You are on page 1of 12

Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research Neuroimaging


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychresns

Decreased response inhibition to sad faces during explicit and implicit tasks MARK
in females with depression: Evidence from an event-related potential study
Fengqiong Yua,b,c,1, Xiaoqing Zhoud,1, Wu Qinga,c, Dan Lib,c, Jing Lia, Xingui Chena,c,
⁎ ⁎
Gongjun Jia,c, Yi Dongc,d, Yuejia Luoe, Chunyan Zhua,c, , Kai Wanga,b,c,
a
Laboratory of Cognitive Neuropsychology, Department of Medical Psychology, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China
b
Department of Neurology, The First Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China
c
Collaborative Innovation Centre of Neuropsychiatric Disorders and Mental Health, Anhui Province, China
d
Anhui Mental Health Center, Hefei, China
e
Institute of Social and affective Neuroscience, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China

A R T I C L E I N F O A BS T RAC T

Keywords: The present study aimed to investigate neural substrates of response inhibition to sad faces across explicit and
Depression implicit tasks in depressed female patients. Event-related potentials were obtained while participants performed
Response inhibition modified explicit and implicit emotional go/no-go tasks. Compared to controls, depressed patients showed
Implicit decreased discrimination accuracy and amplitudes of original and nogo-go difference waves at the P3 interval in
Explicit
response inhibition to sad faces during explicit and implicit tasks. P3 difference wave were positively correlated
Emotion
P3
with discrimination accuracy and were independent of clinical assessment. The activation of right dorsal
prefrontal cortex was larger for the implicit than for the explicit task in sad condition in health controls, but was
similar for the two tasks in depressed patients. The present study indicated that selectively impairment in
response inhibition to sad faces in depressed female patients occurred at the behavior inhibition stage across
implicit and explicit tasks and may be a trait-like marker of depression. Longitudinal studies are required to
determine whether decreased response inhibition to sad faces increases the risk for future depressive episodes
so that appropriate treatment can be administered to patients.

1. Introduction social interactions, inhibiting inappropriate negative emotions con-


veyed by sad faces is an indispensible social skill. Depression patients
Depression is one of the most common psychiatric disorders and a show attentional bias to sad faces and are unable to prevent being
leading cause of disability worldwide (Goodwin et al., 2006). Studies troubled by sad expressions (Gollan et al., 2008; Gur et al., 1992;
suggest that at least 20% of adolescents experience a depressive Sylvester et al., 2015). In one study, depressed adolescents and those
episode before adulthood, which is associated with high morbidity who developed depression at follow-up made more commission errors
and mortality (Brent and Birmaher, 2006). Women show a younger age for sad than for happy faces (Kilford et al., 2015). Moreover,
of onset and higher rate of recurrence of depression than men (Kessler unmedicated offspring of depressed mothers showed greater pupil
et al., 2005; Schuch et al., 2014). Repeated negative thoughts are dilation compared to controls, which was associated with greater
considered a hallmark of depression (Koster et al., 2011), and are depression risk (Burkhouse et al., 2015). Neuroimaging studies have
related to impaired cognitive control over negative information found that depressed individuals exhibit higher activity in the amyg-
(Ochsner and Gross, 2008; Wager et al., 2008). Response inhibition dala in response to sad faces, whereas control subjects show higher
is an important component of this cognitive control system; depressed activity in response to happy faces (Suslow et al., 2010). Depression
individuals are unable to inhibit their response to negative information patients also show less differential activation of the insular cortex in
(De Raedt and Koster, 2010; Gotlib and Joormann, 2010), which is also response to sad vs. happy faces as compared to healthy controls; the
observed in unmedicated offspring of depressed mothers (Joormann difference in activation is negatively correlated with depression severity
et al., 2007). (Henje Blom et al., 2015). These results suggest that depressed
Sad facial expressions are one type of negative social stimulus. In individuals show facilitated negative bias in the processing of sad


Corresponding authors at: Laboratory of Cognitive Neuropsychology, Department of Medical Psychology, Anhui Medical University, Hefei, China.
E-mail addresses: zhuchunyan@163.com (C. Zhu), wangkai1964@126.com (K. Wang).
1
These authors contribute equally to this work.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.10.013
Received 25 February 2016; Received in revised form 22 October 2016; Accepted 27 October 2016
Available online 08 December 2016
0925-4927/ © 2016 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

faces. Exerting cognitive control over sad emotions is critical for the and Pourtois, 2010; Berggren and Derakshan, 2013; Botvinick et al.,
remission of depression, and understanding the neural correlates of 2001).
response inhibition to sad faces in depression can enable the initiation The present study investigated the neural substrates of response
of appropriate interventions. inhibition to sad faces across implicit and explicit tasks in depression
In explicit processing, facial expressions are within the scope of patients based on ERP recordings. We hypothesized that depression
voluntary attention and are directly processed, whereas in implicit patients would show deficits in response inhibition to sad faces across
processing they occur outside the scope of voluntary attention and are both tasks, and would therefore show decreased accuracy in discerning
incidentally processed. As such, attentional resources for stimulus sad faces as well as a shorter response time to sad faces relative to
processing differ between the two conditions. Implicit and explicit control subjects. For ERP recordings, we expected decreased N2 and
facial processing serve different functions (Taylor et al., 2003) and have P3 amplitudes in inhibition-related brain areas such as the rIFC in both
distinct neural substrates (Linden et al., 2010; Valdes-Conroy et al., tasks. We also examined the correlation between behavioral measures,
2014; Winston et al., 2003), and subjects have reported different ERP components, and clinical parameters to determine whether
emotional intensities associated with facial expressions processed decreases in N2 and P3 are markers of depression. We recruited only
explicitly vs. implicitly. For instance, rating pictures was associated women in this study since response inhibition and patterns of brain
with reduced intensity of sadness as compared to passive viewing, activation show gender differences (Yuan et al., 2009) and the
likely because the rating task reduced the activation of brain regions incidence of depression is higher among females (Kessler et al.,
related to emotional experiences (Taylor et al., 2003). Psychiatric 2005; Schuch et al., 2014).
patients show different responses to explicit and implicit emotional
stimuli. For example, schizophrenia and depression patients exhibit
greater automatic amygdala responses to implicit sad faces than 2. Materials and methods
controls, in the former, these responses were positively correlated with
the negative subscale of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale 2.1. Participants
(Rauch et al., 2010). We previously found that response inhibition was
modulated by sad facial information at the action inhibition stage when Female unipolar depression outpatients (n=20; age: 17–46 years)
facial expressions are processed explicitly as opposed to implicitly (Yu were recruited from the depression disorder clinic in Anhui Mental
et al., 2014). Moreover, individuals with generalized anxiety disorder Health Center. Diagnostic assessments were independently completed
showed impaired response inhibition to sad faces in an explicit but not by two psychiatrists based on DSM-IV criteria. DSM-IV is the fourth
in an implicit task (Yu et al., 2015). However, it is not known whether edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
depression patients exhibit deficits in response inhibition across (DSM) which has been praised for standardizing psychiatric diagnostic
implicit and explicit conditions. categories and criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The
To address this issue, we developed a modified emotional go/no-go manual includes all currently recognized mental health disorder and
paradigm consisting of two sections. In the task-related section, thus is usually used by mental health professionals to describe the
participants made their go/no-go decision according to recognition of characteristics of a given mental disorder to distinguish the disorder
facial expressions; that is, the emotional expression was explicitly from other, similar problems. In addition, patients included in the
processed. In the task-irrelevant section, participants responded or study scored > 14 on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD)-
inhibited their response based on identification of the gender of the 17 which is used for adults and to rate the depression severity by
face; that is, the emotional processing was implicit. We used the same probing mood, feeling of guilt, suicide ideation, agitation, retardation,
set of stimuli in the two sections to exclude interference from anxiety, weight loss, and somatic symptom. The scale showed high
additional factors. We hypothesized that response inhibition deficits inter-rater reliability as 0.9 for 70 patients (Hamilton, 1960). Exclusion
in depression are reflected by lower discriminating accuracy and criteria were as follows: any history of mania, psychosis or any other
shorter response time to sad stimuli across explicit and implicit tasks. Axis I disorders; a history of neurological conditions; substance abuse;
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded during the evalua- and current use of psychotropic medication. Patients had never had
tion of behavioral measures. The main advantage of this non-invasive behavioral or drug treatment for depression or had suspended treat-
method is the high time resolution that reflects the time course of brain ment for at least two months prior to the study. Age-matched female
activity (Otten and Rugg, 2004); it has been frequently used to explore adults (n=21) screened for current and past psychiatric and neurolo-
the neural basis of response inhibition (Fabiani et al., 2000), which gical disorders were recruited through internet postings. All partici-
comprises two different cognitive processes—namely, conflict monitor- pants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and HAMD-17 scores
ing and action inhibition (Sehlmeyer et al., 2010). No-go N2 reflects the < 7. Control tests and characteristics of the study population are shown
former, while no-go P3 is associated with conflict resolution and in Table 1. This study was carried out in accordance with the
behavioral inhibition (Donkers and van Boxtel, 2004; Kropotov et al., Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Research Ethics
2011). Neuroimaging studies have revealed that the right inferior Committee of Anhui Medical University. All participants completed the
prefrontal cortex (rIFC) plays a crucial role in response inhibition to experiment on a voluntarily basis with receiving RMB50 yuan.
emotional stimuli (Berkman et al., 2009; Goldstein et al., 2007;
Table 1
Ochsner et al., 2004; Padmala and Pessoa, 2010; Shafritz et al.,
Group characteristics of the DEP group and CON group.
2006); ERP measurements have shown that response inhibition is
impaired in individuals with anxiety (Hum et al., 2013). Depression DEP CON Between group
patients showed decreased no-go N2 amplitudes in a modified auditory Group Group comparison
go/no-go task (Kaiser et al., 2003; Katz et al., 2010). However, another
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) P-value
study in which participants performed a cued emotional conflict task
revealed differences between depression patients and controls only Age(years) 30.6(9.4) 25.9(6.2) t39=1.84, P=0.084
during the late N450 time window (Vanderhasselt et al., 2012). These Education(years) 13.7(3.4) 13.9(0.9) t39=0.25, P=0.80
inconsistent findings may be due to variations in experimental para- BDI 18.5(4.5) 4.9(2.1) t39=12.42, P < 0.001
SAI 50.2(10.3) 36.0(6.9) t39=5.37, P < 0.001
digms and patient inclusion criteria in these studies. Although depres-
TAI 57.3(8.5) 34.4(6.7) t39=9.64, P < 0.001
sion disrupts response inhibition, the neural basis of this effect is not
well understood. It has been suggested that depression is linked to a Abbreviations: DEP, depression; CON, control; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory. SAI,
reduced ability to monitor conflict or to apply active inhibition (Aarts State Anxiety Inventory; TAI, Trait Anxiety Inventory.

43
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

2.2. Materials no-go trial was always preceded by a go trial. This was done in order to
induce pre-potent motor responses and conflict during response
Stimuli used in the present study have been described in our inhibition. At the start of each block, an instruction screen that lasted
previous work (Yu et al., 2014). Sad and neutral faces (n=40 each) were for 5 min was presented in order to prompt participants to press or
selected from the native Chinese Facial Affective Picture System and refrain from pressing the “J” key with their right hand according to the
have been assessed on a 9-point scale by 100 college students from two facial expression or gender. Each trial was initiated by a small white
colleges in Beijing and used in many studies (Fan et al., 2015; Luo cross that was displayed for a variable duration (200–400 ms) on the
et al., 2010). In the present study, 20 female and 20 male faces black background. An emotional face then appeared at the center of the
displaying each type of emotion were presented to subjects. The screen for 1000 ms, and participants were asked to respond as quickly
stimulus were selected such that they differed significantly in valence and accurately as possible. The inter-trial interval which means the
from one another [t=11.65, P < 0.001 (M ± SD, sad: 3.11 ± 0.63, time interval from stimulus offset to stimulus onset was 1200–
neutral: 4.49 ± 0.41)], but were similar in terms of arousal (P > 0.05). 1500 ms. The experimental procedure is outlined in Fig. 1. The
Stimuli were also similar in size, spatial frequency, brightness, back- subjects in this experiment have given written informed consent to
ground, contrast grade, and other physical properties. Each picture was publish these case details. A practice session consisting of 20 trials was
cropped using Adobe Photoshop v.8.0 software into an elliptical shape administered before the experiment using stimuli that differed from
that incorporated facial characteristics. The screen resolution was72 those used in the actual experiment.
pixels per inch, and the viewing angle was 5.7°×4.6°. All stimuli were
displayed in the center of a 17-in. screen. Subjects were seated in a
soundproof room with their eyes approximately 100 cm away from the
screen.
2.4. Event-related potential recording
2.3. Experimental procedures
A Geodesic Sensor Net System (Neuro Scan, Sterling, VA, USA) was
The experimental procedure was similar to that of previous studies used to record 64-channel electroencephalography (EEG) according to
(Yu et al., 2014, 2015) and was developed using E-Prime software the international 10/20 system within an electrically and acoustically
(Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). The experiment shielded room. The system was grounded with a forehead electrode. All
included two parts: the implicit and explicit tasks. In the former, EEG signals were referenced to the left mastoid and were re-referenced
participants were instructed to respond to the presentation of faces off-line to the average of the left and right mastoids. Vertical electro-
depicting one gender (go trials) and to inhibit response after presenta- oculogram (EOG) data were recorded supraorbitally and infraorbitally
tion of the other gender (no-go trial). In the explicit task, participants in the left eye. Horizontal EOG data were recorded as the left versus
were asked to respond or inhibit their response according to facial right orbital rim. EEG and EOG activity was amplified with a 0.01–
expression category. There were 480 trials that included 144 no-go 100 Hz band-pass filter and continuously sampled at 500 Hz/channel.
stimuli and 336 go stimuli (30% vs. 70%) in implicit and explicit tasks, All electrode impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ. Ocular artifacts
which were presented in two separate parts, with the order of the parts were removed from the EEG signals using a regression procedure
counterbalanced across participants. In addition, each part was sub- implemented in Neuroscan software. Trials with remaining EOG
divided into two blocks. Facial stimuli were counterbalanced in terms artifacts, amplifier clipping artifacts, or peak-to-peak deflections ex-
of whether they were indicated as go or no-go trials in each part. Thus, ceeding ± 100 μV were excluded from averaging. The EEG activities
the experimental procedure consisted of four blocks: sad-go/neutral- during correct responses in each condition were aligned and averaged
no-go and neutral-go/sad-no-go in the explicit task, and female-go/ separately. The ERP waveforms were time-locked to the onset of the
male-no-go and male-go/female-no-go in the implicit task. In each face stimuli, and the average epoch was 1200 ms, including a 200 ms
block, go and no-go stimuli were presented pseudo-randomly, and a pre-stimulus base-line.

Fig. 1. Experimental design for (A) explicit and (B) implicit emotional go/no-go tasks. In the explicit task, subjects pressed a response button or inhibited their behavior according to the
facial expression (sad/neutral). In the implicit task, subjects responded to gender (male/female).

44
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

2.5. Data analysis distributions. Yet, the algorithm has been conformed to be cross-modal
validity (Pizzagalli et al., 2002). Studies have shown that sLORETA
The independent variables in the experiment were task (explicit/ results are consistent with those obtained by MRI (magnetic resonance
implicit), valence (sad/neutral), trial type (go/no-go), and group imaging) and functional MRI as well as electrocorticography from
(patient/health control). Data were evaluated by a mixed measures subdural electrodes (Seeck et al., 1998; Worrell et al., 2000). However,
multivariate analysis of variance. The degrees of freedom of F ratios as the limited spacial resolution of sLORETA, the results should be
were adjusted according to the Greenhouse–Geisser epsilon correction confirmed by other brain imaging methodologies with high spatial
in all analyses. The Bonferroni method was used for multiple compar- resolution.
isons. First, To detect the neural mechanism of response inhibition to
facial expression, voxel-based whole brain sLORETA images were
2.5.1. Analysis of behavioral results compared between go and no-go conditions; The sLORETA built-in
Signal detection theory was applied to the analysis of behavioral voxel-wise randomization tests (5000 permutations) based on statis-
results in order to distinguish signal from noise (Snodgrass and tical non-parametric mapping methodology were used to yield a whole-
Corwin, 1988); participants’ discrimination accuracy d' (distance brain statistical correction (Nichols and Holmes, 2002). According to
between signal and noise distributions) and decision bias β (tendency previous sLORETA studies, to reduce the likelihood of false positives,
to respond to signal and noise distributions) were assessed based on we set the nominal significance threshold to P < 0.005, with minimal
this theory. d' was defined as d′=(μs−μn)/σ, where μs and μn are signal cluster at least 5 significant contiguous voxels (Mueller and Pizzagalli,
and noise distributions, respectively, and σ is the common standard 2016). Second, the current source density (CSD) of the clusters that
deviation of both distributions. d' was calculated as d′=Zhit rate−Zfalse were significant in the first step would be subjected to ANOVA in order
alarm rate, which had four possible outcomes: hit (signal present and
to determine inter-group differences and assess the modulatory effects
subject's response is ‘yes’), false alarm (signal absent and subject's of task and valence. Due to limited spatial resolution of sLORETA, we
response is ‘yes’), miss (signal present and subject's response is ‘no’), extracted the mean CSD for all contiguous significant voxels of the
and correct rejection (signal absent and subject's response is ‘no’). β clusters for further statistical analysis to increase the credibility of the
was defined as β=fs(λ)/fn(λ), where fs(λ) is the height of the signal results.
distribution at a given criterion λ, and fn(λ) is the height of the noise
distribution at the same λ; the value was calculated as β= exp (d′×C),
where C=–(Zhit rate+Zfalse alarm rate)/2. A three-way mixed design
analysis of variance (ANOVA) of the amplitudes of each component
was carried out with task (two levels: implicit and explicit), valence 2. Results
(two levels: neutral and sad) as within-subject factors, and group
(depression and control) as a between-subject factor. 2.1. Behavioral performance

2.5.2. ERP measurement and analysis Subjects’ response accuracy and time are presented in Table 2. An
According to the topographical distribution of grand-averaged ERP analysis of accuracy by signal detection theory revealed that the main
activity and previous studies (Righart and de Gelder, 2007; Yu et al., effect of group was significant (F1,39=5.67, P < 0.05). Depression
2009; Yuan et al., 2008), nine electrode sites including F3, Fz, F4, C3, patients showed smaller d' as compared to controls when inhibiting
Cz, C4, P3, Pz, and P4 were selected for statistical analysis of N2 (250– inappropriate responses. The interaction effect of group and valence
350 ms) and P3 (500–600 ms) mean amplitudes. A five-way mixed was also significant (F1,39=4.80, P < 0.05) (see Fig. 2). Further analysis
design analysis of variance (ANOVA) of the amplitudes of each revealed that the group effect was significant for sad faces (F1,39=6.86,
component was carried out with task (two levels: implicit and explicit), P < 0.05). Depression patients had significantly smaller d' values than
valence (two levels: neutral and sad), trial type (two levels: go and no- controls when asked to respond to neutral faces and inhibit response to
go), and electrode as within-subject factors, and group (depression and sad faces, indicating their diminished ability to suppress negative
control) as a between-subject factor. information. However, the inter-group difference in the neutral condi-
tion was not significant (F1,39=0.11, P=0.74), and the difference in d'
2.5.3. Source localization analysis between the two groups was not significant in the other conditions. d'
Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography and β are independent variables, and therefore discrimination accuracy
analysis (sLORETA) was used to determine the neural substrates of does not correlate with decision bias. Discrimination accuracy in
response inhibition (Pascual-Marqui, 2002). Using this method, we response to neutral faces and inhibition of responses to sad faces were
estimated current density distributions in the cortical gray matter and impaired as indicated by d', but decision bias (as measured by β) was
hippocampus in the digitized Montreal Neurological Institute atlas with not significant (F1,39=0.037, P=0.85). The mixed ANOVA of response
6, 239 voxels at a spatial resolution of 5 mm. sLORETA assumes that time showed a significant main effect for group (F1, 39=6.01, P < 0.05)
neighboring neuronal sources active similarity. The assumption was (see Fig. 2), with a shorter response time observed for the depression
implemented by computing the ‘smoothest’ of all possible activity than for the control group.

Table 2
Averaged reaction times for Go stimuli and the response accuracy rates for Go and Nogo trials in explicit and implicit tasks in each group (M ± SD).

Explicit task Implicit task

Negative Neutral Negative Neutral

DEP CON DEP CON DEP CON DEP CON

go ACC (%) 76.6 (23.9) 92.8 (6.8) 86.6 (14.7) 89.1 (9.4) 92.5 (8.1) 96.2 (4.9) 91.8 (6.3) 97.5 (2.9)
nogo ACC (%) 89.3 (17.8) 95.8 (3.1) 93.6 (4.8) 95.5 (3.0) 93.1 (4.9) 92.6 (6.9) 94.6 (2.3) 94.3 (3.2)
go RT (ms) 470.3 (154.9) 522.6 (91.6) 560.1 (99.9) 526.1 (44.9) 608.1 (115.9) 515.9 (40.7) 589.5 (80.1) 519.3 (38.5)

Abbreviations: DEP, depression; CON, control; ACC, accuracy rate; RT, response time.

45
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Fig. 2. Discrimination accuracy (left panel) compared between depression (gray bar) and control (black bar) groups during sad nogo/neutral go (left group) and neutral nogo/sad go
(right group) tasks and response time (right panel) compared between depression (left group) and control (right group) groups in sad (black bar) and neutral (gray bar) conditions. *, P <
0.05.

2.2. Raw ERP waveforms (F1,39=0.02, P=0.89). In addition, the interaction effect between
valence, trial type and group effect was significant (F=4.46, P < 0.05)
ERP waveforms time-locked to go and no-go stimuli were averaged (Fig. 4). Further simple analysis showed that valence and trial type
(Fig. 3). As expected, no-go stimuli elicited larger N2 and P3 interaction effect was significant in control group (F=8.16, P < 0.05).
components than go stimuli. Task, valence, trial type, and group effects The emotion main effect was significant in nogo condition (F=13.63,
are described below. The average N2 and P3 scores for the four factors P=0.001) but not in go condition (F=0.02, P=0.89). P3 amplitudes
are shown in Table 3. induced by sad stimuli were significantly larger than that induced by
neutral stimuli in nogo condition in control group. However, the
2.2.1. . N1 interaction effect between valence and trial type was not significant
The five-way mixed ANOVA showed that valence, group and in depression group (F=1.06, P=0.32). Notably, the interaction effect
electrodes main effects were significant on N1 amplitudes between task, valence, group, and trial type was also marginally
(F1,39=5.49, P < 0.05; F1,39=8.96, P=0.005; F8,312=150.36, P < significant (F1,39=3.92, P=0.055). To clearly illustrate this effect, we
0.0001). Neutral stimuli elicited larger N1 amplitudes than sad stimuli. calculated go/no-go difference waves to determine the dissimilarity
Depression group showed smaller amplitudes than control group. N1 between depression and control groups. No any other main effect or
amplitudes, which were largest at FCz electrode sites, were mainly interaction effect that was detected between this time windows.
distributed in central prefrontal areas. No any other main effect or
interaction effect was detected during this stage. 2.3. Go/no-go differences ERP waveforms

2.2.2. . N2 As described above, to clearly illustrate the interaction effect


The five-way mixed ANOVA showed that N2 amplitudes were between task, valence, group, and trial type during P3 interval and
modulated by trial type (F1,39=20.88, P < 0.001), valence interaction effect between group and trial type during N2 interval,
(F1,39=24.36, P < 0.001), and electrode (F8,312=15.20, P < 0.001). No- amplitudes induced by go trials were directly subtracted from ampli-
go stimuli elicited larger N2 amplitudes than go stimuli, which were tudes induced by nogo trials for each valence condition to more
smaller in the sad than in the neutral condition. N2 amplitudes, which precisely determine go/nogo difference waves.
were largest at FCz electrode sites, were mainly distributed in central
prefrontal areas. In addition, trial type and group interaction effects 2.3.1. N2 difference wave
were significant (F1,39=8.76, P=0.005) (Fig. 4); the former was The main effects of group (F1,39=6.64, P < 0.05) and electrode
significant in the control (F1,20=18.96, P < 0.001) but not in the (F8,312=5.56, P=0.001) were significant. N2 amplitudes were smaller
depression group (F1,19=2.88, P=0.11). There was no any other main in the depression than in the control group and were larger at FC4 and
effect or interaction effect that was detected between this time C4 than at other electrode sites.
windows.
2.3.2. P3 difference wave
2.2.3. . P3 The analysis of P3 difference waves showed significant main effects
The five-way mixed ANOVA of P3 amplitudes showed significant of valence (F1,39=13.55, P=0.001), group (F1,39=7.69, P < 0.01), and
main effects of trial type, valence, group, and electrode (F1,39=9.29, P < electrode (F8,312=13.81, P < 0.001). Sad faces elicited larger P3 ampli-
0.01; F1,39=7.56, P < 0.01; F8,312=46.46, P < 0.001; F1,39=14.59, P < tudes than neutral faces, and the depression group showed smaller P3
0.001). P3 amplitudes were larger in the sad than in the neutral amplitudes than the control group. These were larger at FCz and Cz
condition. The depression group showed smaller P3 amplitudes than than at other electrode sites. We also detected an valence and group
controls. P3 amplitudes induced by no-go stimuli were larger than interaction effect (F1,39=6.72, P < 0.05). The group effect was signifi-
those induced by go stimuli, with Pz eliciting the largest amplitudes. Cz, cant in the sad condition (F1,39=5.27, P < 0.05). The depression group
C4, P3, and P4 elicited larger amplitudes than the other electrode sites. showed lower P3 amplitudes than the control group, but the inter-
The interaction effect between valence and trial type was significant group difference was not significant in the neutral condition
(F1,39=8.84, P=0.005). The valence effect was significant in nogo (F1,39=0.007, P=0.935). Importantly, we found a significant task,
condition (F1,39=15.12, P < 0.001). P3 amplitudes induced by sad valence, and group interaction effect (F1,39=4.27, P < 0.05) (Fig. 5),
stimuli were significantly larger than that induced by neutral stimuli. and the valence and group interaction effect was significant in the
However, the valence effect was not significant in go condition explicit task (F1,39=8.10, P < 0.01). A group effect was also detected in

46
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Fig. 3. Grand averages evoked by go (dashed line) and nogo (solid line) stimuli in sad and neutral conditions under implicit and explicit tasks in the control (red line) and depression
(blue line) groups at FCz and Cz sites. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

the sad condition during the explicit task (F1,39=5.68, P < 0.05). P3 2.3.3. Relationship between clinical characteristics and task-related
amplitudes in the control group were larger than in the depression measures
group in the sad condition during the explicit task, while the group Discrimination accuracy was positively correlated with amplitudes
effect was not significant in the neutral condition during this task. of go/no-go difference waves in the P3 time window at FCz and Cz
However, in the implicit task, the valence and group interaction effect electrode sites during the sad condition in the depression group
was not significant (F1,39=0. 61, P=0.44). (r=0.45, P < 0.05). Discrimination accuracy and P3 amplitudes were

47
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Table 3
Amplitudes in original ERP components of the study participants (M ± SD).

Explicit task Implicit task

Sad Neutral Sad Neutral

Go Nogo Go Nogo Go Nogo Go Nogo

N2(DEP) 3.7(2.6) 3.4(2.3) 2.9(1.9) 2.7(2.5) 2.7(2.1) 2.9(2.1) 2.9(2.5) 2.4(2.5)


N2(CON) 3.3(3.3) 3.5(2.9) 3.2(3.1) 1.7(3.1) 2.9(2.0) 2.8(2.5) 3.8(3.4) 2.2(2.3)
P3(DEP) 5.4(2.8) 7.0(3.4) 5.1(2.4) 6.2(3.1) 4.3(2.3) 6.4(3.5) 4.4(2.5) 6.3(3.5)
P3(CON) 7.0(5.2) 10.5(4.0) 7.3(4.0) 7.8(3.4) 6.9(3.7) 10.4(4.9) 6.7(3.8) 9.7(4.0)

Amplitudes in microvolt. All values expressed in mean (SD). Abbreviations: DEP, depression; CON, control.

not correlated in the other conditions. There were no correlations than in explicit task. The task main effect was not significant in neutral
between clinical measures (HAMD) and discrimination accuracy or condition in control group (F1,39=0.11, P=0.92).
between clinical measures and P3 components in the sad condition
during the explicit task.
3. Discussion

2.3.4. Source localization results This study investigated the neural substrates of response inhibition
Based on ERP results that showed significant task, valence, group, deficits to emotionally sad information in female depressive individuals
and trial type interaction effects during the P3 stage, voxel-based whole across explicit and implicit tasks using high-resolution ERP measure-
brain sLORETA images were compared between go and no-go condi- ment. The findings suggest that depression patients showed decreased
tions at this stage with non-parametric randomization tests. As go/no-go difference waves during the P3 stage when inhibiting
predicted, a cluster in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex responses to sad faces across implicit and explicit tasks. In addition,
(rDLPFC; BA 8/9/10: maximum values obtained at MNI coordinates: the P3 difference wave was positively correlated with discrimination
x=25, y=40, z=35) showed significantly enhanced activity (t39=0.87, P accuracy in the depression group. When inhibiting neutral faces, these
< 0.005; k=54 voxels, volume: 6.75 cm3) in nogo trials compare to go individuals showed decreased P3 difference waves in implicit tasks in
trials during this stage (Fig. 6). To evaluate experimental effects, the which neutral faces were ambiguous and were likely to be recognized as
CSD of the cluster was extracted using coordinates from voxels showing negative information. However, in explicit tasks, we did not detect this
the largest t value in the previous sLORETA comparison as centroid difference between the two groups. The source localization analysis on
point and 22.5 mm as radius which was computed from the volume of the P3 component showed that no-go trials activated the rDLPFC to a
the significant cluster (6.75 cm3). A significant main effect of trial type greater degree than go trials. In addition, the CSD of the rDLPFC was
was observed (F1,39=36.69, P < 0.001). The CSD was higher in the no- higher in the implicit than in the explicit task in the control as
go condition than in the go condition. We detected significant interac- compared to the depression group, confirming that this is a key area
tion effect between task and group (F1,39=6.23, P < 0.05), and the task for top-down cognitive control that plays a critical role in the
effect was significant in the control (F1,20=4.76, P=0.041) but not in the pathophysiology of depression.
depression (F1,19=2.15, P=0.16) group. The CSD of rDLPFC was larger As shown in Fig. 3, at the 40–140 ms interval, the early N1
in the implicit than in the explicit task in the control group. In addition, components were induced by the facial stimuli. Numerous ERP studies
the interaction effect between task, valence, and group was significant confirmed that N1 component over frontal areas were related to facial
(F1,39=8.85, P < 0.05) (Fig. 6). Further analysis showed that the task expressions (Eimer and Holmes, 2002; Holmes et al., 2003). The
and valence interaction effect was significant in control group frontal N1 is related to rapid attentional alerting to facial expressions
(F1,39=5.04, P < 0.05), but not in depression group (F1,39=3.812, (Eimer et al., 2008). Negative faces elicited larger N1 amplitudes
P=0.083). Task main effect was significant in sad condition in control compared with neutral faces at frontal areas (Association, 1994; Luo
group (F1,39=13.59, P=0.001). The CSD was higher in implicit task et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2015). However, in the present study, we

Fig. 4. Histograms described the original N2 (A) and P3 amplitudes (B). (A) The original N2 amplitudes compared between nogo (black bar) and go (gray bar) conditions in depression
(left group) and control (right group) groups. (B) The original P3 amplitudes compared between sad (black bar) and neutral (gray bar) conditions in depression and control groups
during nogo (left panel) and go (right panel) tasks. CON, control; DEP, depression; *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.001.

48
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Fig. 5. P3 difference waves of no-go subtracted from go trials at the FCz site (upper panel) and corresponding scalp topography (middle panel) and amplitude histograms (bottom panel)
of the two groups in sad and neutral condition during implicit and explicit tasks. dep, depression; con, control; im, implicit; ex, explicit.

detected that N1 amplitudes were larger in neutral condition than in Kropotov et al., 2011). During the N2 stage, we detected a significant
sad condition. The inconsistent results may be due to the different task interaction effect of trial type and group (Fig. 4). N2 amplitudes elicited
sets in present study. In previous facial expression studies, participants by no-go trials were decreased in the depression as compared to the
were asked to discern facial expressions or passive viewing (Eimer and control group, indicating that the processing of conflict monitoring was
Holmes, 2002; Luo et al., 2010), while in the present study, we disrupted in depression irrespective of valence and task. These results
required participants to response or inhibit their response according were consistent with previously examined non-emotional tasks such as
to facial expression or facial gender. In the case, emotion information flank (Alderman et al., 2015) and go/no-go (Bailey et al., 2014) tasks,
was distracted stimuli. Participants pay more attention to sad emotion which also showed decreased N2 amplitudes. It is worth noting that
processing, resulting in decreased frontal N1 amplitudes elicited by there was no interaction effect of task, valence, and group within no-go-
response inhibition task. In addition, depression group showed smaller N2. N2 sub-components are sensitive to involuntary cognitive proces-
N1 amplitudes than control group irrespective of task type and valence. sing but not to voluntary top-down cognitive regulation (Kahkonen
Studies reported that depression patients showed obvious attention et al., 2002). In this study, we used a block design and participants
bias to negative expression and also exhibited difficulty in recognizing were required to respond to or inhibit their response according to the
six basic facial expressions, including neutral facial expression same instructions presented at the beginning of each task. As such,
(Feinberg et al., 1986; Leppanen et al., 2004). Therefore, more their attention was focused on facial expression or gender in order to
attention resources were devoted to facial expression processing in perform the task, and was not affected by task or valence.
depression group, resulting in decreased frontal N1amplitudes in It was worth noting that we observed a significant interaction effect
response inhibition task irrespective of task and valence. of group, task, valence, and trial type on the P3 component at 500–
More important, as expected, larger N2 and P3 components were 600 ms, as demonstrated by difference waves. The subtraction of ERPs
elicited by no-go stimuli as compared to go stimuli. Previous electro- elicited by go trials from those elicited by no-go trials in each valence
physiological findings indicated that N2 and P3 represent distinct condition revealed a clear P3 difference wave during this stage. No-go-
functions in response inhibition: the former is related to conflict related P3 represents behavioral inhibition to inappropriate stimuli.
monitoring, while the latter is an indicator of behavioral inhibition We observed a significant interaction effect of valence and group in the
(Albert et al., 2010; Bokura et al., 2001; Donkers and van Boxtel, 2004; difference wave. The depression group showed decreased no-go-P3

49
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Fig. 6. Source localization results on P3 components. (A) sLORETA solutions to non-parametric randomization tests on P3 components showing voxels in which the go/no-go condition
contrast was significant (P < 0.05); (B) Histograms of the CSD of rDLPFC extracted from implicit (black bar) and explicit tasks (gray bar) under sad and neutral conditions in control (left
panel) and depression (right panel) groups. *, P < 0.05.

amplitudes relative to controls when inhibiting sad but not neutral while negative and positive stimuli elicited similar P3 in controls
stimuli as well as lower discrimination accuracy during this task. The (Krompinger and Simons, 2009). This may be explained by the
original P3 amplitudes also showed interaction effect between valence, different P3 sub-components in the two studies. In the present study,
trial type and group. When inhibiting sad stimuli, control group the P3 component was mainly distributed in the midline prefrontal
showed increased P3 amplitudes compared to neutral stimuli, but area (i.e., maximal at FCz, followed by Cz and Pz), which is typically
depression group showed no difference between the two valence stimuli inhibited in association with the no-go P3 component; in the previous
(Fig. 4). Both electrophysiological and behavior data indicated that work, P3 was not distributed at the midline and was actually a P3b
depression patients had impaired response inhibition to sad faces. component related to attention/working memory processing. Another
Behavioral studies that examined interference and inhibition of emo- possible reason is that research subjects in Krompinger and Simons's
tional stimuli in depression subjects based on adapted paradigms have work is sub-clinically depressed individuals which is different from
shown that these subjects fail to inhibit negative information in a clinically depressed patients in current study. The different results
behavioral context (Joormann and Gotlib, 2006, 2007, 2008; Koster between the two studies demonstrate that neural mechanisms of
et al., 2005). Devoting more cognitive resources to the inhibition of response inhibition differ between clinically and sub-clinically de-
avoidance-based motivationally relevant stimuli is an evolutionary pressed populations. A direct comparison between the two groups
adaptation in normal individuals (Cuthbert et al., 2000; Schupp using the same simple task paradigm can confirm or refute this
et al., 2000). Clinically depressed patients had decreased no-go P3 to possibility.
a sad expression, indicating maladapted cognition in this group The interaction effect between task, valence, and group was
resulting in a diminished ability to inhibit response to sad faces. This significant during the P3 difference wave. In the explicit task, ampli-
was underscored by the positive correlation between no-go P3 elicited tudes of P3 difference waves were reduced in depression patients
by sad faces and discrimination accuracy in the sad no-go/neutral go relative to controls in the sad condition, but in the neutral condition,
condition. Depression patients display attention bias to mood-congru- P3 difference waves were similar between the two groups. In the
ent stimuli, which makes inhibition of negative stimuli difficult implicit task, these waves decreased in patients relative to controls in
(Krompinger and Simons, 2009). Negative attention bias and the both the sad and neutral conditions. It is worth noting that depression
inability to inhibit negative information are closely related phenomena patients showed abnormal processing when inhibiting the response to
that play key roles in depression pathogenesis, maintenance, and neutral faces in the implicit but not in the explicit task. In the former,
relapse (Goeleven et al., 2006; Katz et al., 2010; Vanderhasselt et al., participants were asked to inhibit or execute behavior according to face
2012). The electrophysiological evidence showed that selective impair- gender. Under this condition, neutral faces were not the focus of
ment of response inhibition to sad faces in depression patients attention and were therefore ambiguous stimuli. It has been reported
occurred during the late inhibition no-go P3 stage. that depressed individuals are more inclined to interpret ambiguous
Inconsistent with the present study, using a go/no-go task with information in a negative manner (Mogg et al., 2006; Moser et al.,
emotional images as stimuli, Krompinger and colleagues found that 2012). Indeed, in the implicit task, depression patients also showed
individuals with high depression scores showed larger P3 amplitudes to impaired response inhibition to neutral faces, which were recognized as
negative as compared to positive stimuli in both go and no-go trials, negative information. However, in the explicit task in which patients

50
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

were asked to respond to facial expression, patients inhibited neutral imbalance relationship with depression symptoms (Colangelo et al.,
faces normally. These results suggest that interpretation bias in 2012), it remains unclear whether the results are generalizable to
depression can be modified by training individuals to interpret depressed men. Moreover, Yuan and colleagues have reported that
ambiguous information as benign or positive (Mathews and women and men had different behavioral inhibition control ability in
Mackintosh, 2000). human adults (Yuan et al., 2008). Future studies focus on response
No-go N2 amplitudes were smaller in depression patients than in inhibition ability of depression men to negative information are
controls irrespective of valence, while the amplitudes of no-go P3 were required to clarify this question.
decreased only in the sad condition. N2 sub-components are sensitive
to involuntary cognitive processing but not to voluntary top-down Funding
cognitive regulation (Kahkonen et al., 2002), while components emer-
ging in late stages of the P3 family—including no-go P3—are sensitive This research was supported by the National Natural Science
to motivationally relevant stimuli and processing stage during con- Foundation of China (31000503, 91232717, 81301176, 81300944,
sciousness (Bradley et al., 2007; Delplanque et al., 2006). In the N2 and 81300944) and the Natural Science Foundation of the Colleges and
stage, depression patients showed decreased amplitudes as compared Universities in Anhui Province (KJ2016A355). The funders had no role
to controls irrespective of task and valence; consequently, in the later in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
P3 stage, inappropriate neutral stimuli may be intentionally inhibited preparation of the manuscript.
by top-down cognitive control.
During the P3 time window, the rDLPFC was activated in the no-go References
relative to the go condition (Fig. 4). Neuroimaging studies in humans
have reported that this brain region, which is part of the rIFC, plays a Aarts, K., Pourtois, G., 2010. Anxiety not only increases, but also alters early error-
critical role in response inhibition, task switching, updating, and other monitoring functions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 479–492.
Albert, J., Lopez-Martin, S., Carretie, L., 2010. Emotional context modulates response
cognitive control functions (Meyer et al., 2011; Verbruggen et al., inhibition: neural and behavioral data. NeuroImage 49, 914–921.
2010). Our results confirm that the rDLPFC is a critical area for Alderman, B.L., Olson, R.L., Bates, M.E., Selby, E.A., Buckman, J.F., Brush, C.J., Panza,
response inhibition. Furthermore, the cluster analysis found that the E.A., Kranzler, A., Eddie, D., Shors, T.J., 2015. Rumination in major depressive
disorder is associated with impaired neural activation during conflict monitoring.
CSD of the rDLPFC was higher in the implicit than in the explicit task Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 269.
in sad condition in the control rather than in depression group. Indeed, Association, A.P., 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4 ed..
in the explicit task, facial expressions were directly processed and were American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC.
Bailey, N.W., Hoy, K.E., Maller, J.J., Segrave, R.A., Thomson, R., Williams, N.,
within the scope of voluntary attention, while in the implicit task, they
Daskalakis, Z.J., Fitzgerald, P.B., 2014. An exploratory analysis of Go/Nogo event-
were incidentally processed and were within the scope of involuntary related potentials in major depression and depression following traumatic brain
attention. Thus, attentional resources dedicated to stimulus processing injury. Psychiatry Res. 224, 324–334.
Berggren, N., Derakshan, N., 2013. Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: why you
differ for the two tasks. Previous studies have reported that the DLPFC
see them and why you don't. Biol. Psychol. 92, 440–446.
plays a critical role in top-down modulation of selective attention, Berkman, E.T., Burklund, L., Lieberman, M.D., 2009. Inhibitory spillover: intentional
working memory, response inhibition, and other cognitive control motor inhibition produces incidental limbic inhibition via right inferior frontal
functions (Harding et al., 2015; Shin and Kim, 2015). The differential cortex. NeuroImage 47, 705–712.
Bokura, H., Yamaguchi, S., Kobayashi, S., 2001. Electrophysiological correlates for
activation of the rDLPFC for the two tasks in the P3 phase that was response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clin. Neurophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin.
observed in normal subjects suggests that it is critical for top-down Neurophysiol. 112, 2224–2232.
cognitive control. However, there was no difference between the two Botvinick, M.M., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S., Cohen, J.D., 2001. Conflict
monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652.
tasks in the depression group, indicating that functional abnormalities Bradley, M.M., Hamby, S., Low, A., Lang, P.J., 2007. Brain potentials in perception:
exist in the rDLPFC of depression patients. Various reports have picture complexity and emotional arousal. Psychophysiology 44, 364–373.
highlighted the key role of the DLPFC in the pathophysiology of Brent, D.A., Birmaher, B., 2006. Treatment-resistant depression in adolescents:
recognition and management. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 15, 1015–1034.
depression (Koenigs and Grafman, 2009); exogenous stimulation of Brunoni, A.R., Boggio, P.S., De Raedt, R., Bensenor, I.M., Lotufo, P.A., Namur, V.,
this brain area has been shown to reduce depression symptoms Valiengo, L.C., Vanderhasselt, M.A., 2014. Cognitive control therapy and
(Brunoni et al., 2014; Loo and Mitchell, 2005; Marangell et al., transcranial direct current stimulation for depression: a randomized, double-
blinded, controlled trial. J. Affect. Disord. 162, 43–49.
2007). The present results indicate that impairment in response Burkhouse, K.L., Siegle, G.J., Woody, M.L., Kudinova, A.Y., Gibb, B.E., 2015. Pupillary
inhibition and deficits in the rDLPFC underlie depression. reactivity to sad stimuli as a biomarker of depression risk: evidence from a
There were several limitations to this study. Firstly, only sad prospective study of children. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 124, 498–506.
Colangelo, L.A., Craft, L.L., Ouyang, P., Liu, K., Schreiner, P.J., Michos, E.D., Gapstur,
expressions were used as experimental stimuli; further study is
S.M., 2012. Association of sex hormones and sex hormone-binding globulin with
required to determine how other basic emotions are related to response depressive symptoms in postmenopausal women: the Multiethnic Study of
inhibition in female depression patients across implicit and explicit Atherosclerosis. Menopause 19, 877–885.
tasks. Secondly, depression is often accompanied by anxiety. Anxiety Cuthbert, B.N., Schupp, H.T., Bradley, M.M., Birbaumer, N., Lang, P.J., 2000. Brain
potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and
scores in our depression group were high, suggesting that differences affective report. Biol. Psychol. 52, 95–111.
between depression patients and controls may be influenced by De Raedt, R., Koster, E.H., 2010. Understanding vulnerability for depression from a
anxiety. Future studies should distinguish between depression patients cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new
conceptual framework. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 50–70.
with and without anxiety. Thirdly, the spatial resolution of the Delplanque, S., Silvert, L., Hot, P., Rigoulot, S., Sequeira, H., 2006. Arousal and valence
electrophysiological method used in this study was low, and source effects on event-related P3a and P3b during emotional categorization. Int. J.
localization analysis was carried out according to a mathematical and Psychophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Organ. Psychophysiol. 60, 315–322.
Donkers, F.C., van Boxtel, G.J., 2004. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict
not a physiological approach. In addition, many sub-cortical areas are monitoring not response inhibition. Brain Cognit. 56, 165–176.
involved in response inhibition to facial stimuli such as the amygdala, Eimer, M., Holmes, A., 2002. An ERP study on the time course of emotional face
ventral striatum, and fusiform gyrus (Kret et al., 2011; Shafritz et al., processing. Neuroreport 13, 427–431.
Eimer, M., Kiss, M., Holmes, A., 2008. Links between rapid ERP responses to fearful
2006). Due to the limitations of electrophysiological source analysis, we faces and conscious awareness. J. Neuropsychol. 2, 165–181.
were unable to record from sub-cortical areas. Additional studies using Fabiani, M., Gratton, G., Goles, M.G., 2000. Hand Book of Psychophysiology. Cambridge
other brain imaging methodologies with high spatial resolution, such as University Press, London.
Fan, C., Chen, S., Zhang, L., Qi, Z., Jin, Y., Wang, Q., Luo, Y., Li, H., Luo, W., 2015. N170
functional magnetic resonance imaging, are needed to substantiate
changes reflect competition between faces and identifiable characters during early
these findings. Last but not least, the participants recruited in the visual processing. NeuroImage 110, 32–38.
present study were just depression and control women. Given the Feinberg, T.E., Rifkin, A., Schaffer, C., Walker, E., 1986. Facial discrimination and
evidence that sex hormones, such as testosterone and ovarian show emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Arch. Gen.

51
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Psychiatry 43, 276–279. 1857–1867.


Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Baert, S., Koster, E.H., 2006. Deficient inhibition of emotional Marangell, L.B., Martinez, M., Jurdi, R.A., Zboyan, H., 2007. Neurostimulation therapies
information in depression. J. Affect. Disord. 93, 149–157. in depression: a review of new modalities. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 116, 174–181.
Goldstein, M., Brendel, G., Tuescher, O., Pan, H., Epstein, J., Beutel, M., Yang, Y., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., 2000. Induced emotional interpretation bias and anxiety. J.
Thomas, K., Levy, K., Silverman, M., Clarkin, J., Posner, M., Kernberg, O., Stern, E., Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 602–615.
Silbersweig, D., 2007. Neural substrates of the interaction of emotional stimulus Meyer, T., Qi, X.L., Stanford, T.R., Constantinidis, C., 2011. Stimulus selectivity in dorsal
processing and motor inhibitory control: an emotional linguistic go/no-go fMRI and ventral prefrontal cortex after training in working memory tasks. J. Neurosci.:
study. NeuroImage 36, 1026–1040. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 31, 6266–6276.
Gollan, J.K., Pane, H.T., McCloskey, M.S., Coccaro, E.F., 2008. Identifying differences in Mogg, K., Bradbury, K.E., Bradley, B.P., 2006. Interpretation of ambiguous information
biased affective information processing in major depression. Psychiatry Res. 159, in clinical depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1411–1419.
18–24. Moser, J.S., Huppert, J.D., Foa, E.B., Simons, R.F., 2012. Interpretation of ambiguous
Goodwin, R.D., Jacobi, F., Bittner, A., 2006. Text Book of Mood Disorders 2006. social scenarios in social phobia and depression: evidence from event-related brain
American Psychiatric Publishing Washington, Washington, 33–54. potentials. Biol. Psychol. 89, 387–397.
Gotlib, I.H., Joormann, J., 2010. Cognition and depression: current status and future Mueller, E.M., Pizzagalli, D.A., 2016. One-year-old fear memories rapidly activate human
directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285–312. fusiform gyrus. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 308–316.
Gur, R.C., Erwin, R.J., Gur, R.E., Zwil, A.S., Heimberg, C., Kraemer, H.C., 1992. Facial Nichols, T.E., Holmes, A.P., 2002. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional
emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings in depression. Psychiatry Res. 42, neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25.
241–251. Ochsner, K.N., Gross, J.J., 2008. cognitive emotion regulation: insights from social
Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, cognitive and affective neuroscience. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 153–158.
56–62. Ochsner, K.N., Ray, R.D., Cooper, J.C., Robertson, E.R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J.D., Gross,
Harding, I.H., Yucel, M., Harrison, B.J., Pantelis, C., Breakspear, M., 2015. Effective J.J., 2004. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down-
connectivity within the frontoparietal control network differentiates cognitive control and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage 23, 483–499.
and working memory. NeuroImage 106, 144–153. Otten, L.J., Rugg, M.D., 2004. Event-Related Potentials: A Methods Handbook. The MIT
Henje Blom, E., Connolly, C.G., Ho, T.C., LeWinn, K.Z., Mobayed, N., Han, L., Paulus, Press, United States of America.
M.P., Wu, J., Simmons, A.N., Yang, T.T., 2015. Altered insular activation and Padmala, S., Pessoa, L., 2010. Interactions between cognition and motivation during
increased insular functional connectivity during sad and happy face processing in response inhibition. Neuropsychologia 48, 558–565.
adolescent major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 178, 215–223. Pascual-Marqui, R.D., 2002. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic
Holmes, A., Vuilleumier, P., Eimer, M., 2003. The processing of emotional facial tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 24
expression is gated by spatial attention: evidence from event-related brain potentials. (Suppl. D), S5–S12.
brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 16, 174–184. Pizzagalli, D.A., Nitschke, J.B., Oakes, T.R., Hendrick, A.M., Horras, K.A., Larson, C.L.,
Hum, K.M., Manassis, K., Lewis, M.D., 2013. Neural mechanisms of emotion regulation Abercrombie, H.C., Schaefer, S.M., Koger, J.V., Benca, R.M., Pascual-Marqui, R.D.,
in childhood anxiety. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 54, 552–564. Davidson, R.J., 2002. Brain electrical tomography in depression: the importance of
Joormann, J., Gotlib, I.H., 2006. Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of symptom severity, anxiety, and melancholic features. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 73–85.
emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. Rauch, A.V., Reker, M., Ohrmann, P., Pedersen, A., Bauer, J., Dannlowski, U., Harding,
115, 705–714. L., Koelkebeck, K., Konrad, C., Kugel, H., Arolt, V., Heindel, W., Suslow, T., 2010.
Joormann, J., Gotlib, I.H., 2007. Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery Increased amygdala activation during automatic processing of facial emotion in
from depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 80–85. schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 182, 200–206.
Joormann, J., Gotlib, I.H., 2008. Updating the contents of working memory in Righart, R., de Gelder, B., 2007. Impaired face and body perception in developmental
depression: interference from irrelevant negative material. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, prosopagnosia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104, 17234–17238.
182–192. Schuch, J.J., Roest, A.M., Nolen, W.A., Penninx, B.W., de Jonge, P., 2014. Gender
Joormann, J., Yoon, K.L., Zetsche, U., 2007. Cognitive inhibition in depression. Appl. differences in major depressive disorder: results from the Netherlands study of
Prev. Psychol. 12, 128–139. depression and anxiety. J. Affect. Disord. 156, 156–163.
Kahkonen, S., Ahveninen, J., Pekkonen, E., Kaakkola, S., Huttunen, J., Ilmoniemi, R.J., Schupp, H.T., Cuthbert, B.N., Bradley, M.M., Cacioppo, J.T., Ito, T., Lang, P.J., 2000.
Jaaskelainen, I.P., 2002. Dopamine modulates involuntary attention shifting and Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational
reorienting: an electromagnetic study. Clin. Neurophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Fed. Clin. relevance. Psychophysiology 37, 257–261.
Neurophysiol. 113, 1894–1902. Seeck, M., Lazeyras, F., Michel, C.M., Blanke, O., Gericke, C.A., Ives, J., 1998. Non-
Kaiser, S., Unger, J., Kiefer, M., Markela, J., Mundt, C., Weisbrod, M., 2003. Executive invasive epileptic focus localization using EEG-triggered functional MRI and
control deficit in depression: event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task. Psychiatry electromagnetic tomography. Clin. Phys. Physiol. Meas.: Off. J. Hosp. Phys. Assoc.
Res. 122, 169–184. Dtsch. Ges. Med. Phys. Eur. Fed. Organ. Med. Phys. 106, 508–512.
Katz, R., De Sanctis, P., Mahoney, J.R., Sehatpour, P., Murphy, C.F., Gomez-Ramirez, M., Sehlmeyer, C., Konrad, C., Zwitserlood, P., Arolt, V., Falkenstein, M., Beste, C., 2010.
Alexopoulos, G.S., Foxe, J.J., 2010. Cognitive control in late-life depression: ERP indices for response inhibition are related to anxiety-related personality traits.
response inhibition deficits and dysfunction of the anterior cingulate cortex. Am. J. Neuropsychologia 48, 2488–2495.
Geriatr. Psychiatry: Off. J. Am. Assoc. Geriatr. Psychiatry 18, 1017–1025. Shafritz, K.M., Collins, S.H., Blumberg, H.P., 2006. The interaction of emotional and
Kessler, R.C., Chiu, W.T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K.R., Walters, E.E., 2005. Prevalence, cognitive neural systems in emotionally guided response inhibition. NeuroImage 31,
severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National 468–475.
Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 617–627. Shin, G., Kim, C., 2015. Neural correlates of cognitive style and flexible cognitive control.
Kilford, E.J., Foulkes, L., Potter, R., Collishaw, S., Thapar, A., Rice, F., 2015. Affective NeuroImage 113, 78–85.
bias and current, past and future adolescent depression: a familial high risk study. J. Snodgrass, J.G., Corwin, J., 1988. Perceptual identification thresholds for 150
Affect. Disord. 174, 265–271. fragmented pictures from the Snodgrass and Vanderwart picture set. Percept. Mot.
Koenigs, M., Grafman, J., 2009. The functional neuroanatomy of depression: distinct Skills 67, 3–36.
roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 201, Suslow, T., Konrad, C., Kugel, H., Rumstadt, D., Zwitserlood, P., Schoning, S., Ohrmann,
239–243. P., Bauer, J., Pyka, M., Kersting, A., Arolt, V., Heindel, W., Dannlowski, U., 2010.
Koster, E.H., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., De Raedt, R., 2011. Understanding Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in
depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 155–160.
disengagement hypothesis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 138–145. Sylvester, C.M., Hudziak, J.J., Gaffrey, M.S., Barch, D.M., Luby, J.L., 2015. Stimulus-
Koster, E.H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., Crombez, G., 2005. Mood-congruent driven attention, threat bias, and sad bias in youth with a history of an anxiety
attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement disorder or depression. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.
from negative information. Emotion 5, 446–455. Taylor, S.F., Phan, K.L., Decker, L.R., Liberzon, I., 2003. Subjective rating of emotionally
Kret, M.E., Denollet, J., Grezes, J., de Gelder, B., 2011. The role of negative affectivity salient stimuli modulates neural activity. NeuroImage 18, 650–659.
and social inhibition in perceiving social threat: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia Valdes-Conroy, B., Aguado, L., Fernandez-Cahill, M., Romero-Ferreiro, V., Dieguez-
49, 1187–1193. Risco, T., 2014. Following the time course of face gender and expression processing:
Krompinger, J.W., Simons, R.F., 2009. Electrophysiological indicators of emotion a task-dependent ERP study. Int. J. Psychophysiol.: Off. J. Int. Organ. Psychophysiol.
processing biases in depressed undergraduates. Biol. Psychol. 81, 153–163. 92, 59–66.
Kropotov, J.D., Ponomarev, V.A., Hollup, S., Mueller, A., 2011. Dissociating action Vanderhasselt, M.A., De Raedt, R., Dillon, D.G., Dutra, S.J., Brooks, N., Pizzagalli, D.A.,
inhibition, conflict monitoring and sensory mismatch into independent components 2012. Decreased cognitive control in response to negative information in patients
of event related potentials in GO/NOGO task. NeuroImage 57, 565–575. with remitted depression: an event-related potential study. J. Psychiatry Neurosci.:
Leppanen, J.M., Milders, M., Bell, J.S., Terriere, E., Hietanen, J.K., 2004. Depression JPN 37, 250–258.
biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Res. 128, 123–133. Verbruggen, F., Aron, A.R., Stevens, M.A., Chambers, C.D., 2010. Theta burst stimulation
Linden, S.C., Jackson, M.C., Subramanian, L., Wolf, C., Green, P., Healy, D., Linden, dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex. Proc.
D.E., 2010. Emotion-cognition interactions in schizophrenia: implicit and explicit Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 13966–13971.
effects of facial expression. Neuropsychologia 48, 997–1002. Wager, T.D., Davidson, M.L., Hughes, B.L., Lindquist, M.A., Ochsner, K.N., 2008.
Loo, C.K., Mitchell, P.B., 2005. A review of the efficacy of transcranial magnetic Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron
stimulation (TMS) treatment for depression, and current and future strategies to 59, 1037–1050.
optimize efficacy. J. Affect. Disord. 88, 255–267. Winston, J.S., O'Doherty, J., Dolan, R.J., 2003. Common and distinct neural responses
Luo, W., Feng, W., He, W., Wang, N.Y., Luo, Y.J., 2010. Three stages of facial expression during direct and incidental processing of multiple facial emotions. NeuroImage 20,
processing: ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. NeuroImage 49, 84–97.

52
F. Yu et al. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 259 (2017) 42–53

Worrell, G.A., Lagerlund, T.D., Sharbrough, F.W., Brinkmann, B.H., Busacker, N.E., response inhibition: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport 20, 25–30.
Cicora, K.M., O'Brien, T.J., 2000. Localization of the epileptic focus by low-resolution Yu, F., Zhu, C., Zhang, L., Chen, X., Li, D., Zhang, L., Ye, R., Dong, Y., Luo, Y., Hu, X.,
electromagnetic tomography in patients with a lesion demonstrated by MRI. Brain Wang, K., 2015. The neural substrates of response inhibition to negative information
Topogr. 12, 273–282. across explicit and implicit tasks in GAD patients: electrophysiological evidence from
Yang, P., Wang, M., Jin, Z., Li, L., 2015. Visual short-term memory load modulates the an ERP study. Front. Psychol. 6, 275.
early attention and perception of task-irrelevant emotional faces. Front. Hum. Yuan, J., He, Y., Qinglin, Z., Chen, A., Li, H., 2008. Gender differences in behavioral
Neurosci. 9, 490. inhibitory control: ERP evidence from a two-choice oddball task. Psychophysiology
Yu, F., Ye, R., Sun, S., Carretie, L., Zhang, L., Dong, Y., Zhu, C., Luo, Y., Wang, K., 2014. 45, 986–993.
Dissociation of neural substrates of response inhibition to negative information Yuan, J., Luo, Y., Yan, J.H., Meng, X., Yu, F., Li, H., 2009. Neural correlates of the
between implicit and explicit facial Go/Nogo tasks: evidence from an females' susceptibility to negative emotions: an insight into gender-related
electrophysiological study. PloS One 9, e109839. prevalence of affective disturbances. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3676–3686.
Yu, F., Yuan, J., Luo, Y.J., 2009. Auditory-induced emotion modulates processes of

53

You might also like