You are on page 1of 19

OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

CH A PT E R
Magical Thinking
4
Karl S. Rosengren and Jason A. French

Abstract
This chapter examines the concept of magical thinking and how this form of thought develops over
the course of childhood and is maintained in adulthood. Rather than assuming magical thinking to be
a remnant of childhood and an immature mind, it is suggested that a number of universal cognitive
processes lead to the emergence of magical thinking in early childhood and its maintenance into
adulthood. This chapter suggests that magical thinking reaches a peak in childhood partly because of
the emergence of pretense and imagination as well as substantial cultural support for magical beliefs
in children. Although this form of thinking declines somewhat in later childhood and adulthood,
unconscious cognitive processes serve to maintain magical thinking throughout the life span. The
chapter also explores the relation between magical thinking and religious thoughts and beliefs,
superstition, and other forms of strange beliefs in adulthood.
Key Words: cognitive biases, cognitive development, essentialism, magical belief, magical thinking,
similarity and contagion

Every day we are confronted with a variety of a variety of factors, including innate biases, socio-
events for which we clearly know the cause. We’re cultural support for particular types of explana-
hungry because we were in a hurry and missed break- tions, and individual differences in the willingness
fast. Sue realizes that John is mad at her because she to accept one or more possible explanations (see
forgot to meet him after work. But for other events chapters 3 and 22). Our goal in the current chapter
the causes are less evident. Why does the house creak is to provide a review of the literature on the devel-
at night? Is it because of changes in the weather con- opment of one form of reasoning, magical think-
ditions, strong winds, or some other cause? Could ing, a form of reasoning that takes a nonscientific
it be a ghost? Why did the streetlight go off just as perspective with respect to mysterious and anoma-
we walked by? Was it because of a burned out bulb, lous events. In doing so, we relate magical thinking
faulty wiring, or is it caused in some way by our to other forms of cognition and explore the situa-
physical presence? How do we explain the myriad tions and contexts that appear to activate magical
of events that we witness on a daily basis, especially thinking. Why is it for example that individuals
those for which no discernable cause is observed? feel uncomfortable handling the clothes of a dead
This search for causality in everyday life, for both person, especially, if the individual performed some
mundane and mysterious events, seems to be a uni- heinous crime? Why is it that we tend to avoid loca-
versal cognitive process. How particular individuals tions where some bad event has occurred, such as a
reason about different events and make reference to murder? And why is it that we take causal credit for
different types of explanation seems to depend on the occurrence of seemingly random events? Many

42

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 42 1/10/2013 2:56:21 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

researchers attribute the feelings, thoughts, and magical thinking, in terms of illogical and irratio-
behaviors associated with these types of events to nal thought, is considered a general characteristic
magical thinking. of young children (Rosengren, Miller, Gutierrez,
But what really is magical thinking? Although & Schein, in press). Others, such as Joan Didion,
magical thinking has been the focus of both anthro- have used the term to describe seemingly irrational
pological (Lévy-Bruhl, 1948; 1966; Malinowski, thoughts and behaviors that in her case dominated
1954; Tambiah, 1990) and psychological (Piaget, her days following the sudden and unexpected death
1929, 1930; Subbotsky, 1985, 1992, 2004) inqui- of her husband (Didion, 2005). But these uses of
ries for many years, there is no clearly agreed upon magic generally go beyond merely thinking or act-
definition of what is meant by the term. For this ing irrationally. That is, they seem at some level to
reason we begin our review with an exploration of connect to a set of beliefs that treat certain individu-
both traditional and more current perspectives on als, events, or occurrences as “special” and “out of
magical thinking and then offer our own perspec- the ordinary.” We will return to this issue shortly.
tive on how this particular term should be defined. Similar uses of the term magical thinking are
Afterward we consider whether magical thinking is found in the fields of anthropology and psychol-
still a commonplace activity in the everyday lives ogy, although there is quite a lot of variation in how
of both children and adults. We then explore the this term has been used. As Mayr (1982, p. 44) has
research on the development of magical thinking pointed out, many of the controversies in the his-
in children. Although never quite a main focus of tory of science can be attributed to scientists in dif-
developmental psychologists, this topic was exten- ferent, opposing camps using the same term for very
sively explored by Piaget (1929, 1930), and research different concepts. Much of the early research exam-
in this area has recently seen resurgence. In this sec- ining magical thinking characterized the thought
tion we also examine the relation between magical processes of preindustrialized “primitive” cultures
thinking and the emergence of pretense and the (Frazer, 1911) and children (Piaget, 1929) as magi-
imagination in childhood. We suggest that these cal, dominated by illogical, irrational thoughts
three behaviors—magical thinking, imagination, and beliefs (Rosengren & Hickling, 2000). By this
and pretense—appear to emerge in concert in late definition any thought process that is not logical,
infancy and early childhood and provide a mutually systematic, or scientific might be characterized as
supportive environment enabling each to flourish magical. This is not that dissimilar to the definition
in their own right. We also examine how magical provided by Eckblad and Chapman (1983, p. 215),
thinking might be related to other forms of think- who suggested that magical thinking involves
ing. Specifically, we explore the relation among “belief in forms of causation that by conventional
magical thinking, pretense, and imagination and standards are invalid.”
creativity; between magical and religious thinking; In our view there are a number of problems with
between magical thinking and superstition; and last this definition of magical thinking. First, this per-
the relation between magical thinking and psycho- spective uses magical thinking as a pejorative label
pathology. We end the chapter with a summary and for thinking that differs either from that of educated
some thoughts on future directions for research in adults in technologically advanced societies or the
this area. majority of society in general. This pejorative view
was quite common in the study of magic from an
Magical Thinking Defined anthropological perspective before the 1970s. With
On the surface, the term magical thinking seems an increase in more relativistic cultural accounts,
quite easy to define. It is a phrase that is used in such as situated cultural cognition, and distancing
both common everyday language and more esoteric from notions of primitivism, the study of magic in
academic discussions. In common usage the term different cultures declined substantially (Sorensen,
magic is tied to a wide range of events, from a beau- 2007). Second, this definition ignores the fact that
tiful sunset or rainbow to seemingly unexplainable even in technological advanced societies many deci-
phenomena or events. The label is also used for par- sions are not based on rational and logical thought
ticular individuals who think or behave in certain (Gilovich, Griffin, & Kahneman, 2002; Tversky &
ways. Magic has also been used to label thinking Kahneman, 1974, 1979). For example, when gam-
that is characterized to be illogical or irrational. bling, people often place too much importance on
Indeed, for many preschool teachers, especially one aspect of an event, which produces an error
those schooled in a Piagetian view of development, in predicting future outcomes of that event. These

Rosengren, French 43

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 43 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

decisions appear to be made on the basis of cogni- conventional wisdom of the majority of scientists.
tive biases and heuristic. Although these kinds of Although the cutting edge of science may be labeled
decisions are not based on formal logic or reason, as “magical,” as in Arthur C. Clarke’s third law (“Any
they are not generally labeled as magical. sufficiently advanced technology is indistinguishable
A third critique of the traditional view of magical from magic,” Clarke, 1962), we suggest that very
thinking is that it ignores the fact that thinking that few individuals engaging in the rigorous process of
appears irrational or illogical to an educated adult scientific inquiry would be labeled as engaging in
may be the result of lack of knowledge or experi- magical thinking. Thus, there is something more
ence in a particular domain or different types of to magical thinking than being ignorant, thinking
knowledge or experience. For example, under most illogically or irrationally, or holding beliefs that go
circumstances most adults will agree that 1 + 1 = 2. against the established science of the day.
This is a basic number fact that children acquire at
a very early age (Wynn, 1990; Wynn, Bloom, & Piaget’s View of Magical Thinking
Chiang, 2002). However, if we consider drops of Piaget provided one of the most comprehensive
water, adding two drops together yields one slightly definitions of magical thinking (1929, 1930). He
larger drop. In this case, the answer “one” is sensible, argued that magical thinking dominated young
even though in most circumstances it would seem children’s thinking, and suggested that their every-
irrational or illogical. A child’s response may seem day thoughts could be characterized by a number of
illogical or irrational, based on logic that seemingly distinct types of errors in everyday causal attribu-
defies the normal situation, but in reality may stem tions. One type of error described by Piaget as part
from reasoning based on different knowledge or a of magical thinking was when children confused
different context. cause–effect relations. A child’s mistaken assump-
We suggest that a seemingly irrational or illogical tion that two random events were linked causally,
statement, such as 1 + 1 = 1, should not be labeled such as the case of the individual assuming he or she
as magical thinking unless two conditions are met. is the cause of a streetlight turning off after merely
The first condition is that the child (or adult) must walking by, would have been characterized as magi-
realize that under normal situations the world works cal thinking.
in a particular way. That is, in the everyday world Piaget used the term magic by participation
adding one item with another yields two items. The to describe three forms of magical thinking that
second condition is that the child (or adult) holds involved thoughts, objects, and events with no
a belief that in some situations the world can be actual causal link. For example, magic by participa-
“altered” so the normal causal relations are in some tion between thoughts and things was used by Piaget to
way circumvented by some other, “special” process describe situations in which children believed their
or processes that lead to an “alternative” outcome thoughts could alter reality. He used the expression
that varies from the norm. That is, they must believe magic by participation between actions and things to
that some sort of supernatural or alternative causal describe a child’s belief that the performance of an
power exists that can render 1 + 1 = 1. It is this addi- action, such as saying a word or waving a hand,
tional belief in some “unnatural” or “supernatural” could alter reality. The third form of participation,
process that we argue should be the defining prop- magic by participation between objects, was used to
erty of magical thinking. describe a child’s belief that one object could influ-
We also argue that reasoning that goes against ence another object when no natural, logical causal
the norm or established mindset of science should relationship was present between the objects. The
not be necessarily labeled as magical thinking. A notion that one’s razor would never get dull if stored
fairly large number of individuals hold beliefs that under a glass pyramid is an example of this type of
reject the “majority” view of scientists. Although participation. The final form of participation, magic
rejection of evolution or climate change in the face by participation of purpose, incorporates animistic
of overwhelming scientific evidence in support for beliefs, whereby the child endows the inanimate
both of these phenomena might seem illogical or world with animate properties and in some extreme
irrational, we suggest that individuals who hold forms believes the will or purpose of the object can
these divergent views would rarely if ever be labeled be controlled by the child. A classic Piagetian exam-
as “magical” in their thinking. ple of this is a child believing that the sun, clouds,
At the frontiers of science, individuals may also or moon is purposefully following him or her as the
hold views that vary greatly or even contradict the child walks by (see chapter 18).

44 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 44 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

An essential aspect of Piaget’s notions of partici- that in many circumstances a young child’s belief
pation is the idea that children hold a belief that the in the reality of certain fantasy figures (e.g., Santa
participatory relation is causal. A number of other Claus, the Tooth Fairy) and their actions brought
researchers have also cited the importance of belief about by atypical forms of causality could be char-
in some sort of underlying causality. For example, acterized as involving magical thinking. It is not
Pronin and colleagues have suggested that magi- clear that belief in these figures or in their special
cal thinking is the “belief in the ability to influence powers necessarily follows the principles of similar-
events at a distance with no known physical expla- ity and contagion. We explore the concepts of simi-
nation” (Pronin, Wegner, McCarthy, & Rodriguez, larity and contagion in more detail in a later section.
2006, p. 218). This view is very similar to Piaget’s We also explore the notion of essences, as we sug-
first form of participation. We suggest that belief in gest that this concept deserves particular focus, as
the efficacy of a nontypical causal link is central to a belief in essences is likely an unconscious process
classifying a thought as magical. But it does not go that may serve as a “built-in bias” in human reason-
far enough. Rather, we argue that it is a particular ing more generally.
type of belief, a belief in the existence of alterna-
tive forms of causality operating in the world, which Our View of Magical Thinking
works to bring about events that violate the normal To summarize our view of magical thinking, if
causal order of the world. This belief constraint on a child merely lacks particular knowledge, is con-
the definition removes from the magical thinking fused, or incorrect about some event or situation,
category most instances of decision making based we should refrain from labeling his or her think-
on emotion, or “reasoning without knowledge of, ing as magical. An individual’s thinking can’t merely
or on the basis of some sort of misconception about be wrong, confused, irrational, driven by emotions,
causality, or about natural laws more generally” based on inaccurate knowledge, or different from
(Woolley, 1997, p. 991). Rather, as Woolley con- our own or the conventional wisdom to be labeled
tinues, we need to consider “ . . . belief in an entity magical thinking. If this were sufficient, the thought
or process that is unsupported by what we gener- processes of many consumers and theoretical physi-
ally consider to be the principles of nature . . . Thus cists would need to be labeled as magical. Rather, we
one might think of these phenomena as violating, suggest that for thought to be labeled as magical an
or at least being inconsistent with our naïve theories individual must (1) realize that objects and events
of the world” (p. 991). Here the notion of “belief ” in the world generally follow a certain pattern or
goes beyond the idea that the two random events order governed by the laws of nature (or physics);
are connected causally and extends to a particular (2) realize that the observed object or event in some
type of “belief ” in alternative forms of causality. way “violates” or “contradicts” the normal order of
things in the world; and (3) hold a belief in some
Nemeroff and Rozin’s View of form of supernatural or alternative form of causality
Magical Thinking that extends beyond those that govern the natural
Belief is also central to the definition of magical world. An important point is that the belief need
thinking provided by Nemeroff and Rozin (2000), not be conscious for thinking to be labeled “magi-
who have presented one of the more detailed defini- cal.” In fact, some researchers, such as Subbotsky
tions of magical thinking to date. They suggest that (2000a,b,c), argue that magical thinking in most
magical thinking should be viewed as “. . . . the cog- cases is not under conscious control.
nitive intuition or belief in the existence of imper- Our definition of magical thinking removes the
ceptible forces or essences that transcend the usual pejorative aspect of many past views of this behavior
boundary between the mental/symbolic and physi- and provides a clearer demarcation between magical
cal/material realities, in a way [that] (1) diverges thinking and other forms of thought. We explore
from the received wisdom of the technocratic elite, particular links to other forms of cognition in later
(2) serves important functions, and (3) follows sections of the chapter.
the principles of similarity and contagion” (p. 5).
Although we find many aspects of this definition Relation Between Magical Thinking and
compelling, we feel that not all cases of magical Magical Belief
thinking necessarily serve a clear function for the Earlier we suggested that part of the require-
individual, nor do they always involve principles of ments for characterizing thought as magical was an
similarity and contagion. For example, we suggest accompanying belief in the idea that some sort of

Rosengren, French 45

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 45 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

alternative form of causality was possible. By alter- similarity


native form of causality we mean one that is not The law of similarity specifies that objects that
part of accepted scientific explanations and one that resemble one another share certain fundamental
is generally not used to describe everyday phenom- properties that are sufficient to link the objects
ena in the world. Zusne and Jones (1989, p. 229) causally. This causal link is thought to take the form
suggest that beliefs are cognitions that refer to what of an underlying essence that gives rise to the over-
an individual regards as true or false, and the beliefs all similarity of the two entities. With the law of
serve to help the individual make sense of the world similarity, appearance equates reality in a proximal/
around him or her. That is, these beliefs guide inter- distal relationship with deep roots in humans’ past.
pretations and help remove ambiguity. One’s beliefs Evaluating each plant for poisonous content is far
can often be viewed negatively as irrational or illogi- more dangerous than classifying all plants as poi-
cal because they are personal, and generally oper- sonous that resemble one known to be dangerous.
ate without any type of external validation (Zusne This is not to relegate and overgeneralize the law
& Jones, 1989). Although beliefs can be explicit in of similarity as an ancient heuristic, as we shall see,
nature, as in a stated “belief in God,” many beliefs, but instead to identify it as a useful magical law for
especially magical ones, are intuitive and implicit establishing causal inferences that has deep evolu-
(Nemeroff & Rozin, 1994). It is these intuitive tionary roots.
magical beliefs, such as those that operate with
the laws of contagion and similarity that we suggest contagion
are at the core of magical thinking and are discussed The law of contagion also involves the notion
in the next section. of an underlying essence, but this law concerns the
transfer of physical, moral, or behavioral proper-
Magical Thinking in Everyday Life ties from source to recipient through some form
To what extent is magical thinking present in of contact, direct or indirect (Nemeroff & Rozin,
the everyday life of children and adults? In this 1992, 2000; Rozin, Millman, & Nemeroff, 1986).
section, we present a brief overview of sympa- It is this law that is thought to influence one’s
thetic magic, one form of magical thinking that is reluctance to don the sweater previously owned by
thought to be ubiquitous in the thought of both some despised, diseased, or mentally ill individual.
children and adults. We also discuss the role of This law also is thought to operate indirectly, as
this form of magical thinking to everyday life. In in the case of action performed on a voodoo doll.
particular, we examine how it influences feelings Here the action is believed to be magically trans-
of disgust in American culture and general eating ferred from source to recipient. However, the law
preferences. of contagion is slightly more complex in that phys-
ical contact between the source and recipient also
Sympathetic Magic: Similarity and the establishes a continual link or contract between
Contagion Concept in Magical Thinking the two through which further essence can be
Sympathetic magic is comprised of the laws of transmitted.
similarity, opposites, and contagion, with the sec- In addition, contagious contact may be made
ond considered a subset of the first (Frazer, 1911; through intermediary and even twice-removed
Nemeroff & Rozin, 2000). Importantly, these laws objects, such as a pair of gloves instead of direct
continue to operate and influence one another essence transmission between source and recipient.
throughout the life span, regardless of scientific This transmission is more commonly exemplified by
training that should eliminate some of their most the aversion of Westerners to wearing second-hand
salient and tangible effects. Similarity and contagion clothing from stores, despite more attractive prices
are central to the magical beliefs of various diver- than found in first-hand stores (Rozin, Markwith,
gent cultures and ethnic groups and are considered & McCauley, 1994). Here, the aversion to essence
the basis for universal primitive human thought. manifests in a quantifiable price gap. This aversion
That is, these forms of magical thinking have been remains, even after thorough washing or steriliza-
hypothesized to provide useful heuristics that guide tion of the “offending” garment or object. Together,
individuals’ behaviors in ways that promote the evo- the laws of sympathetic magic form a highly intui-
lutionary value for survival (Boden & Berenbaum, tive concept that can permeate even pronounced
2004; Frazer, 1911; Mauss, 1972; see Nemeroff & and thorough scientific training (Nemeroff &
Rozin, 1994, for cultural details). Rozin, 1992).

46 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 46 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

research on similarity and contagion by magical thinking. By focusing on Jewish adults,


Rozin, Millman, and Nemeroff (1986) have Nemeroff and Rozin (1992) were able to explore
demonstrated these two laws in daily Western life how sympathetic magic interacts with individual
by comparing responses to disgusting, dangerous, differences in adherence to kosher laws. They found
or valued objects using both direct response and that magical thinking often overrode kosher labels.
questionnaires. In one such study, the role of con- Specifically, a simple label often was enough to
tagion and similarity were explored. Specifically, render kosher, contamination-free food, inedible.
participants’ responses to roach contamination of a Variation among the participants in the sensitiv-
glass of water, labeling the glass of water as cyanide, ity to contagion and similarity effects suggests that
and imitation dog feces were examined. They found the two laws may at times work independently of
that individuals generally avoided substances that each other. Interestingly, highly observant Jews
resembled either disgusting or dangerous substances had strong magical reactions to disgust, but not to
even though the substances were only water or other kosher nonviolations. Here, Nemeroff and Rozin
harmless substances. This is a case of magical think- note that the attentiveness to contamination may
ing based on similarity. They also found significant cause an overconcern that may serve to strengthen
reluctance of participants to come into contact or specific rule sets. Also, individuals who are more
ingest substances that had been contaminated by sensitive to feelings of disgust may exhibit a greater
something disgusting (a roach or spit), even if the propensity toward observing certain food rules sup-
substance was completely sterilized. Similar reluc- ported by the culture. In this way the magical laws
tance was found in participants’ willingness to put may serve to increase compliance with particular
on clothing that had been worn by a disliked per- cultural or religious norms or obligations.
son. These effects are quite strong. The mere label Aspects of the principle of contagion can be
“cyanide” placed on a glass of water filled from found in modern germ theory, showing that at
the tap in the participants’ full view is enough to times magical thought can align with current sci-
activate an avoidance response. Generally, these entific views. Indeed, in medieval Europe intel-
researchers (Rozin & Zellner, 1985; Rozin et al., lectuals viewed natural magic as a branch of
1986) have found stronger effects for negative science focused on discovering the hidden powers
contagion (transfer of an essence from something of nature (Kieckhefer, 1989). The similarity of the
disgusting or disliked) than for positive contagion disease transmission involving germs with the magi-
(transfer of an essence from some liked or positive cal law of contagion suggests further support for
source). Humans may have a bias, whether innate the idea that contagion beliefs are highly adaptive
or conceptual, which increases the disgust of nega- for survival against microbial contaminants such
tive items while minimizing the effect of positive as bacteria and viruses. This also may account for
items. It would seem adaptive to have a stronger the negativity bias discussed earlier. Furthermore,
negative bias, as this would lead individuals to Nemeroff and Rozin (1994) have found that people
avoid potentially harmful substances. Erroneously differentiate different sources of contagion, treating
avoiding something will not kill you. In contrast, physical sources of contagion differently from inter-
not approaching something with a positive asso- personal–moral sources of contagion. To illustrate,
ciation is less likely to lead to a negative outcome. a more severe aversion can be found to second-hand
Recent evidence suggests that different domains of garments with moral or behavioral contagions than
disgust elicited by merely written words activate to physical contagions.
specific brain regions (frontal, temporal, and lim- Individuals also expect these different forms
bic networks), suggesting an automatic response to of contagion to produce divergent consequences.
particular stimuli (Moll, de Oliveira-Souza, Moll, Although the physical contagion can be bleached
Ignácio, Bramati, Caparelli-Dáquer, & Eslinger, out of the object and properly disinfected, the moral
2005). Again this suggests that unconscious pro- and behavioral contagions of objects are much more
cesses may drive aspects of magical thinking. difficult to eliminate and generally require some
The principles of contagion and similarity have type of ritual that may not be effective in completely
also been shown by Nemeroff and Rozin to influ- eliminating the moral contagion. For example, some
ence the eating practices observed in many cultures religious fundamentalists purify themselves in ritual
and religions. In one study, Nemeroff and Rozin ceremony weekly in fear of becoming contaminated
(1992) explored individual differences in the sus- again by moral containment or in fear of not hav-
ceptibility of Jewish participants to be influenced ing completely removed it the previous week. What

Rosengren, French 47

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 47 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

these findings suggest is that physical sources tend to as children’s cognitive structures matured in inter-
follow the contagion model, whereas interpersonal action with experiences via the processes of assimi-
sources of contagion tended to invoke reactions lation, accommodation, and equilibration. In this
more in accordance with the law of similarity. Also, replacement model, children’s thinking was trans-
responses indicated that individuals act as if there formed in a stagelike fashion. With each major
is a personal–soul–essence concept. The research- developmental step magical thinking is driven far-
ers suggest that physical-substance contagion effects ther and farther out of the child’s mind until she
that remain after cleansing may be symbolic in part. reaches the rational, logical level of adults. This
This symbolic aspect of contagion, in which a word accomplishment was thought to occur sometime
or idea carries the “essence,” also suggests that the around age 12 with the onset of formal operational
boundary between physical and interpersonal con- thinking (Piaget, 1929).
tagion may not be clear-cut, and these two forms Piaget also referred to a different type of magi-
of contagion may both operate across different cal thinking, mainly culturally supported magi-
domains. cal beliefs. These include beliefs in entities such as
Clearly, contagion continues to operate in our Santa Claus, the Tooth Fairy, witches, ghosts, and
thinking even in the modern world because of its goblins (see chapter 5). Because much of his focus
survival value in some instances. Implicit models of was on uncovering universals in cognitive develop-
this can be found in studies of disgust. Rozin et al. ment, he was much less interested in these beliefs,
(1986, 1989) have extensively explored contagion which he viewed as culturally dependent. For this
in the interpersonal domain among college stu- reason he did not devote any time explicitly exam-
dents. They found that evidence of magical thinking ining children’s beliefs in these magical entities.
remains despite high levels of education and scien-
tific training. These results provide further evidence the secular hypothesis
that magical thinking does not stem from irrational In many ways, Piaget’s theory captures at the
or uneducated individuals. Rather, it suggests that individual level what the secular hypothesis cap-
implicit models of contagion appear to be a common tures at the level of culture. Specifically, one of the
aspect of cognition even in highly educated, scien- main ideas behind the secular hypothesis (Norris
tifically trained adults. It should also be noted that & Inglehart, 2004) is that as cultures become more
germ theory, a scientifically valid notion of illness scientifically and technologically advanced, scien-
contagion, could be classified as an example of an tific thought and rationality eventually drive out
explicit exemplification of interpersonal-contagion, and replace all magical or illogical thought. Indeed,
lending further support to the idea that magical much of the early research on superstitious beliefs
contagion may have both evolutionary roots and and behaviors was conducted under the assumption
improve one’s chances for survival. that these beliefs were caused by failures in educa-
tion. With expanded access and training in science
The Development of Magical Thinking and mathematics education, it was thought that
How does magical thinking develop? Where these beliefs and behaviors would fade from exis-
does it come from? Is it a universal aspect of human tence (Zusne & Jones, 1989, p. 231). There does
behavior? Obviously the answers to these questions appear to be some evidence supporting the notion
depend on one’s definition of magical thinking. We that traditional superstitious beliefs have decreased
begin this section with some of the traditional per- over the years (Zusne & Jones, 1989). That is, fewer
spectives on the development of magical thinking people today seem to believe in the efficacy of lucky
and then provide our own account that attempts to numbers, rubbing a rabbit’s foot, or the dangers of
integrate ideas from diverse aspects of development a black cat crossing one’s path. At the same time
and culture describe of the origins and development as this decrease, however, they suggest that there
of magical thinking. has been a complementary increase in more cog-
nitively complex beliefs (Zusne & Jones, 1989).
Piaget’s View of the Development of These more complex beliefs, such as those in extra-
Magical Thinking sensory perception (ESP) or auras, often have the
Piaget viewed magical thinking as a univer- illusion of scientific support, or individuals hold-
sal aspect of young children’s cognition. For him, ing such beliefs have the expectation that science
children’s early magical thinking was gradually will eventually provide either a natural (e.g., more
replaced with more logical, even scientific thinking evolved minds in the case of ESP) or supernatural

48 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 48 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

explanation (e.g., find that ghosts really do exist). At indicating that magical thinking never is completely
present, the data seem to suggest that magical and driven out of the human mind. This conclusion is
even irrational thought seem to be quite resistant to supported by many adult’s justification expressing
replacement or extinction. Magic does not seem to credulity regarding the existence of magical processes
be something relegated to our or others “primitive” in the world following experimental manipulation.
past or thought, but appears to be relatively ubiqui- Subbotsky’s view on magical thinking and its
tous in all historical periods, cultural settings, and development is shared to a large extent by Woolley
even modern society (Luhrman, 1989; Sorenson, and her colleagues (Woolley, 1997; Woolley,
2007). In our view this provides further evidence Browne, & Boerger, 2006), who suggest that magi-
in support of the idea that aspects of magical think- cal thinking remains abundant in today’s culture in
ing operate as unconscious biases that are universal the minds of both children and adults. She and her
aspects of human cognition. colleagues have argued that certain factors influ-
ence the dominance of both magical thoughts and
Subbotsky’s Coexistence View of the beliefs, including the particular context and social
Development of Magical Thinking cost of either believing or not believing. They have
A more recent account of the development of also found that children with a greater fantasy ori-
magical thinking is that of Subbotsky (2010). His entation are more likely to adopt a belief in a novel
starting point is a Piagetian view of magical think- fantasy entity (the Candy Witch; Woolly, Boerger, &
ing, defined as involving sympathetic magic and Markman, 2004). One implication of these results
notions of participation. However, rather than sug- is that both context, especially the current social
gesting that magical thinking is replaced by scientific situation, and particular individual differences, may
thought, Subbotsky argues strongly that magical influence the occurrence of different forms of magi-
thinking is merely driven underground by social cal thinking. Thus, one should perhaps not think of
and cultural pressures that place a greater value on magical thinking as a general orientation toward the
science, logic, and rational thought. Specifically, he world, as Piaget thought, but as a type of thought
suggests that in late childhood, magical thinking that varies in likelihood as a function of age, as well
becomes suppressed but never extinguished. as individual and situational factors.
In a series of clever experiments Subbotsky
shows that both children and adults, given the right An Integrated Account of the Development
context or situation, can be shown to act as if they of Magical Thinking
believe magic is real. For example, in a number of Our own view is that magical thinking arises out
studies, Subbotsky (1994, 1996, 1997) primed his of a number of universal cognitive processes. This
participants with either a story or an example of view is not all that different than Zusne and Jones’
magical events, produced a seemingly impossible (1989, p. 250) view that magical thinking stems
outcome, and then placed the participants in a situ- from the nature of the human cognitive architec-
ation in which they could exhibit behavior indica- ture and thus is a universal cognitive process. In par-
tive of magical beliefs. In one such study, Subbotsky ticular, they have suggested that magical thinking
presented children with a story about a magical box arises from the misattribution of causal factors to a
that could cut a postage stamp in half when a par- particular event when in fact the attributed causes
ticular magic word was uttered. He then showed the play no role in the actual event. They also think that
children an actual box and told them it was the box magical thinking stems from a universal cognitive
in the story. He then left the room and observed process that involves what they refer to as the “reifi-
surreptitiously whether children would utter the cation of the subjective.” This idea is tied historically
magic word. Many six- to nine-year-olds did exactly to notions of magic that place a large emphasis on
that in an attempt to test the magic of the box. symbolic thought. The notion is that the ability to
In later studies Subbotsky found that both chil- experience subjective experiences arises from sym-
dren and adults were generally unwilling to place bolic thought, which in turn enables an individual
a valued object in the box after being shown that to think of oneself as separate and distinct from the
it could “magically” destroy objects when a magic world. It is this separate sense of self that is thought
word was uttered. This behavior, the reluctance to to open up the realm of reality to extend the self
utter a magic word or allow the experimenter to out into the world in dreams, spirits, or transformed
place a valued object (one’s driver’s license) into the bodies. Zusne and Jones suggest that magical think-
“magical box,” has been interpreted by Subbotsky as ing involves endowing the subjective self with the

Rosengren, French 49

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 49 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

properties of the objective world. This is what they passed on to another person. Gelman (2003) has
mean by reification of subjective. They suggest that argued that the tendency to essentialize the world is
this reification may be expressed by the use of magic likely a universal aspect of human cognition.
spells, incantations, wishing for desired outcomes,
or beliefs that different forms of energy can emanate role of causality and essentialism in
from the human mind. magical thinking
Our own view of the development of magical We suggest that these two processes, the search
thinking draws on some of these ideas, but places for causality and the tendency to essentialize, are
less emphasis on misattributions and the role of present in early infancy and are maintained through-
symbolism. We also extend Zusne and Jones’ (1989) out life. This is one reason why stereotypes are dif-
ideas by providing greater detail about the cognitive ficult to override or eliminate, even with extensive
process that may underlie the emergence and main- knowledge and training. We also suggest that these
tenance of magical thinking. basic human tendencies to search for causality and
to essentialize are at the core of sympathetic magic
search for causality and the belief that our mere presence influenced the
One particularly important cognitive process streetlight to go off or why we feel disgust at the
involved in magical thinking is the human mind’s thought of the wearing Hitler’s sweater, even after
tendency to causally link events close in time and frequent washing.
proximity even when the events are clearly random.
This tendency has been documented in numerous the role of knowledge
research investigations tracing back to the work of These two processes, the search for causality and
Michotte (1962), who showed that adults treat white the tendency to essentialize, are not by themselves
dots moving around a projection screen as causally sufficient for the development of magical thinking.
influencing one another. This notion that children We suggest that a third process is also instrumen-
and adults search for and assign causality in numer- tal in the development of magical thinking, that of
ous domains and situations is a central assumption knowledge acquisition. This is another process that
of the theory-theory approach to cognitive devel- is clearly present from birth. Indeed, infants are par-
opment (Gopnik & Wellman, 1994; Wellman & ticularly good at absorbing knowledge. The knowl-
Gelman, 1992). We suggest that this innate drive to edge acquired in infancy and beyond, coupled with
search for and assign causal links to events is one of the drive to make causal linkages, and the tendency
the key processes involved in the development and to essentialize, enables the infant, child, or adult
maintenance of magical thinking. to categorize events into distinct domains. It is the
formation of the foundational domains of physics,
tendency to essentialize psychology, and biology, which enable the child
A second universal cognitive process that we and adult to reason rationally about the world of
view as central to the development and mainte- inanimate objects, psychological entities, and bio-
nance of magical thinking is a basic tendency to logical things (Wellman & Gelman, 1992). But as
essentialize. The concept of psychological essential- children acquire new knowledge, they sometimes
ism is the notion that individuals treat objects and miscategorize entities and events. Children may
entities in the world as if they have an underlying treat nonliving things as alive or treat nonsentient
core nature or “essence” that determines the prop- beings or entities as motivated by thoughts, desires,
erties and characteristics of that object or entity and intentions. These are the classic Piagetian
(Medin & Ortony, 1989). Essentialism appears to examples of magical thinking in young children.
play a key role in a number of aspects of magical We, however, suggest that they should be classified
thinking. Specifically, it can be seen as playing the as such, if and only if the children recognize that
pivotal role in the magical principles of similarity categorizing nonliving entities as living ones and
and contagion discussed earlier. With respect to nonsentient entities as sentient ones violates the
the principle of similarity, similar objects are per- nature of the everyday world. If they do not, then
ceived to share some underlying essence that creates we argue that these are no more than examples of
a magical connection between them. In the case of individuals who lack specific knowledge failing to
the contagion principle, the idea is that the essence categorize correctly or misattributing an entity to
of an individual or disease gets transferred when an the wrong causal foundational domain. Similarly, if
object, such as a sweater of deceased individual, is children misattribute life properties, thoughts, and

50 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 50 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

or feelings to nonbiological or psychological entities be convinced that a machine can physically shrink
we shouldn’t label them as thinking magically. To objects or even an entire room (DeLoache, Miller,
gain the label of magical thinking, children must & Rosengren, 1997). The work by Rosengren and
know something about the physical, biological, and Hickling (2000) suggests that as children gain more
psychological worlds and treat events as somehow knowledge about the physical world they begin to
transcending the rules of normal physics, psychol- distinguish particular events that violate their expec-
ogy, or biology. tations of how things normally function. Parents,
With age and experience, children acquire teachers, or other adults often label these events as
greater knowledge of the physical, natural, and psy- “magic,” leading to the emergence of a special cate-
chological worlds and become keenly aware of per- gory of “magic” somewhere around the age of three.
ceived violations. These violations may arise at the Support for magic by parents and the larger culture
boundaries—where misattributing animacy may coupled with increasing developments in the realm
have some survival value (Guthrie, 1993), from the of the imagination help this magical category grow
interaction of the processes discussed in the preced- and become more well defined. For this reason,
ing section (causality, essentialism), or from the there appears to be a peak in the magical beliefs of
observation of strange (an insect that looks like a children between the ages of three and six. Even in
stick) or wonderful (a rainbow) events that appear children of this age range, however, magical think-
to defy everyday reasoning. ing is neither ubiquitous nor the dominant form of
cognitive interaction with the world, as Piaget had
cultural support for magical thinking thought. Rather, research suggests that in most situ-
An additional process involved in the develop- ations children’s default form of reasoning is based
ment and maintenance of magical thinking involves on natural and physical causality that conforms to
cultural support for an alternative belief system that what is normally considered possible and typical in
transcends everyday reasoning in the foundational the world. It is only when children’s everyday causal
domains. For children growing up in the dominant expectations are violated or cultural support is pro-
culture in the United States, cultural support for a vided, that magical thinking prevails (Rosengren &
variety of magical beliefs is quite common. Parents Hickling, 1994, 2000).
actively support beliefs in magic and magical entities For most children in the mainstream cul-
(Rosengren & Hickling, 1994; Rosengren, Hickling, ture of the United States, belief in magic changes
Kalish, & Gelman, 1994), and young preschool into the understanding that what appears to be
children appear surprisingly open to the acquisition magic is brought about by tricks and deception
of new magical characters, such as the Candy Witch (Rosengren & Hickling, 2000; Woolley, 1994). But
(Woolley, Boerger, & Markman, 2004). Research as both Subbotsky and Woolley suggest, magical
by Rosengren et al. (Rosengren & Hickling, 1994; thinking does not appear to be extinguished, even
Rosengren et al., 1994) has shown that in the United if magical entities such as magicians, Santa Claus,
States, middle class parents tend to actively support and the Tooth Fairy are demoted from the realm
beliefs in magic in children younger than five. Once of reality to the realm of pretense and the imagi-
children enter formal schooling, the family and cul- nation. Rather, the magical principles of similarity
tural support for magic declines and parents either and contagion coupled with the search for cau-
turn back the child’s questions regarding magic (e.g., sality (even when there is no causal link) and the
“What do you think, is magic real?” Rosengren & tendency to essentialize maintain a certain level of
Hickling, 2000) or begin to actively deny the exis- magical thinking in older children and adults. We
tence of magic in the real world (e.g., “There is no suggest that this later type of magical thinking is
such thing as real magic, it’s just a trick.”). present throughout the life span, lurking under the
The period of greatest cultural support for magi- surface in even the most rational of minds. Magical
cal beliefs, ages three to five or six in the United thinking can be brought back to the surface in a
States, occurs at the same time children are acquir- variety of different ways (Subbotsky, 2010).
ing a lot of new information about the world. Before
age three, children often lack the knowledge of what Magical Thinking and Its Relation to Other
is and is not possible in the real world. This is one Cognitive Processes
reason why professional magicians generally do not In this section we explore how magical thinking
like to perform in front of children under the age of relates to other forms of cognition, including pre-
three and why children under the age of the three can tense and the imagination. We also briefly explore

Rosengren, French 51

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 51 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

the role of magical thinking in creativity. We then Subbotsky (2010) suggests that “ancient forms of
examine possible links between magical thinking the magical thinking live on in dreams and imagi-
and religious thought. Finally, we explore possible nation,” and he suggests that magical thinking
links between magical thinking in adulthood to plays a central role in the creativity that is pres-
superstitious beliefs and psychopathology. ent in literature, the arts, and movies. Indeed, he
views magical thinking as opening up the realm of
Magic, Pretense, and the Imagination what is possible, and by doing so magical thinking
How does magical thinking relate to pretense serves to stimulate and foster creativity. Recently, he
and the imagination? There are a number of simi- tested this idea by presenting children with scenes
larities between these different cognitive processes. of magic from movies and examining whether chil-
In particular, each of these processes tends to open dren exposed to magical ideas in movies express
up the realm of what is possible. In magical think- greater creativity than children who observed non-
ing, impossible causal connections or mechanisms magical scenes from the same movies. This research
are thought to be possible. In pretense, one object appears to support the idea that magical thinking
is symbolically transformed into another. In the may serve to increase creativity, but more research
imagination, reality may be extended in a myriad is needed in this area. At issue here is whether it is
of ways beyond the physical, biological, or psycho- actually magical thinking that stimulates creativity
logical world. Each of these processes appears to or whether it is imagination involving some magical
emerge in the minds of children around the same elements that actually fosters creativity. The general
time, although pretense and imagination may pre- idea is that considering alternative forms of causal-
date the emergence of a clear conceptualization of ity may enable individuals to broaden the perspec-
magic. Each of these processes also likely stems from tive and hence respond on a given task in a more
some of the other cognitive processes discussed ear- creative manner.
lier, as well as alongside the emergence of symbolic
thought (see chapter 12). Magical Thinking and Religion
We suggest that these different cognitive processes Many psychologists and anthropologists include
serve to mutually support and enhance one another. religious thoughts and beliefs in the realm of magi-
For most children, the boundaries between the real cal thinking (see chapter 3). Indeed, a wide range of
and pretend or the real and imagined are well defined. scholars has examined the relation between magic
For example, by age two, children appear to be able and religion across a variety of disciplines (i.e.,
to distinguish a pretend entity from a real one (Leslie, Boyer, 1994; Kieckhefer, 1989; Neusner, Ferichs, &
1987). By age three, there is evidence that children Flesner, 1989; Sorensen, 2007).
can distinguish real from imagined (Wellman & Clearly, belief in the existence of God, angels,
Estes, 1986). Leslie has suggested that children serve and miracles fits the definition of magical think-
to cordon off pretense from reality, enabling them to ing outlined in a previous section. Neusner et al.
reason about the pretend and real world separately point out that the term magic, as used in the context
with few misattributions from pretense into reality. of religion, is often meant in a pejorative sense, in
It appears that the same occurs in the realms of the which it is used to label the religious leaders of some
imagination and magic. That is, children appear to other group “magicians.” Similarly, many psycholo-
cordon off magical events from typical ones, just as gists and scientists more generally treat religion
they appear to cordon off pretense from reality, and as a form of magical thinking, suggesting that to
the imagined from the real. What is different from embrace religious beliefs is irrational, illogical, and
magical thinking is that for children between the ages nonscientific. Here we briefly review a number of
of about three and six (in the United States and likely different views on the relation between magic and
in some other cultures), magical causes are a part of religion.
reality (although they are not typical). That is, with
magic, certain events are treated as distinct, separate, similarities between religious and
or special, and are included in the realm of what it magical thinking
possible, just not what is typical. Boyer and Walker (2000) have suggested that
magic and religion share similar fundamental ontol-
Magical Thinking and Creativity ogies. They suggest that these two forms of think-
Throughout the life span it is likely that magical ing may start out as one single form. With age and
thinking serves to foster imagination and creativity. experience, undifferentiated magical beliefs separate

52 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 52 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

into, on the one hand, institutionalized magical is particularly useful for learning about things that
beliefs in the form of various religions, and on the are generally unobservable. Because of the institu-
other hand, a set of noninstitutionalized magical tionalization of religion, children are likely to view
beliefs. The institutionalized beliefs include notions religious thought and beliefs as relatively consis-
of God and other spiritual beings and particular rit- tent and coherent. This is particularly true in the
uals and practices that are believed by the practitio- United States, where the vast majority of the cul-
ners to have sacred values and certain causal powers ture expresses belief in God and tends to practice
to bring about some of sort of meaningful change some form of organized religion (Evans, 2000;
(i.e., heal a sick child). The central role of some Gallup, 2009; Poling & Evans, 2004). In contrast,
sort of deity and the role of sacred values appear to magical beliefs and thinking are likely to be more
provide a distinct form of causality that serves to idiosyncratic and less coherent in most individuals
further differentiate religious thinking from magical in this same culture because of lack of widespread
thinking. cultural support for magic except during particular
A similar argument to that of Boyer and Walker times in early childhood. Thus, we suggest that at
(2006) has been made by Woolley (2000), who has least in the mainstream culture in the United States,
described parallels between making a wish and pray- magical and religious thinking could be viewed as
ing. Both making a wish and praying involve men- relatively distinct. However, there is also consider-
tal processes, often accompanied by language that able overlap between these concepts. For example,
is generally performed so as to bring about some Sorensen (2007) has suggested that although magic
sort of meaningful change. Both of these processes is involved in most religious rituals, religion covers
also involve belief in some sort of alternative form a much wider range of human behavior than magic.
of causality. Research by Woolley and her colleagues It is also likely that nature of this overlap is highly
(Woolley & Phelps, 2001; Woolley, Phelps, Davis, dependent on the culture and religion that is being
& Mandell, 1999) suggests that these two processes examined.
become differentiated some time between the ages
of four and eight years of age, with a tendency with Magical Thinking and Other Forms of
increasing age for children to restrict the efficacy of Thought and Behavior
wishes to stories but to show an increasing belief in superstition
the efficacy of prayer. One area in which magical thinking remains
Like magical thinking, the emergence of religious strong in adults is superstition. According to
thoughts and beliefs are also substantially influ- Zusne and Jones (1989), to act superstitiously is
enced by cultural support. Many religious concepts to “momentarily become a small magician.” Like
are highly abstract and embedded in rich knowl- the term magical thinking, superstition has been
edge frameworks that provide an elaborate symbolic variously defined, often with a pejorative conno-
context for religious experiences. For this reason, tation (Lindeman & Aarnio, 2007; Vyse, 1997).
many religious concepts and ideas may be acquired Lindeman and Aarnio (2007) argue that supersti-
at a later developmental point than those related to tious, magical, and paranormal beliefs should all
magic and potentially continue to increase past the be viewed as category mistakes in which the core
time many magical beliefs become suppressed. For attributes of foundational theories (physics, biol-
example, Harris and Giménez (2005) have found ogy, and psychology; Wellman & Gelman, 1992)
the belief in the afterlife increases between the ages are confused with one another. In their view, the
of seven and eleven. This is a time when few chil- way to distinguish these particular category mis-
dren in the United States would endorse the exis- takes arises from the confusion of core knowledge.
tence of real magic in the natural world. We argue for the same reasons that we used for
magical thinking that category mistakes by them-
differentiating magical and selves should not be categorized as magic unless
religious thinking they are accompanied by a belief in some alterna-
Other support for differentiating magical think- tive form of causality. That is, we view supersti-
ing from religious thinking comes from the work on tions as one form of magical thinking in which
testimony. Harris and Koenig (2006; see chapter 3) individuals hold beliefs, either idiosyncratic or cul-
argue that children are quite sensitive to variations tural, that certain actions or behaviors will influ-
in the consistency, coherence, and plausibility of ence the outcome of some event by some sort of
others’ testimony. They also suggest that testimony supernatural means.

Rosengren, French 53

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 53 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

Superstitious behavior can be readily observed provide a general feeling of control in chaotic or
in many situations. For example, in many athletic unpredictable situations (Keinan, 2002; Schippers
events, especially those in which highly skilled per- & Van Lange, 2006; Womack, 1992). Subbotsky
formance can be viewed as only slightly better than (personal communication) has argued that these
chance, athletes engage in a variety of superstitious same conditions foster magical thinking more gen-
behaviors (Vyse, 1997). Often athletes will only erally. There is some indication that in particular
wear certain clothes on a game day, refuse to wash task situations, superstitious behavior may actually
certain items of clothing during a particular streak benefit performance. For example, Buhrman and
of good performance, or perform rituals before a Zaugg (1981) found that in competitive basket-
game or particular event within a game. All of these ball, the teams that perform the best exhibit more
behaviors are done with the notion that they might superstitious behaviors than less competitive teams.
potentially influence the outcome. Each of these Likewise, superior players on those superior teams
behaviors involves a number of characteristics we also exhibit greater superstitious behaviors than
have described with respect to magical thinking. their teammates. Although routine, ritual move-
First, they often involve aspects of similarity and ments before performance does appear to improve
contagion. Either individuals perform a ritual that overall levels of motor performance in tasks such as
in some way connects them to a higher power (e.g., a basketball free throw (Lobmeyer & Wasserman,
a batter performing the sign of the cross before step- 1986), superstitious behaviors are thought to pro-
ping up to the plate in a baseball game) or they pre- vide added benefits through their magical connec-
serve the “essence” of their recent good performance tion (Damisch, Stoberock, & Mussweiler, 2010).
by refusing to wash a particular garment until the Damisch et al. have shown that activation of
winning streak is broken. These behaviors also per- superstitions related to luck (e.g., providing some-
petuate the belief that some alternative form of cau- one with a “lucky” golf ball or having a personal
sality might influence the outcome of the event. good luck charm present) improves success at a put-
Superstitious reasoning is not reserved for sport- ting task, a motor dexterity task, and a memory task.
ing events, but appears to be quite common in the These researchers suggest that superstitious behav-
everyday lives of children and adults. Children may iors lead to an increase in perceived self-efficacy,
avoid cracks on the sidewalks for fear of influencing which in turn leads to greater persistence in the
their mother’s health, avoid black cats because they task. Although these results are intriguing, this
might bring bad luck, or carry lucky charms to help research was generally conducted with individu-
them in particular situations. Many of these behav- als who hold relatively strong superstitious beliefs.
iors share an important aspect of cultural transmis- For individuals who profess not to be superstitious,
sion that we suggest is a key process in the emergence activation of these types of beliefs might serve to
of magical thinking more generally. But other super- decrease the overall levels of performance. This sug-
stitious behaviors seem highly idiosyncratic (Vyse, gests that examining the role of implicit and explicit
1997), suggesting that many of these behaviors and superstitious beliefs might be an interesting area of
practices stem from the unconscious cognitive pro- further investigation. It also suggests that it might
cesses involved in magical thinking more generally. be interesting to determine whether magical think-
For example, we recently encountered a student ing in general leads to performance benefits in situ-
who insisted on taking every exam with a particular ations in which magical thinking might enhance the
pen that he used to take notes in class and he used as perception of control.
he studied for the exam. It was as if the pen carried
the essence of the information from learning and Magical Thinking and Peculiar Beliefs
studying into the exam. Idiosyncratic superstitions Magical thinking and superstition have also been
are also likely to occur when incomplete meanings linked with peculiar thoughts and beliefs in adults
of rituals are passed down from parents and adults more generally (Berenbaum, Boden, & Baker, 2009;
within a culture to children. Boden & Berenbaum, 2004). Peculiar beliefs have
Researchers have also shown that individuals been defined as those beliefs that the established
engage in superstitious behaviors under conditions scientific community do not view to be veridical
of high stress and/or uncertainty and low levels (Berenbaum, Kerns, & Raghavan, 2000). Generally,
of perceived control (Keinan, 1994; Malinowski, paranormal beliefs fit within this category.
1954; Whitson, & Gallinsky, 2008). Superstitions As with many beliefs, a spectrum of intensity
are thought to regulate stress and uncertainty and exists. In one form, peculiar beliefs can result in

54 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 54 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

paranormal beliefs, such as belief in ghosts, the exis- ESP, have been linked to a variety of forms of psy-
tence of extra terrestrials, alien abductions, and ESP. chopathology (American Psychiatric Association,
Like magical thinking, these beliefs are not con- 2000). For example, magical thinking in the form
sistent with current empirical observations in the of thought–action fusion has been related to anxi-
world—and are thought to be unscientific. However, ety disorders and obsessive–compulsive disorders
although some of these beliefs and thoughts can be (Berle & Starcevic, 2005; Muris, Meesters, Rassin,
viewed as magical, in our view others do not meet Merckelbach, & Campbell, 2001). Thought–
the criteria for magical thinking. That is, we sug- action fusion is a cognitive distortion that results in
gest that certain paranormal beliefs typically asso- the belief that certain negative events are brought
ciated with parapsychology, such as psychokinesis about by intrusive thoughts (Rachman & Shafran,
and ESP, are substantially different from magical 1999). These researchers report on one young
beliefs in that individuals who hold these particular woman who was afraid that her unwanted thoughts
beliefs often think that these phenomena do exist about her parents dying could cause a car accident
in the natural world, and believe that they can be through magical “vibes” (p. 82). Other researchers AQ:
explained by some natural explanation. For exam- have found that individuals with an excessive focus There is a
ple, believers in the notion that individuals can on weight and body shape sometimes hold simi- mismatch
in the
move or alter objects with their minds (psychoki- lar magical beliefs about food (Garner & Bemis, reference”
nesis) or read others’ thoughts (ESP) often suggest 1992). Garner
that these behaviors stem from a highly evolved Magical thinking has also been associated with & Bemis,
1982”.
mind, rather than some supernatural form of cau- schizophrenia (Claridge & Broks, 1984; Hewitt The date
sality. Interestingly, psychologists are the least likely & Claridge, 1989; Lee, Cougle, & Telch, 2005). is 1992 in
to advocate the reality of these extraordinary pow- Claridge and Broks (1984) found magical thinking the citation
where as
ers (Zusne & Jones, 1989), and tend to treat them to be one of three primary factors resulting from a in the list
in a pejorative fashion. Likewise, many individuals factor analysis of the Schizotypal Personality Scale it is 1982.
who advocate the existence of aliens or report alien along with factors related to paranoid ideation and Kindly
check.
abductions do not treat these events as supernatural, unusual perceptual experiences. Similar results have
but within the realm of possibility based on the idea been found by other researchers looking at adults
that because humans evolved on this planet, other (Joseph & Peters, 1995; Wolfradt & Straube, 1998),
beings might have evolved in a different form on although some researchers have identified additional
other distant planets. factors (Rawlings, Claridge, & Freeman, 2001).
The notion that scientific investigation could The majority of research examining magical
one day show that individuals have greater mental thinking and psychopathology has been conducted
powers than has been assumed or that aliens exist with adults. One of the few studies to examine
extend the normal views of mental and biological this relationship in children and adolescents was
processes, but they do not necessarily embrace the conducted by Bolton, Dearsley, Madronal-Luque,
same type of belief in an alternative form of causality and Baron-Cohen (2002). They used the a Magical
that we suggest is at the heart of magical thinking. Thinking Questionnaire adapted from an earlier
Indeed, although the majority of evidence does not questionnaire designed to assess thought–action
support the existence of either psi (anomalous pro- fusion (Muris, Meesters, Rassin, Merckelbach, &
cesses of information or energy transfer) or aliens, Campbell, 2001) and found that in children and
researchers continue to use the scientific method adolescents’ obsessive–compulsive thoughts and
to investigate their possible existence in psychol- actions were associated with magical thinking.
ogy (Bem & Honorton, 1994) and in the Search These researchers found that this relation did not
for Extraterrestrial Intelligence (SETI) project in change from childhood to adolescence, suggesting
astronomy (Tarter, 2001). that there may be continuity from childhood to
adulthood in magical thinking, at least in popula-
Magical Thinking and Psychopathology tions diagnosed with a psychological disorder.
In more extreme forms, magical and peculiar Clearly, the research indicates that certain forms
beliefs may be labeled as delusional (Berenbaum, of psychopathology appear to involve magical
1996) and have been linked to various forms of thinking to some extent. However, this conclusion
pathology (Eckblad & Chapman, 1983). But even must be tempered a bit, as magical thinking is often
in less extreme forms, these beliefs, including belief part of the diagnostic criteria for determining the
in such things as ghosts, good luck charms, and existence of a particular disorder. In addition, the

Rosengren, French 55

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 55 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

scales used to assess magical thinking and particu- driving out irrational thoughts and behaviors, but
lar disorders often assess a wide range of peculiar even the most rational of individuals might think
beliefs, all treated as magical, as they are considered twice about buying a house where someone has been
non-veridical and inconsistent with subcultural murdered or experience some “irrational” feelings if
norms. However, we suggest that some of the beliefs told that the sweater they had just been asked to
assessed are not necessarily magical (e.g., beliefs in put on was formerly owned by a notorious criminal.
ESP, feelings of unexplainable senses of danger). At best, we can be aware of our tendency to think
Finally, it is not at all clear how the magical think- magically and use reason to counteract the influence
ing of children, psychologically healthy adults, and of these cognitive biases.
individuals with particular psychological disorders
are related. On the surface they share similar quali- Future Directions
ties, yet they clearly differ under the surface. The Research on magical thinking has waxed and
issue is whether they differ on a continuum, similar waned over the years. Before the late 1970s research
to peculiar beliefs in general, or whether they are on magical thinking was an active area of research by
categorically different. anthropologists and psychologists. In the late 1970s
to the 1990s there appeared to be little interest in
Conclusion this area of research in either discipline, although
The Future of Magical Thinking interest remained strong among clinical psycholo-
Much of the research and scholarship investigat- gists interested in particular psychological disorders.
ing magical thinking in general, and superstitious Since that time there has been a renewed interest
beliefs more specifically, has been conducted under among both cognitive developmentalists in psy-
the assumption that these types of thoughts and chology and anthropologists interested in the role
beliefs were caused by errors in thinking, poten- of ritual in various cultures in the study of magical
tially brought about by failures in education or psy- thinking. This renewed interest has opened up new
chopathology. At least with psychologically healthy avenues of research and spotlighted areas that need
children and adults, the notion has been that by further investigation.
providing them with expanded access to science One of the most interesting directions for future
and mathematics education, magical thinking and research would be to examine how magical thinking
beliefs eventually would be eliminated. This appears in children relates to various forms of adult thought
to be an overly optimistic view. At present, magical in both psychologically healthy individuals and those
thinking appears to be just as prevalent today as in with particular psychological disorders. Specifically,
times past. The specific manifestations of magical it would be useful to explore more carefully how
thinking and particular beliefs common in differ- magical thinking in childhood relates to alternative
ent cultures may have shifted over time, but the fact or strange beliefs in adulthood and the emergence of
that magical thinking still exists in the minds of different forms of pathology. This research direction
both children and adults cannot be refuted. would involve investigating the source and nature of
Our own view is that magical thinking is here to individual differences in magical thinking in young
stay because it stems to a large extent from our cog- children and tracking continuity and change in
nitive architecture. This cognitive architecture leads magical thinking over developmental time. At pres-
us to search for causality, even when no actual causal ent, cross-sectional studies have shed some light on
link between a behavior and an outcome exists. It the developmental time course of magical thinking,
also leads us to essentialize, making us susceptible but have failed to provide much information about
to the sympathetic magic of contagion and simi- continuity of magical thinking in some individuals.
larity. These cognitive processes coupled with the Relatedly, it would be important to determine if
acquisition of a special category of magic, one that the magical thinking of children relates on a contin-
includes objects and events that violate our causal uum with that of psychologically healthy adults and
expectations about how the world normally works, individuals with particular disorders or whether the
serve to nurture magical thinking in young children nature of magical thinking in these populations is
and then maintain it over the course of our lives. categorically different. One of the challenges of pur-
These processes bias initial reactions to particular suing this issue involves developing better measures
phenomena, ones that violate the norm and likely of magical thinking that are based on stronger theo-
increase under times of stress or high emotional retical grounds. A related challenge would be for
investment. Some individuals may work hard at researchers focusing on these different populations

56 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 56 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

to agree on how magical thinking should be defined. potential benefits to thinking magically? Like the
As we have suggested, magic as a pejorative label magical thought involved in superstition, does mag-
for individuals who think “differently” should be ical thinking sometimes enhance performance? And
avoided and the belief in some form of alternative are there individuals or situations in which thinking
form of causality outside the domain of scientific magically leads to decreases in performance? Does
inquiry should be central to this definition. We hope too much stimulation of magical thinking lead to
our definition of magical thinking prevails because strange beliefs that negatively affect one’s ability to
it removes the pejorative aspect of the magical label function in the real world?
and more clearly defines what should or should not
be characterized as magical. Our definition also Further Readings
serves to redefine magic as situated in the particu- Hood, B. M. (2009). Supersense: Why we believe in the unbeliev-
lar beliefs of a culture. As more and more research able. New York: HarperCollins.
Rosengren, K. S., Johnson, C. N., & Harris, P. L. (2000). Imagining
shows that cognition is highly influenced by culture the impossible: Magical, scientific, and religious thinking in chil-
(e.g., Nisbett, 2003) it is even more important to dren. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
clarify the definition of magical thinking. Subbotsky, E. (2010). The magic of today: Mechanisms, functions
Clearly, culture influences the beliefs that are and development of magical thinking and behavior. Oxford,
considered to be natural and supernatural. Thus, UK: Oxford University Press.
Vyse, S. A. (1997). Believing in magic. The psychology of supersti-
what may be magical thought in one culture may tion. New York: Oxford University Press.
not in fact be magical in another. Being sensitive to
cultural differences, rather than labeling these dif-
References
ferences as magical in the pejorative sense, will help American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statis-
us to understand more clearly how and when magi- tical manual of mental disorders (4th edition, test revision).
cal thinking might aid or hinder performance in a Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
given task or situation. Thus, an additional direction Bem, D., & Honorton, C. (1994). Does psi exist? Replicable
for future work would be to explore cultural varia- evidence for an anomalous process or information transfer.
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 4–18.
tion in magical thinking. We suggest that it would Berenbaum, H., Boden, M. T., & Baker, J. P. (2009). Emotional
be fruitful to reopen this area of research with a goal salience, emotional awareness, peculiar beliefs, and magical
of understanding how culture shapes cognition in thinking. Emotion, 18, 197–205.
important ways and explicitly explore the social Berenbaum, H., Kerns, J. G., & Raghavan, C. (2000). Anomalous
transmission of aspects of magical thinking. experiences, peculiarity, and psychopathology. In E, Cardena,
S. Lynn, & S. Krippner (Eds.), The varieties of anomalous expe-
Another interesting direction for future research rience, pp. 25–46. Washington, DC: American Psychological
would be to explore the relation between magical and Association.
other forms of thinking. In particular, magical and Berle, D., & Starcevic, V. (2005). Thought-action fusion: Review
scientific thinking are often thought of as incom- of the literature and future directions. Clinical Psychology
patible. Recent research suggests that different types Review, 25, 263–284.
Bolton, D., Dearsley, P., Madronal-Luque, R., & Baron-Cohen,
of explanation (magical, religious, scientific) can be S. (2002). Magical thinking in childhood and adolescence:
AQ:
used to reason about the same phenomena (Evans, Please note
Development and relation to obsessive compulsion. British that the
Legare, & Rosengren, 2011). This use of multiple Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 479–494. citation for
explanations appears to be evident in the think- Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas. A cognitive the- reference
ing about illness (Legare & Gelman, 2008), the ory of religion. Berkley, CA: University of California Press. Boyer,
Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2008). Evolutionary perspectives on P., &
origins of life (Evans, 2000), and death (Harris & religion. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 111–130. Bergstrom,
Giménez, 2005; Rosengren, Miller, Gutierrez, Boyer, P., & Walker, S. (2000). Intuitive ontology and cultural B. (2008)
Chow, Schein, & Anderson, in preparation). What input in the acquisition of religious concepts. In Rosengren, is missing.
is not at all clear is how children and adults choose a K. S., Johnson, C. N., & Harris, P. L. (Eds.), Imagining the
particular form of explanation in a given situation, impossible. Magical, scientific and religious thinking in children,
pp. 130–156. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
and under what conditions these different types of Claridge, G., & Broks, P. (1984). Schizotypy and hemisphere
explanations are combined to form some kind of function. I. Theoretical considerations and the measure-
blended or synthetic explanation. ment of schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 5,
Finally, the role of magical thinking in support- 633–648.
ing creativity and imagination should be investi- Clarke, A. C. (1962). Profiles of the future: An inquiry into the
limits of the possible. New York: Warner Books.
gated more fully. Does thinking magically enable Damisch, L., Stoberock, B., & Mussweiler, T. (2010). Keep your
individuals to be more creative? If so, what best fingers crossed! How superstition improves performance.
ways to stimulate this connection? Are there other Psychological Science, 21, 1014–1020.

Rosengren, French 57

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 57 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F., & Rosengren, K. S. (1997). The atory frameworks across development. Cognitive Science: A
incredible shrinking room: Very young children’s perfor- Multidisciplinary Journal, 32, 607–642.
mance with symbolic and nonsymbolic relations. Psychological Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of
Science, 8, 308–313. “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.
Didion, J. (2005). The year of magical thinking. New York: Alfred Lévy-Bruhl, L (1966). Primitive mentality. Boston: Beacon Press
A. Knopf. (original work published 1923).
Eckblad, M., & Chapman, L. J. (1983). Magical ideation as an Lévy-Bruhl, L. (1984). How natives think. Salem, NH: Ayer
indicator of schizotopy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Publishers (original work published 1926).
Psychology, 51, 215–225. Lindeman, M., & Aarnio, K., (2007). Superstitious, magical,
Evans, E. M. (2000a). The emergence of beliefs about the origins and paranormal beliefs: An integrative model. Journal of
of species in school-age children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: A Research in Personality, 41, 731–744.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 46, 221–254. Lobmeyer, D., & Wasserman, E. A. (1986). Preliminaries to free
Evans, E. M. (2000b). Beyond Scopes: Why creationism is here throw shooting: Superstitious behaviour? Journal of Sport
to stay. In K. Rosengren, C. Johnson, & P. Harris (Eds.) Behavior, 9, 70–78.
Imagining the impossible: Magical, scientific and religious think- Luhrman, T. M. (1989). Persuasions of the witch’s craft: Ritual
ing in children, pp. 305–331. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge magic and witchcraft in present-day England. Oxford, UK:
University Press. Blackwell.
Evans, E. M., Legare, C., & Rosengren, K. S. (2011). Engaging Malinowski, B. (1954). Magic, science, and religion and other
multiple epistemologies. In R. Taylor & M. Ferrari (Eds.), essays. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Epistemology and science education: Understanding the evolu- Mauss, M. (1972). A general theory of magic. New York: W.W.
tion vs. intelligent design controversy (pp. 111–139). London: Norton & Co.
Routledge. Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought: Diversity, evolution,
Frazer, J. G. (1911). The golden bough: A Study of magic and reli- and inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
gion. London: Macmillan. Medin, D. L., & Ortony, A. (1989). Psychological essentialism.
Garner, D. M., & Bemis, K. M. (1982). A cognitive-behavioural In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds), Similarity and ana-
approach to anorexia nervosa. Cognitive Therapy and Research, logical reasoning, pp. 179–195. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
6, 123–150. University Press.
Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in Michotte, A. (1962). The perception of causality. Andover, MA:
everyday thought. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Methuen.
Gilovich,T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.). (2002). Heuristics Muris, P., Meesters, C., Rassin, E., Merckelbach, H., & Campbell,
and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press. J. (2001). Thought-action fusion and anxiety disorders symp-
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In toms in normal adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.). Mapping the mind: 39(7), 843–852. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00077-2.
Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge, UK: Nemeroff, C. (1995). Magical thinking about illness virulence:
Cambridge University Press. Conceptions of germs from “safe” versus “dangerous” others.
Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. Health Psychology, 14, 147–151.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Nemeroff, C., & Rozin, P. (1992). Sympathetic magical beliefs
Harris, P. L., & Giménez, M. (2005). Children’s acceptance of and kosher dietary practice: The interaction of rules and feel-
conflicting testimony: The case of death. Journal of Cognition ings. Ethos, 20, 1, 96–115.
and Culture, 5, 143–164. Nemeroff, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). The contagion concept in
Harris, P. L., & Koenig, M. (2006). Trust in testimony: How adult thinking in the United States: Transmission of germs
children learn about science and religion. Child Development, and an interpersonal influence. Ethos, 22, 2, 158–186.
77, 505–524. Nemeroff, C., & Rozin, P. (2000). The making of the magi-
Hewitt, J. K., & Claridge, G. (1989). The factor structure of cal mind: The nature and function of sympathetic magi-
schizotypy in a normal population. Personality and Individual cal thinking. In K. S. Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, & P. L.
Differences, 10, 323–329. Harris (Eds.), Imagining the impossible: Magical, scientific
Joseph, S., & Peters, E. R. (1995). Factor structure of schizotypy and religious thinking in children, pp. 1–34. Cambridge, UK:
with normal subjects: A replication of Hewitt and Claridge Cambridge University Press.
1989. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 437–440. Neusner, J. (1989). Religion, science, and magic: In concert and in
Keinan, G. (1994). Effects of stress and tolerance of ambigu- conflict. New York: Oxford University Press.
ity on magical thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Nisbett, R. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and
Psychology, 67, 48–55. Westerners think differently . . . and why. New York: Free Press.
Keinan, G. (2002). The effects of stress and desire for control Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). Sacred and secular: Religion
on superstitious behavior. Personality and Social Psychology and politics worldwide. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge univer-
Bulletin, 28, 102–108. sity Press.
Kieckhefer, R. (1989). Magic in the Middle Ages. Cambridge, Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. London:
UK: Cambridge University Press. Kegan Paul.
Lee, H., Cougle, J. R., & Telch M. J. (2005). Thought-action Piaget, J. (1930). The child’s conception of physical reality. London:
fusion and its relationship to schizotypy and OCD symp- Kegan Paul.
toms. Behavior Research and Therapy, 43, 29–41. Poling, D. A., & Evans, E. M. (2004). Are dinosaurs the rule or
Legare, C. H., & Gelman, S. A. (2008). Bewitchment, biology, the exception? Developing concepts of death and extinction.
or both: The co-existence of natural and supernatural explan- Cognitive Development, 19, 363–383.

58 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 58 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

Rachman, S., & Shafran, R. (1999). Cognitive distortions: P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the
Thought-action fusion. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, mind, pp. 227–347. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
6, 80–85. Subbotsky, E. (2000b). Phenomenalistic perception and ratio-
Rawlings, D., Claridge, G., & Freeman, J. L. (2001). Principal nal understanding in the mind of an individual: A fight for
components analysis of the Schizotypal Personality Scale dominance. In K. S. Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, & P. L.
(STA) and the Borderline Personality Scale (STB). Personality Harris (Eds), Imagining the impossible. Magical, scientific and
and Individual Differences, 31, 409–419. religious thinking in children, pp. 35–74. Cambridge, UK:
Rosengren, K. S., & Hickling, A. K. (1994). Seeing is believ- Cambridge University Press.
ing: Children’s explanations of commonplace, magical, Subbotsky, E. (2000c). Phenomenalistic reality: The develop-
and extraordinary transformations. Child Development, 65, mental perspective. Developmental Review, 20, 438–474.
1605–1626. Subbotsky, E. (2004). Magical thinking in judgments of causa-
Rosengren, K. S., & Hickling, A. K. (2000). Metamorphosis tion: Can anomalous phenomena affect ontological causal
and magic: The development of children’s thinking beliefs in children and adults? British Journal of Developmental
about possible events and plausible mechanisms. In K. S. Psychology, 22, 123–152.
Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Imagining Subbotsky, E. (2010). The magic of today: Mechanisms, functions
the impossible. Magical, scientific, and religious think- and development of magical thinking and behavior. Oxford,
ing in children, pp. 75–98. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge UK: Oxford University Press.
University Press. Tambiah, S. J. (1990). Magic, science, religion, and the scope of
Rosengren, K. S., Kalish, C. W., Hickling, A. K., & Gelman, rationality. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
S. A. (1994). Exploring the relation between preschool chil- Tarter, J. (2001). The search for extraterrestrial intelligence
dren’s magical beliefs and causal thinking. British Journal of (SETI). Annual Review of Astronomy and Astrophysics, 39,
Developmental Psychology, 12, 69–82. 511–548.
Rosengren, K. S., Miller, P. J., Gutiérrez, I. T., Chow, P., Schein, Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncer-
S., & Anderson, K. A. (in press). Children’s understanding of tainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.
death: toward a contextualized and integrated account. Vyse, S. A. (1997). Believing in magic. The psychology of supersti-
Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & McCauley, C. R. (1994). The nature tion. New York: Oxford University Press.
of aversion to indirect contact with another person: AIDS Wellman, H. M., & Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of
aversion as a composite of aversion to strangers, infection, mental entities: A re-examination of childhood realism.
moral taint and misfortune. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Child Development, 57, 910–923.
103, 495–504. Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive develop-
Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Nemeroff, C. (1992). Magical con- ment: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review
tagion beliefs and fear of AIDS. Journal of Applied Social of Psychology, 43, 337–375.
Psychology, 22, 1081–1092. Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control
Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Ross, B. (1990). The sympathetic increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322, 115–117.
magical law of similarity, nominal realism, and neglect of Wolfradt, U., & Straube, E. R. (1998). Factor structure of schizo-
negatives in response to negative labels. Psychological Science, typal traits among adolescents. Personality and Individual
1, 383–384. Differences, 24, 201–206.
Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of Womack, M. (1992). Why athletes need ritual: A study of magic
laws of sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. among professional athletes. In S. J. Hoffman (Ed.), Sport and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 703–712. religion, pp. 191–202. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Schippers, M. C., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2006). The psycho- Woolley, J. D. (1997). Thinking about fantasy: Are children
logical benefits of superstitious rituals in top sport: A study fundamentally different thinkers and believers from adults?
among top sports persons. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Child Development, 98, 991–1011.
36, 2532–2553. Woolley, J. D. (2000). The development of beliefs about direct
Sorensen, J. (2007). A cognitive theory of magic. New York: mental-physical causality in imagination, magic, and reli-
AltMira Press. gion. In S. Rosengren, C. N. Johnson, & P. L. Harris, (Eds.),
Subbotsky, E. V. (1985). Preschool children’s perception of Imagining the impossible. Magical, scientific and religious think-
unusual phenomena. Soviet Psychology, 23, 91–114. ing in children, pp. 99–129. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Subbotsky, E. V. (1992). Foundations of the mind. Children’s University Press.
understanding of reality. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Woolley, J., Boerger, E. A., & Markman, A. B. (2004). A visit
Subbotsky, E. V. (1994). Early rationality and magical think- from the Candy Witch: Factors influencing children’s belief in
ing in preschoolers: Space and time. British Journal of a novel fantastic entity. Developmental Science, 7, 456–468.
Developmental Psychology, 12, 97–108. Woolley, J. D., Browne, C. A., & Boerger, E. A. (2006).
Subbotsky, E. V. (1996). Explaining impossible phenomena: Constraints on children’s judgments of magical causality.
Object permanence beliefs and memory failures in adults. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 253–277.
Memory, 4, 199–223. Woolley, J. D., & Phelps, K. E. (1994). Young children’s practical
Subbotsky, E. V. (1997). Explanations of unusual events: reasoning about imagination. British Journal of Developmental
Phenomenalistic causal judgments in children and adults. Psychology, 12, 1, 53–67.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology,15, 13–36. Woolley, J. D., Phelps, K. E., Davis, D. L., & Mandell, D. J.
Subbotsky, E. V. (2000a). Causal reasoning and behavior in (1999). Where theories of mind meet magic: The develop-
children and adults in a Technologically advanced society: ment of children’s beliefs about wishing. Child Development,
Are we still prepared to believe in magic and animism? In 70, 571–587.

Rosengren, French 59

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 59 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM


OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Thu Jan 10 2013, NEWGEN

Woolley, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (1993). Origin and truth: Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Chiang, W. (2002). Enumeration of collec-
Young children’s understanding of imaginary mental repre- tive entities by 5-month-old infants. Cognition, 83, B55–B62.
sentations. Child Development, 64, 1, 1–17. Zusne, L., & Jones, W. H. (1989). Anomalistic psychology. A
Wynn, K. (1990). Children’s understanding of counting. study of extraordinary phenomena of behavior and experience.
Cognition, 36, 155–193. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

60 Magical Thinking

04_MarjorieTaylor_Ch04.indd 60 1/10/2013 2:56:23 PM

You might also like