You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield]

On: 30 December 2014, At: 11:50


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Aging & Mental Health


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/camh20

Life review in groups? An explorative analysis


of social processes that facilitate or hinder the
effectiveness of life review
a a a a
J. Korte , C.H.C. Drossaert , G.J. Westerhof & E.T. Bohlmeijer
a
Department of Psychology, Health and Technology, Faculty of Behavioural Sciences,
University of Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
Published online: 09 Oct 2013.

Click for updates

To cite this article: J. Korte, C.H.C. Drossaert, G.J. Westerhof & E.T. Bohlmeijer (2014) Life review in groups? An
explorative analysis of social processes that facilitate or hinder the effectiveness of life review, Aging & Mental Health,
18:3, 376-384, DOI: 10.1080/13607863.2013.837140

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2013.837140

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of
the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied
upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall
not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other
liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or
arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Aging & Mental Health, 2014
Vol. 18, No. 3, 376–384, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2013.837140

Life review in groups? An explorative analysis of social processes that facilitate or hinder the
effectiveness of life review
J. Korte*, C.H.C. Drossaert, G.J. Westerhof and E.T. Bohlmeijer
Department of Psychology, Health and Technology, Faculty of Behavioural Sciences, University of Twente,
Enschede, The Netherlands
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

(Received 28 August 2012; accepted 15 August 2013)

Objectives: Life review can be implemented within a group as well as on an individual level. There have been few discus-
sions about which the format is most effective. This study investigates the social aspects of a life-review group intervention
from the perspective of the client within the context of a large, randomized controlled trial.
Method: This was an exploratory study using a qualitative methodology. We conducted semi-structured interviews to
explore how participants felt about the benefits and barriers of receiving life review in a group. Transcripts of the interviews
were analyzed using inductive analysis.
Results: The social processes of life review in a group included experiencing a sense of belonging, feeling accepted, find-
ing good company, disclosing oneself, learning to express oneself, finding recognition, realizing that others have problems
too, being more successful at coping than others, learning from others, and being able to help others. Negative processes
were less often mentioned and included having difficulties with sharing in a group, finding no recognition, and anxiety
caused by the prospect of finding no recognition. These social processes can be divided into three categories: first, having a
good atmosphere in the group; second, disclosure to peers; and third, relating to others.
Conclusion: Our results reveal a variety of social processes that may facilitate the effects of life-review therapy. Future
research, however, is needed to further examine the importance of these social processes and their effects on depression.
Keywords: qualitative study; group processes; life review

Introduction reminiscence functions in a life-review intervention leads


Life review can be regarded as an evidence-based treat- to a reduction in the symptoms of depression and anxiety
ment for depression in later life (Bohlmeijer, Smit, & (Korte, Westerhof, & Bohlmeijer, 2012b).
Cuipers, 2003; Korte, Bohlmeijer, Cappeliez, Smit, & An important next step in intervention research is to
Westerhof, 2012a; Pinquart, Duberstein, & Lyness, 2007; study the mechanisms of life-review interventions. The
Pinquart & Forstmeier, 2012). It refers to a structured present study investigates the added value of implement-
evaluation of one’s life that involves two distinct but ing life review in a group format from the perspective of
related adaptive processes; integrative reminiscence and the client. In general, group therapy has many advantages
instrumental reminiscence (Butler, 1963; Haight, 1992; such as time and cost savings per patient and the possibil-
Westerhof, Bohlmeijer, & Webster, 2010; Wong, 1995). ity of treating a greater number of people and thereby
Integrative reminiscence is the integration of both positive reducing waiting lists (Jeffrey, 1999; Lewinsohn &
and negative memories within the life story, whereas Clarke, 1999; Morrison, 2001; Vinogradov & Yalom,
instrumental reminiscence concerns the use of past memo- 1994). In addition, the effects of group cohesion, imitative
ries to cope with the present problems (Cappeliez, 2002; behaviour, interpersonal learning, and opportunity for
Watt & Cappeliez, 2000; Westerhof et al., 2010; Wong, group members to serve as co-therapists and offer mutual
1995). The life-review interventions seem especially support are claimed as positive by-products of group
suited for older adults who experience loss of meaning in treatment. Moreover, group settings provide the group
life and who hold a negative view of themselves. Critical members with the opportunity to recognize common
life events may increase the use of specific negative func- experiences shared among other group members
tions of reminiscence such as reviving bitter experiences (Lewinsohn & Clarke, 1999; Morrison, 2001; Spence,
or escaping from past problems (Cappeliez, 2002; Wink 1989; Toseland & Siporin, 1986; Vinogradov & Yalom,
& Schiff, 2002; Wong, 1995). Associations between these 1994). However, there are also disadvantages associated
functions and poor mental health have been found with group therapy. Examples are the risk of one patient
(Cappeliez & O’Rourke, 2006; Cappeliez, O’Rourke, & monopolizing the group sessions, the start of conflicts
Chaudhury, 2005; Cully, LaVoie, & Gfeller, 2001; Korte, between group members, the development of subgroups,
Bohlmeijer, Westerhof, & Pot, 2011; O’Rourke, and the propensity for groups to descend into small talk
Cappeliez, & Claxton, 2011). Moreover, it has been (Morrison, 2001; Vinogradov & Yalom, 1994). Advan-
recently demonstrated that diminishing these negative tages of individual treatment include the ability to design

*Corresponding author. Email: j.korte@utwente.nl

Ó 2013 Taylor & Francis


Aging & Mental Health 377

therapy around the patient’s unique needs and to invest Understanding clients is important from a clinical per-
more therapist time for each patient (Lewinsohn & Clarke, spective. Clients can help us to understand what works in
1999; Morrsion, 2001; Spence, 1989). Disadvantages day-to-day in descriptive layman’s terms. This yields
include high fiscal and time costs, and missed valuable information about how interventions should be
opportunities to benefit from the effects of group dynam- developed in order to address specific problems as they
ics (Lewinsohn & Clarke, 1999; Morrison, 2001). are perceived by clients (Gallegos, 2005; Hanson &
There had been few discussions about which the for- Young, 2012). This manuscript addresses the above gaps
mat, group or individual, is most effective in life-review by investigating what clients regard as the positive and
interventions. Haight, Coleman, and Lord (1995) stressed negative aspects of receiving life review in a group set-
that conducting the intervention at an individual level is ting. The present study was conducted in the context of a
the linchpin of life review as it provides a sense of safety large, randomized controlled trial (RCT) (Korte et al.,
for discussing and sharing sensitive issues. In addition, 2012a, 2012b). In this trial, it was shown that, compared
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

individual sessions can be easily adapted to the personal to care-as-usual, participants in the life-review condition
needs of the participant and he/she might be more willing reported significantly decreased symptoms of depression
to talk about critical experiences (Pinquart & Forstmeier, with moderate effect sizes, both at post-treatment and
2012). On the other hand, others underscore the usefulness 3-month follow-up. The aim of the present study is two-
of group formats, as they promote social exchange with fold. First, we will explore which social processes take
other group members, which offer possibilities for place in the life-review group intervention according to
exchanging life experiences and learning from life experi- the participants. Second, to gain more insight into the
ences of other group members (Birren & Deutschman, effectiveness of the group format, we will investigate
1991; Bohlmeijer, Westerhof, & Emmerik-de Jong, 2008; which processes might facilitate or hinder its effectiveness
Korte et al., 2012a, 2012b; Pinquart & Forstmeier, 2012; on depression.
Watt & Cappeliez, 2000). It has been hypothesized that
individual life-review interventions may be more signifi-
cant in reducing depression and enhancing self-esteem Methods
and that life-review group interventions may be more Design
effective in enhancing life satisfaction, psychological
well-being, and social integration (Haight & Dias, 1992; This was an exploratory study using a qualitative method-
Pinquart & Forstmeier, 2012). Two large meta-analyses, ology. We conducted semi-structured interviews with
including 113 studies on the effect of life review on 20 participants to explore how they value the social
depression, show that the majority (65%) of life-review aspects of a life-review group intervention. Transcripts of
interventions is conducted in a group setting (Bohlmeijer the interviews were analysed using inductive analysis.
et al., 2003; Pinquart & Forstmeijer, 2012). It was found
that individual and group life-review formats are equally
effective in reducing depressive symptoms. The question Context
regarding which format is more effective might also The present study was part of a large, multi-site, prag-
depend on the setting in which life review is implemented. matic RCT that evaluated the effectiveness of ‘The stories
It has been shown that individual life review is effective we live by’; a life-review group intervention for older
for frail, older adults who are in the last years of their lives adults with mild to moderate depressive symptoms (Korte
(e.g., Haight, Michel, & Hendrix, 1998; Haight, Michel, & et al., 2012a, 2012b). The participants were randomly
Hendrix, 2000; Serrano, Latorre, Gatz, & Montanes, assigned to the life-review group intervention (n ¼ 100)
2004). The positive effects of life review in groups have or to care-as-usual (n ¼ 102). The design of the RCT has
been demonstrated in younger older adults (e.g., been described in detail elsewhere (Korte, Bohlmeijer, &
Bohlmeijer et al., 2008; Cappeliez, 2002; Pot et al., 2010; Smit, 2009).
Korte et al., 2012a, 2012b). These findings might suggest The life-review intervention was developed by
that old older adults benefit more from more traditional Bohlmeijer and Westerhof (2010) and consisted of eight
life review, which was originally described by Butler weekly sessions of 2 hours. The intervention had three
(1974) as an individual process of coming to terms with core elements. First, the integration of difficult life events
unresolved conflicts from the past and with approaching from the past; second, the development of meaningful and
vulnerability and death. Young older adults might benefit agentic life stories that help the participants to better cope
more from life review in a group setting which involves with the present life events and to set new goals; third, the
evaluating one’s entire lifespan and setting realistic future retrieval of specific positive memories which can serve as
goals in the presence of like-minded people (Westerhof building blocks for the new life stories. The intervention
et al., 2010). was implemented in small groups of four people. This
In summary, although there is some knowledge about small group format guarantees that every participant has
the advantages and disadvantages of group therapy in gen- enough time during each session to discuss his or her auto-
eral, insights into how social processes might facilitate or biographical writings. In addition, the therapists who
hinder the effectiveness of life review are still missing. offered the interventions were responsible for facilitating
Moreover, the group processes of life review have never the group process. In the introductory session, they
been studied from the perspective of the client. made arrangements with the participants regarding
378 J. Korte et al.

confidentiality. During the intervention meetings, partici- informed consent. The interviews were audiotaped with the
pants had the opportunity to exchange and discuss their participants’ permission and transcribed verbatim. Tran-
experiences with one another. The therapists ensured that scripts were anonymized.
each individual participant had an equal amount of time
to share his or her life story.
Data analysis
All data were analysed by two coders (JK and CHCD).
Sample The coders were blinded with regard to the effects of the
In the current study, 20 participants were selected for the intervention on the participants’ depression. During the
semi-structured interviews, which took place three weeks first stage of the process, both coders read the transcripts
after the final session of the intervention. To gain more in their entirety, several times, to familiarize themselves
insights into the social processes of life review, which with the data and identify emerging social themes.
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

might facilitate or hinder its effectiveness, participants Relevant quotes were selected and divided into themes
were selected based upon the effectiveness of the inter- and subthemes by each of the two coders independently
vention on depressive symptoms. Hence, the current study using inductive analysis (Patton, 1990). The two coders
sample includes two groups of participants. The first met to discuss their findings and resolve any differences,
group consisted of 10 participants who demonstrated a and subsequently developed a thematic framework that is
substantial reduction in depressive symptoms after the presented in Table 1. During the next step of the process,
intervention (a difference of 5 on the Center for each interview was independently re-read and recoded
Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D); using this thematic framework. A coding sheet was filled
Beekman et al., 1997), while the second group consisted out for each participant to identify whether a particular
of 10 participants whose depressive symptoms did not social theme was present or not (Table 2). Each theme
improve (a difference of 1 or an increase on the could be rated only once per participant and one sentence
CES-D). These two groups of participants were randomly could be rated into one or more categories. The results
selected and did not differ with regard to age and sex. were discussed to reach absolute consensus. As the two
Participants had a mean age of 62 years and 80% of them coders had already discussed the categories in the previ-
were female, which is representative for the total sample ous step, there was little disagreement.
of participants in the RCT. All quotes presented in this article were translated
from Dutch into English by a native English speaker. To
ensure anonymity, we removed all identifying information
Data collection from the quotes.
Participants were interviewed individually, following a
semi-structured interview schedule to allow flexibility,
individual contextualization and probing of issues that Results
arose (Smith, 1995). The interviews were part of a pro- In general, clients mentioned many more benefits (posi-
cess evaluation and were conducted by three inter- tive processes) than barriers (negative processes) when
viewers. We made sure that these interviewers were asked about their experiences of taking part in the life-
trained to address positive as well as negative aspects review intervention in a group setting. All themes that
of group processes. The participants were questioned in derived from the analysis have been summarized in
an open and non-directive way about their positive and Table 1 and discussed below.
negative experiences with the intervention, and particu-
lar attention was also paid to the specific components
of the intervention. The main focus of this study was Positive social processes
how the participants experienced life review in a group Ten positive social processes of life review were identi-
setting both positively and negatively. Therefore, the fied, which could be divided into three categories.
most important question encapsulating the essence of
this study was: ‘How did you feel about sharing your
personal experiences in a group?’ Participants were Good group atmosphere
encouraged to elaborate on this issue by various neutral The first category was described as ‘good group atmos-
probing questions such as, ‘Which were positive or neg- phere’ and included a sense of belonging, feeling
ative experiences?’, ‘Could you give an example for accepted, and finding good company. Respondents men-
this?’, ‘Could you say something more about this?’, ‘Is tioned a good atmosphere as being a precondition for
this all there is?’, and ‘Do you know any other sharing, but many also described the atmosphere in the
aspects?’ (Emans, 2002). In addition, all interviews group as a beneficial, healing process in its own right.
were scanned for any other relevant quotes that referred
to social aspects of life review and in which there was
an explicit relation to other participants. Sense of belonging
All interviews were conducted in the participants’ Most of the participants experienced a sense of belonging
homes. The interviews lasted between 45 and 120 minutes. to the group (65%): ‘You don’t really know each other, so
Before the interview, all participants gave their written the fact that you can actually come together in a group
Aging & Mental Health 379

Table 1. Positive and negative social processes of life review.

Themes Typical quote

Positive processes
Good group atmosphere
Sense of belonging It was very open. No holds were barred: you could say whatever you wished, all the
emotional issues were dealt with too.
Feeling accepted I was taken seriously and that felt great.
Finding good company I found it intimate and cosy.
Disclosure to peers
Disclosing oneself I thought that I would never be as open as that, and that I would never dare to talk about such
things after everything I have been through. And yet, a kind of energy is released, with all
of us together, so that it just ends up coming out.
Learning to express oneself I believe that I have become more self-assured, I also find it much easier to speak out.
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

Relating to others
Finding recognition Something that also happened with amazing regularity was that we found recognition in each
other’s stories. It is fantastic to find such recognition.
Realizing that others have It is a fantastic feeling to know that so many other people are also struggling with all sorts of
problems too problems.
Being more successful at coping Despite all the horrible things that I have experienced, when I look at myself and compare
than others myself to the other participants, then I actually come out very well.
Learning from others Through their personal experiences, they have given me some extra bonus points. This is why
it is good to talk to other people once in a while.
Being able to help others The fact that you are capable of helping others in some small way too. Offering them a
listening ear and being open to their needs.
Negative processes
Difficulties with sharing in a I found it difficult to reveal things about myself in front of others. I just don’t feel
group comfortable talking about myself.
Finding no recognition I believe in God and this gives me a lot of inner strength, but others in the group just said:
well, I don’t believe in God at all.
Anxiety of no recognition When you had to tell your own personal story, then all the old fears came to the surface
again: Will I be understood and can I actually say this? Am I sharing too much?

and say: ‘like we have just done now. . .’ – that is what good atmosphere enabled participants to feel safe enough
creates a bond’. Besides, participants valued the good and to share intimate experiences: ‘Opening up came very nat-
open atmosphere within the group: ‘It was very open. No urally to us, actually. We felt safe enough to share our
holds were barred. People could say whatever they experiences’ and ‘Don’t forget: It was a safe and trusted
wanted, very emotional issues were also dealt with’. This group. The following was also said: we expect all of you

Table 2. Differences between participants with and without improved depressive symptoms.

Total sample (n ¼ 20) Improvementa (n ¼ 10) No improvementb (n ¼ 10)

Themes N % N % N %

Positive processes
Good group atmosphere
Sense of belonging 13 65 7 70 6 60
Feeling accepted 9 45 5 50 4 40
Finding good company 3 15 2 20 1 10
Disclosing to peers
Disclosing oneself 8 40 5 50 3 30
Learning to express oneself 3 15 1 10 2 20
Relating to others
Finding recognition 8 40 5 50 3 30
Realizing that others have problems too 7 35 5 50 2 20
Being more successful at coping than others 4 20 2 20 2 20
Learning from others 8 40 5 50 3 30
Being able to help others 4 20 1 10 3 30
Negative processes
Difficulties with sharing in a group 4 20 0 0 4 40
Finding no recognition 3 15 2 10 1 10
Anxiety about not finding recognition 4 20 2 20 2 20
Difference of 5 on the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D).
a

Difference of 1 or an increase on the CES-D.


b
380 J. Korte et al.

to be sure that whatever you say in the group will stay in to speak out’. Other participants explicitly gained better
the group. That meant that each person found it easier to personal insights into the way they act and present them-
take the plunge and share something in the group. And so, selves through direct comments from others: ‘During the
then you just came to expect it of each other’. sessions, it was revealed that I simply assume that other
people will automatically understand me, without me hav-
ing to say anything to them. That is not the case. So I
Feeling accepted learned that in fact I must say something if I want to be
A related aspect to experiencing a sense of belonging was sure that somebody else has truly understood me’.
that many participants felt accepted, understood, and
heard by the other participants and therapists (45%).
Many participants perceived this acceptance by others as Relating to others
beneficial and rewarding: ‘I was taken very seriously The third category referred to the process of ‘relating to
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

when I was there: that did me a lot of good’ and ‘I was others’ and included finding recognition, realizing that
never able to say stuff like “how do you experience this?” others have problems too, being more successful at coping
or “how did you feel about that?” It was definitely nice to than others, learning from others, and being able to help
be in a new situation for a change and to be able to talk others.
about your past. In an environment where I was not seen
as someone who only ever cried their eyes out or
complained’. Finding recognition
Many participants found recognition in the experiences of
other participants (40%): ‘Something that also happened
Finding good company with amazing regularity was that we found recognition in
Finally, some participants mentioned that they simply each other’s stories. It is fantastic to find such recog-
appreciated the personal contact with other peers (15%): nition’. Mainly, participants recognized themselves in
‘I found it intimate and cosy’. In some cases, participants other participants because they were of the same age and
even became friends with each other. generation: ‘We all belong to the same generation, so you
hear a lot of things like Oh yes, look at these photos, the
outfits, that image of the 1950s, and those parents and
Disclosure to peers how they behaved’ and ‘The generations where the focus
The second category referred to disclosure to peers, which was heavily on upbringing – that keeps coming up time
involved disclosing oneself and learning to express and time again’.
oneself.

Realizing that others have problems too


Disclosing oneself Many participants compared their own experiences with
Many interviewees indicated that they felt good, proud, or those of the other participants (35%). They realized that
relieved about sharing experiences and expressing emo- they were not the only ones who had been through a lot
tions with others (40%): ‘I experienced it as being very during their lives and they felt good about that insight: ‘It
invigorating and sometimes also a great relief that you is a fantastic feeling to know that so many other people
could share such things with each other’. Sometimes, the are also struggling with all sorts of problems’ and ‘In my
participants admitted that they had never spoken before case, it suddenly dawned on me that other people have
about those emotional experiences: ‘I thought that I would also had their own personal struggles to contend with.
never be as open as that, and that I would never dare to There are very few people who go through life without
talk about such things after everything I have been encountering a single problem’. By comparing their own
through. And yet, a kind of energy is released, with all of experiences with others, participants noticed that almost
us together, so that it just ends up coming out’. Some par- everybody has their own problems, which helped to put
ticipants said that they were able to disclose their experi- their problems and experiences into perspective: ‘You put
ences only because they were in the company of people everything into perspective. In the sense that other people
whom they felt were in the same boat: ‘Any feelings of also go through a lot. It is just part of life’.
embarrassment tend to leave you when you are in the mid-
dle of such an exercise. You can be totally open about
yourself because everyone is in this together’. Being more successful at coping than others
Another way in which participants compared their own
experiences with those of the other participants was that
Learning to express oneself few noticed that they were better able to cope with certain
Aside from the fact that participants felt good about shar- life experiences and problems than their peers in the group
ing experiences, a few participants mentioned explicitly (20%): ‘I thought that I was actually the strongest one out
that they had learned to express themselves better by shar- of the other three participants when it came to conjuring
ing their experiences with others (15%): ‘I believe that I up positive memories’. Other participants felt proud about
have become more self-assured, I also find it much easier the very fact that they had survived despite all the misery
Aging & Mental Health 381

they had experienced: ‘Despite all the horrible things that I was cut off mid sentence; they just misunderstood me. I
I have experienced, when I look at myself and compare found that awful’.
myself to the other participants, then I actually come out
very well’.
Anxiety about the prospect of not finding recognition
Sometimes participants had an implicit anxiety about not
Learning from others being recognized by the other participants (20%): ‘When
Many participants found it beneficial to learn from the you had to tell your own personal life story, then all the
other participants, how others have coped with particular old fears came to the surface again: Will I be understood?
life experiences, situations, or problems (40%). Examples and Can I actually say this? Am I sharing too much?’.
are: ‘Through their personal experiences they have given Some participants were afraid that their story was not
me some extra bonus points. This is why it is good to talk interesting enough to share: ‘I feel that my own life story
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

to other people once in a while’ and ‘When we discussed was not interesting enough, but of course that was not
it with each other, then the other participants sometimes such a big issue’. Other participants worried about not
gave very wise answers’. being a good storyteller: ‘I was worried that I would not
be able to tell my story in a spirited enough way’.

Being able to help others


Participants not only reported that they learned something Differences between participants
from others, some of them also discovered that they can Table 2 shows the number of participants that mentioned
offer something meaningful to somebody else (20%): a particular social process at least once in their interview.
‘The fact that you are capable of helping others in some Although the data should be interpreted with caution due
small way too, offering them a listening ear and being to the small sample size, Table 2 shows the differences
open to their needs’. Some participants demonstrated to between participants that had a decrease in depressive
other participants how they coped with certain situations. symptoms after the interventions, and participants those
In this way, they experienced that they could be a good did not. For each participant, we calculated a difference
example to others: ‘I helped those other ladies too. After score between the number of positive and negative social
all, I interpreted it in my own way. When you get older, processes mentioned by that participant. Using a t-test, we
you become more laid back in the way you think about tested whether the participants without an improvement in
things and this, in my view, was the advantage that I had depressive symptoms had lower scores than the partici-
compared to the other participants’. pants who did improve. This resulted in a marginally sig-
nificant trend; t(17) ¼ 1.7, p ¼ 0.098. Therefore,
participants without an improvement mention on average
Negative social processes somewhat fewer positive social processes than negative
Negative social processes could be divided into three social processes. More specifically, the largest differences
themes, which included difficulties with sharing in a for positive processes (difference of 20% or more) were
group, finding no recognition, and anxiety caused by the that more participants who benefited from the intervention
prospect of finding no recognition. were able to disclose themselves, find recognition, realize
that others have problems too, and learn from others. The
negative process that was mentioned by more participants
Difficulties with sharing in a group who did not improve was difficulties with sharing in a
A few participants did not acknowledge the good atmo- group.
sphere in the group (20%). They felt uncomfortable about
sharing their own personal experiences in a group setting:
‘I found it difficult to reveal things about myself in front Discussion
of others. I just don’t feel comfortable talking about In this study, we aimed to explore the social processes of
myself’. Other participants realized that they are not life review that might facilitate or hinder its effectiveness
much of a group person: ‘I’m not really a group person, from the perspective of the client. The results of this study
actually. In fact, I’m a bit reserved and quite shy’ and show that conducting life review in a group is highly
‘I’m not a great talker. I’m quite good at talking to people appreciated by the participants and that the group format
on a one-to-one basis, but when I am in the company of a might facilitate the effects of life review. In general,
lot of people, I tend to become very nervous’. clients reported many more benefits (positive processes)
than barriers (negative processes) in relation to receiving
life review in a group.
Finding no recognition Ten social processes were identified that may facilitate
Some participants explicitly experienced that they were the effect of life review. These processes could be further
not recognized by the other participants or by the thera- divided into three categories. The first category refers to
pists (15%): ‘I believe in God and get a lot of support having a good atmosphere in the group. The most impor-
from this, others simply said: oh, I don’t believe in God at tant themes within this category were experiencing a
all’. One participant even felt misunderstood: ‘Sometimes sense of belonging and feeling accepted and feeling safe
382 J. Korte et al.

enough to share life stories with others. These aspects of each other’s experiences. This requires another partici-
seem to be prerequisites for an effective life-review group pant to have had similar experiences and, hence, under-
intervention. When sharing experiences with peers, a stand them (Helgeson & Gottlieb, 2000). Conversely,
warm and accepting atmosphere must be present for some participants found no recognition or experienced
health benefits to appear (Lepore & Helgeson, 1999; anxiety about the prospect of not finding recognition. One
Lepore, Silver, Wortman, & Wayment, 1996). This reason why validation might not occur is that not all group
accepting group atmosphere is related to the concept of members respond in the same way to a certain level of dis-
wisdom environment (Randall & Kenyon, 2002), which is tress (Coates & Winston, 1983), or that the group is too
defined as an environment where stories are listened to in heterogeneous, and participants therefore have too few
an open, non-judgmental manner, in order to help co- experiences in common (Helgeson & Gottlieb, 2000).
author life stories. Having a good atmosphere in the group Other social aspects reported by participants involve
is not only a prerequisite, but might also be a direct benefi- social comparison. In times of uncertainty or stress,
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

cial consequence of participating in a group intervention. people compare themselves with others to evaluate their
It may be a very rewarding (and healing) experience to feelings and abilities (Festinger, 1954). Realizing that
have full attention, not only from a therapist, but also others have problems too may normalize the experience
from people who are able to understand your situation in a (Helgeson & Gottlieb, 2000). Indeed, in our study many
way that does not happen in daily life. Unfortunately, participants reported that they found it helpful to learn
some participants felt uncomfortable when sharing their that others face similar problems. Participants also com-
experiences in a group. This aspect seems to be related to pared themselves with others who were less successfully
the effectiveness of life review, as it was reported more coping with their problems than they were. This so-called
frequently by participants who showed no improvement ‘downward social comparison’ is supposed to enhance
in their depression. Our findings underscore the impor- self-esteem, as participants are enabled to feel fortunate
tance of having a good atmosphere in a group. Therefore, about their own circumstances in comparison with others
we stress the importance of creating a warm, open, and who are worse off (Wills, 1981). Another social process
accepting atmosphere. This might be achieved by using was that participants learned from other participants. It is
strategies to facilitate the group process as used in the most likely that participants are able to provide sound
present study, i.e., implementing life-review interventions advice and useful ideas about ways of coping because
in small groups, making arrangements with the partici- they have first-hand experience with the stressor
pants regarding confidentiality, and taking care that each (Helgeson & Gottlieb, 2000). In addition to participants
individual participant has an equal amount of time to being able to learn from others, they were able to help
share his or her life story. In addition, these results have others. This refers to the ‘helper-therapy principle’
implications for the inclusion of participants in a life- (Riessman, 1965). Helping others leads to feelings of self-
review group intervention. It should be a requirement that efficacy and competence, which might increase positive
participants are willing to review their lives in a group feelings about the self. These aspects might be optimally
and that they are willing to share their experiences with utilized when group leaders adopt an attitude of curiosity
others. If people are not willing to do this or are unable to and not knowing. In this way, co-authorship is created,
do this, they should be referred to individual life-review which enables participants to restructure their life stories
therapy. Another theme that was linked to having a good according to their own preferences (White & Epston,
atmosphere in a group was that some participants found 1990). The above processes help participants to develop
that they enjoyed the company of each other. the skills required to cope with their problems, which
The second category involves disclosing to peers. according to Carkhuff (1973) is a main ingredient in the
Many of the respondents described it as a relief that they effectiveness of group therapy. These processes may
were able to disclose their personal life story to others. It strengthen instrumental reminiscence, an adaptive mecha-
has been shown that disclosing and expressing feelings nism of life review (Korte et al., 2012a, 2012b; Wong,
about traumatic events has health benefits (Pennebaker & 1995). To effectively deal with life experiences, people
Beall, 1986; Pennebaker, Colder, & Sharp, 1990). Being need a sense of mastery and competence. By comparing
able to disclose and express oneself to peers can be seen as their own experiences with other participants, people
important steps in the process of integrating painful memo- become aware of adverse and successful coping strategies,
ries; an important adaptive mechanism of life review called which might help them to effectively cope with present
integrative reminiscence (Korte et al., 2012a, 2012b; difficulties (Cappeliez, 2002; Wong, 1995).
Wong, 1995). By recounting painful memories, participants One might wonder whether the social processes as
will be able to become aware of the impact of painful reported in this study are unique to group therapy or life-
events in their lives, work through the emotions, and begin review interventions. Some processes, like feeling
the process of acceptance and integration with the present. accepted or disclosing oneself, may also be part of indi-
In this way, both the positive and negative experiences of vidual therapy as non-specific factors in individual ther-
the past can be incorporated into participants’ self-concepts apy are often discussed in this way as well (Corey &
(Cappeliez, 2002; Wong, 1995). Corey, 2002; Horvath & Bedi, 2002). However, clients
The third category of group processes refers to relating expressed these processes as part of their relations to their
to others. An important process in this category is finding peers and the group as a whole, rather than to the thera-
recognition from the group, which involves the validation pists. Disclosing and finding acceptance and recognition
Aging & Mental Health 383

by peers may have another function than by a trained ther- studies that also allow addressing the inter-coder reliability.
apist. In a new online study, we therefore compare life A fourth limitation is that the semi-structured interview we
review as an individual intervention guided by a counsel- used was developed specifically for the aims of this study
lor to life review in a non-guided peer group. To gain a and was not derived from previous studies on group versus
better understanding of the mechanisms of life review, we individual settings. However, the articles we mention in
compared the processes that were mentioned by partici- the introduction on group and individual processes only
pants who benefited from the therapy with those men- describe the advantages and disadvantages of group versus
tioned by participants who did not get benefited (i.e., individual therapy settings. These assumptions were not
improved on scores on depression). Although the results based on empirical studies. To the best of our knowledge,
must be interpreted with some caution due to the small empirical studies on these advantages and disadvantages
sample size, patients without an improvement mention on are rather scarce.
average somewhat fewer positive social processes and To conclude, this study suggests that several social
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

more negative social processes. More specifically, it processes might facilitate the effect of life review in
seems that participants who benefited from the interven- groups. Feeling accepted and feeling safe enough to share
tion make more social comparisons, in that they were bet- life stories with others as well as experiencing a sense of
ter able to find recognition, to realize that others have belonging, seem to be prerequisites for an effective life-
problems too and to learn from others. This might indicate review intervention. Processes that were mentioned most
that they are better able to normalize their experiences. frequently were: being able to disclose oneself, finding
These results suggest that social comparisons facilitate recognition, learning from others, and realizing that others
the effectiveness of life review on depression. have problems too. Experimental studies are needed to
To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first that investigate which social processes moderate or mediate
investigated the additional value of offering life review in the effectiveness of life review.
a group setting and from the perspective of the client.
Some limitations should be acknowledged. First, it should
be noted that our groups were rather small (up to four peo-
References
ple). Therapists were instructed to guard that each partici-
pant had 20–30 minutes’ time to review his or her Beekman, A.T.F., Deeg, D.J., van Limbeek, J., Braam, A.W., de
Vries, M.Z., & van Tilburg, W. (1997). Criterion validity of
individual life stories. As a result, some of the common the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES-
disadvantages of group settings may not have been D): Results from a community-based sample of older subjects
reported. In addition, the life-review intervention in this in the Netherlands. Psychological Medicine, 27, 231–235.
study is a combination of home-task writings and talking Birren, J.E., & Deutchman, D.E. (1991). Guided autobiography
about these writings in a group. Like this, participants have groups for older adults. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins
University Press.
even more time for individual processes in which they can Bohlmeijer, E., Smit, F., & Cuipers, P. (2003). Effects of reminis-
transfer the content of the intervention into their lives. One cence and life review on late-life depression: A meta-analysis.
further reason why most comments of the participants were International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 1088–1094.
in favour of the group setting might be that mainly those Bohlmeijer, E.T., & Westerhof, G.J. (2010). The stories we live
people participated that like groups and profit from group by [in Dutch]. Amsterdam: Boom Uitgeverij.
Bohlmeijer, E.T., Westerhof, G.J., & Emmerik-de Jong, M.
discussions. A second limitation is that the study sample (2008). The effects of integrative reminiscence on meaning
was rather small. As we achieved data saturation, we can in life: Results of a quasi-experimental study. Aging & Men-
assume that all relevant social processes have been identi- tal Health, 12, 639–646.
fied. However, to gain more insight into the extent to Butler, R.N. (1963). The life-review: An interpretation of remi-
which each of these social processes is experienced, more niscence in the aged. Psychiatry, 26, 65–76.
Butler, R.N. (1974). Successful aging and the role of the life
quantitative studies are needed. Moreover, experimental review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 22(12),
studies are needed to investigate whether a group life- 529–535.
review intervention is indeed more effective than an indi- Cappeliez, P. (2002). Cognitive-reminiscence therapy for
vidual life-review intervention, and to investigate whether depressed older adults in day hospital and long-term care. In
social processes causally hinder or facilitate the effective- J.D. Webster & B.K. Haight (Eds.), Critical advances in
reminiscence work: From theory to application (pp. 300–
ness of life-review group interventions. These issues will 313). New York, NY: Springer.
be addressed by our future study in which we will use a Cappeliez, P., & O’Rourke, N. (2006). Empirical validation of a
large RCT to evaluate the effectiveness of two different model of reminiscence and health in later life. Journal of
life-review formats; a self-help intervention versus an inter- Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social
vention with online peer support. A third limitation is that Sciences, 61, 237–244.
Cappeliez, P., O’Rourke, N., & Chaudhury, H. (2005). Functions
we are unable to calculate an intra-class correlation. In cod- of reminiscence and mental health in later life. Aging &
ing the interviews, we made sure that consensus was Mental Health, 9, 295–301.
arrived between the two independent coders in all phases Carkhuff, R.R. (1973). The art of problem-solving: A guide for
of the process, both in the development of thematic frame- developing problem-solving skills for parents, teachers,
work and in categorizing the data. An indication of the counselors, and administrators. Amherst, MA: Human
Resource Development Press.
validity of this thematic framework is that it fits the group Coates, D., & Winston, T. (1983). Counteracting the deviance of
processes that are discussed in the literature rather well. depression: Peer support groups for victims. Journal of
However, the framework needs to be validated in future Social Issues, 39, 169–194.
384 J. Korte et al.

Corey, M.S., & Corey, G. (2002). Groups: Process and Practice. O’Rourke, N., Cappeliez, P., & Claxton, A. (2011). Functions of
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. reminiscence and the psychological well-being of young-old
Cully, J.A., LaVoie, D., & Gfeller, J.D. (2001). Reminiscence, and older adults over time. Aging & Mental Health, 15,
personality, and psychological functioning in older adults. 272–281.
Gerontologist, 41, 89–95. Patton, M.Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research meth-
Emans, B. (2002). Interviewing. Theory, technique and training ods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
[in Dutch]. Groningen: Stenfert Kroese. Pennebaker, J.W., Beall, S.K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. event: Toward an understanding of inhibition and disease.
Human Relations, 7, 117–140. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 274–281.
Gallegos, N. (2005). Study client perspectives on what contrib- Pennebaker, J.W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L.K. (1990). Accelerating
utes to symptom relief in psychotherapy: A qualitative the coping process. Journal of Personality and Social
outcome. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 45, 355–382. Psychology, 58, 528–537.
Haight, B.K. (1992). Long-term effects of a structured life- Pinquart, M., Duberstein, P.R., & Lyness, J.M. (2007). Effects of
review process. Journal of Gerontology, 47, 312–315. psychotherapy and other behavioral interventions on clini-
Downloaded by [Computing & Library Services, University of Huddersfield] at 11:50 30 December 2014

Haight, B.K., Coleman, P., & Lord, K. (1995). The linchpins of a cally depressed older adults: A meta-analysis. Aging &
successful life review: Structure, evaluation and individuality. Mental Health, 11, 645–657.
In B.K. Haight & J.D. Webster (Eds.), The art and science of Pinquart, M., & Forstmeier, S. (2012). Effects of reminiscence
reminiscing: Theory, research, methods and applications (pp. interventions on psychosocial outcomes: A meta-analysis.
179–192). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis. Aging & Mental Health, 16, 541–558.
Haight, B.K, & Dias, J.K. (1992). Examining key variables in Pot, A.M., Bohlmeijer, E.T., Onrust, S., Melenhorst, A.S.,
selected reminiscing modalities. International Psychogeriat- Veerbeek, M., & de Vries, W. (2010). The impact of life-
rics, 4, 279–290. review on depression in older adults: A randomized con-
Haight, B.K., Michel, Y., & Hendrix, S. (1998). Life review: trolled trial. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 572–581.
Preventing despair in newly relocated nursing home resi- Randall, W., & Kenyon, G. (2002). Reminiscence as reading our
dents short- and long-term effects. International Journal of lives: Toward a wisdom environment. In J. Webster & B.
Aging and Human Development, 47, 119–142. Haight (Eds.), Critical advances in reminiscence: Theoreti-
Haight, B.K., Michel, Y., & Hendrix, S. (2000). The extended cal, empirical, and clinical perspectives (pp. 233–253). New
effects of the life review in nursing home residents. Interna- York, NY: Springer.
tional Journal of Aging and Human Development, 50, 151–168. Reissman, F. (1965). The helper therapy principle. Social Work,
Hanson, B., & Young, M.A. (2012). Why depressive symptoms 10, 29–38.
cause distress: The clients’ perspective. Journal of Clinical Serrano, J.P., Latorre, J.M., Gatz, M., & Montanes, J. (2004).
Psychology, 68, 860–874. Life-review therapy using autobiographical retrieval practice
Helgeson, V.S., & Gottlieb, B.H. (2000). Support groups. In S. for older adults with depressive symptomatology. Psychol-
Cohen, L.G. Underwood, & B.H. Gottlieb (Eds.), Social support ogy and Aging, 19, 270–277.
measurement and intervention: A guide for health and social Smith, J.A. (1995). Semi-structured interviewing and qualitative
scientists (pp. 221–245). Oxford: Oxford University Press. analysis. In J.A. Smith, R. Harre, & L. van Langenhove
Horvath, A.O., & Bedi, R.P. (2002). The therapeutic alliance. In (Eds.), Rethinking methods in psychology (pp. 9–26).
J.C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy relationships that work: London: Sage.
Therapist relational contributions to effective psychotherapy Spence, S. (1989). Cognitive-behavior therapy in the manage-
(pp. 37–69). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. ment of chronic, occupational pain of the upper limbs.
Jeffrey, N. (1999). Health care: HMOs face questions over push Behavior Research and Therapy, 27, 435–446.
for group therapy. The Wall Street Journal, 11, A.25–A.28. Toseland, R., & Siporin, M. (1986). When to recommend
Korte, J., Bohlmeijer, E.T., Cappeliez, P., Smit, F., & Westerhof, group treatment: A review of the clinical and the research
G.J. (2012a). Life-review therapy for older adults with mod- literature. International Journal of Psychotherapy, 36,
erate depressive symptomatology: A pragmatic randomized 171–201.
controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 42, 1163–1173. Vinogradov, S., & Yalom, I. (1994).Group therapy. In R. Hales,
Korte, J., Bohlmeijer, E.T., & Smit, F. (2009). Prevention of S. Yudofsky & J. Talbott (Eds.), The American psychiatric
depression and anxiety in later life: Design of a randomized press textbook of psychiatry (2nd ed.) (pp. 143–1175).
controlled trial for the clinical and economic evaluation of a Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
life-review intervention. BMC Public Health, 9, 250. Watt, L.M., & Cappeliez, P. (2000). Integrative and instrumental
Korte, J., Bohlmeijer, E.T., Westerhof, G.J., & Pot, A.M. (2011). reminiscence therapies for depression in older adults: Inter-
Reminiscence and adaptation to critical life-events in older vention strategies and treatment effectiveness. Aging &-
adults with mild to moderate depressive symptoms. Aging Mental Health, 4, 166–177.
and Mental Health, 15, 638–646. Westerhof, G.J., Bohlmeijer, E.T., & Webster, J.D. (2010).
Korte, J., Westerhof, G.J., & Bohlmeijer, E.T. (2012b). Mediat- Reminiscence and mental health: A review of recent prog-
ing processes in an effective life-review intervention. ress in theory, research, and intervention. Ageing & Society,
Psychology and Aging, 27, 1172–1181. 30, 697–721.
Lepore, S.J., & Helgeson, V.S. (1999). Psychoeducational sup- White, M., & Epston, D. (1990). Narrative means to therapeutic
port group enhances quality of life after prostate cancer. ends. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Cancer Research, Therapy and Control, 8, 81–92. Wills, T.A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social
Lepore, S.J., Silver, R.C., Wortman, C.B., & Wayment, H.A. psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245–271.
(1996). Social constraints, intrusive thoughts, and depressive Wink, P., & Schiff, B. (2002). To review or not to review? The
symptoms among bereaved mothers. Journal of Personality role of personality and life-events in life review and adapta-
and Social Psychology, 70, 271–282. tion to older age. In J.D. Webster & B.K. Haight (Eds.),
Lewinsohn, P., & Clarke, G. (1999). Psychosocial treatment for Critical advances in reminiscence work: From theory to
adolescent depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, application (pp. 44–60). New York, NY: Springer.
329–342. Wong, P.T. (1995). The processes of adaptive reminiscence. In
Morrison, N. (2001). Group cognitive therapy: Treatment of B.K. Haight & J.D. Webster (Eds.), The art and science of
choice or sub-optimal option? Behavioural and Cognitive reminiscing: Theory, research, methods, and applications
Psychotherapy, 29, 311–332. (pp. 23–35). Pennsylvania, PA: Taylor & Francis.

You might also like