You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [95.146.221.

128]
On: 28 November 2013, At: 23:56
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Creativity Research Journal


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcrj20

Changes in Children's Pretend Play Over Two Decades


a a
Sandra W. Russ & Jessica A. Dillon
a
Case Western Reserve University
Published online: 09 Nov 2011.

To cite this article: Sandra W. Russ & Jessica A. Dillon (2011) Changes in Children's Pretend Play Over Two Decades, Creativity
Research Journal, 23:4, 330-338, DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2011.621824

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.621824

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 23(4), 330–338, 2011
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1040-0419 print=1532-6934 online
DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2011.621824

Changes in Children’s Pretend Play Over Two Decades


Sandra W. Russ and Jessica A. Dillon
Case Western Reserve University

There is growing recognition that children have less time to engage in play, and, concur-
rently, recent evidence suggests a decrease in divergent thinking ability in young chil-
dren. This study investigated changes in pretend play ability during a 23-year period.
The same standardized measure of pretend play, the Affect in Play Scale (APS; Russ,
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

1993; 2004), was the measure of pretend play in all studies. This puppet play task is
videotaped and scored from the tapes. Fourteen studies of children from 6 to 10 years
of age in school-based samples from 1985–2008 were included in the analyses. A
cross-temporal meta-analysis examined correlations between weighted mean scores
and year of data collection. Main findings were that imagination in play and comfort
with play significantly increased over time. There was no evidence of change in organi-
zation of the story or in overall expression of affect in play. When one outlier was
removed, there was a significant decrease in negative affect expression in play. Even
though children have less time to play, cognitive processes that occur in play are con-
tinuing to develop. Whether these pretend abilities are being transferred to creative pro-
duction is a key question for future investigation.

Pretend play has important functions in child develop- pleasing (Sternberg, Kaufman, & Pretz, 2002).
ment. There is a consensus in the field that pretend Although children cannot make major creative contri-
play relates to many areas of child development butions to a discipline, they can produce ideas, art,
(Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff, 2003; Russ, 2004; Singer & and inventions that are novel and good for their age
Singer, 1990). For example, pretend play relates to cre- group. They can also express everyday creativity in
ative problem solving (Dansky, 1999), perspective- daily life. A number of theorists have thought of pre-
taking (Harris, 2000), coping ability (Russ, Robins, & tend play as being a source of creativity for children
Christiano, 1999), and emotion regulation (Berk, Mann, (Erikson, 1963; Fein, 1987; Singer & Singer, 1990;
& Ogman, 2006). One of the most robust findings in the Vygotsky, 1967).
literature is the relationship between pretend play and Pretend play has been defined as a symbolic behavior
divergent thinking (Fisher, 1992), and by extrapolation, in which ‘‘one thing is playfully treated as if it were
creativity. There is less consensus in the field as to something else’’ (Fein, 1987, p. 282). Pretend play
whether or not pretend play actually facilitates the involves make-believe, fantasy, imagination, and affect.
development of creativity. Nevertheless, many of the Fein thought that pretense is charged with emotion, so
processes we observe in play are important in creativity that affect is intertwined with pretend play. Cognitive
and child development. and affective processes that occur in play are also impor-
There is a long history of investigation into the tant in creativity (Russ, 1993, 2004). Divergent thinking
relationship between pretend play and creativity. Many is a cognitive process that occurs in both play and crea-
of the processes important in creative production occur tivity. Divergent thinking is the generation of a variety
in pretend play. A creative product is defined as being of ideas and solutions to problems (Guilford, 1968).
both novel and appropriate to the task or aesthetically By generating many ideas, some requiring remote asso-
ciations, the likelihood of coming up with a novel idea is
increased. Singer and Singer (1990) described pretend
Correspondence should be sent to Sandra W. Russ, Department of
Psychology, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH 44106- play as practice with divergent thinking. In pretend play,
7123. E-mail: swr@case.edu children generate many different stories and transform
CHANGES IN PRETEND PLAY 331

objects into representations of different things. The child DECREASING PLAY TIME
is playing with ideas in their imagination and recombin-
ing images and ideas into new configurations, which is One implication of the impact of play on creativity fol-
also an important component of creativity (Guilford, lows from the growing recognition that children have
1968). less time to themselves to engage in free play activities.
Affective processes also occur in play and are impor- Over the past few decades, the amount of free play for
tant in creative production. Children express positive children has decreased (Hirsh-Pasek & Golinkoff,
(happiness, affection) and negative (anger, sadness, fear) 2003; Hirsh-Pasek, Golonkoff, Berk, & Singer, 2009).
emotions in play. Affect-laden themes and images (mon- Research examining how time is allocated in elementary
sters, food, guns) are also expressed in fantasy play. Fein schools confirms that play time is swiftly disappearing
(1987) described how children manipulate affect-laden from many classrooms in America, despite evidence sug-
symbols in pretend play and that this affective symbol gesting play time is essential. According to surveys con-
system was important in creativity. Access to ducted by the National Association of Elementary
affect-laden ideas, thoughts, and memories could be School Principals (NAESP), although 96% of school
especially important for creativity in the arts. Positive systems reported having at least one recess period in
affect facilitates creativity in a number of studies (Baas, 1989, this number reduced to only 70% of school sys-
Carsten, & Nijstad, 2008). Negative affect themes in pre- tems just 10 years later. More recently, a 2010 Gallup
tend play have related to divergent thinking (Russ & poll sponsored by the Robert Wood Johnson Foun-
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

Schafer, 2006). Finally, in play, children become dation, NAESP, and Playworks surveying 1,951 schools,
absorbed in the task and often experience deep pleasure. found that 92% of schools reported having recess, but
This is similar to the flow experience described by over half of the respondents reported that their school
Csikszentmihalyi (1990) as being part of the creative had 30 minutes or less for recess each day, suggesting
experience. These are just some of the reasons that pre- that children are receiving minimal or no time for
tend play should be related to divergent thinking and unstructured or child-directed activities during the day.
creativity in children. Yet, although play time is being squeezed out of cur-
Many research studies have found relationships ricula, this same 2010 study found that 8 out of 10 prin-
between pretend play and creativity in both preschool cipals report recess as having a positive impact on
and elementary school populations, usually inde- academic achievement, and two-thirds of the principals
pendent of intelligence (Russ, 2004). In a meta-analysis reported that students listened and focused better after
of 46 play studies, Fisher (1992) found the largest effect recess. Furthermore, 96% of principals reported believ-
sizes for relationships between play and divergent ing that recess had a positive impact on both social
thinking (ES ¼ .387) and perspective-taking (ES ¼ development and student’s overall well-being. Thus,
.392). Both cognitive processes and affective processes there is a puzzling discrepancy between the research
in play have related to divergent thinking. Children and principals’ views, and the reality of how school sys-
who have more imagination in play, more organized tems are currently being run. Despite a clearly observed
make-believe stories, and more emotion and affect- connection between recess and positive student
laden themes generate more responses and more orig- socio-emotional and academic achievement, 77% of
inal responses on divergent thinking tests (Russ & principals reported that their school takes away recess
Grossman-McKee, 1990). There is some support for as a typical punishment, and 20% of principals reported
stability of the association between play and divergent that they had decreased recess time due to annual aca-
thinking over time as well. For example, imagination demic testing.
in first- and second-grade children predicted divergent One can imagine the many reasons why schools are
thinking in the 5th and 6th grade (r ¼ .42, p < .01; Russ struggling to provide unstructured play time; in the
et al., 1999). age of the No Child Left Behind Act, schools are under
An important question is whether engaging in pre- increased pressure to produce strong test scores, and to
tend play only reflects creative processes or also facili- pack every minute of the day with structured material.
tates the development of those processes. There are Additionally, many schools struggle with funding for
some experimental studies in the literature that found better playgrounds and enough teachers to monitor play
that pretend play facilitates insight in problem-solving time. But the trend toward less play time is not solely the
(Vandenberg, 1980) and divergent thinking (Dansky, fault of schools or government regulations: Parents, too,
1980). Although there are many methodological chal- seem oriented toward childhoods with less time for play.
lenges with this type of research, Dansky (1999) con- After a review of the research in a report by the
cluded that there were well-done studies that American Academy of Pediatrics, Ginsburg (2007) out-
controlled for experimenter effects that found that pre- lined a range of possible factors contributing to this
tend play did facilitate creative thinking. reduction in play. Among the factors is the increase in
332 RUSS AND DILLON

single parents or households in which both parents that you can use. Be sure to have the puppets talk out
work, leaving children in child care or after-school loud. The video camera will be on so that I can remem-
activities more hours out of the day, or to spend more ber what you say and do. I will tell you when to stop. Go
hours in front of the television or video game console. ahead.
Furthermore, American culture increasingly views good
parenting as building a child’s skills, especially academic The researcher does not engage with the child in the
skills, and producing as many opportunities for them as play, and follows standard prompts during the 5-minute
possible. College admissions and preschool admissions period.
for that matter, promote the pressure that parents feel The APS measures pretend play ability with five
to help their child be well-rounded and well-prepared. scores:
More practically, for some children the communities in
which they live do not allow them to play safely outside. 1. Organization rates the child’s play (1–5) on the
There is growing recognition that children have less quality and complexity of the plot, considering
time to play (Elkind, 2007). This decrease in play time cause and effect and plot integration.
has been widely documented. The Academy of Pedi- 2. Imagination rates the child’s play (1–5) on its
atrics has called for an increasing recognition of the fantasy elements, block transformations, and
importance of play in child development (Ginsberg, number of novel ideas, characters, or events.
2007). There is little research on actual play ability in 3. Comfort is a rating (1–5) of the child’s comfort,
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

children during this period of decreasing play time. Is involvement in the play and enjoyment of the
the decrease in pretend play time resulting in poorer pre- play.
tend play ability? There is some evidence reported by 4. Frequency of Affect is a tally count of the num-
Kim (2011) that there has been a decrease in creativity. ber of units of verbal and nonverbal emotion
What about pretend play? We were in a unique position expressed during the play. A unit is defined as
of having samples of pretend play of various child popu- one expression by an individual puppet that can
lations during a 23-year period that used the same stan- be scored. For example, every time one puppet
dardized play task with standardized administration and said, ‘‘I like you’’ or ‘‘I hate this’’ a unit is coded.
scoring in all studies. The purpose of this investigation The total number of units expressed during the
was to compare play ability in children from 6 to 10 5-minute period is the frequency of affect score.
years of age during a 23-year period to determine Frequency of positive affect units and negative
whether play processes are improving, staying the same, affect units are also obtained.
or decreasing. The Affect in Play Scale (APS) was the 5. Variety of Affect is a tally count of the number of
measure used in all studies. affect categories that are expressed during the
play, drawing from 11 categories: Anger=
Aggression, Anxiety=Fear, Happiness=Pleasure,
Frustration=Dislike=Disappointment, Sadness=
METHOD Hurt, Oral, Oral Aggression, Anal, Competition,
Sexual, and Nurturance=Affection. These cate-
Measure gories can be combined into positive affect
The measure used in this study was the APS (Russ, 1993; expressions and negative affect expressions.
2004), a 5-min, unstructured play task that measured
both cognitive and affective processes in play. Each An extensive scoring manual has been developed for the
child was met with individually and given two scale (Russ, 1993; 2004). Raters achieve good interrater
neutral-looking puppets and three blocks with which reliability (usually in the .80 s and .90 s). Split-half
to play however he or she would like. The task is appro- reliability for frequency of affect was good (r ¼ .85; Seja
priate for children ages 6–10, in grades 1 to 4. The play & Russ, 1999). A variety of validity studies have been
was videotaped and scored according to criteria in a carried out with different researchers and child popula-
scoring manual. The APS is relatively unstructured so tions that have found relationships between the APS
that there is room for the child to structure the play and theoretically relevant criteria. For example, the
and present affect themes that are habitual to him or APS has been related to divergent thinking, coping abil-
her. The instructions for the task are: ity, amount of emotion in memories, and emotional
understanding (Russ & Grossman-McKee, 1990; Russ
I am here to learn about how children play. I have here 2 et al., 1999; Russ & Schafer, 2006; Seja & Russ, 1999,
puppets and would like you to play with them any way respectively). Most of the relationships between the
you like for 5 minutes. For example, you can have the APS and criteria were independent of intelligence. The
puppets do something together. I also have some blocks APS has not related to verbal intelligence in most
CHANGES IN PRETEND PLAY 333

studies. Occasionally, there has been a low positive Data Analytic Strategy
correlation between imagination and verbal IQ. Several
We analyzed how the five main APS scores may have
studies have supported the conceptualization behind the
changed over time. This was done by performing a
scale of two separate but related factors, one cognitive
cross-temporal meta-analysis, examining correlations
and one affective (Russ, 2004).
between mean scores and year of data collection. The
All the studies included in this comparison used the
means were weighted by the sample size of each study
same props, administration, and scoring manual. Only
to give better estimates of the population mean. This
very minor scoring changes have been made over the
data analytic procedure followed that used by Jean M.
years.
Twenge examining similar changes in test scores over
time (Twenge, Konrath, Foster, Campbell, & Bushman,
Inclusion of Studies 2008; Twenge, Zhang, & Im, 2004).
The analyses were performed using SPSS. It is impor-
All studies that used the APS with a school-based popu-
tant to note that there were varying numbers of data
lation were included. There were a total of 14 studies
points for each of the main APS scores because different
over a 23-year period from 1985 to 2008. These included
studies focused on different scores, and not every study
published studies, as well as unpublished Masters Theses
used or reported every score. For example, some studies
and Dissertations. All were carried out in the research
did not examine positive or negative affect scores, thus
program of Sandra Russ at Case Western Reserve Uni-
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

the number of studies included in the cross-temporal


versity. There were a number of different examiners and
meta-analysis for these scores is fewer. Table 2 presents
raters. All raters had been trained and checked for
a breakdown of APS mean scores by study and year.
adequate interrater reliability.
We also calculated the magnitude of the change in
Possible data points for the analysis were included or
APS scores, by using regression equations and the aver-
excluded on the basis of specific inclusion rules. To be
aged standard deviation (SD) of the individual samples.
included in the analysis, a study had to meet the following
To find the mean scores for specific years, we used the
criteria: (a) participants were children between the ages of
regression equation produced by the statistical analyses
6 and 10; (b) participants were taken from school-based
to draw the regression line. The regression equation fol-
samples, not clinical groups or at-risk populations; (c)
lows the formula: y ¼ Bx þ C, where y ¼ the predicted
means were reported for unselected groups of parti-
mean APS score, B ¼ the unstandardized regression
cipants, not those chosen for scoring in any particular
coefficient, x ¼ the year, and C ¼ the constant or inter-
way on the APS or any other measure; and (d) the study
cept. This formula yielded the position of the regression
had used the original version of the APS, not the brief
line for certain years. We then calculated the difference
rating version or the preschool version. Table 1 lists the
between a score’s predicted value in the earliest year that
author(s), year the APS was administered, publication
score was obtained and the predicted value in the most
status, number of children in the sample, type of school,
recent year that score was used, obtaining a number
and grades included in the study.

TABLE 1
Data Sets Included in the Analysis

Sources Year Data Collected Published n School Type Grade

Grossman-McKee (1985); Russ and Grossman-McKee 1990) 1985 Yes 60 Suburban public 1, 2
Grossman-McKee (1989) 1988 No 89 Military base 1, 2
Russ and Peterson (1988); Russ, Robins, and Christiano (1999) 1988 Yes 121 Suburban public 1, 2
Niec (1994); Niec and Russ (1996) 1993 Yes 49 Suburban parochial 1, 2, 3
Russ and Kaugars (2001) 1995 Yes 80 Suburban public 1, 2
Niec (1998); Niec and Russ (2002) 1996 Yes 86 Suburban public 3, 4
Seja (1998); Seja and Russ (1999) 1997 Yes 66 Suburban public 1, 2
Goldstein (1999); Goldstein and Russ (2001) 1997 Yes 55 Suburban public 1
Russ and Schafer (2006) 1999 Yes 46 Suburban public 1, 2
Goldstein (2002) 1999 No 76 Suburban public 3, 4
Russ, Moore, and Pearson (2007) 2002 No 45 Inner city public 1, 2
Sacha Cordiano, Russ, and Short (2008) 2006 Yes 30 Suburban parochial 1, 2
Cain Spannagel, Russ, and Niec (2008); Niec and Spannagel (2006) 2006 No 58 Suburban parochial 1, 2, 3, 4
Christian, K. (2009) 2008 No 33 Suburban parochial 1, 2, 3
Inner city parochial
334 RUSS AND DILLON

TABLE 2
Affect in Play Scale (APS) Mean Scores by Study

Affect in Play Scale (APS) Scores

Frequency Variety Positive Negative


Sources Year n Organization Imagination Comfort of Affect of Affect Affect Affect

Grossman-McKee (1985); Russ, and 1985 60 2.03 2.40 12.35


Grossman-McKee (1990)
Grossman-McKee (1989) 1988 89 2.46 1.90 2.33
Russ and Peterson (1988); Russ, Robins, 1988 121 2.76 2.58 2.96 10.99 2.97
and Christiano (1999)
Niec (1994); Niec and Russ (1996) 1993 49 2.41 13.96 2.57
Russ and Kaugars (2001) 1995 80 14.80 8.50 6.30
Niec (1998); Niec and Russ (2002) 1996 86 2.15 2.20 2.78 16.58 3.58
Seja (1998); Seja and Russ (1999) 1997 66 2.48 2.41 3.53 12.71 2.77
Goldstein (1999); Goldstein and Russ (2001) 1997 55 2.40
Russ and Schafer (2006) 1999 46 2.26 2.57 3.13 9.04 5.00 4.17
Goldstein, A. (2002) 1999 76 22.23 4.01 7.76 14.47
Russ, Moore, and Pearson (2007) 2002 45 3.16 2.91 3.22 8.78 2.82 5.58 3.20
Sacha Cordiano, Russ, and Short (2008) 2006 30 2.25 2.54 3.04 11.07
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

Cain Spannagel, Russ, and Niec (2008); Niec 2006 58 2.93 2.76 3.57 7.72 2.66 5.38 2.34
and Spannagel (2006)
Christian (2009) 2008 33 2.73 2.82 3.21 9.39 2.48 6.82 2.58

representing the predicted amount a score would change Statistical analyses gave an unstandardized regression
across those years. coefficient (B) of .006 and a constant (C) of 9.86. The
We also calculated the average SD by averaging the regression equation (y ¼ Bx þ C, detailed in the method)
within-sample SDs reported in the data sources; thus, is: Predicted APS Organization score ¼ .006  year of
this reflected the average variance of the measure in a data collection 9.86. When the oldest and most recent
sample of individuals. The previously calculated pre- years with data on Organization were plugged into the
dicted amount that a score would change across years regression equation, APS Organization scores of 2.07
was divided by this average variance across those years, for the year 1988 and 2.19 for the year 2008 were pre-
to determine the number of SDs a score was predicted to dicted. By subtracting the predicted score for 1988 from
shift across time. This, too, was the exact procedure the predicted score for 2008, we obtained a difference of
used by Twenge et al. (2008). 0.12, meaning that the regression equation, being linear,
It is important to note, as Twenge et al. (2008), predicted the APS Organization score would increase
pointed out, that this method avoids the ecological fal- 0.12 every 20 years.
lacy, also known as alerting correlations (Rosenthal, This predicted change over time is more meaningful
Rosnow, & Rubin, 2000) by using the SD of the individ- when reported in terms of how many SDs the score has
ual studies to capture the variance of the scale, rather changed or is predicted to change. For APS Organization
than using the variation in mean scores. scores, the average SD reported for the individual sam-
ples was 1.33. We divided the APS Organization mean
score change of 0.12 units by the average SD of 1.33, to
calculate that APS Organization scores have changed
RESULTS .09 SDs over the 20 years from 1988 to 2008. This further
confirmed the nonsignificant relationship between year
Organization and children’s organization in pretend play.
For children ages 6 to 10 in nonclinical samples, there
was no evidence of change in their scores of Organiza-
Imagination
tion on the APS across time. No significant correlation
was found between APS scores of Organization and Unlike Organization scores, children ages 6 to 10 from
the year of data collection when weighted by sample size nonclinical samples were found to score progressively
(b ¼ .146, p ¼ .707, k ¼ 9). No correlation between score higher on Imagination in pretend play between 1985
and time suggests that there has not been a significant and 2008 (see Figure 1). There was a significant and
change in how well children are able to organize their positive correlation between APS scores of Imagination
pretend play over time, from 1988 to 2008. and year of data collection when weighted by sample
CHANGES IN PRETEND PLAY 335

FIGURE 1 Children’s imagination scores over time. FIGURE 2 Children’s comfort scores over time.
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

size (b ¼ .663, p ¼ .026, k ¼ 11). Thus, more recently, sample size (b ¼ .132, p ¼ .659, k ¼ 11). Thus, there
children were more imaginative in their play. was no evidence of a change in how much affect children
The regression equation produced by the statistical express during their pretend play over time. The unstan-
analysis is: Predicted APS mean Imagination scor- dardized regression coefficient was .081, thus the
e ¼ .028  year 53.65. The regression equation yielded regression equation (predicted APS frequency of affect
a score of 1.93 for the year 1985, and 2.57 for the year score ¼ .081  year þ 175.38) yielded a score of 14.60
2008. The average SD reported for the individual sam- for 1985, and 12.73 for 2008, a difference of 1.87. The
ples (from all the studies collected) was 1.25. Thus, average SD reported for the individual samples was
APS scores of Imagination increased 0.51 SDs from 14.22. Thus, APS scores of Frequency of Affect
1986 to 2008. This is a medium effect size using Cohen’s decreased 0.13 SDs from 1985 to 2008, further confirming
(1977) guidelines. no effect of time on frequency of affect in pretend play.

Comfort Variety of Affect


Children also scored progressively higher on scores of No changes in children’s scores on the Variety of Affect
Comfort between 1985 and 2008 (see Figure 2). There expressed on the APS across time were found. No signifi-
was a significant and positive correlation between APS cant correlation was found between APS scores of Fre-
scores of Comfort and the year of data collection when quency of Affect and the year of data collection when
weighted by sample size (b ¼ .710, p ¼ .014, k ¼ 11). weighted by sample size (b ¼ .107, p ¼ .801, k ¼ 8).
More recently, children displayed more comfort engag- The regression equation (predicted APS variety of affect
ing in fantasy play. The regression equation (predicted score ¼ .009  year þ 20.16) yielded a score of 2.27 for
APS mean imagination score ¼ .041  year 78.87) 1988, and a score of 2.09 for 2008, a difference of 0.19.
yielded a score of 2.52 for the year 1985 and 3.46 for The average SD reported for the individual samples
the year 2008. The average SD reported for the individual was 2.05. Thus, APS scores of Variety of Affect decreased
samples (from all the studies collected) was 1.40. Thus, .09 SDs from 1988 to 2008, confirming no effect of time
APS scores of Comfort increased 0.67 SDs from 1985 on variety of affect expressed during pretend play.
to 2008. This is a medium-to-large effect size (between
.50 and .80) according to Cohen’s (1977) guidelines.
Positive Affect
No evidence of changes in scores of positive affect
Frequency of Affect
across time was found. No significant correlation was
Children, ages 6 to 10, from nonclinical samples showed found between APS scores of Positive Affect and the
no clear directional change in their scores on Frequency year of data collection when weighted by sample size
of Affect on the APS across time. No significant corre- (b ¼ .627, p ¼ .183, k ¼ 6). Thus, there has not been a
lation was found between APS scores of Frequency of change in the amount of positive affect that children
Affect and year of data collection when weighted by express during their pretend play across time. The
336 RUSS AND DILLON

regression equation (predicted APS positive affect


score ¼ .197  year þ 401.27) yielded a score of 8.26
for 1995 and a score of 5.69 for 2008. The average SD
reported for the individual samples was 6.91; therefore,
APS scores of Positive Affect have decreased .37 SDs
from 1995 to 2008, confirming no effect of time on the
amount of positive affect expressed during pretend play.

Negative Affect
No significant correlation was found between APS
scores of Negative Affect and the year of data collection
when weighted by sample size (b ¼ .472, p ¼ .345,
k ¼ 6). However, the regression equation (predicted
APS score of negative affect ¼ .496  year þ 999.02)
yielded a score of 9.50 for 1995 and a score of 3.05 for
2008. The average SD reported for the individual sam- FIGURE 3 Children’s negative affect scores over time with outlier
ples was 9.29, therefore, APS scores of Negative Affect removed.
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

have decreased .69 SDs from 1995 to 2008, a medium-


to-large effect size (between .50 and .80) according to that these cognitive and affective processes in play have
Cohen’s (1977) guidelines. remained the same or improved, given the decline in
An examination of Table 2 revealed that one data set unstructured time for children.
(Goldstein, 1999) had an unusually high negative affect What can we make of these results in light of the fact
score; the rest of the data points showed a relatively lin- that there has been a reduction in time for free play over
ear decrease in Negative Affect scores over time. To these decades? There are several possible explanations.
explore this further, the Goldstein (1999) data point First, children are resilient and may be finding ways to
whose score was unusually high was removed from the develop imagination and make-believe abilities other than
analysis. With this study removed, the regression equa- through play. These abilities are then reflected in the pre-
tion was recalculated, showing a significant negative tend play task. For example, Singer and Singer (2005)
relationship between time and Negative affect explored whether the use of videogames and computer
expression (b ¼ .318, p ¼ .01, k ¼ 5). This result sug- games could enhance imagination in some ways. It is also
gests that the amount of negative affect that children possible that children are finding time to play in spite of
express during pretend play is decreasing over time their heavily structured schedules. Elkind (2007) sug-
(see Figure 3). The regression equation (predicted APS gested that children maintain the same desire to play, even
score of negative affect ¼ .318  year þ 640.63) yielded though there are fewer opportunities to play. Children
a score of 6.22 for 1995 and a score of 2.09 for 2008. continue to play games and create learning experiences.
Since the average SD reported for the individual samples It is also possible that the complexities and challenges
changed to 7.35, APS scores of Negative Affect have of modern culture require creative problem-solving and
decreased .56 SDs from 1995 to 2008, again producing imagination to function from day to day.
a medium-to-large effect size (between .50 and .80) Second, it is possible that the play task used in these
according to Cohen’s (1977) guidelines. Similar analysis studies is not sensitive to aspects of play that are in
excluding Goldstein (1999) did not change results for the decline. For example, the imagination scale is a 1–5
other APS scores. Likert scale that is a global measure of imagination in
play. Perhaps if one looked at specific imagination beha-
viors, like number of transformations of the blocks, the
DISCUSSION results would differ. However, this global rating is based
upon consideration of a number of specific indicators of
The main findings of this study were that there was no imagination and has related to measures of divergent
evidence of change in some aspects of children’s pretend thinking in a number of studies.
play and improvements in other areas. There has been Third, it is possible that there were other differences
no evidence of change in children’s pretend play, from between the most recent samples of children and earlier
1985 through 2008, in terms of organization of the play samples that confounded the results. More recent sam-
story and amount and range of affect expression. ples were parochial schools when compared to earlier
Imagination has increased in recent play samples as samples. However, the type of neighborhood was similar
has comfort engaging in the play task. It is encouraging to earlier samples. Because there are only 14 studies, it is
CHANGES IN PRETEND PLAY 337

possible that these samples are not representative of the Berk, M., Mann, T., & Ogman, A. (2006). Make-believe play: Well-
larger population. spring for development of self-regulation. In D. Singer, R.
Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Play ¼ Learning: How plan
The finding that negative affect expression in play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social–emotional
decreased, after removing one outlier, is suggestive of growth (pp. 74–100). New York: Oxford University Press.
a downward trend and should be explored in future 
Cain Spannagel, S. A., Russ, S. W., & Niec, L. N. (2008, August). The
research. If this finding is supported with more play stu- relationship between pretend play, prosocial moral reasoning, and
dies, then the implications are important. The prosocial behavior. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the
American Psychological Association, Boston, MA.
expression of negative affect themes in play has related 
Christian, K. (2009). Effects of anxious mood induction on play pro-
to divergent thinking (Russ & Schafer, 2006). Russ cesses. Unpublished master’s thesis, Case Western Reserve Univer-
and Schafer also found that positive and negative affect sity, Cleveland, OH.
themes related to expression of emotions in memory Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
narratives. Russ and Grossman-McKee (1990) found a (rev. ed.). New York: Academic Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experi-
relationship between affect-laden primary process think- ence. Grand Rapids, MI: Harper and Row.
ing on the Rorschach and affect in play. The results of Dansky, J. (1980). Make-believe: A mediator of the relationship between
these two studies suggest that expression of affect and play and associative fluency. Child Development, 51, 576–579.
affect-laden themes is a cross-situational ability. Access Dansky, J. (1999). Play. In M. Runco & S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclo-
to affect-laden imagery could be especially important to pedia of creativity (pp. 393–408). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Elkind, D. (2007). The power of play: Learning what comes naturally.
creativity in the arts (Russ, 2009). Constriction of nega-
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

New York: Da Capo.


tive affect expression in play, if supported in future Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
research, could have consequences for creative Fein, G. (1987). Pretend play: Creativity and consciousness. In P.
expression. It is important to point out that negative Gorlitz & J. Wohlwill (Eds.), Curiosity, imagination and play (pp.
affect expression in pretend play is still pretend and chil- 281–304). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Fisher, E. (1992). The impact of play on development: A
dren know it is pretend. Many negative affect themes are meta-analysis. Play and Culture, 5, 159–181.
accompanied by positive affect (e.g., a child might say Ginsburg, K. R. (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy
that a shark is happily eating the small fish). child development and maintaining strong parent–child bonds.
In general, the major findings of this study are not con- Pediatrics, 119, 182–191.

sistent with the recent findings reported by Kim (2011) Goldstein, A. (1999). Children’s fantasy ability and its relationship to
understanding literature. Unpublished master’s thesis, Case Western
that creativity scores, as measured by the Torrance Tests Reserve University, Cleveland, OH.
of Creativity (Torrance, Ball, & Saften, 1992) have signifi- 
Goldstein, A. (2002). The effect of affect-laden reading passages on
cantly decreased since 1990. Kim reported that this children’s emotional expressivity in play. Unpublished doctoral dis-
decrease was especially clear in younger children, kinder- sertation, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH.
garten through sixth grade. Given the large body of Goldstein, A., & Russ, S. W. (2001). Understanding children’s litera-
ture and its relationship to fantasy ability and coping. Imagination,
research that shows that pretend play ability correlates Cognition, and Personality, 20, 105–126.
with creativity, with the APS as well, how do we account 
Grossman-McKee, A. (1985). Affective expression in fantasy play and
for the finding that play ability is either stable or increas- its relationship to divergent thinking and achievement: an exploration
ing? One explanation could be that even though children of sex differences among first and second grade children. Unpublished
are finding ways to develop different abilities in play, they master’s thesis, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH.

Grossman-McKee, A. (1989). The relationship between affective
are not using the abilities to transfer to creativity tasks. expression in fantasy play and pain complaints in first and second
This possibility should be explored in future research. grade boys. (Doctoral Dissertation, Case Western Reserve Univer-
The findings with these samples of children that play sity, 1989). Dissertation Abstracts International, 50.
ability has remained stable or increased is encouraging Guilford, J. P. (1968). Intelligence, creativity, and their education impli-
and may speak to the resilience of children living in com- cations. San Diego, CA: Knapp.
Harris, P. (2000). The work of the imagination. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
plex times. Nevertheless, given recent evidence that play Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2003). Einstein never used flash-
time can facilitate development, society should continue cards: How our children really learn—and why they need to play more
to address the issue of reduced time for unstructured play. and memorize less (Revised ed.). Emmaus, PA: Rodale.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R., Berk, L., & Singer, D. (2009). A man-
date for playful learning. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.
REFERENCES Kim, K. H. (2011, August). Creativity crisis in children. Paper presented
at meeting of American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.

Niec, L. (1994). The relationship between affect and interpersonal themes
Data sets included in the meta-analysis are preceded by in children’s fantasy and interpersonal functioning. Unpublished mas-
an asterisk. ter’s thesis, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH.

Niec, L. (1998). Relationships among children’s internal representa-
Baas, M., Carsten, K. W., & Nijstad. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 tions, affect and fantasy in play, and interpersonal functioning.
years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone activation or regu- Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Case Western Reserve Univer-
latory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806. sity, Cleveland, OH.
338 RUSS AND DILLON

Niec, L., & Russ, S. (1996). Relationship among affect in play, inter- Russ, S. W., Robins, A., & Christiano, B. (1999). Pretend play: Longi-
personal themes in fantasy and children’s interpersonal behavior. tudinal prediction of creativity and affect in fantasy in children.
Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 645–649. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 129–139.

Niec, L., & Russ, S. (2002). Children’s internal representations, empa- Russ, S. W., & Schafer, E. D. (2006). Affect in fantasy play, emotion
thy and fantasy play: A validity study of the SCORS-Q. Psychologi- in memories, and divergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal,
cal Assessment, 14, 331–338. 18, 347–354.

Niec, L., & Spannagel, S. (2006). [Pretend play, prosocial moral Sacha Cordiano, T. J., Russ, S. W., & Short, E. J. (2008). Develop-
reasoning, and prosocial behavior]. Unpublished raw data. ment and validation of the Affect in Play Scale–Brief Rating
Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. (2010, February). The State of Version (APS-BR). Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 52–60.

Play: Gallup Survey of Principals on School Recess. Retrieved Seja, A. L. (1998). Children’s fantasy play and emotional understand-
September 5, 2010 from http://d6test.naesp.org/resources/1/ ing. Unpublished master’s thesis, Case Western Reserve University,
Gallup_Poll/StateOfPlayFeb2010.pdf Cleveland, OH.
Rosenthal, R., Rosnow, R. L., & Rubin, D. R. (2000). Contrasts Seja, A. L., & Russ, S. W. (1999). Children’s fantasy play and emotion-
and effect sizes in research: A correlational approach. New York: al understanding. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 269–277.
Cambridge University Press. Singer, D. G., & Singer, J. L. (1990). The house of make-believe: Chil-
Russ, S. W. (1993). Affect in creativity: The role of affect and play in the dren’s play and the developing imagination. Cambridge, MA:
creative process. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Harvard University Press.
Russ, S. W. (2004). Play in child development and psychotherapy: Singer, D., & Singer, J. (2005). Imagination and play in the electronic
Toward empirically supported practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence age. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2002). The creativity
Russ, S. W. (2009). Pretend play, emotional processes and developing conundrum. New York: Psychology Press.
Downloaded by [95.146.221.128] at 23:56 28 November 2013

narratives. In S. Kaufman & J. Kaufman, (Eds.), The psychology of Torrance, E. P., Ball, E. O., & Safter, H. T. (1992). Torrance Tests
creative writing (pp. 247–263). New York: Cambridge University of Creative Thinking: Streamlined score guide Figural A and B.
Press. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.
Russ, S. W., & Grossman-McKee, A. (1990). Affective expression in Twenge, J. M., Konrath, S., Foster, J. D., Campbell, W. K., &
children’s fantasy play, primary process thinking on the Rorschach, Bushman, B. J. (2008). Egos inflating over time: A cross-temporal
and divergent thinking. Journal of Personality Assessment, 54, meta-analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of
756–771. Personality, 76, 875–901.

Russ, S. W., & Kaugars, A. (2001). Emotion in children’s play Twenge, J. M., Zhang, L., & Im, C. (2004). It’s beyond my control: A
and creative problem solving. Creativity Research Journal, 13, cross-temporal meta-analysis of increasing externality in locus of con-
211–219. trol, 1960–2002. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 308–319.

Russ, S. W., Moore, M., & Pearson, B. (2007). The effects of play Vandenberg, B. (1980). Play, problem-solving, and creativity. New
training on play, creativity and emotional processes. Unpublished Directions for Child Development, 9, 49–68.
manuscript, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH. Vygotsky, L. S. (1967). Vaobraszeniye i tvorchestvov deskom voraste

Russ, S. W., & Peterson, N. (1988). [The Affect in Play Scale: Predict- [Imagination and creativity in childhood]. Moscow: Prosvescheniye.
ing creativity and coping in children]. Unpublished raw data. (Original work published 1930).

You might also like