You are on page 1of 16

ARTICLE

Why dizziness is likely to


increase the risk of cognitive
dysfunction and dementia in
elderly adults
Paul F Smith

ABSTRACT
Dementia is recognised to be one of the most challenging diseases facing society, both now and in the
future, with its prevalence estimated to increase substantially by 2050. The potential contributions of
age-related sensory deficits have attracted little attention until recently, when a landmark study
suggested that hearing loss could be a greater risk factor for dementia than hypertension, obesity,
smoking, depression, physical inactivity or social isolation. Over the last decade, evidence has been
gradually accumulating to suggest that the other part of the inner ear, the balance organs or ‘vestibular
system’, might also be important in the development of cognitive dysfunction and dementia. Increasing
evidence suggests that dizziness associated with vestibular dysfunction, a common reason for patients
consulting their GPs, increases the risk of cognitive dysfunction, including dementia, and our understanding
of the basic neurobiology of this sensory system supports this view. This paper aims to review and critically
evaluate the relevant evidence.

D
izziness is reported to be one of the In 2016, the mortality rate associated with
most common reasons for patients falls in the US was estimated to be 122.2 per
consulting a general practitioner.1 100,000 persons.4,5
Although not all consultations regarding diz- The impact of vestibular dysfunction
ziness are related to the vestibular system, on loss of balance and falls is due both to
for example, they can be due to cardiovas- its effects on brainstem vestibular reflex
cular dysfunction, approximately 20–50% pathways as well as higher cognitive
are believed to be due to balance disorders processing of self-motion signals from the
related to the peripheral vestibular system vestibular system. The vestibular system
(see Table 1).1 Recent studies have estimat- (Figure 1) senses head movement (strictly
ed that 35% of US adults over the age of speaking ‘head acceleration’ or change in
35 years suffer from vestibular disorders, head velocity) in different planes, as well
increasing to 85% aged 80 and over.2 Unfor- as linear acceleration by gravity.6 The three
tunately, no reliable statistics are available semi-circular canals in each inner ear sense
on the prevalence of vestibular disorders for angular rotation of the head, and the two
New Zealand. However, the Health Quality otoliths, the utricle and the saccule, sense
and Safety Commission reported that even linear movement.7,8 This linear movement
between January and March, 2020, there not only includes movement of the head,
were 65,893 fall injuries reported to the ACC, forward and backward, and left and right,
of which 8,544 were classified as serious.3 but linear acceleration of the head by
Overseas, impairment of the vestibular sys- gravity; the saccule, in particular, senses
tem has been estimated to increase the odds linear acceleration by gravity (Figure 2
of falling by over 12-fold, and nearly 30% for explanation).7 The utricle and saccule
of adults aged 65 and over fall each year.2,4,5 detect linear acceleration as a result of

112
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

‘otoconia’ (calcium carbonate crystals) that the environment.17 In 2014, the Nobel Prize
generate an inertial force on the hair cells in Medicine or Physiology was awarded to
during head movement, causing a change John O’Keefe, Edvard Moser and Britt-Mayer
in electrical potential (see Figure 2).7 In this Moser for these discoveries, which have
respect, it is important to note that the most become known as the brain’s ‘global posi-
primitive form of the otoliths (‘statoliths’) tioning system’.18 Since then, both place cells
are estimated to have evolved approxi- and grid cells have been demonstrated to rely
mately 670 million years ago, and they exist on vestibular information from the inner
in invertebrates such as jellyfish. This is ear.11–13 Therefore, one reason why vestibu-
their only means of detecting upright, which lar-related dizziness contributes to falls, is
is necessary for survival. Therefore, given that not only does it impair fast vestibular
their evolutionary age, the otoliths might be reflexes such as the VORs and VSRs, but it
expected to have developed major contribu- impairs the ability of the brain to integrate
tions to balance in humans.9,10 The vestibular self-motion information and to navigate
system, through short-latency brainstem through the spatial environment and form
pathways, generates rapid eye movements spatial memories. Information from the
that compensate for the unintentional vestibular system is distributed widely
movement of the head, eg, movement of throughout the central nervous system and
the head due to the pulse beat (the vestibu- is involved in higher cognitive function.11–15,18
lo-ocular reflexes or VORs) and maintains There is increasing evidence that the otoliths
the stability of the visual image of the world may be important for cognitive processing
on the retina.6 The vestibular system also independently of the semi-circular canals;19
generates rapid vestibulo-spinal reflexes this is one reason why the evolutionary age
(VSRs) which adjust posture for uninten- of the otoliths is of interest.
tional movement, enabling us to keep our In recent years, a substantial amount of
balance.6 Without a normal vestibular epidemiological evidence has been published
system, vision would become blurred to support the idea that age-related hearing
(a condition known as ‘oscillopsia’) and loss is a risk factor for dementia.20 For
balance and locomotion become disrupted.6 example, in a seminal study published in
Information about angular and linear the Lancet, it was reported that the contri-
head movement is also transmitted to higher bution of hearing loss to the incidence of
centres of the brain, where it contributes to dementia was greater than hypertension,
the conscious experience of moving through obesity, smoking, depression, physical
the environment and to cognitive processes inactivity and social isolation.20 This result
such as memory.11–15 As we move through seemed surprising, because sensory systems
the environment, the vestibular hair cells in had never been considered particularly
the semi-circular canals and otoliths detect important to dementia, except perhaps for
every head movement, and transmit this olfactory function as a potential biomarker.21
information to areas of the brain such as the It is important to note that the Livingstone
hippocampus, where it is assimilated and et al study20 was based on data from high-
stored to provide a spatial map of our move- income countries and the evidence from
ments.11–15 This information is integrated low-to-middle income countries is less
with other sensory information, such as that convincing in this respect.22 Over the last
from the visual, auditory, tactile, olfactory several years, further evidence in support of
and proprioceptive systems, and formulated the importance of hearing loss for the devel-
into mathematical maps of the spatial world, opment of dementia has been published,
allowing us to navigate through it more although it is not entirely consistent.22–27
effectively.11–15 In the 1970s, specific neurons Less attention has been given to the
were discovered in the hippocampus that other part of the inner ear, the vestibular
selectively discharged in response to specific system (see Figure 1); however, evidence
areas of the environment. These became is mounting that age-related vestibular
known as ‘place cells’.16 In the 1990s, related disorders could also be a significant risk
cells were discovered in the medial ento- factor for the development of cognitive
rhinal cortex, known as ‘grid cells’, which dysfunction and dementia, along with
discharged in response to multiple areas in

113
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

Table 1: Common causes of dizziness in primary care practice. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

Category Percent Examples

Peripheral vestibular 20–50% Benign paroxysmal positional vertigo (BPPV), labyrinthitis,


disease vestibular neuritis

Cardiovascular disease 10–30% Arrhythmia, congestive heart failure, vasovagal conditions


(eg, carotid sinus hypersensitivity)

Systemic infection 10–20% Systemic viral and bacterial infections

Psychiatric conditions 5–15% Depression, anxiety, hyperventilation

Metabolic disturbances 5–10% Hypoglycemia, hyperglycemia, electrolyte disturbances,


thyrotoxicosis, anemia

Medications 5–10% Anti-hypertensives, psychotropic drugs

hearing loss.28 The vestibular system is or both, possibly with a decrease in


known to degenerate with age, as with other vestibular perception; however, it is possible
sensory systems, with decreases in hair cells for age-related vestibular symptoms to be
in the semi-circular canals and otoliths, a sub-clinical and therefore harder to detect .
reduction in the number of neurons in the
Animal studies supporting the
vestibular nerve and brainstem vestibular
nucleus, and a deterioration of vestibular
role of the vestibular system in
reflex responses.1,2 As shown in the Iceberg cognitive function
model in Figure 3, clinical presentation of The literature relating the vestibular
age-related vestibular symptoms usually system to cognitive function, especially
includes ‘presbystasis’ (the imbalance of spatial memory, dates back to the 1960’s.14
disequilibrium) or ‘presbyvertigo’ (vertigo), Numerous studies have reported evidence

Figure 1: Human inner ear anatomy. The cochlea and vestibular system, which encompasses the three
semicircular canals (horizontal, superior (anterior vertical) and posterior vertical) and the two otolith
organs (utricle and saccule), are depicted. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

114
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

Figure 2: (A) Schematic representation of the plates of the otolithic receptors (the utricular and saccular
maculae). The arrows show the preferred polarization of the hair cell receptors across the maculae. The
dashed lines are lines of polarity reversal (lpr). The striola refers to a band of receptors on either side
of the lpr. Schematics of type I (B,D) and type II receptors (C,E) show how linear acceleration acts on the
otoliths and so deflects the hair bundles of individual receptors. From Curthoys et al7 with permission.

115
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

Figure 3: Iceberg model of age-related vestibular loss.

VSR: vestibulo-spinal reflex; VOR: vestibulo-ocular reflex. ‘Presbystasis’: age-related imbalance of disequilibrium.
‘Presbyvertigo’: age-related vertigo. From Agrawal et al1 with permission.

that unilateral or bilateral lesions of the (‘head direction cells’), are also dysfunc-
peripheral vestibular system impair spatial tional following BVL.36 Together, these
memory in various maze and foraging abnormalities in the function of place cells,
tasks.14,29–31 In some cases, these have been grid cells and head direction cells, are likely
conducted even 14 months after bilateral to underlie the spatial memory deficits
vestibular lesions (BVL) in rats, and the observed in animals.14,15 Furthermore, trans-
spatial memory deficits remain.32 A variety genic mice without otoconia and therefore
of potentially confounding factors have without otolith function (‘otolith deficient
been controlled for, including vision, tilted mice’), but with normal semi-cir-
degree of motor activity, anxiety and cular canal function, have been shown to
auditory function, and the results have been have aberrant hippocampal place cell and
consistent.29–32 BVL has been demonstrated head direction cell activity.36,37 In addition,
to impair the function of neurons in the a variety of neurochemical changes have
hippocampus that encode places in the envi- been documented in the hippocampus
ronment (‘hippocampal place cells’),11,12 EEG following BVL, including changes in the
activity in the theta frequency range,33–35 N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) subtype of
which is thought to regulate place cell glutamate receptor and muscarinic acetyl-
function, and theta EEG activity among grid choline (ACh) receptors,38–40 both of which
cells of the entorhinal cortex.13 Neurons in are implicated in hippocampal learning and
the thalamus which encode head direction memory processes.

116
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

Taken together, the data from animal pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus


studies strongly indicate an important (PPTg), then via various nuclei to the medial
role for the vestibular system in spatial septum, which releases ACh into the hippo-
cognition. Although the specific pathways campus; a ‘head direction pathway’, from
through which vestibular information the VNC via head direction cells of the
reaches areas of the brain such as the anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (ADN)
hippocampus, are yet to be fully elucidated, to the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), to the
projections from the vestibular nucleus and hippocampus; a major thalamic pathway
cerebellum are likely to transmit infor- which transmits vestibular information to
mation via multiple routes, particularly the parietal cortex and then on to the MEC
the thalamus (see Figure 4).41 It is specu- and hippocampus; and a transcerebellar
lated that there is a ‘theta pathway’, which pathway.41–43 The detailed pathways are
involves projections from the brainstem depicted in Figure 4.
vestibular nucleus complex (VNC) to the
Figure 4: ADN, anterodorsal nucleus of the thalamus; DTN, dorsal tegmental nucleus; Interpositus N, anterior and posterior interpositus
nuclei; LMN, lateral mammillary nuclei; MEC, medial entorhinal cortex; MG, medial geniculate nucleus; NPH, nucleus prepositus hypo-
glossi; Parietal C, Parietal cortex; PaS, parasubiculum; Perirhinal, Perirhinal cortex; PoS, posterior subiculum (i.e dorsal part of the pre-
subiculum); Post HT, posterior hypothalamus; Postrhinal, postrhinal cortex; PPTg , pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus of Gudden; Pulv,
pulvinar; RPO, reticularis pontis oralis; SUM, supramammillary nucleus; ViM, ventralis intermedius nuclei of the thalamus; VLN, ventral
lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VNC, vestibular nucleus complex; VPi, ventral posterior inferior nucleus of the thalamus; VPL, ventral
posterior lateral nucleus of the thalamus; VPM, ventral posterior medial nuclei of the thalamus. From Hitier et al.41 with permission.

117
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

Vestibular contributions to with behavioural assays, have also provided


compelling evidence that vestibular sensory
cognitive function in humans
inputs are important for human spatial
Consistent with the studies in animals,
cognition. In a seminal study of patients
many studies conducted over the last
with neurofibromatosis type 2 (NF2) who
two decades have demonstrated that
underwent bilateral vestibular nerve
vestibular disorders are associated with
section, it was observed that the NF2 patients
the impairment of cognitive function in
exhibited significantly poorer spatial
otherwise normal healthy adults.44–53 The
navigation skills, measured using a virtual
studies reviewed were identified using a
Morris Water Maze Task, which required
Pubmed search between 1989 and 2020,
no movement other than that of a mouse to
using ‘vestibular’ and ‘cognition’ as key
control a cursor on a computer screen. These
words. All of the studies were included;
spatial cognitive deficits were correlated
they consisted of two main types: survey
with reduced hippocampal volumes
and epidemiological studies; and clinical
(approximately 17%) compared to age- and
experimental studies (Table 2). Among the
sex-matched controls.51 Only one of these
symptoms that have been reported are
patients exhibited total hearing loss post-op-
difficulty concentrating, deficits in attention
eratively and all of them were 8–10 years
and spatial memory, verbal fluency, mental
post-BVL. Subsequent studies have provided
rotation, and dyscalculia and other forms of
further evidence of impaired spatial memory
numerical cognition (see Table 2). Although
and hippocampal atrophy in patients
some 51 such studies have been published
with other vestibular disorders such as
since 1989, only a subset of them (21)
Meniere’s disease.52,71–74 A recent study of
have controlled for hearing loss, either by
over 100 healthy adults reported that poorer
excluding patients with hearing loss or by
vestibular function was correlated with
controlling for it statistically in the analysis
significantly reduced hippocampal volume.75
of the data.54–69 Table 2 provides a summary
In our most recent study, we have found
of the studies that have controlled for
that age is statistically related to a bilateral
hearing loss and therefore where the deficits
decrease in the volume of the hippocampus
can be considered to be mainly vestibular in
and the left entorhinal cortex.76
origin. Dobbels et al70 have argued that few
of the studies reporting cognitive deficits Vestibular dysfunction as a risk
in humans with vestibular disorders, have factor for dementia
controlled for hearing loss. However, a As a result of the animal and human
careful review of the literature suggests that studies demonstrating that peripheral
is not the case. Of course, there are cases in vestibular lesions caused spatial memory
which vestibular and auditory symptoms deficits, Previc77 suggested that loss of
present together, for example, in Meniere’s vestibular function might be implicated in
disease, in which case they are both likely the development of dementia, including
to contribute to cognitive deficits. Beyond Alzheimer’s disease (AD). His argument was
controlling for hearing loss, the major based partly on the limitations of the β-am-
weaknesses of the epidemiological studies yloid (Aβ) hypothesis of AD, but the idea
are that they often include ‘heterogeneous can be considered independently of that
vestibular disorders’ (bilateral vestibular hypothesis. The central vestibular system,
loss, unilateral vestibular loss, vestibular including the brainstem VNC, contributes
neuritis, benign paroxysmal positional to major cholinergic inputs to the hippo-
vertigo (BPPV), etc.) and do not include the campus, which is damaged in AD.78 Bilateral
same controls as the experimental studies vestibular loss in rats also results in a
(see Table 2). The available experimental decrease in acetylcholine (ACh) receptors in
studies are based on samples of patients the hippocampus.38
with different vestibular disorders, but each
There have been only a few studies that
one of them includes a sample of patients
have directly investigated the relationship
which is compared to a control group
between vestibular function and AD, all of
without vestibular dysfunction (see Table 2).
them conducted by the same group. The
Studies in humans which have combined first studies investigated the statistical rela-
structural and functional neuro-imaging tionship between vestibular dysfunction and

118
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

Table 2: Studies conducted in humans that have reported cognitive deficits associated with different types of vestibular dysfunction, in
which hearing loss has been controlled for in some way, either by excluding subjects with hearing loss or by controlling for it statistically
in a multiple logistic regression model. Where ‘No ()’ occurs, the first number in the brackets indicates the number of subjects without
hearing loss and the second, the total sample size.

Authors Diagnosis Cognitive impairment Hearing loss?

Epidemiological

Sang et al (2006)54 Heterogeneous vestibular Difficulty concentrating, thoughts seem No (33/50)


disorders blurred

Jauregui-Renaud et al (2008)55 Heterogeneous vestibular Difficulty concentrating, thoughts seem No (37/50)


disorders blurred

Jauregui-Renaud et al (2008)56 Heterogeneous vestibular Difficulty concentrating, thoughts seem No


disorders blurred

Semenov et al (2016)46 Vestibular dysfunction Digit symbol substitution test No

Bigelow et al (2016)44 Vestibular vertigo Cognitive impairment No

Bigelow et al (2020)57 Vertigo Attention, learning No

Clinical experimental

Risey and Briner (1990/1991)58 Vertigo Dyscalculia No

Redfern et al (2004)59 Unilateral vest. nerve Increased RT for complex tasks No


section

Brandt et al (2005)51 Bilateral vest. nerve Impaired spatial memory No


section

Talkowski et al (2005)60 UVL Auditory reaction time No

Gomez-Alvarez (2011)61 UVL Impaired spatial orientation, difficulty No


concentrating

Caixeta et al (2012)62 Chronic vestibular Verbal fluency No


dysfunction

Candidi et al (2013)63 BPPV, vestibular neuritis Mental rotation No

Bigelow et al (2015) 45
Vestibular dysfunction Spatial cognition No

Kremmyda et al (2016)52 Bilateral vest. nerve Impaired spatial memory No (13/15)


section

Moser et al (2017a)64 Vestibular neuritis Impaired numerical cognition No

Moser et al (2017b)65 Vestibular neuritis increased redundancy, impaired No


generation of random numbers

Lofti et al (2017)66 Vestibular deficits, ADHD Choice reaction time No

Sugaya et al (2018)67 Dizziness Trail-making test No (53/60)

Deroualle et al (2019)68 UVN Embodied spatial cognition No

Dobbels et al (2019)70 Bilateral vestibular loss Attention No

Pineault et al (2020)69 Various type of of Benton visual retention test, Trail No


vestibular loss making test
Abbreviations: UVL, unilateral vestibular loss; BPPV, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo; UVN, unilateral vestibular neurectomy; ADHD, attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder; RT, reaction time.

119
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

clinical syndromes of cognitive impairment, the studies were cross-sectional, and the
such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) samples may not have been representative
and AD. Harun et al79 investigated the preva- of a broader population.80 In particular,
lence of vestibular dysfunction in 32 patients patients with both vestibular and cognitive
with AD and 15 with MCI, compared to 94 impairment may have been more likely to
controls, and estimated that patients with present than those with either condition
bilaterally absent cervical vestibular-evoked alone, resulting in a potential overestimation
myogenic potentials (cVEMPs, a measure of the proportion of vestibular dysfunction
of saccular function), had a greater than in AD (‘Berksonian bias’).80 Fourth, it is
three-fold increased odds of AD (OR 3.42, conceivable that the poor performance
95% CI 1.32–8.91, P=0.011). Furthermore, by AD patients on the cVEMP and oVEMP
a 1 µV increase in cVEMP amplitude was testing could have been due to their inability
associated with a decreased odds of AD (OR to understand and follow instructions;
0.28, 95% CI 0.09–0.93, P=0.038). Higher however, the authors reported that this was
ocular VEMP (oVEMP, indicative of utricular not the case.79,80 Finally, the relationship
function) amplitude was associated with between cVEMP/oVEMP function and AD was
a decreased odds of AD (OR 0.92, 95% CI a statistical one involving logistic regression
0.85–0.99, P=0.036). However, there was and does not necessarily indicate a causal
no significant difference between the MCI relationship.79,80 For example, aside from
group and the controls. Importantly, there the possibility that vestibular dysfunction
was no significant association between VOR contributed to the development of AD, it
function and AD, indicating that semi-cir- is possible that AD pathology might have
cular canal function was not implicated, caused vestibular dysfunction.
only the otoliths, the most primitive part of One potential explanation for the rela-
the vestibular system. tionship between vestibular dysfunction
In a follow-up study, Wei et al80 examined and AD might be that AD pathology (eg,
vestibular function in 51 patients with AD, β-amyloid (Aβ)) extends into the central
26 with MCI and 295 matched controls. The vestibular pathways from the vestibular
cVEMP, the oVEMP and the VOR were all nucleus to the thalamus and beyond,
tested. Compared to controls, they found thereby impairing vestibular function.
that people with cVEMP impairment had a Although there have been no specific studies
3–4 fold increase in the odds of being in the of Aβ deposition in ‘vestibular-related
MCI group (OR 3.0, 95% CI 1.1–8.5, P=0.04) areas’ of the brain, vestibular information is
and those with oVEMP impairment had an distributed widely,41 (see Figure 4), therefore
almost four-fold increased odds of being it is likely that AD pathology extends to
in the MCI group (OR 3.9, 95% CI 1.4–11.3, many brain regions receiving vestibular
P=0.01). Compared to controls, they found input. However, in a recent study of
that people with impaired cVEMPs had a vestibular function in 98 participants aged
five-fold increased odds of being in the AD 77.3 (±8.26) from the BLSA, Aβ deposition
group (OR 5.0, 95% CI 2.0–12.3, P=0.001) and was measured using amyloid C-11 Pittsburgh
those with abnormal oVEMPs had a greater Compound B (11C-PB).81 The authors found
than four-fold increased odds of being in the that 22.4% of the sample were positive
AD group (OR 4.2, 95% CI 1.9–9.1, P≤0.001). for PiB; however, there was no statisti-
Importantly, VOR gain (the ratio of head cally significant relationship between the
velocity to eye velocity) was not significantly extent of Aβ deposition and any measure of
related to group membership. vestibular function. This study was designed
There are a number of limitations of these to investigate preclinical AD, but no such
studies which must be noted, however. study has been performed in patients
First, hearing loss was not controlled for in diagnosed with AD. Another possible expla-
these studies of AD and MCI, so it is possible nation is that vestibular impairment directly
that it may have been a contributing factor. contributes to medial temporal lobe neuro-
Second, the sample sizes of AD patients were degeneration and AD, possibly as a result of
relatively small, n=32 and 51, and therefore reduced vestibular sensory input to areas
the studies need to be replicated.79,80 Third, of the brain such as the hippocampus, as
occurs for auditory input.82

120
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

Further studies have concentrated on were newly diagnosed with LOAD between
whether vestibular loss is associated with 2007 and 2013, who were then matched to
specific phenotypes of AD, especially those 4,600 controls by both age and sex. Using
with spatial cognitive deficits. Some AD multivariate logistic regression and path
phenotypes are characterised by predom- analysis, the authors reported that the inci-
inantly amnestic symptoms compared to dence of LOAD was positively correlated
others which are characterised by more with prior anxiety (ICD code 300), functional
motoric and spatial impairment.83 In a digestive disorder (ICD code 564), psychopa-
study of 50 patients with MCI or AD, Wei et thology-specific symptoms (ICD code 307),
al84 observed that patients with vestibular disorders of the vestibular system (ICD code
loss were significantly more likely to 386), concussion (ICD code 850), disorders of
exhibit impairment in neurocognitive tests the urethra and urinary tract (ICD code 599),
of spatial skills, for example the Money disorders of refraction and accommodation
Road Map test (MRMT). When patients (ICD code 367) and hearing loss (ICD code
were divided into ‘spatially normal’ and 389). While the authors conclude that these
‘spatially impaired’ groups based on their data suggest that vestibular dysfunction
performance in the MRMT, only 25% of the may therefore be a risk factor for LOAD,
spatially normal patients were found to the limitations of the study include limited
have vestibular dysfunction compared to information regarding other confounding
96% of the spatially impaired patients.84 In a factors such as body mass index, blood
further study of 60 patients with MCI or AD, pressure, diet, smoking, diabetic therapy
patients with vestibular dysfunction were etc.; and the specific nature of the diag-
significantly more likely to have difficulty nosis may have varied according to factors
driving, an activity closely linked to spatial affecting access to a neurologist. At present,
cognitive ability.85 It is possible, therefore, there are no comparable data available
that vestibular dysfunction contributes to on potential vestibular contributions to
the development of a ‘spatial’ subtype of dementia associated with Parkinson’s
AD, increasing the probability of symptoms disease or fronto-temporal dementia.
such as spatial disorientation, wandering, One of the intriguing aspects of the
and an increased risk of falling.84 However, studies in cognitively-normal and vestib-
these two studies have similar limitations ular-impaired adults is the demonstration
to the original one by Harun et al:79 there of a link between saccular function and
were no controls for hearing loss, the sample cognition.45,47,69,75,76,79,80,85 We have recently
sizes were relatively small, the studies were reported that saccular function is a statis-
cross-sectional and in Wei et al,85 the postural tically significant predictor of the decrease
measurements were not specific to vestibular in hippocampal volume that occurs with
function. Finally, the statistical association age.76 The saccule, one of the two otoliths,
reported does not indicate causality. is the part of the peripheral vestibular
The only available study that provides system which detects the orientation of the
any evidence that vestibular loss might be head with respect to gravity and, together
causally involved in the development of with the utricle, is the oldest component
cognitive impairment and dementia is by of the vestibular system in evolutionary
Liao et al86 They investigated prior medical terms (see Figure 2). Patients with AD
conditions that were associated with late- exhibited specifically poorer saccular and
onset Alzheimer’s disease dementia (LOAD) utricular function compared to age-matched
using a population-based matched case controls.79,85 Saccular stimulation has
control study based on the National Health been demonstrated to activate the multi-
Insurance Research database of Taiwan sensory vestibular cortex involved in
and the Catastrophic Illness Certificate spatial information processing.87,88 In guinea
database, between the years 1997 and 2013. pigs and rats, selective electrical stimu-
The definitions of prior diseases were based lation of the utricle and saccule has been
on the first three digits of the International shown to cause widespread activation of
Classification of Diseases, 9th Revision, the hippocampus.89,90 There is increasing
Clinical Modification (ICD-9-CM). The total evidence that the otoliths, the saccule in
case group consisted of 4,600 patients who particular, have a critical role in spatial

121
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

memory due to their importance in the


perception of gravitational vertical.19 We
Conclusions
have recently reported that mice lacking The available evidence suggests that
otolithic function from birth, exhibit major vestibular dysfunction, including that asso-
developmental delays by post-natal day 9, ciated with age-related vestibular loss, has
including spatial memory deficits.91 Inter- a significant negative impact on cognitive
estingly, considerable electrophysiological function. Vestibular impairment may
evidence suggests that the neurons in the therefore be a risk factor for the devel-
VNC that subserve the VOR are separate opment of dementia, including AD. Previc93
from those involved in the VSR pathways has recently suggested that, given the
and the pathways to the limbic system and increased prevalence of vestibular disorders
neocortex.92 This means that it is possible for in females,94 vestibular dysfunction may
the vestibular pathways that give rise to the contribute to their increased incidence of
conscious perception of self-movement and AD. However, the majority of the evidence
contribute to spatial memory, to be compro- to date is correlational; therefore, caution
mised, without VOR deficits necessarily must be exercised in interpreting these
being exhibited, and that only VSRs such as findings. The potential combined effects
VEMPs would indicate a vestibular deficit. of both hearing loss and vestibular loss
Figure 5 summarises the hypothesis that are unknown but could be expected to be
saccular dysfunction, in particular, might much greater. Of course, it is important to
contribute to the development of cognitive note that some otological disorders involve
dysfunction that preferentially includes both auditory and vestibular symptoms
spatial cognitive deficits. (eg, Meniere’s disease). Understanding the

Figure 5: Conceptual model of impact of aging on vestibular function (notably saccular function), which
contributes to neurodegeneration of neural circuits involved in vestibular processing and deterioration,
specifically in spatial cognitive ability. From Agrawal et al28 with permission.

122
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

full implications of vestibular dysfunction reported that vestibular rehabilitation can


for cognitive decline is potentially of great improve cognitive function in healthy adults
importance for the health of the elderly, and in patients with intractable dizziness.67,97
since effective therapies are available to However, at present, studies from the US
treat vestibular disorders.95 One of the prin- indicate that only a small number of people
cipal treatments for vestibular impairment with AD are referred for vestibular rehabil-
is vestibular rehabilitation, a suite of itation.98 Since vestibular impairment may
physical therapy-based exercises in which be a modifiable risk factor for dementia, the
head movements are used to stimulate the impact of vestibular loss on cognition should
vestibular system and gradually encourage be considered along with hearing loss as a
the brain to adapt to the loss of normal critical area for research.
vestibular function.96 Several studies have

Competing interests:
Nil.
Acknowledgements:
I would like to thank the three anonymous referees for their constructive and helpful com-
ments on the manuscript.
Author information:
Paul F Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences
and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New
Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance
Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.
Corresponding author:
Prof Paul Smith, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, School of Biomedical Sciences
and the Brain Health Research Centre, University of Otago, Dunedin; Brain Research New
Zealand Centre of Research Excellence; Eisdell Moore Centre for Hearing and Balance
Research, University of Auckland, Auckland.
paul.smith@otago.ac.nz
URL:
www.nzma.org.nz/journal-articles/why-dizziness-is-likely-to-increase-the-risk-of-cognitive-
dysfunction-and-dementia-in-elderly-adults

REFERENCES:
1. Agrawal Y, Smith PF, 3. Health Quality and Safety Institute on Aging/National
Merfeld D. Dizziness, Commission New Zealand, Institute of Deafness and
imbalance and age-related 2020. http://public.tableau. Communication Disorders
vestibular loss. In: Straka com/profile/hqi2803#!/ Workshop. J. Gerontol.:
H, editor, Reference vizhome/FallsFracture- Med. Sci. 2020, in press.
Module in Neuroscience sOutcomesFramework/ 6. Goldberg JM, Wilson VJ,
and Biobehavioral Psychol- Landing Angelaki DE, et al. The
ogy. Elsevier, NY, 2020. 4. Hartholt KA, Lee R, Burns Vestibular System: A Sixth
2. Agrawal Y, Carey JP, Della ER, Van Beeck EF. Mortal- Sense. New York: Oxford
Santina CC, et al. Disorders ity from falls among US University Press, 2012.
of balance and vestibular adults aged 75 years or 7. Curthoys IS, Grant JW,
function in US adults: older, 2000–2016. JAMA. Burgess AM, et al. Otolith
data from the national 2019; 321(21):2131–2133. receptor mechanisms for
health and nutrition 5. Agrawal Y, Merfeld D, vestibular-evoked myogen-
examination survey, 2001- Horak F, et al. Aging, vestib- ic potentials: A review.
2004. Arch. Intern. Med. ular function and balance: Front. Neurol. 2018; 9:366.
2009; 169(10):938–944. Proceedings of a National

123
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

8. Hitier M, Sato G, Zhang entorhinal cortex. Nature. 26. Liu CM, Lee CT. Associa-
Y, et al. Anatomy and 2015; 436(7052):801–6. tion of hearing loss with
surgical approach to 18. Moser EI, Moser MB, dementia. JAMA Netw
rat’s vestibular sensors McNaughton BL. Spatial Open. 2019; 2(7): e198112.
and nerves. J. Neurosci. representation in the 27. Curhan SG, Willett
Meth. 2016; 270:1–8. hippocampal formation: WC, Grodstein F, et al.
9. Urciuoli A, Zanolli C, Beau- a history. Nat Neurosci. Longitudinal study of
det A, et al. The evolution 2017; 20(11):1448–1464. self-reported hearing loss
of the vestibular apparatus 19. Smith PF. The growing and subjective cognitive
in apes and humans. evidence for the impor- function decline in women.
Elife. 2020; 9. pii:e51261. tance of the otoliths Alzheimers Dement.
10. Ramos de Miguel A, for spatial memory. 2020; 16(4):610–620.
Zarowski A, Sluydts M, Front. Neural Circ. 28. Agrawal Y, Smith PF,
et al. The superiority 2019; 3(66):1–14. Rosenberg PB. Vestibular
of the otolith system. 20. Livingston G, Sommerlad impairment, cognitive
Audiol Neurootol. A, Orgeta V, et al. Dementia decline and Alzheimer’s
2020; 25(1–2):35–41. prevention, intervention, Disease: Balancing the
11. Stackman RW, Clark AS, and care. Lancet. 2017; evidence. Aging and Mental
Taube JS. Hippocampal 390(10113):2673–2734. Health. 2020; 24(5):705–708.
spatial representations 21. Bathini P, Brai E, Auber 29. Smith PF, Zheng Y. From
require vestibular input. LA. Olfactory dysfunction ear to uncertainty:
Hippocampus 2002; in the pathophysiological Vestibular contributions
12:291–303. continuum of demen- to cognitive function.
12. Russell NA, Horii A, Smith tia. Ageing Res Rev. Front. Integrat. Neuro-
PF, et al. Long-term effects 2019; 55:100956. sci. 2013; 7:84.
of permanent vestibular 22. Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, 30. Smith PF, Geddes LH,
lesions on hippocampal Huntly J, et al. Population Baek J-H, et al. Modulation
spatial firing. J. Neurosci. attributable fractions for of cognitive function
2003; 23(16):6490–6498. risk factors for dementia in by vestibular lesions
13. Jacob P-Y, Poucet B, low-income and middle-in- and galvanic vestibular
Liberge M, et al. Vestibular come countries: an analysis stimulation. Front.
control of entorhinal using cross-sectional survey Neurol. 2010; 1(141):1–8.
cortex activity in spatial data. The Lancet Global 31. Smith PF, Darlington
navigation. Front. Integrat. Health. 2019; 7:e596–e603. CL, Zheng Y. The effects
Neurosci. 2014; 8:38. 23. Michalowsky B, Hoffmann of complete vestibular
14. Besnard S, Lopez C, Brandt W, Kostev K. Association deafferentation on spatial
T, et al. (editors) The vestib- between hearing and memory and the hippocam-
ular system in cognitive vision impairment and pus in rat: The Dunedin
and memory processes in risk of dementia: Results experience. In: Ferre, E.R,
mammals. Front. Integrat. of a case-control study Harris, L. (editors), Vestib-
Neurosci. Lausanne: based on secondary data. ular Cognition. Brill,
Frontiers Media. doi: Front Aging Neurosci. Leiden, 2017; pp. 461–485.
10.3389/978-2-88919-744-6 2019; 20;11: 363. 32. Baek JH, Zheng Y, Darling-
(ebook) pp. 1–246, 2016. 24. Golub JS, Brickman ton C L, et al. Evidence that
15. Smith PF. The vestibular AM, Ciarleglio AJ, et al. spatial memory deficits
system and cognition. Association of subclinical following bilateral vestibu-
Curr. Opin. Neurol. hearing loss with cogni- lar deafferentation in rats
2017; 30(1):84–89. tive performance. JAMA are probably permanent.
Otolaryngol Head Neck Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
16. O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The
Surg. 2020; 146(1):57–67. 2010; 94(3):402–413.
hippocampus as a spatial
map. Preliminary evidence 25. Loughrey DG, Parra 33. Russell N, Horii A, Smith
from unit activity in the MA, Lawlor BA. Visual PF, et al. Lesions of the
freely-moving rat. Brain short-term memory vestibular system disrupt
Res. 1971; 34(1):171–5. binding deficit with hippocampal theta rhythm
age-related hearing loss in the rat. J. Neurophysi-
17. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden
in cognitively normal ol. 2006; 96:4–14.
S, et al. Microstructure
of a spatial map in the older adults. Sci Rep. 34. Neo P, Carter, D, Zheng, Y,
2019; 9(1):12600. et al. Septal elicitation of

124
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

hippocampal theta rhythm the ventrolateral and 50. Smith L, Wilkinson D,


did not repair the cognitive laterodorsal thalamus Bodani M, et al. Short-term
and emotional deficits in mice. Front Neural memory impairment in
resulting from vestibular Circuits. 2019; 13:51. vestibular patients can
lesions. Hippocampus. 43. Watson TC, Obiang P, arise independently of
2012; 22:1176–1187. Torres-Herraez A, et al. psychiatric impairment,
35. Tai SK, Ma J, Ossenkopp Anatomical and physi- fatigue, and sleeplessness.
KP, et al. Activation of ological foundations of J Neuropsychol. 2019;
immobility-related hippo- cerebello-hippocampal 13(3):417–431.
campal theta by cholinergic interaction. Elife. 51. Brandt T, Schautzer
septohippocampal neurons 2019; 8:e41896. F, Hamilton DA, et al.
during vestibular stim- 44. Bigelow RT, Semenov Vestibular loss causes
ulation. Hippocampus. YR, Du Lac S, et al. hippocampal atrophy and
2012; 22(4):914–25. Vestibular vertigo and impaired spatial memory
36. Cullen KE, Taube JS. Our comorbid cognitive and in humans. Brain. 2005;
sense of direction: prog- psychiatric impairment: 128(11):2732–2741.
ress, controversies and The 2008 national health 52. Kremmyda O, Huffner
challenges. Nat Neurosci. interview survey. J. K, Flanagin VL, et al.
2017; 20(11):1465–1473. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psych. Beyond dizziness: Virtual
37. Harvey RE, Rutan SA, 2016; 87(4):310–319. navigation, spatial anxiety
Willey GR, et al. M. Linear 45. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR, and hippocampal volume
self-motion cues support Trevino C, et al. Association in bilateral vestibulop-
the spatial distribution and between visuo- spatial athy. Front. Human
stability of hippocampal ability and vestibular Neurosci. 2016; 10:139.
place cells. Curr. Biol. function in the Baltimore 53. Ayar DA, Kumral E,
2018; 28(11):1810.e5. Longitudinal Study of Celebisoy N. Cognitive
38. Aitken P, Benoit A, Zheng Aging. J. Am. Geriat. Soc. functions in acute vestib-
Y, et al. Hippocampal and 2015; 63(9):1837–1844. ular loss. J. Neurol. 2020,
striatal M1-muscarinic 46. Semenov YR, Bigelow RT, in press. doi: 10.1007/
acetylcholine receptors are Xue Q, et al. Association s00415-020-09829-w.
down-regulated following between vestibular and 54. Sang FY, Jáuregui-Re-
bilateral vestibular loss cognitive function in naud K, Green DA, et
in rats. Hippocampus. US adults: Data from al. Depersonalisation/
2016; 26:1509–1514. the National Health and derealisation symptoms
39. Besnard S, Machado Nutrition Examination in vestibular disease. J
ML, Vignaux G, et al. Survey. J. Gerontol. Series Neurol Neurosurg Psychi-
Influence of vestibular A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. atry. 2006; 77(6):760–6.
input on spatial and 2015; 71(2):243–250. 55. Jáuregui-Renaud K, Sang
nonspatial memory and 47. Xie Y, Bigelow RT, FY, Gresty MA, et al. Deper-
on hippocampal NMDA Frankenthaler SF, et al. sonalisation/derealisation
receptors. Hippocampus. Vestibular loss in older symptoms and updating
2012; 22:814–826. adults is associated orientation in patients
40. Truchet B, Benoit A, with impaired spatial with vestibular disease. J
Chaillan F, et al. Hippocam- navigation: Data from the Neurol Neurosurg Psychi-
pal LTP modulation and Triangle Completion Task. atry. 2008; 79(3):276–83.
glutamatergic receptors Front Neurol. 2017; 8:173. 56. Jáuregui-Renaud K,
following vestibular 48. Wackym PA, Balaban Ramos-Toledo V, Agui-
loss. Brain Struct. Funct. CD, Mackay HT, et al. lar-Bolaños M, et al.
2019; 224:699–711. Longitudinal cognitive Symptoms of detachment
41. Hitier M, Besnard S, and neurobehavioral from the self or from
Smith PF. Vestibular functional outcomes before the environment in
pathways involved in and after repairing otic patients with an acquired
cognition. Front. Integrat. capsule dehiscence. Otol deficiency of the special
Neurosci. 2014; 8:59. Neurotol. 2016; 37(1):70–82. senses. J Vestib Res.
49. Popp P, Wulff M, Finke K, 2008; 18(2–3):129–37.
42. Bohne P, Schwarz MK,
Herlitze S, Mark MD. A et al. Cognitive deficits in 57. Bigelow RT, Semenov YR,
new projection from the patients with a chronic Hoffman HJ, et al. Asso-
deep cerebellar nuclei vestibular failure. J Neurol. ciation between vertigo,
to the hippocampus via 2017; 264(3):554–563. cognitive and psychiatric

125
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

conditions in US children: math achievement in volume and its relevance


2012 National Health patients with acute with inner ear function
Interview Survey. Int J vestibular neuritis. Neuro- in Meniere’s disease
Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. psychologia. 2017; 107:1–8. patients. Auris Nasus
2020; 130:109802. 66. Lofti Y, Rezazadeh N, Larynx. 2016; 43(6):620–5.
58. Risey J, Briner W. Dyscalcu- Moossavi A, et al. Prelimi- 74. Göttlich M, Jandl NM,
lia in patients with vertigo. nary evidence of improved Sprenger A, et al. Hippo-
J. Vest. Res. 1990; 1:31–37. cognitive performance campal gray matter volume
59. Redfern MS, Talkowski following vestibular reha- in bilateral vestibular
ME, Jennings JR, et al. bilitation in children with failure. Hum Brain Mapp.
Cognitive influences combined ADHD (cADHD) 2016; 37(5):1998–2006.
on postural control of and concurrent vestibular 75. Kamil RJ, Jacob A,
patients with unilateral impairment. Auris Nasus Ratnanather JT, et al.
vestibular loss. Gait and Larynx. 2017; 44:700–707. Vestibular function and
Posture. 2004; 19:105–114. 67. Sugaya N, Arai M, Goto hippocampal volume in
60. Talkowski ME, Redfern F. Changes in cognitive the Baltimore Longitudinal
MS, Jennings JR, et al. function in patients with Study of Aging (BLSA). Otol.
Cognitive requirements intractable dizziness Neurotol. 2018; (6):765–771.
for vestibular and ocular following vestibular 76. Jacob A, Tward D, Resnick
motor processing in rehabilitation. Sci. Reps. S, et al. Vestibular func-
healthy adults and patients 2018; 8:9984. tion and cortical and
with unilateral vestibular 68. Deroualle D, Borel L, sub-cortical alterations
lesions. J. Cog. Neurosci. Tanguy B, et al. Unilateral in an aging population.
2005; 17(9):1432–1441. vestibular deafferentation Heliyon. 2020, 6: e04728.
61. Gomez-Alvarez FB, impairs embodied spatial 77. Previc FH. Vestibular
Jáuregui-Renaud K. cognition. J. Neurol. 2019; loss as a contributor to
Psychological symptoms 266(Suppl. 1: S149–S159. Alzheimer’s disease.
and spatial orientation 69. Pineault K, Pearson D, Wei Med. Hypoth. 2013;
during the first 3 months et al. Association between 80(4):360–367.
after acute vestibular saccule and semi-circular 78. Chapleau M, Aldebert
neuritis. Arch. Med. canal impairments and J, Montembeault M,
Res. 2011; 42:97–103. cognitive performance Brambati SM. Atrophy
62. Caixeta GC dos S, among vestibular patients. in Alzheimer’s Disease
Dona F, Gazzola JM. Ear Hear. 2020; 41:686–692. and Semantic Dementia:
Cognitive processing 70. Dobbels B, Peetermans O, An ALE Meta-analysis of
and body balance in Boon B, et al. Impact of voxel-based morphometry
elderly subjects with bilateral vestibulopathy studies. J Alzheimers Dis.
vestibular dysfunction. on spatial and nonspatial 2016; 54(3):941–955.
Braz. J. Otorhinolaryngol. cognition: A systematic 79. Harun A, Oh ES, Bigelow
2012; 78(2):87–95. review. Ear Hear. 2018; RT, et al. Vestibular
63. Candidi M, Micarelli A, 40(4):757–765. impairment in dementia.
Viziano A, et al. Impaired 71. zu Eulenburg P, Stoeter P, Otol. Neurotol. 2016;
mental rotation in benign Dieterich M. Voxel-based 37(8):1137–1142.
paroxysmal positional morphometry depicts 80. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et
vertigo and acute vestibu- central compensation after al. Vestibular loss predicts
lar neuritis. Front. Hum. vestibular neuritis. Annal. poorer spatial cognition in
Neurosci. 2013; 7:783. Neurol. 2010; 68:241–249. patients with Alzheimer’s
64. Moser I, Vibert D, Caver- 72. Van Cruijsen N, Hiemstra disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis.
saccio MD, et al. Acute WM, Meiners LC, et al. 2017; 61(3):995–1003.
peripheral vestibular Hippocampal volume 81. Kamil RJ, Bilgel M, Wong
deficit increases the redun- measurement in patients DF, et al. Vestibular
dancy in random number with Ménière’s disease: a function and beta-amyloid
generation. Exp. Brain pilot study. Acta Otolaryn- deposition in the Balti-
Res. 2017; 235:627–637. gol. 2007; 127(10):1018–23. more Longitudinal Study
65. Moser I, Vibert D, Caver- 73. Seo YJ, Kim J, Kim SH. The of Aging. Front Aging
saccio MD, et al. Impaired change of hippocampal Neurosci. 2018; 10:408.

126
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal
ARTICLE

82. Cui B, Zhu L, She X, et al. in the human cerebral of Alzheimer’s Disease
Chronic noise exposure cortex. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. in females? Clin. Case.
causes persistence of tau 2005; 1039(1):124–131. Revs. 2019; 2(1):1–2.
hyperphosphorylation and 88. Schlindwein P, Mueller M, 94. Smith PF, Agrawal Y,
formation of NFT tau in the Bauermann T, et al. Cortical Darlington CL. Sexual
rat hippocampus and pre- representation of saccular dimorphism in vestibular
frontal cortex. Exp. Neurol. vestibular stimulation: function and dysfunc-
2012; 238(2):122–129. VEMPs in fMRI. NeuroIm- tion. J. Neurophysiol.
83. Mapstone M, Steff- age. 2008; 39(1):19–31. 2019; 121:2379–2391.
enella TM, Duffy CJ. A 89. Cuthbert PC, Gilchrist DP, 95. Hillier S, McDonnell M. Is
visuospatial variant of Hicks SL, et al. Electro- vestibular rehabilitation
mild cognitive impairment: physiological evidence effective in improving
getting lost between for vestibular activation dizziness and function
aging and AD. Neurol. of the guinea pig hippo- after unilateral peripheral
2003; 60(5):802–808. campus. Neuroreport. vestibular hypofunction?
84. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, et 2000; 11(7):1443–47. an abridged version of
al. Increased prevalence 90. Smith PF, Hitier M, Zhang, a Cochrane Review. Eur.
of vestibular loss in mild Y-F, et al. Vestibular J. Phys. Rehab. Med.
cognitive impairment modulation of hippocampal 2016; 52(4):541–556.
and Alzheimer’s Disease. and striatal function. J. 96. Herdman SJ, Clendaniel R.
Curr Alzheimer Res. Vest. Res. 2019; 29(1):18. Vestibular Rehabilitation
2019; 16(12):1143–1150. (4th ed.). Philadelphia,
91. Le Gall A, Hilber P,
85. Wei EX, Oh ES, Harun A, Chesneau C, et al. The PA: F.A. Davis. 2014.
et al. Saccular impairment critical role of vestibular 97. Rogge AK, Röder B, Zech
in Alzheimer’s disease graviception during A, et al. Balance train-
is associated with driv- cognitive-motor devel- ing improves memory
ing difficulty. Dement. opment. Behav. Brain and spatial cognition
Geriatric Cog. Dis. 2017; Res. 2019; 372:112040. in healthy adults. Sci
44(5–6):294–302. Rep. 2017; 7(1):5661.
92. Cullen KE. The vestibular
86. Liao JY, Lee CT, Lin TY, et system: multimodal 98. Gandhi P, Klatt BN,
al. Exploring prior diseases integration and encoding Agrawal Y. Physical
associated with incident of self-motion for motor and vestibular physical
late-onset Alzheimer’s control. Trends Neurosci. therapy referrals in people
disease dementia. PLoS 2012; 35(3):185–96. with Alzheimer Disease.
One. 2020; 15(1):e0228172. Alzheimer Dis Assoc
93. Previc F. Does vestibular
87. Miyamoto T, Fukushi- dysfunction contribute to Disord. 2020 doi: 10.1097/
ma K, Takada T, et al. the increased prevalence WAD.0000000000000390.
Saccular projections

127
NZMJ 25 September 2020, Vol 133 No 1522
ISSN 1175-8716 © NZMA
www.nzma.org.nz/journal

You might also like