. There iors in staff evaluations, and so forth.
270 MARK A. MATTAINI

Maintaining these contingencies, how- vention (say, sending teachers to a one-


ever, requires continuing action on the day training workshop) that targets a
part of administrators. (There may, of single practice rather than the overall
course, be multiple administrative lev- contingency network. The relation-
els present; they are collapsed into one ships among multiple variables emerge
for simplicity here, but in a particular clearly in the diagrams, which thereby
setting may need to be carefully elab- preclude oversimplification.
orated.) As the phenomena being analyzed
Although there are likely to be long- become more complicated, so do the
term advantages for agency staff from practice diagrams depicting them. The
registration, including shifting power utility of the diagrams is likely to be
to their clients and eliciting funding highest when the situation being ex-
support, these small, cumulative ad- plored is complex. The diagram rep-
vantages are unlikely to be effective re- resenting it may then require signifi-
inforcers for staff behavior. What may cant effort-and even training-to
be required is to identify ways that na- comprehend. Note, however, that this
tional organizations could provide is also true in other fields, including
more immediate and certain reinforcers medical imaging, computer systems
to agency administrators, perhaps by analysis, and meteorology, but those
publicizing rates of registration among who become skilled in the technology
constituent agencies, instituting con- come to read the images rapidly and
tests, or experimenting with other cre- efficiently. Achieving the pragmatical-
ative alternatives. Agency administra- ly most functional balance between
tors might then, in turn, provide re- simplicity and detail will require ex-
quired incentives for staff. perimentation, and no single answer
may be right for all circumstances.
CONCLUSION Moving toward the design of desir-
able cultural practices, in some cases
The analysis and design of cultural the analyst may want to work from di-
practices and the interlocking contin- agrams of the existing situation, iden-
gencies that support them are "new tifying currently active antecedents and
frontiers" for applied work in behavior consequences that might be transferred
analysis (Biglan, 1995; Mattaini, to the new practice. In other cases, one
1996)-in some ways a new subdiscip- may need to work outward from the
line that one reviewer described as desired practice, identifying necessary
"applied behaviorocultural analysis." contingencies to establish and maintain
Diagrams like those presented here it, then identifying those practices else-
may be useful for this challenging where in the network that are required
work in several ways. The complexi- to establish and maintain those contin-
ties involved in understanding the gencies, continuing outward until a sta-
problems of the American educational ble interlocking behavioral system
system, for example, are immense. But emerges.
they can be studied; it is possible to The physical preparation of dia-
identify current practices, and to sug- grams like those illustrated here need
gest and iteratively test variations in not be a complicated process. The cur-
antecedents and consequences that rent versions of standard word proces-
may be associated with more effective sors (e.g., WordPerfect® or Microsoft
alternatives (Greer, 1996). As data are Word@) include simple graphic tools
collected, they can be slotted into prac- for drawing rounded rectangles, lines,
tice diagrams until the overall config- and arrows and for embedding text
uration of the system begins to emerge. within the figures, and these are the
When the issues are contextualized in only elements required to construct
this fashion, scientists or policy makers practice diagrams. Drawing and saving
may be less likely to rely on an inter- a blank template can save time later.
ENVISIONING CULTURAL PRACTICES 271

When more than one person is working porary social problem: 65 million unregis-
with the diagram, we have sometimes tered citizens. Unpublished manuscript, Hu-
man Serve, Inc., 622 West 113th Street, New
found it useful to print the basic tem- York, NY 10025.
plate on transparencies, so the image Fawcett, S. B., Seekins, T., & Silber, L. (1988).
can be projected on the wall and grad- Low-income voter registration: A small-scale
ually and iteratively refined in the evaluation of an agency-based registration
course of discussions. Related infor- strategy. American Journal of Community
Psychology, 16, 751-758.
mation can be found in Mattaini Friedman, E. H. (1991). Bowen theory and
(1995a, 1995b). therapy. In A. S. Gurman & D. P. Kniskern
As is true for contingency diagrams, (Eds.), Handbook of family therapy (Vol. 2,
practice diagrams can be valuable pp. 134-170). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Glenn, S. S. (1988). Contingencies and meta-
teaching tools, in addition to their util- contingencies: Toward a synthesis of behavior
ity for analysis and cultural design. In analysis and cultural materialism. The Behav-
the author's community practice ior Analyst, 11, 161-179.
course, students develop and annotate Glenn, S. S. (1991). Relations among behav-
detailed contingency diagrams and ioral, cultural and biological evolution. In P.
A. Lamal (Ed.), Behavioral analysis of soci-
practice diagrams. They report that eties and cultural practices (pp. 39-73). New
these tools enhance both their under- York: Hemisphere.
standing of the cultural phenomena Glenn, S. S., & Malagodi, E. F. (1991). Process
with which they are dealing, as well as and content in behavioral and cultural phe-
nomena. Behavior and Social Issues, 1, 1-14.
their appreciation for the broad appli- Gould, S. J. (1995). Evolution by walking. Nat-
cability to important human questions ural History, 104(3), 10, 12-15.
of analyses rooted in the science of be- Greer, R. D. (1996). Our educational crisis:
havior (Mattaini, 1995b). Such appli- Contributing factors and solutions. In M. A.
cations may be the principal profes- Mattaini & B. A. Thyer (Eds.), Finding so-
lutions to social problems: Behavioral strat-
sional and sociocultural challenge fac- egies for change (pp. 13-146). Washington,
ing behavior analysts in the 21st cen- DC: APA Books.
tury. Harris, M. (1979). Cultural materialism: The
struggle for a science of culture. New York:
Vintage.
REFERENCES Harris, M. (1989). Our kind. New York:
HarperCollins.
Ainslie, G. (1993). A picoeconomic rationale Lamal, P. A. (Ed.). (1991). Behavioral analysis
for social constructionism. Behavior and Phi- of societies and cultural practices. New York:
losophy, 21(2), 63-75. Hemisphere.
Bertin, J. (1983). Semiology of graphics: Dia- Lee, V. L. (1994). Organisms, things done, and
grams networks maps. Madison: University of the fragmentation of psychology. Behavior
Wisconsin Press. and Philosophy, 22, 7-48.
Biglan, A. (1995). Changing culturalpractices: Malott, R. W. (1988). Rule-governed behavior
A contextualistframeworkfor intervention re- and behavioral anthropology. The Behavior
search. Reno, NV: Context Press. Analyst, 11, 181-203.
Canada, G. (1995). Fist, stick, knife, gun. Bos- Malott, R. W (1992a). Saving the world with
ton: Beacon Press. contingency diagraming. ABA Newsletter,
Catania, A. C., & Harnad, S. (Eds.). (1988). 15(1), 45.
The selection of behavior: The operant be- Malott, R. W. (1992b). The three-contingency
haviorism of B. F. Skinner: Comments and model of performance management. ABA
consequences. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni- Newsletter, 15(2), 6.
versity Press. Malott, R. W, & Garcia, M. E. (1991). Role of
Chisholm, D. C., & Cook, D. A. (1995). A private events in rule-governed behavior. In L.
model of cause-effect relations in the study of J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on
behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 99-111 . verbal behavior (pp. 237-254). Reno: Context
Citizen Education Fund. (no date). The national Press.
reclaim our youth crusade "Back to School" Malott, R. W., Whaley, D. L., & Malott, M. E.
pledge campaign. Citizen Education Fund, (1993). Elementary principles of behavior
1700 K Street, NW, Suite 802, Washington, (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice
DC 20006. Hall.
Coad, P., & Yourdon, E. (1991). Object-orient- Mattaini, M. A. (1993). More than a thousand
ed analysis (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: words: Graphics for clinical practice. Wash-
Yourdon Press. ington, DC: NASW Press.
Conley, J., & Freedman, N. (1995). Contem- Mattaini, M. A. (1 995a). Contingency diagrams
272 MARK A. MATTAINI
as teaching tools. The Behavior Analyst, 18, Patterson, G. R. (1976). The aggressive child:
93-98. Victim and architect of a coercive system. In
Mattaini, M. A. (1995b). Teaching cultural de- E. J. Mash, L. A. Hamerlynck, & L. C. Handy
sign: Shaping new behaviorists. Behavior and (Eds.), Behavior modification and families
Social Issues, 5, 21-28. (pp. 267-316). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Mattaini, M. A. (1996). Public issues, human Sema, L. A., Schumaker, J. B., Sherman, J. A.,
behavior, and cultural design. In M. A. Mat- & Sheldon, J. B. (1991). In-home generaliza-
taini & B. A. Thyer (Eds.), Finding solutions tion of social interactions in families of ado-
to social problems: Behavioral strategies for lescents with behavior problems. Journal of
change (pp. 13-40). Washington, DC: APA Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 733-746.
Books. Skinner, B. E (1957). Verbal behavior. Engle-
Mattaini, M. A., T`wyman, J. S., Chin, W., & wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Nam, K. (1996). Youth violence. In M. A. Skinner, B. F (1981). Selection by conse-
Mattaini & B. A. Thyer (Eds.), Finding so- quences. Science, 213, 501-504.
lutions to social problems: Behavioral strat- Skinner, B. F (1987). The evolution of behav-
egies for change (pp. 75-111). Washington, ior. In Upon further reflection (pp. 65-74).
DC: APA Books. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. (Reprint-
McDowell, J. J. (1988). Matching theory in nat- ed from Joumnal of the Experimental Analysis
ural human environments. The Behavior An- of Behavior, 41, 217-221, 1984)
alyst, 11, 95-109. Skinner, B. F (1989). The behavior of the lis-
McGinnies, E. (1970). Social behavior: A func- tener. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed be-
tional analysis. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. havior: Cognition, contingencies, and instruc-
Michael, J. (1995). What every student of be- tional control (pp. 85-96). New York: Ple-
havior analysis ought to learn: A system for num.
classifying the multiple effects of behavioral Sloane, H. N. (1988). The good kid book.
variables. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 273-284. Champaign, IL: Research Press.
Michael, J., & Shafer, E. (1995). State notation Tufte, E. R. (1983). The visual display of quan-
for teaching about behavioral procedures. The titative information. Cheshire, CT: Graphics
Behavior Analyst, 18, 123-140. Press.
Minuchin, S. (1974). Families andfamily ther- lIbfte, E. R. (1990). Envisioning information.
apy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Cheshire, CT: Graphics Press.
Press. Vargas, E. A. (1993). A science of our own
Minuchin, S., & Nichols, M. P. (1993). Family making. Behaviorology, 1, 13-22.
healing. New York: Free Press. Walsh, F (Ed.). (1993). Normal family process-
Newman, B. (1992). The reluctant alliance: Be- es. New York: Guilford.
haviorism and humanism. Buffalo, NY: Pro- Yourdon, E. (1989). Modern structured analy-
metheus. sis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Yourdon Press.

You might also like