You are on page 1of 22

Fingerpoint-Reading of Memorized Text: What Enables Beginners to Process the Print?

Author(s): Linnea C. Ehri and Jennifer Sweet


Source: Reading Research Quarterly, Vol. 26, No. 4 (Autumn, 1991), pp. 442-462
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the International Reading Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/747897 .
Accessed: 22/06/2014 03:39

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and International Reading Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to Reading Research Quarterly.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
442

? InternationalReadingAssociation7031-4620/91/US$1.25+ .00
LinneaC. Ehri
JenniferSweet
Universityof California,Davis

Fingerpoint-reading of memorized text:


What enables beginners to process the print?
IN A LABORATORY study, we investigatedthe kinds of print-relatedknowledgethat emergent
readersmust possess in order to learn to point to the words of a text as they recited it from
memory (fingerpoint-reading)and to rememberinformationabout the print from this activ-
ity. Childrenwhose ages rangedfrom 4.5 to 6 years completedseveraltests of readingskill.
Then they practiced fingerpoint-readinga simple text they had memorized. We assessed
the impact of this practice on their reading capabilities with the text. Regression analyses
revealedthat differenttypes of print knowledge facilitateddifferent aspects of fingerpoint-
reading.Knowinghow to reada few preprimerwords was importantfor learningto readnew
words in the text. Phonemic segmentationwas importantfor learning to point to printed
words at the same time as they were spoken, and for rememberinghow to read individual
words in text. Letter knowledge was importantfor noticing that letters in the text had been
altered,and for locatingwordsin text. These resultshelp to unravelthe complex relationships
between various knowledgesources as they are used by beginningreadersto process written
text in a focused word-by-wordmanner.

Lire en suivant du doigt des textes memorises: Comment les lecteurs


dkbutants traitent-ils l&crit?
DANS UNE RECHERCHEen laboratoire, nousavons6tudieles typesde connaissances reli6es a
l'6critque les apprentis-lecteursdoivent poss6derpour apprendrea pointerdu doigt en lisant
des mots d'untexte qu'ilsont au pr6alablem6moris6set pour se souvenirde l'informationsur
l'6crit acquise a travers cette activit6. Des enfants ag6s entre 4.5 et 6 ans ont compl6t6
diff6rentstests d'habilet6sen lecture. Par la suite ils se sont entrain6sa suivre du doigt en
lisant un texte qu'ils avaient m6moris6 au pr6alable. Nous avons alors 6valuer l'impactde
cette activit6 sur leurs habilet6sa lire le texte. Des analyses de regression montrerentque
diff6rentstypes de connaissances sur l'6crit facilitaientdiff6rentsaspects de la capacit6 de
suivre du doigt en lisant. Savoir lire certains mots appris auparavantest apparuimportant
pour apprendrea lire des mots nouveaux dans le texte. La segmentation phon6tique est
apparueimportantepour pouvoir suivre les mots en meme temps qu'ils 6taientprononc6sa
l'oral et pour se souvenir comment prononcercertains mots du texte. La connaissance des
lettres6taitimportantepour 8trecapablede remarquerque certaineslettresavaientchangede
caligraphiedans le texteet pour rep6rerdes mots dans le texte. Ces r6sultatsaidenta d6nouer
les relationscomplexes qui existententrediff6rentstypes de connaissancestelles qu'ellessont
mises en applicationpar les lecteursd6butantsdans une tache contr616ede lecturede texte.

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI & SWEET 443

La lecturasehalando con el dedo de un textomemorizado:iQue es lo que


permitea los principiantesprocesar la palabra impresa?
EN UN ESTUDIO de laboratorio, investigamos los tipos de conocimiento relacionadocon la
letra impresa que los lectores incipientes necesitan poseer para aprender a sefialar las
palabrasen un texto conforme lo recitan de memoria (lectura sehalando con el dedo) y a
recordarinformaci6nacerca de la palabraimpresade esta actividad.Los nifios, cuyas edades
variaban de 4.5 a 6 afios de edad, completaron varias pruebas de habilidad de lectura.
Despu6s practicaron lectura sefialando con el dedo en un texto simple que habian
memorizado. Valoramosel impacto de esta prictica en sus capacidadesde lectura con el
texto. Los anailisisde regresi6n revelaronque diferentestipos de conocimientode la palabra
impresa facilitabandiferentesaspectos de la lectura sefialandocon el dedo. El saber c6mo
leer unas pocas palabrassencillas era importantepara aprendera leer nuevas palabrasdel
texto. La segmentaci6n fon6mica fue importante para aprender a sefialar las palabras
impresasal mismo tiempo que estas eran pronunciadas,y para recordarc6mo leer palabras
sueltasen el texto. El conocimientode las letras fue importantepara notarque las letrasen el
texto habian sido alteradasy para localizar palabrasen el texto. Estos resultadosayudana
desenredarlas complejasrelacionesque existen entre varias fuentesde conocimientoy c6mo
son usadaspor los lectoresprincipiantesparaprocesartexto escrito de maneraque se enfoque
la atenci6nen cada palabra.

Fingerlesen eines auswendiggelerntenTextes:Wasbewirktbei Anfangern


die Verarbeitungvon gedrucktemText?
IN EINER Versuchsstudieuntersuchtenwir die Arten des Wissens, das sich auf gedruckten
Text bezieht und das Anfiinger im Lesen besitzen miissen, um zu lernen, daB sie auf die
W6rter eines Textes beim auswendigenAufsagen zeigen sollen (Fingerlesen), und daB sie
sich anhanddieser Ubung an Informationenfiber das Gedruckteerinnernsollen. Kinder im
Alter von 4 bis 6 Jahrennahmenan verschiedenenLesefertigkeitsfibungenteil. AnschlieBend
fibten sie das Fingerlesen eines einfachen Textes, den sie auswendig gelernt hatten.
Wir bewerteten den EinfluB dieser Ubung auf die Textlesefiihigkeiten der Kinder.
Regressionsanalysen zeigten, daB unterschiedliches Wissen von gedrucktem Text
verschiedene Aspekte des Fingerlesens erleichterte. Es war z.B. wichtig zu wissen, wie
einige Worterder Vorschulstufezu lesen sind, um das Lesen neuerW6rterim Textlernen zu
k6nnen. Die phonemischeGliederungwar wichtig, um zu lernen, parallelzum Aufsagen auf
die gedrucktenW6rterzu zeigen und sich daranzu erinnern,wie einzelne W6rterim Textzu
lesen sind. Buchstabenkenntnissewaren wichtig, um erkennenzu k6nnen, daB Buchstaben
im Text verdindertworden waren, und um W6rter finden zu k6nnen. Die Ergebnisse
dieser Studie erleichtern ein Verstlindnis der komplexen Beziehungen zwischen den
unterschiedlichen Wissensquellen, die von Leseanfdingernverwendet werden, wenn sie
gedrucktenTextWortfiir Wortlesen.

of emergent literacy have revealed Yaden & Templeton, 1986). Very young chil-
Studies
that childrenbecome readersmuch earlier dren become readers as they gain experience
than is commonly recognized- in fact, long be- listening to stories and learning how they are
fore they can read novel words and stories on structured semantically, in terms of ideas, as
their own (Sulzby, 1985; Teale & Sulzby, 1987; well as visually, in terms of appearance on the

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
444 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

printedpage. One of the earliest forms of read- forms by watchingthe print.


ing among preschoolershas been referredto as Sulzby (1985) provides evidence for this
pretend reading or emergentstorybookreading very interestingcourse of developmentof pre-
(Sulzby, 1985). This occurs when childrenturn tend reading. However,it is not clear from this
the pages of a book they have heardmany times work what causes children to advance in the
and recite the story from memory. A more ad- hierarchy and to begin processing the print,
vanced form of pretendreadinginvolves point- whether pretendreading practice is the impor-
ing to the words as the text is recited verbatim tant factor,whetherother kinds of printexperi-
from memory,orfingerpoint reading. ences are critical as well, and if so, what these
Some educators have argued that it is es- experiences might be. There is reason to doubt
sential for beginners to practicethese forms of that pretendreading by itself has the power to
reading in order to come to understandwhat move children closer to independent reading,
reading is all about and how the act of read- because successful pretendreadingdoes not re-
ing is executed (Clay, 1972; Holdaway, 1979; quire readersto pay any attentionto the print.
Mason & Allen, 1986; Mason, Kerr, Sinha, & Emergent readers can pretend-readadequately
McCormick, 1990; Taylor, 1986). Withoutthis by studying the pictures and linking them to
type of practice, mechanicsof the process such what they recall of the spoken text. There is no
as letter-soundrelationsand isolatedword read- "press"from the task itself necessitatingfurther
ing do not makemuch sense to learners(Dyson, adaptation.In their reading of storiesp to chil-
1984). Holdaway (1979) cites Martinand Bro- dren, adultsmay not revealto listenersthatthey
gan's (1972) assertions that "book experience are doing anythingotherthanlooking at the pic-
should precede word experience in bringing a tures and recallingthe story.
child to print"and that "[a]n emerging reader Studies of children'sreading of signs such
needs a battery of books that he [or she] can as STOPand McDonalds have shown that envi-
zoom throughwith joyous familiarity." ronmental print reading does not bring chil-
The focus of the presentstudywas upon the dren any closer to reading the print itself.
transitionfrom memorizedtext readingto inde- Masonheimer,Drum, and Ehri (1984) selected
pendentreading.We were interestedin how this 102 preschoolerswho could read at least 8 out
transitionoccurs, andwhetherthereare types of of 10 labels and signs. They modified the labels
print-relatedknowledge that emergent readers and signs in severalways: by removingthe con-
must possess in order to learn to fingerpoint- texts and logos, by printingthem in manuscript
read a memorizedtext and rememberinforma- type, and by alteringletters in the labels, such
tion aboutthe printfrom this activity. as in changing Pepsi to Xepsi. They found that
Sulzby (1985) studied pretend reading in practicallyall of their experts lost their ability
young children. She assessed readingstrategies to "read"the print when it was modified. Few
by having kindergartenchildrenselect their fa- could identify the words out of context. Most
vorite storybookand "pretend-read it."Based on failed to recognize the letter alterations. The
her observations,she proposeda developmental fact that letters were not processed in the signs
hierarchyto distinguishmore maturefrom less indicates that extensive experience with envi-
maturestrategiesfor this activity. Readerspro- ronmental print does not move preschoolers
gress from simply reacting to the pictures to into reading of the sort that involves noticing
rendering an interpretation that follows the and rememberingprintbecause doing so would
story line. At first, their stories exhibit oral serve no purpose. Childrencan read the labels
story-telling language. Later on, they utilize and signs successfully by paying attention to
language characteristicof written text, such as more obvious contextual cues, such as golden
repetitive sentence patterns. Initially, readers arches. The same may be true of pretendread-
generate written language forms from the pic- ing: Using pictures to promptmemory for the
tures combined with their memory for the text. text may be sufficient.
Later on, readers produce written language A more difficult form of pretendreadingis

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 445

fingerpoint-reading,which requireschildren to present study was to examine whether begin-


pay closer attentionto the print. Such "reading" ners need to know something about the alpha-
of text that has been memorizedhas been incor- betic system in order to succeed in learning to
porated into instructional programs currently fingerpoint-read.
marketedby basal text publishersfor use in the Morris and Henderson (1981; Morris,
early grades. The approachfollows Holdaway's 1983) have studied children'sability to finger-
(1979) proceduresfor conductinga sharedbook point-readmemorizedtext, which they refer to
experience. Teachers are directed to read and as reflectinga child's"conceptof word."In their
reread books that have been enlarged suffi- task, studentsfirst learn to recite a poem, then
ciently so thatthe text and picturesare visible to observe and practice fingerpoint-reading,then
the entireclass. The teacherreadseach line and try to read selected words in the text. Three lev-
points to each word as it is read. This is done els of fingerpoint-reading have been distin-
for several reasons: so that children will see guished. The least mature students exhibit no
how text is read, from left to right; so that they ability to point to words or read them after-
will come to understandhow printis structured wards. Transitionalstudentsuse spacing cues to
to correspondto speech, at the word and letter point to words, but tend to monitor spoken syl-
levels; so that they will become able to recite lables ratherthan words and do not self-correct
and fingerpoint-read smaller versions of the these errors by noticing the lack of correspon-
books on their own; and so that they will begin dence between the letters pointed to and the
to acquirea sight vocabularyof familiarwords. sounds spoken. When asked to read a wordbur-
Advocates of whole language instruction be- ied in a line, these studentsrespondby attempt-
lieve that practice at memorized text reading ing to rereadthe line up to that word. The most
enables beginners to move into independent advancedstudentscan point accuratelyto words
reading. The purpose of the present study was as they are said, can correcterrorson two-sylla-
to investigatethis hypothesis in the laboratory. ble words by noticing the mismatch between
Whereas preschoolers may find it easy to sounds and letters, and can learn to read new
learnpretendreadingthatrelies on picturesplus words from a fingerpoint-readinglesson. Stu-
memoryof a story,pretendreadingthatrequires dents at this level, according to Morris and
pointing to the words in a text may prove more Henderson, have achieveda "conceptof word."
difficult. Not all pretendreadersmay be able to In a longitudinalstudy conductedwith kin-
learn to follow print with their fingers or to dergartenchildren, Morris (1989, in press) ex-
process and rememberprintedwords and letters amined precursors of fingerpoint-reading.He
during pretend-reading practice. In order to found that students achieved proficiency with
trackspeech, they may need to know something initial consonants before they achieved profi-
about print and how it works, such as how to ciency with the "concept of word,"indicating
segment speech into constituent words and that beginners may need to know how to seg-
sounds, how to recognize letter shapes, how in- ment and spell beginning consonant sounds in
dividual letters correspondto sounds, and how wordsbefore they can succeed at matchingprint
writtenwords correspondto spoken words. De- with speech in their fingerpoint-reading.
spite the fact that empty spaces are available as Morris's (1989) measure of beginning conso-
cues markingthe breaks between words, some nantknowledgewas a compositemeasureof let-
knowledge about the alphabeticsystem may be ter knowledge and phonemic segmentation
needed. Speakers do not pause between the skill. His measure of "conceptof word"was a
words they utter, so word units are not as obvi- composite of several aspects of fingerpoint-
ous in speech as they are in print. Readersmay reading. One purpose of the present study was
need to recognize how initial letters in printed to examine components of fingerpoint-reading
words correspond to initial sounds in spoken and printknowledgeseparately,in orderto pin-
words in orderto distinguishand point to words point possible causal relations between these
in a line of memorizedtext. One purposeof the variables.

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
446 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

The design of the presentstudyinvolvedse- Method


lecting 4- to 6-year-oldswho had not advanced
beyonda novice level in readingwordsby sight. Subjects
Most could read no words. Subjects practiced The subjectswere 36 children, 18 male and
fingerpoint-readinga text. Several aspects of 18 female, rangingin age from 4.5 to 5.8 years
fingerpoint-readingwere then measured:recit- (M = 5.1 years), all proficientin English. Sub-
ing lines verbatimfrom memory, matching up jects were drawn from local groups willing to
spoken and printed words in the child's point- cooperatein a researchstudy:two preschools, a
ing, rememberinghow to read individualwords day care center, and a kindergartenclassroom.
in print, and detecting letter alterationsin the Children younger than 4.5 years were not in-
lines of text. In addition, severalaspects of sub- cluded. Although some of the children had en-
jects' print knowledge were assessed: letter gaged in reading readiness activities, they had
naming, preprimer word reading, phonemic not received any formal, structuredreadingin-
segmentation,and discriminationof letters dis- struction. Also, they had not participated in
tinguishing similarly spelled words. We were emergentliteracyprograms.
interestedin determiningwhich types of print Additional subjects included in the initial
knowledge were related to various aspects of sample were not included in the study for the
successful fingerpoint-reading. following reasons: One child read some target
Of special interestwas the contributionof words and words above the preprimer level;
letter knowledge. In other reading tasks, letter four childrenwere unavailablefor a second day
knowledge has been found to be a strong pre- of testing; one child was unableto memorizeor
dictor of reading. Share, Jorm, Maclean, and participatein choral reading of the story; and
Matthews (1984) found that letter knowledge three childrenrefusedto cooperate.
measuredat the beginning of kindergartenwas
one of the two best predictors of reading
achievementat the end of kindergartenand first Procedure
grade; the other predictor was phonemic seg- The assessment and training were con-
mentation skill. Also, Ehri and Wilce (1985) ducted individuallyon 2 successive days, with
and Masonheimeret al. (1984) found that mas- each day's session lasting 20-30 minutes. On
tery of letters distinguishedbeginnerswho were Day 1, each child completed the pretests mea-
able to learn to read words effectively from suring letter knowledge and word readingabil-
those who were not. ity. (Some subjects completed these pretests a
Of interest here was whether letter knowl- day earlier.)Then the subjectwas introducedto
edge would distinguish beginners who could fingerpoint-reading, and practiced reading a
learn to fingerpoint-readfrom those who could story and fingerpointingwith the experimenter
not. It may be thatletter knowledgeenables be- (one of the authors). After several trials, the
ginners to adaptto the fingerpointingtask and, subject fingerpoint-read the story alone. Fi-
with practice, to figure out how printedwords nally, the child completed the first part of the
correspond to spoken words. Moreover, letter trainingfor the phonemicsegmentationtask.
knowledge may enable beginners to remember On Day 2, subjects received additional
how to readthe individualwordsthey encounter practice in choral and fingerpoint-reading,in-
in the text. Accordingto Ehri's(1987) theory of terspersedwith attemptsto read the text them-
beginning word reading, knowledge of letters selves. We then assessed what the childrenhad
provides the basis for forming connections learned about printedwords from their finger-
between the letters seen in spellings and the point-reading.Finally, they completedthe pho-
sounds detectedin pronunciations,and for stor- nemic segmentationtask.
ing these associationsin memory in orderto re- Pretests
memberhow to readthose words when they are
seen again. Three tests of letter and word knowledge

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 447

were given on Day 1, prior to the fingerpoint- the title of this nursery rhyme was written in
readingtraining. capitalletters.
In addition, a story of 8 lines was creat-
1. Reading target words. Subjects were ed by modifying one verse of a poem by
asked to read 12 words from the story that Olive A. Wadsworth entitled "Over in the
would be used for the fingerpoint-readingtrain- Meadow."Wordswere changedto make the ini-
ing. Each word was printedin capital letters on tial letters more varied, to make the text less fa-
a flash card. The words were blue, old, dived, miliar,andto includemore wordswith an initial
fishes, and, mother,jungle, the, swim, where, letter that symbolized the initial sound in the
down, and two. Five picture cards were mixed word as well as a sound in the name of the letter
in with the word cards to minimize nonreaders' (e.g., M, named "em"'containing/m/, the first
sense of failure. Subjects who could read more sound in Mother). The 8 lines were printed in
than one target word were excluded from the capitalletters, one line per page:
study. Two of the subjects who were retainedin
the sample were able to read one target word DOWNIN THEJUNGLE
each; the remainingsubjectscould not readany WHERETHERIVERRUNSBLUE
targetwords. LIVEDAN OLDMOTHERFISH
AND HERLITTLEFISHESTWO.
2. Naming letters. Subjects named 26 up- "SWIM," SAIDTHEMOTHER.
"WEARESWIMMING" SAIDTHETWO.
percase letters presentedin randomorder on a
card. Scores of subjects ranged from 0 to 26; SO THEY PADDLED AND THEYDIVED
25 percent knew 0-10 letters, 22 percent knew WHERETHERIVERRUNSBLUE.
11-21 letters, and 53 percent knew 22-26 let-
ters. Each line was accompaniedby a relevant pic-
ture in color. The 8 pages formed a soft-cover
3. Reading preprimerand Slosson words. booklet. Capitalletters were used because they
Subjects were shown 11 preprimer words on are more familiarto preschoolers.
flash cards mixed with 5 picture cards. If the
child read at least one word, then he or she was Training.On Day 1, each child was first in-
shown 11 more preprimerwords mixed with 3 troduced to fingerpoint-readingusing the pic-
picture cards. Subjects who read more than 2 ture of "Peter,Peter,PumpkinEater"along with
words were given the Slosson OralReadingTest the printedtitle. The experimenterread the title
(1963). Subjects who scored above the pre- twice, pointing to each word as she said it. She
primer level on this test were excluded from explainedthat she knew which wordsto pointto
the study. Scores of subjects who remained in by attendingto the correspondencebetween ini-
the study ranged from 0 to 9 preprimerwords tial letters and sounds in the words. Then the
correct; 67 percent read 0 words, 28 percent child attemptedto point to each word as he or
read 1-3 words, and the 2 remaining subjects she spokethe four-wordtitle. If this attemptwas
read 6 and 9 words, respectively. Thus, the unsuccessful, the experimentermodeled how to
sample comprised a large group of nonreaders point at the words until the child was able to do
(n = 24) and a small group of novice readers so correctly, up to a maximum of three times.
(n = 12). Most children (86%) were successful within
two trials.
Fingerpoint-reading training Next, the experimenterreadthe story aloud
Materials. Two sets of materialswere used while the subject looked at the pictures in the
for the trainingin fingerpoint-reading.The ma- booklet. Then she read the story again more
terials for the introductorytask consisted of an slowly while pointingher finger at the words as
illustrationof the nursery rhyme "Peter,Peter, the child watched. The child and experimenter
PumpkinEater"and a sheet of paper on which then readthe book together in chorus, and both

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
448 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

pointed to the words- with the experimenter 1. Withthe bookclosed, the child was asked,
pointing above the line, and the child below the "Werethe wordsin thatbookjust like the
line. The child then fingerpoint-readthe first wordswe readbeforein the otherbook?"If
line alone. The experimenterand child read the the child answered"No,"the experimenter
story together again in chorus using finger- asked,"Howweretheydifferent?"
2. With the book open, the experimenter
pointing. Then the child fingerpoint-readthe pointedto the first line of printandasked,
whole story alone. This was followed by one fi- "Dothesewordslookjust likethewordswe
nal choral reading. readbefore?" If thechildanswered"No,"the
On Day 2, the story was read five times. experimenter asked,"Howweretheydiffer-
On Trials 1, 2, and 4, the experimenterand the ent?"
child read the story in chorus while pointing to 3. Withthe firstbookopenandpositionedon
the words. On Trials 3 and 5, the child finger- top of the secondbook so thatthe original
point-readthe story alone. andalteredlinesof printwerevisible,theex-
perimenter said, "Lookat thesewords.Are
Taskperformance. Performanceon the fi- thesejustlikethose?"
nal solo readingof the 8 lines of text was scored
for three characteristics:(a) correctly reciting Childrenwere assigned 4 points for spontane-
each line, (b) pointing to words in each line, ous recognition of the altered text before any
and (c) voice-point matching (i.e., pointing to questions were asked, 3 points for answering
words at the same time as they were spoken). that the text had been changed when the book
The scoring criteriafor the first two characteris- was closed, 2 points for recognitionwith the al-
tics were as follows: If subjectsrespondedto a tered book open, 1 point for recognition upon
line perfectly,they received a score of 3; if they seeing the two books open side-by-side, and 0
respondedcorrectlyto only some of the words, points for no recognition.
they received a score of 1; if their responsewas 2. Reading isolated lines of text. Subjects
totally incorrect, they received a score of 0. were asked to read three lines from the text
Nonlinear values were adopted to lend greater
(Lines 3, 1, and 7, in that order) with the pic-
weight to perfectperformances.Scores were to- tures removed.
taled for the 8 lines, for a maximum score of
24. The scoring criterion for the voice-point 3. Reading isolated target words. This was
matching measure was the numberof lines re- a repeat of the Day 1 pretest. Subjects were
spondedto perfectly (maximumscore = 8). asked to read 12 words from the story printed
on flashcards,with 5 picturecards mixed in.
Posttests
Six tasks measuringletterand word knowl- 4. Readingtargetwordsin text. The experi-
edge were given after the fingerpoint-reading menter turned to various pages of the text,
task (i.e., the last solo reading)on Day 2. pointed to words in the text out of order, and
asked subjects to read the words. These were
1. Recognizingaltered text. Subjects were the same 12 target words that were tested in
shownan alteredversion of the story booklet, in isolation.
which the pictures, the length of the words, and
the line length remainedthe same as in the orig- 5. Recognizing words in text. The experi-
inal, but new letters were substituted.For ex- menteropened the book and pronouncedwords
ample, the first line, DOWNIN THEJUNGLE, on severalpages, and asked the subjectto point
was changed to WSWSAL WOH OEWNIH. to these words. Five of the target words used
Childrenwere askedto readthe first four pages. above plus 5 new words were used. (Subjects
The experimenternoted whetherthe child spon- were not betterat recognizingpreviouslytested
taneously commented on the altered text, then than untestedwords, indicatingthat testing did
asked three questions: not teach the words.)

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 449

6. Letter discrimination. The purpose of mouthblocks in a phonemic segmentationtask.


this task was to assess whether subjects proc- First, the child was shown how to select two
essed print globally or analyzed letter details blocks to depict the blend /me/ ("mae").Then
when asked to compare pairs of similarly the subject was given a blend orally and was
spelled words. Subjectswere shown 7 nonsense asked to segment it into constituentsounds by
word pairs. Each pair was writtenin capitallet- selecting a picture of each sound as the subject
ters on a flashcard, with one word appearing4 spoke it. A total of 24 blends were presented,
centimetersabovethe other. The two wordsdif- which includeda mix of CV (consonant-vowel),
feredby one letter, which was in initial, medial,VC, CVC, VCC, and CCV sequences (e.g.,
or final position. There were 6 pairs of three- /le/, /es/, /mel/, /em/, /sle/, /els/, /lem/). If the
letter words, of which 3 were pronounceable child performedthe task incorrectly,the experi-
(e.g., WOR/DOR)and 3 were unpronounceable menter modeled the correct response and de-
(e.g., BPC/BPX). The 7th pair contained two scribedher mouthmovements.
consonant blends (CLUST/CLUSP). Subjects Three measures were derived from per-
were asked if the wordswere the same or differ- formance on the phonemic segmentationtask:
ent, and if different, how. Any explanationthat (a) the number of trials to reach criterion the
involved referringto the lettersthat were differ-first time in learning associations between the
ent was acceptedas correct. four mouth pictures and sounds; (b) the total
numberof individualsounds spoken and repre-
Phonemic segmentation training sented correctly with mouth blocks in the
Materials. Fourmoveableblocks, each 2 x blends (maximum = 60); and (c) the numberof
21/2 inches in size, were covered with pictures blends correctly segmented (maximum = 24).
of mouth positions depicting four different Because this task followed the fingerpoint-read-
sounds: A smiling mouth portrayed/e/ (long a ing task, it did not influence reading perform-
as in ate), a mouth with lips closed portrayed ance.
/m/, a mouth open slightly with tongue against
teeth portrayed/s/, and a mouth open with the
tongue up touching the top of the mouthbehind Results
the teeth portrayed/1/. The /s/ block also dis-
played a pictureof a snake. The mouthpictures To determinewhich of several readerchar-
were adapted from those used by Lindamood
and Lindamood(1975). acteristicsaccountedfor unique variance in the
measures of text reading and word readingthat
Training. On Day 1 (after completing the followed fingerpoint-readingpractice, stepwise
pretests and the fingerpoint-readingtraining), regressionanalyses were conducted. The maxi-
subjectswere shown the blocks and were taught mum R2 improvement technique (in the SAS
to associate the mouthpictureswith sounds. To statisticalprogram)was used to find the best re-
make the connection, children observed their gression model for each dependent variable,
own mouths making those sounds in a mirror. with the best model defined as the one in which
They learnedthese associationsto a criterionof the factorsyielded the highest value for R2 and
8 errorlesstrials. in which each factoraccountedfor uniquevari-
On Day 2 (aftersubjectshad completedthe ance thatwas significant. Significancewas indi-
fingerpoint-readingtraining and the posttests), cated if removal of the factor resulted in a
the training in associating the mouth pictures statistically significant drop in the amount of
with sounds was continuedor reinstatedto a cri- varianceaccountedfor by the model at p <.05.
terion of 8 errorlesstrials. The independentvariables that were tested
in these regression analyses were age, letter-
Taskperformance. After the association name knowledge, preprimerword readingskill,
training on the second day, subjects used the letter discriminationskill, and two measuresof

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
450 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

Table 1 Performanceon all measures:Mean scores, standarddeviations, mean percentage


correctscores, and percentagesof subjectswith scores of zero and with perfect scores

Mean %with %with


Measure Max. M SD % zero perfect
correct score score

Age (months) - 61.27 4.42


Letter/word knowledge
Namingcapital letters 26 17.83 8.45 68% 3% 25%
Readingpreprimerwords 22 0.92 1.90 4% 67% 0%
Letterdiscrimination 7 5.50 2.20 79% 6% 56%
Phonemic segmentation
Trialsto criteriona 4 5.63 2.75 - 6% 44%
Correctphonemessegmented 60 36.97 13.43 62% 6% 1%
Blends segmentedcorrectly 24 6.64 5.56 28% 14% 0%
2-phonemeblends 12 5.11 3.62 43% 14% 3%
3-phonemeblends 12 1.53 2.52 13% 53% 3%

Fingerpoint-reading
Recitinglines verbatim 24 17.39 5.06 72% 0% 14%
Pointingto words 24 10.47 7.74 44% 6% 3%
Voice-pointmatching 8 1.83 2.36 23% 44% 3%
Recognizingalteredtext 4 1.47 1.34 37% 19% 17%
Readingwordsin isolation 12 0.50 1.11 4% 78% 0%
Readingwordsin text 12 2.11 2.94 18% 58% 0%
Locatingwords in text 10 2.56 2.22 26% 19% 0%
Note. N = 36.
aTheminimum(notmaximum) of subjectswitha perfectscoreis thepercentage
numberof learningtrialswas4. Thepercentage achieving
criterionin theminimum of trials;thepercentage
number thatneverreachedcriterion.
of subjectswitha zeroscoreis thepercentage

phonemic segmentation(numberof correct in- We examinedfirst the subjects'initial letter


dividualphonemes, and numberof correctpho- and word knowledge before any training. We
neme blends). The dependent variables were then examined subjects' fingerpoint-reading
the measuresof subjects'ability to fingerpoint- performance and the variables that best ex-
read text following practice, their recognition plained this performance.Finally, we looked at
that the text's letter configurationhad been al- the measures of subjects'ability to read words
tered, and their ability to read individualwords afterthe trainingin fingerpoint-reading.
in the text.
Performance of the subjects on all mea- Initial letter and word knowledge
sures is reportedin Table 1, which shows the From Table 1, it is apparentthatthe sample
means, standarddeviations, and mean percent- consistedof subjectswho were typically 5 years
age correct scores, as well as the percentageof old, knew over half the letter names but could
subjects who scored 0 and scored perfectly on read few if any preprimerwords, and were able
each task. The coefficients of correlation be- to attendto and discriminatelettersdistinguish-
tween these variables are reportedin Table 2. ing most pairs of letter strings. In the phonemic
Single and multi-factormodels listing the vari- segmentation task, all but 2 subjects reached
ables that provedsignificantare reportedin Ta- criterionin learningto associate mouthpictures
ble 3. Two variableswere not significantin any with sounds. In using the pictures to segment
model: age and the number of individualpho- blends, subjects could distinguish many single
nemes correctlysegmented. phonemes correctly (M = 62% correct), but

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 451

Table 2 Intercorrelationsbetween all measures

Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Age - -.01 .12 .11 .31 .23 .07 .38 .31 -.04 .25 .15
2. Naming letters - .46 .08 .64 .50 .34 .47 .48 .61 .55 .54
3. Readingpreprimerwords - .08 .47 .70 .05 .57 .51 .51 .72 .63
4. Letterdiscrimination - .16 .07 .42 .13 .29 .28 .26 .07
5. Correctphonemes segmented - .83 .32 .64 .53 .44 .55 .53
6. Blends segmentedcorrectly - .28 .70 .60 .47 .68 .58
7. Recitinglines verbatim - .26 .38 .46 .22 .18
8. Pointingto words - .89 .38 .61 .57
9. Voice-pointmatching - .48 .61 .48
10. Recognizingalteredtext - .56 .55
11. Readingwords in text - .51
12. Locatingwords in text
Note. r > .32 is significantly
differentfrom0 atp <.05; r > .40 is significantly
differentfrom0 atp <.01.

were not good at segmenting entire blends (M than to reciting accuracy (r = .38). This find-
= 28% correct). Most of the blends that were ing indicatesthatthe majorproblemin learning
correctly segmented consisted of two pho- to fingerpoint-readis not memorizing the text
nemes, either CV or VC (M = 43%). Se- but pointingto the word segments.
quences of threephonemeswere much harderto In the regressionanalysis on scores for re-
segment (M = 13%). citing the text accurately,letter discrimination
ability and letter-name knowledge accounted
Fingerpoint-reading performance for unique variance. However,as shown in Ta-
Three aspects of subjects'ability to learn to ble 3, the amountof varianceaccountedfor was
fingerpoint-read the story were measured in fairly small, .27. What explains this relation-
their final solo readingattempt:accuracyat re- ship is not clear.
citing each line of text verbatim, accuracy at The best regression models for the word
pointingto the wordsin each line, and ability to pointing and voice-point matching measures
coordinate these two behaviors to match print were identical, which is not surprisingbecause
with speech. From the mean percentagecorrect the two measures were highly correlated (see
values in Table 1, it is apparentthat subjects Table 3). One independentvariable explained
learnedto recite the text fairly well. They were the variance: phonemic segmentation skill as
less able to point to the words in each line and measuredby the number of blends segmented
least able to match their voice with their point- correctly. This finding suggests that phonemic
ing. In fact, 44 percent of the subjects scored segmentationis a critical skill enabling begin-
zero on the measure of voice-point matching. ners to learn to fingerpoint-readfamiliartext. It
Inspection of the children's shortcomings re- may be that the hardestpart of the fingerpoint-
vealed that subjects would often slide their fin- reading task is finding the appropriatephone-
gers along beneath the line of text ratherthan mic segments in speech to match up with the
pointing to individual words in the line. How- printedletters.
ever, this behavior was apparentonly in sub- To verify this interpretation,we examined
jects without much skill in fingerpoint-reading; scores on the measures of phonemic segmen-
none of the subjects who fingerpoint-read at tation and voice-point matching. Figure 1
least one line accuratelyever did this. presents a scatter plot of these scores. Data
As shown by the correlations in Table 2, points are distinguishedfor two groups of sub-
accuracy at voice-point matching was more jects: weak segmenters (those who segmented
highly related to pointing accuracy (r = .89) mainly two-phoneme blends) and strong seg-

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
452 READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

Table 3 Best models found in stepwise regressionanalyses (as judged by maximumR2


improvement)
F F to
Model R2 statistica removeb p

Dependent variable: Reciting lines verbatim


Single-factormodel .174 7.14*
Letterdiscrimination 7.14* .0115
Two-factormodel .267 6.02"
Letterdiscrimination 6.86* .0132
Naming letters 4.23* .0477

Dependent variable: Pointing to words correctly during recitation


Single-factormodel .497 33.60**
Blends segmentedcorrectly 33.60** .0001

Dependent variable: Matching word pointing with recitation


Single-factormodel .363 19.39**
Blends segmentedcorrectly 19.39** .0001

Dependent variable: Recognizing altered text


Single-factormodel .369 19.85**
Naming letters 19.85** .0001

Dependent variable: Reading target words in text


Single-factormodel .519 36.71**
Readingpreprimerwords 36.71** .0001
Two-factormodel .582 22.94**
Readingpreprimerwords 9.28* .0045
Blends segmentedcorrectly 4.93* .0334

Dependent variable: Locating target words in text


Single-factormodel .400 22.70**
Readingpreprimerwords 22.70** .0001
Two-factormodel .482 15.36**
Readingpreprimerwords 11.88* .0016
Naming letters 5.20* .0291
Note. df = 1,34 for single-factormodels; df = 2,33 for two-factormodels.
aTheF statisticfromtheANOVA is theratioof regressionmeansquareto errormeansquare. bTheF to removeis theratioof thesumof
squares(SS)thatis addedto theerrorSSif thatvariableis removedfromthemodel,to errorSSwithoutthevariableremoved.
"*
p <.05 **p <.0001

menters (those who segmented some CV and segmentation skill may emerge before finger-
VC words plus at least 2 of the three-phoneme point-readingand may be a necessary but not
blends). The distributionof the weakest scores sufficient condition for its development. Note
is especially interesting(see lower left cornerof that the phonemic segmentationskill examined
figure): All 8 subjectswho correctlysegmented here is a rudimentaryform: segmenting two-
only 1 or 0 blends failed to fingerpoint-readany phonemeblends.
lines correctly. However, of the 16 subjects in After the fingerpoint-readingpractice, sub-
total who did not fingerpoint-readany lines cor- jects were also given a version with the letters
rectly, 69 percent were able to segment from I alteredand were asked to read it. Subjectsvar-
to 8 blends. These data suggest that phonemic ied in their recognitionthat the print had been

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI & SWEET 453

Figure 1
Scatterplot of scores on phonemicsegmentationtask and fingerpoint-readingtask (with datapoints
distinguishedby whethersubjectswere able to segment at least two 3-phonemeblends)

g
Length of Blends
O CV/VC(N=18)
"8 U CV/VC+ at least
0- 7 - 2 CVC/VCC/CCV
"13 (N=12)
r- 60-
o ? ?
"13 5 0

S2- o
S 0
80 0
0- 000 000 0O
S 00 0
E 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24
zNumber of Blends Segmented into Phonemes

changed: 6 noticed it spontaneously as they knowledge was the critical skill underlyingper-
were reading, 2 mentioned it when they were formance, very likely because it enabled sub-
questioned with the book closed, 2 noticed it jects to distinguish and rememberelements of
when questionedwith the book open, 18 did not the original letterconfiguration.
see it until the two books were presentedside- The distribution of scores on these two
by-side for comparison, and 8 never noticed it measures- detecting altered text and naming
at all. Thus, 26 subjects, or 72 percent, exhib- letters-were plotted in a scatter diagram, pre-
ited no memory for the letter configuration sented in Figure 2. Interestingly,letter knowl-
in the text they had practiced reading. This edge was highly tied to success or failurewhen
percentage is similar to results reported by subjects did not have the two texts side-by-side
Masonheimer et al. (1984), who found that to compare. The subjects who detected the al-
about two thirdsof the subjectswho could read terationspontaneouslyor when asked with the
environmentalprint failed to detect the differ- book either closed or open were subjects who
ence between original and altered signs (e.g., had named the majority of the letters. In con-
Xepsi vs. Pepsi), even when the signs were pre- trast, those who never recognizedthe alteration
sented side-by-side. knew fewer than 18 letters, and most of these
In the regressionanalysis for scores on rec- subjects knew fewer than 10 letters. This indi-
ognizing altered text, the best model included cates that near-masteryof letterswas crucial for
only one factor, letter-nameknowledge, which complete success in the detection task. Letter
accounted for 37 percent of the variance (see knowledge was less importantamong subjects
Table3). These results suggest that letter-name who detectedthe alterationwhen they could see

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
454 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

Figure 2
Scatterplot of scores on the letter-namingtask and alteredtext task

Low
0 4 0 @ Spontaneous
c)

.2 3 - Bookclosed

o2 Bookopen

x 1 * ** * *** : : Sidebyside

) 0 * * * ** Norecognition
d)
0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26
Numberof Letters Named

the books side-by-side; these subjects varied was not centralto this task (r = .08).
greatly in their letter knowledge (see Figure 2). After the altered text task, subjects were
This is not surprising,because the information asked to read three different lines without the
needed to make the comparisonwas at hand, so pictures. Only 7 subjects(19%) read any of the
letter memory was not essential. lines correctly,and none readmorethan2 lines,
One subjectwith weak letter knowledgere- indicatingthat readinglines out of context was
sponded to this task in a way that revealedthat very difficult. All but one of the subjects who
he had not paid attentionto the letters during were able to readat least one line had displayed
fingerpoint-reading. During the latter part of word reading ability on the pretest, indicating
the task, when the altered and original lines that word readingability facilitatedmemory for
of print were exposed together, this subject re- lines of text.
positionedthe books in orderto see both sets of
pictures before answering the question about Word reading after training
whetherthe words were the same or different. Three word reading tests were given fol-
The alteredprinttask was similarto the let- lowing the fingerpoint-reading task. When
ter discrimination task in that both required words from the text were presentedin isolation,
readers to compare letters in two print speci- only 8 subjects(22 %)readany wordscorrectly,
mens. However,performanceswere not correl- and these subjects read no more than 4 apiece
ated (r = .28, p >.05), revealing that the (out of 12). All but one of these subjects had
ability to detect letter differences when two read at least one preprimerword on the pretest.
words are seen side-by-side did not account The low scores on this task reveal that even
for variance in performanceon the alteredtext novice word readersdid not learn to read many
task. Moreover,even subjects with weak letter individual words out of context as a result of
knowledgeperformedwell in the letter discrim- fingerpoint-reading practice. Several factors
ination task, indicating that letter knowledge may be responsible, including subjects'lack of

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 455

success in fingerpoint-reading many lines of read any preprimerwords in the pretest. All of
text, the rudimentarynature of these subjects' the subjects who read any words either in text
word readingskill, the greatersalience of other or in isolation (i.e., the circles and squares
non-printcues such as the pictures, and the ab- shown in Figure 3 for subjectswho read at least
sence of pressureto process print cues because 2 words correctly) also displayedsome phone-
the text had been memorized. Because 78 per- mic segmentationskill. In addition, there were
cent of the subjects received a word reading 14 subjectswho displayedsome phonemic seg-
score of zero, a regressionanalysis was not con- mentationskill but read no words (i.e., the cir-
ducted. cles shownfor subjectswho read0 wordsin text
Subjects read more words when the words but segmentedat least 1 blend). However,there
appearedin text. Of the 17 subjects who read were no subjects who read words but lacked
any words, 15 read more words in text than in phonemic segmentation skill. This indicates
isolation, indicating the facilitating effect of that having some phonemic segmentationskill
context (Goodman, 1965; Stanovich, 1986). In was necessary but not sufficient for learning to
the regression analysis, two factors explained readwords in text.
unique variance in the numberof words read in Observations of subjects' strategies for
text: the number of preprimer words subjects reading the words in text suggested that only
had read on the pretest, and the number of one subject identified words by reciting and
blends correctly segmented into phonemes (see pointing to all the preceding words in the line.
Table3). Thus, the relationship between word reading
A scatter plot of scores was examined to and phonemic segmentationis not explainedby
clarify this relationship.In Figure 3, datapoints skill in fingerpoint-readingthe text to deduce
are distinguishedaccordingto whether subjects words. A more common strategywhen subjects

Figure 3
Scatterplot of scores on the phonemic segmentationtask and word readingin text (with datapoints
distinguishedby whethersubjectsread any preprimerwords on pretest)

12
x PreprimerWords
m Read (N=12)
10
er O Not Read (N=18)

cis 88

" 6
O
l 0
4 - O

0 0 0
"2 O0
.0

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24
Number of Blends Segmented into Phonemes

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
456 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

were uncertain about words was to recite the Success overall was greater when subjects
poem to themselves in an attempt to pluck a examined lines of text to locate single words
likely candidatefromthe set of lexical possibili- spoken by the experimenter than when they
ties. Two subjects were observed doing this read individualwords pointed to by the experi-
with a couple of words. However, most words menter.Fewersubjects(19 percent)failed com-
were read directly without any observable de- pletely in this task than had failed at reading
ductive processing, indicating that subjects words in text (58 percent). In the regression
were using printcues to retrievethe wordsfrom analysis, two factorsaccountedfor uniquevari-
memory. ance in word location scores: preprimerword
Phonemic segmentation skill may have reading ability and letter-name knowledge.
provedimportantfor rememberinghow to read Wordreadingability may have enabledsubjects
words for two reasons. It may have helped sub- to rememberhow to read the words or how to
jects store words in memory to read them, as a readneighboringwords, which helped them de-
process of forming access routes into memory duce the location of targetwords. Knowinglet-
by linking letters in spellings to phonemes in ter names may have enabled subjects to locate
pronunciations(Ehri, 1987, in press). Alterna- words using these cues. One child verbalized
tively, phonemic segmentation may have en- this strategy,saying that 'paddled was easy to
abled subjectsto pluck words covertly from the find because of the p." Six subjects used the
memorized lines by helping them analyze first method of reciting the entire line, pointing to
soundsin words. The fact thatword readingwas each word in turnuntil they arrivedat the target
better in context than in isolation indicatesthat word.
subjectswere using contextin some way to read A scatterplot of scores was constructedto
the words. Decoding skill does not explain the clarify these relationships.In Figure4, the data
relationshipbecause all of the subjectswere un- points distinguish subjects according to their
skilled decoders, and the words were not easily preprimerword readingskill. It is apparentthat
decoded. subjects who were most successful in locating
Inspection of word misreadings revealed words in text (i.e., those who located more than
that many subjects respondedwith whole lines half the wordscorrectly)were mostly preprimer
when asked to read single words. For example, word readerswho knew almost all the letters. In
subjects misread the word blue as where the contrast,subjectswho were partiallysuccessful
river runs blue. The weaker readerswere more at locating words showed a range of letter
likely to respondwith whole lines thanwere the knowledge.
other subjects. Of the subjectswho had readno
preprimerwords and knew fewer than 15 letter
names (n = 12), 92 percent responded with
lines rather than words at least once. In con-
trast, of those who had read some preprimer
Discussion
wordsand knew most of the letters, only 33 per-
cent gave entire lines ratherthan words. Lines Resultsof the presentstudy indicatethat, in
may have been given for several reasons: Sub- orderfor emergentreadersto achieve some suc-
jects may have been "reading" the pictures cess learning to fingerpoint-read memorized
rather than the print, or they may have asso- text and to remember information about the
ciated lines rather than words with the letter print from this activity, they need to possess
sequences, or they may have had difficulty dis- some print-relatedknowledge. Phonemic seg-
tinguishing word boundaries in speech. Line mentation skill enables beginners to learn to
reading appeared to be a default response fingerpoint-readmemorizedtext and to remem-
among subjects who could read some words, ber how to read individual words in the text.
for subjects did not recite lines when they were Also, being able to read even a few preprimer
able to read single wordscorrectly. words enables beginners to rememberhow to

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 457

Figure 4
Scatter plot of scores on the letter-namingtask and on locating words in text (with data points
distinguishedby whethersubjectsreadany preprimerwords on pretest)

10
x PreprimerWords
9
Read (N=12)
.• 8 0 Not Read (N=18)
a 7
o 6 O-
".J 5 -
4-
0
3- 0 000
0 2 0 00 0O
I- OU0

8 0 o8
:0 0 0 0 0 00 0

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26
Number of Letters Named

read individualwords in the text. Knowing let- had expected this factor to have a greater im-
ter names enables beginners to locate words in pact.
lines of text they have read and to recognize Perhapsif we had measuredsubjects'abil-
when letters in the text have been altered. ity to use letters for spelling words, as Morris
One limitation of this study should be did (1983, 1989), or subjects' letter naming
noted. Because results are correlational,causal speed (Adams, 1990), or their knowledge of
interpretationsof the data must remain tenta- letter-sound correspondences, then letter
tive. Furtherexperimentalresearchis needed to knowledge might have accounted for unique
show that trainingin these capabilitiesbenefits variancein other dependentmeasures. Alterna-
fingerpoint-readingin the ways described. tively, perhapsletterknowledgewas less impor-
One purpose of the study was to examine tant for fingerpoint-readingbecause subjects
the importance of letter-name knowledge for did not have to rely exclusively on letter cues in
fingerpoint-readingand word reading. Results print to read correctly. Instead they could use
indicatedthat letter knowledgeenabled subjects their memory for the text, the pictures accom-
to locate individual words spoken by the ex- panying the text, and white spaces separating
perimenterin text, but it did not enable them letter sequences in the lines of text. Phonemic
to rememberhow to read individualwords, nor segmentation skill, by contrast, may have
to fingerpoint-read memorized text. Because provedof uniqueimportancebecause therewere
letter-nameknowledge is a powerful predictor no other means available for analyzing units in
of readingachievement(Shareet al., 1984), we speech correctly.

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
458 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

Phonemic segmentation skill contributed conclusions differ somewhat from those drawn
not only to subjects'ability to rememberhow to by Morris (1989). Morris interpretedhis longi-
read individual words in text, but also to their tudinal data as indicatingcausal links between
abilityto matchprintwith speech duringfinger- successively developing capabilities. He con-
point-reading. It proved even more important cluded that knowledge of initial consonantlet-
than preprimer word reading skill for finger- ters/sounds in words enables mastery of the
point-readingperformance,indicatingthatpho- ability to matchwrittenwords to spokenwords,
nemic units may be more centralin learningthis which in turnenables masteryof phonemicseg-
operationthan lexical units. Phonemicsegmen- mentation,which enables masteryof word read-
tation skill has been found to be an important ing. Our data suggest that letter knowledge,
contributorto reading skill by many other re- phonemic segmentationskill, and word reading
searchers(Ehri, 1979; Juel, Griffith, & Gough, ability each enable differentaspects of the abil-
1986; Liberman, Shankweiler, Liberman, ity to matchwrittenwordsand spoken words.
Fowler, & Fischer, 1977; Lundberg, Frost, & Differences in the difficulty levels of the
Petersen, 1988; Morris, 1983; Perfetti, Beck, tasks may accountfor some of the disparitybe-
Bell, & Hughs, 1987; Share,Jorm, Maclean, & tween Morris's(1989) conclusions and our own.
Matthews, 1984; Stanovich, Cunningham, & Morris measured more advanced phonemic
Feeman, 1984; Treiman& Baron, 1983; Tun- segmentation,whereaswe looked at a rudimen-
mer & Nesdale, 1982, 1985; Vellutino & tary form, possibly more comparable to his
Scanlon, 1987). The measureof phonemic seg- word-initialconsonanttask. Also, it may be that
mentation used here differed from measures causal relationshipsbetween these variablesare
used by others. Also, the subjects tested were reciprocal rather than unidirectional (Ehri,
younger and less literate than those in other 1979; Stanovich, 1986): Entrylevels of phone-
studies. mic segmentation and word reading abilities
In our phonemic task, preschoolers were may enable various aspects of fingerpoint-
taughtto associate four picturesof mouthposi- reading, which in turn may promote further
tions with isolated sounds and then to segment developmentof segmentationand word reading.
the soundsin wordsby pronouncingeach sound The importance of letter knowledge for
separatelyand selecting the appropriatemouth processingprintduringfingerpoint-readingwas
picture. Lindamoodand Lindamood(1975) in- also measured. We found that subjects who
cluded such a task in their programto teach au- were able to detectletter alterationsin text prior
ditory conceptualizationskill. We thought that to seeing the original and altered texts side-
this activity wouldprovidechildrenwith a more by-side were those who knew most of the letters
concrete method of analyzing sounds than the (see Figure 2). Our explanation is that letter
method of counting or tapping out phonemes knowledge providedthe basis for remembering
used by others, and hence might be more sensi- elements of the print configuration. It enabled
tive to individualdifferences between nonread- subjects to store aspects of the original text in
ers who lack spelling knowledgeto help them in memory, such as the first letter or the first and
these tasks (Ehri & Wilce, 1980; Tunmer & final letters in salient words. However,an alter-
Nesdale, 1982). From Table 1 and Figure 3, it native interpretationneeds to be considered.
is apparentthat we did create a task that could Subjectsmay have recognizedthe orthographic
be grasped by the least mature readers. Most abnormality or illegality of the altered text
(86 percent) were able to learn the picture- (e.g., WSWSAL WOHOEWNIH).Three facts
sound associations to criterion and to segment make such an explanation unlikely. First, the
some blends into phoneme segments. print was all in capital letters, so there were no
One purpose of the present study was to deviant word shapes signaling that something
discover the relations between beginners' was amiss. Second, most if not all of our sub-
knowledge of print and their success in various jects had insufficient experience with conven-
aspects of the fingerpoint-reading task. Our tional print to be sensitive to orthographic

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 459

structure. Third, research findings have indi- jects in Masonheimeret al.'s(1984) studyand in
cated that the ability to distinguish legal from the presentstudy did not notice letter alterations
illegal orthographicsequences does not emerge is that they did not know letters well enough to
typically until second grade (Allington, 1978; store the print in memory in order to detect the
Gibson & Levin, 1975; Juola, Schadler, Cha- changes.
bot, & McCaughey, 1978; Leslie & Shannon, Findings in the present study also indicate
1981; Leslie & Thimke, 1986; Massaro& Hes- that beginnersdo not rememberhow to read in-
tand, 1983). Our subjects were in preschool or dividual words from letter cues as a result of
kindergartenand had not even begun formal simply being exposed to the written words and
reading instruction.We conclude that sensitiv- their spoken matches in the course of finger-
ity to orthographicstructure does not explain point-reading memorized text. In the present
why subjects with letter knowledge were better study, it was primarilythose subjectswho dem-
able to detect letter changes in the text. The onstratedword reading ability prior to the ex-
more likely explanationis that letter knowledge perimentwho rememberedhow to read any of
enabled subjectsto rememberwhat the original the targetwords when the wordswere presented
text had looked like. in isolation. It is noteworthy,in fact, that these
The presentfindings suggest resolutionof a subjects did learn how to read some individual
discrepancy arising in two previous studies. words, even though there were plenty of other
Masonheimeret al. (1984) found that children non-printcues available, such as picturesand a
who were experts at reading environmental well-rehearsedtext. These results suggest that
print did not detect letter alterationsin familiar word reading memory may require a special
signs (e.g., Xepsi for Pepsi) and concluded that mechanism. The unprepared mind does not
these children paid little attention to letters. pick up words and rememberhow to read them
However,McGee, Lomax, and Head (1988) ob- spontaneously.On the other hand, the prepared
served environmentalprint readerspoint to and mind does not avoid using printcues to remem-
comment on letters in signs, even when they ber how to read words, even when there are
could not readthe signs, indicatingthatthey did other non-printcues available. Subjects in the
pay attention to letters. The present findings present study were novice readerslacking well-
suggest a possible distinction between paying developed word memory mechanisms, so the
attentionto letters and being able to remember strong showing of this variableis noteworthy.
letters. Further research may be needed to estab-
In the letter discriminationtask, most sub- lish the generalizability of our findings. Our
jects were able to attendto lettersto distinguish text was printed in uppercase letters, because
similarly spelled words. Yet, in the alteredtext beginners are known to be more familiar with
task, only those with substantialletter knowl- uppercasethan lowercaseletters (Smythe, Sten-
edge had sufficient memory of the original text nett, Hardy, & Wilson, 1970-1971), and be-
to recognize letter alterations when the texts cause uppercase letters are easier to
were not side-by-side. The correlationbetween discriminate. However, most big books are
these two tasks was low and was not signifi- printed in a mix of upper and lower case. We
cantly different from zero. These results show believe that the processes and relationshipsob-
that attendingto letters in print is not the same served in this studywould be apparentalso with
capability as being able to remember letters. text printed conventionally. However, this re-
Immaturereaders may notice letters and even mains to be verified.
name them, but unless they know letters well, In considering the implications of the
they will not be able to store letter information presentfindings for early readinginstruction,it
in memory. It is letter memory, not letter dis- is importantto recognize thatwe examinedonly
crimination,that is critical for developingread- the effects of short-termpracticeon fingerpoint-
ing skill (Ehri, 1987; Ehri & Wilce, 1985, reading, in a study conductedin the laboratory.
1987a, 1987b). Our explanationfor why sub- The practicethat Holdaway(1979) advocatesis

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
460 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

long-term, extending over 3 months, during exception: The teacher-instructed group did
which time "sixty books, twenty poems, ten better on the concepts-about-printtest. These
songs, and a chant or two"might be processed. resultsindicatethatimmersionin printis suffic-
Moreover,advocatesof memorizedtext reading ient for moving childreninto reading.However,
do not claim that practice alone moves child- it shouldbe notedthat immersionin this experi-
ren closer to independentreading. Holdaway's ment went beyond the simple practice effects
(1979) shared book experience includes more examined in the present study. Although the
thanjust practice; in addition, teachers are di- teacher-ledinstructionwas designed as part of
rected to model fingerpoint-reading,to explain only one of the treatments, it is likely that
and discuss the structureof alphabeticprint, to teacher explanation and modeling were also
draw attentionto printedwords, empty spaces, presentin the other immersioncondition, given
and letter-sound associations, and to provide the kinds of activities that studentsperformed.
corrective feedback and reinforcementfor stu- In conclusion, our results should not be in-
dents'efforts. terpretedas challenging the use of memorized
What the results of this study and the one text reading in classrooms. On the contrary,
by Morris (1989) indicateis thatbeginnersmay other researchindicatesthatthis is a very effec-
be unableto make much progressin various as- tive means of moving children into reading.
pects of memorized text reading without also Clay (1979) has incorporatedthe method into
acquiring specific print-related knowledge. her Reading Recovery program. It may be the
Holdaway(1979) and othersare less clear about most effective way to bring at-risk youngsters
the importanceof these skills for making pro- with limited home literacy experiences to the
gress, and when they are needed. To validate brinkof reading(Masonet al., 1990). Whatour
our conclusions, trainingstudies should be per- research does is to point out how children's
formedin which memorizedtext readingis con- success with such proceduresis influenced by
tinued over a longer period of time. Not only what they already know about print. It may be
practice but also instruction in print-related important to ensure that these skills are in
skills might be manipulatedexperimentallyto place before children are expected to display
assess their respective contributionsto reading fingerpoint-readingon their own. If childrendo
acquisition. not know lettersand do not have a graspof rudi-
Although the design of their study was mentaryphonemic segmentation,they will have
quasi-experimental,Reutzel, Oda, and Moore trouble performing the necessary operations.
(1989) have reportedone trainingstudy lasting However,this is not to say that childrenlacking
6 months. They compareda traditional"readi- these skills should not be exposed to
ness"kindergartenclassroomto a classroomus- fingerpoint-reading experiences. There are
ing an "immersion in print" approach and a many other benefits to be gained from teacher-
classroom using immersionplus teacher-ledin- modeled memorized text reading, such as
struction. The immersion approach included left-to-rightorientation,familiaritywith written
shared book experience following Holdaway's languagestructures,and realizationthat spoken
(1979) recommendations,plus language exper- language corresponds to written language.
ience and writing using invented spelling. Moreover, such advance exposure may enable
Teacher-ledinstructionconsisted of "short,ex- beginners to catch on very quickly to
plicit lessons on selected print concepts during fingerpoint-readingonce the enabling skills are
the daily rereadingof a big book."This included acquired.
instruction on "concept of word" and how to
identify words in text. Both immersion groups REFERENCES
ADAMS, M.J. (1990). to read: Thinking and
were found to surpass the readiness group on Beginning
learning aboutprint. Cambridge,MA: MIT Press.
print awarenessmeasures, readinesstests, and ALLINGTON, R.L.(1978).Sensitivityto orthographic
struc-
word readingtests. The two immersion groups tureas a functionof gradeandreadingability.Journal
performedequally well on these tests, with one of ReadingBehavior,10, 437-439.

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fingerpoint-reading EHRI& SWEET 461

CLAY,M.M.(1972). Reading: Thepatterningof complexbe- LUNDBERG, I., FROST, J., & PETERSEN, O. (1988).Effects of
haviour.Auckland, New Zealand:Heinemann. an extensive program for stimulating phonological
CLAY,M.M.(1979). The early detection of reading difficul- awareness in preschool children. Reading Research
ties. Auckland, New Zealand:Heinemann. Quarterly,23, 263-284.
DYSON, A.H. (1984). Learning to write/learning to do MARTIN, B., & BROGAN, P. (1972). Teacher's guide, instant
school: Emergent writers'interpretationsof school lit- readers. New York:Holt, Rinehart& Winston.
eracy tasks. Research in the Teachingof English, 18, MASON,J., & ALLEN,J. (1986). A review of emergent liter-
233-264. acy with implicationsfor researchand practice in read-
EHRI,L.C.(1979). Linguistic insight: Thresholdof reading ing. In E.Z. Rothkopf (Ed.), Review of research in
acquisition. In T.G. Waller& G.E. MacKinnon(Eds.), education(Vol. 13, pp. 3-47). Washington,DC: Ameri-
Reading research: Advances in theory and practice can EducationalResearchAssociation.
(Vol. 1, pp. 63-114). New York:AcademicPress. MASON, J., KERR, B., SINHA, S., & MCCORMICK, C. (1990).
EHRI,L.C. (1987). Learningto read and spell words. Jour- Shared book reading in an Early Start program for
nal of ReadingBehavior, 19, 5-31. at-risk children. In J. Zutell, S. McCormick, M.
EHRI,L.C.(in press). Reconceptualizingthe developmentof Connolly, & P. O'Keefe(Eds.), Literacytheoryand re-
sight word reading and its relationshipto recoding. In search: Analysesfrom multipleparadigms. Thirty-ninth
P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri, & R. Treiman(Eds.), Reading yearbookof the National Reading Conference(pp. 189-
acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 198). Chicago, IL: National ReadingConference.
EHRI, L.C., & WILCE, L.S. (1980). The influence of orthog- MASONHEIMER, P.E., DRUM, P.A., & EHRI, L.C. (1984).
raphy on readers' conceptualization of the phonemic Does environmentalprint identification lead children
structureof words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 371- into word reading? Journal of Reading Behavior, 16,
385. 257-272.
EHRI, L.C., & WILCE, L.S. (1985). Movementinto reading: MASSARO, D., & HESTAND, J. (1983). Developmentalrela-
Is the first stage of printedword learningvisual or pho- tions between reading ability and knowledge of ortho-
netic? ReadingResearch Quarterly,20, 163-179. graphic structure. Contemporary Educational
EHRI, L.C., & WILCE, L.S. (1987a). Cipher versus cue read- Psychology, 8, 174-180.
ing: An experimentin decoding acquisition. Journal of MCGEE, L.M., LOMAX, R.G., & HEAD, M.H. (1988). Young
EducationalPsychology, 79, 3-13. children'swritten language knowledge: What environ-
EHRI, L.C., & WILCE, L.S. (1987b). Does learning to spell mental and functionalprint reading reveals. Journal of
help beginners learn to read words? Reading Research ReadingBehavior,20, 99-118.
Quarterly,22, 47-65. MORRIS,D. (1983). Concept of word and phoneme aware-
GIBSON, E., & LEVIN, H. (1975). Thepsychology of reading. ness in the beginning reader. Research in the Teaching
Cambridge,MA: MIT Press. of English, 17, 359-373.
GOODMAN,K.S. (1965). A linguistic study of cues and mis- MORRIS, D. (1989). The relationshipbetween word aware-
cues in reading.ElementaryEnglish, 42, 639-643. ness and phonemeawareness in learningto read:A lon-
HOLDAWAY, D. (1979). The foundations of literacy. Sidney, gitudinal study in kindergarten.Manuscriptsubmitted
Australia:Ashton Scholastic. for publication.
JUEL, C., GRIFFITH, P.L., & GOUGH, P.B. (1986). Acquisi- MORRIS, D. (in press). Concept of word: A pivotal under-
tion of literacy: A longitudinalstudy of childrenin first standingin the learningto read process. In E. Hender-
and second grade. Journal of EducationalPsychology, son, S. Templeton, & D. Bear (Eds.), Developmentof
78, 243-255. orthographic knowledge: The foundations of literacy.
JUOLA, J., SCHADLER, M., CHABOT, R., & MCCAUGHEY, M. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(1978). The development of visual information- MORRIS, D., & HENDERSON, E.H. (1981). Assessing the be-
processing skills related to reading. Journal of ginning reader's'concept of word' Reading World,20,
ExperimentalChildPsychology, 25, 459-476. 279-285.
LESLIE, L., & SHANNON, A. (1981). Recognition of ortho- PERFETTI, C.A., BECK, I., BELL, L., & HUGHS, C. (1987).
graphic structureduring beginning reading. Journal of Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are recip-
ReadingBehavior, 13, 313-324. rocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children.
LESLIE, L., & THIMKE, B. (1986). The use of orthographic Merrill-PalmerQuarterly,33, 283-319.
knowledge in beginning reading. Journal of Reading REUTZEL, D.R., ODA, L.K., & MOORE, B.H. (1989). Devel-
Behavior, 18, 229-241. oping print awareness:The effect of three instructional
LIBERMAN, I.Y., SHANKWEILER, D., LIBERMAN, A.M., approacheson kindergarteners' printawareness,reading
FOWLER, C., & FISCHER, EW. (1977). Phonetic segmen- readiness, and wordreading.Journalof ReadingBehav-
tation and recoding in the beginning reader. In A.S. ior, 21, 197-217.
Reber & D.L. Scarborough(Eds.), Towarda psychol- SHARE, D.L., JORM, A.E, MACLEAN, R., & MATTHEWS, R.
ogy of reading (pp. 207-225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (1984). Sources of individualdifferencesin readingac-
LINDAMOOD, C.H., & LINDAMOOD, P.C. (1975). The audi- quisition.Journal of EducationalPsychology, 76, 1309-
tory discrimination in-depth program. Boston, MA: 1324.
TeachingResourcesCorporation. SLOSSON ORAL READING TEST. (1963). East Aurora, NY:
Slosson EducationalPublications.

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
462 READING RESEARCHQUARTERLY * 1991 XXVI/4

SMYTHE, P.C., STENNETT, R.G., HARDY, M., & WILSON, H.R. TUNMER, W., & NESDALE, A.R. (1985). Phonemicsegmenta-
(1970-1971). Developmental patterns in elemental tion skill and beginningreading.Journal of Educational
skills: Knowledge of upper-case and lower-case letter Psychology, 77, 417-427.
names. Journalof ReadingBehavior,3, 24-33. VELLUTINO, .ER., & SCANLON, D.M. (1987). Phonological
K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some
STANOVICH, coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability:
consequences of individual differences in the acqui- Evidence from a longitudinaland experimentalstudy.
sition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, Merrill-PalmerQuarterly,33, 321-363.
360-406. YADEN, D.B., & TEMPLETON,S. (Eds.). (1986). Metalin-
STANOVICH, K.E., CUNNINGHAM, A.E., & FEEMAN, D.J. guistic awareness and beginning literacy. Portsmouth,
(1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading NH: Heinemann.
progress. ReadingResearchQuarterly,19, 178-303.
SULZBY,E. (1985). Children'semergent readingof favorite
storybooks: A developmentalstudy. Reading Research Footnotes
Quarterly,20, 458-481. This research was supported by Grant No. HD-23719
TAYLOR,N.E. (1986). Developing beginning literacy con- awardedby the National Instituteof Child Health and Hu-
cepts: Contentand context. In D.B. Yaden, & S. Tem- man Development.
pleton (Eds.), Metalinguisticawareness and beginning We express our gratitude to the Davis Parent Nursery
literacy (pp. 173-184). Portsmouth,NH: Heinemann. School, the Redbug Montessori School, The Children's
TEALE, W.H., & SULZBY, E. (1987). The culturalpracticeof Center at North Davis ElementarySchool, and the Davis
storybook reading:Its effects on young children'sliter- Joint Unified School District for their cooperationand as-
acy development.In D.A. Wagner(Ed.), Thefuture of sistance in the conductof this study.
literacy in a changing world (pp. 111-130). New York: Requests for reprintsshould be sent to Linnea C. Ehri,
Pergamon. PhD Programin EducationalPsychology, CUNY Graduate
TREIMAN, R., & BARON, J. (1983). Phonemicanalysis train- School, 33 West 42nd St., New York, NY 10036-8099
ing helps children benefit from spelling-sound rules. (USA).
Memoryand Cognition, 11, 382-389.
TUNMER, W., & NESDALE, A.R. (1982). The effects of di- Received October30, 1989
graphs and pseudowordson phonemic segmentationin Revisionreceived August 23, 1990
young children.AppliedPsycholinguistics,3, 299-311. Final revision acceptedApril 9, 1991

This content downloaded from 62.122.72.154 on Sun, 22 Jun 2014 03:39:59 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like