You are on page 1of 15

Thehttp://nro.sagepub.

com/
Neuroscientist

Brain Plasticity Mechanisms and Memory: A Party of Four


Elodie Bruel-Jungerman, Sabrina Davis and Serge Laroche
Neuroscientist 2007 13: 492
DOI: 10.1177/1073858407302725

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://nro.sagepub.com/content/13/5/492

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for The Neuroscientist can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://nro.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://nro.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://nro.sagepub.com/content/13/5/492.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Sep 27, 2007

What is This?

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Brain Plasticity Mechanisms and Memory:
A Party of Four
ELODIE BRUEL-JUNGERMAN, SABRINA DAVIS, and SERGE LAROCHE
Laboratoire de Neurobiologie de l’Apprentissage, de la Mémoire et de la Communication, CNRS, Univ Paris-Sud, Orsay, France

A defining characteristic of the brain is its remarkable capacity to undergo activity-dependent functional and
morphological remodeling via mechanisms of plasticity that form the basis of our capacity to encode and retain
memories. Today, it is generally accepted that the neurobiological substrate of memories resides in activity-
driven modifications of synaptic strength and structural remodeling of neural networks activated during learn-
ing. Since the discovery of long-term potentiation, the role of synaptic strengthening in learning and memory
has been the subject of considerable investigation, and numerous studies have provided new insights into how
this form of plasticity can subserve memory function. At the same time, other studies have explored the con-
tribution of synaptic elimination or weakening; synaptogenesis, the growth of new synaptic connections and
synapse remodeling; and more recently, neurogenesis, the birth and growth of new neurons in the adult brain.
In this review, based on work in the hippocampus, the authors briefly outline recent advances in their under-
standing of the mechanisms and functional role of these four types of brain plasticity in the context of learn-
ing and memory. While they have long been considered as alternative mechanisms of plasticity underlying the
storage of long-term memories, recent evidence suggests that they are functionally linked, suggesting the
mechanisms underlying plasticity in the brain required for the formation and retention of memories are multi-
faceted. NEUROSCIENTIST 13(5):492–505, 2007. DOI: 10.1177/1073858407302725
KEY WORDS Memory, Synaptic plasticity, Long-term potentiation, Long-term depression, Synaptogenesis, Neurogenesis, Hippocampus

The capacity to form, retain, and use memories is a fun- that memories are encoded as dynamic spatio-temporal
damental property of the brain essential for survival in all patterns of synchronized cellular activity within wide-
organisms. For example, an aplysia will learn to withdraw spread neural networks and that this dynamic, reverberat-
its gill in response to noxious stimuli; a rodent will learn ing activity progressively results in altered patterns of
to map environments to remember where it can access connectivity among the coactivated neurons. Within this
food and avoid places in which danger may be apparent. framework, any memory representation would correspond
Humans have a rich array of memories associated with with specific sets of patterns of activity in overlapping
emotion, acquired skills and habits, facts about life, and networks. The neural code embedded within these pat-
specific episodes of experiences with personal tags. terns of activity in large part defies our understanding.
Collectively, these allow us to form and constantly elabo- Nonetheless, it has long been recognized that this dynamic
rate our own definition of the world, giving us our indi- activity, transient in nature, cannot persist long enough to
viduality. How do we form memories; how are they be the actual substrate of long-term memory. Thus, it has
encoded and stored in the brain? The brain is not a passive been postulated that there should exist a second state of
recorder of experiences as if information were merely pro- memory encoded as changes at the cellular level to store
jected onto a mental screen; it is a dynamic system that these representations. A process of stabilization or consol-
creates information. To process and store a lifetime of idation would lead to what Hebb called a “structural
memories, some form of plasticity in the brain that goes trace,” a memory trace that is maintained in some form of
beyond that known to occur during early development is a dormant state but has the capacity to return to an “active
required. Following Cajal’s original ideas and Hebb’s pre- state” to evoke recall whenever a subset of the original
scient and influential dual-trace theory, it is now believed information, or related information, is available. Although
it has been suggested that once a long-term memory had
been established it was stable and remained immune to
Grant sponsor: Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique. European any form of disruption, Lewis in 1979 suggested this was
Union, Grant RTN-CT-2003-504231 to SL. Ministère de la Recherche,
ACI-NEURO-NIC-0027 to SD. Fellowship from the Fondation pour la not the case. A so-called established, or consolidated,
Recherche Médicale to EB-J. memory when reactivated enters a dynamic but fragile
state, requiring further stabilization via synaptic changes
Address correspondence to: Serge Laroche, Laboratoire de Neurobiolo-
gie de l’Apprentissage, de la Mémoire et de la Communication, UMR to be available once again for recall, a process now known
8620, CNRS, University Paris-Sud, 91405 Orsay, France (e-mail: serge as reconsolidation (reviewed in Alberini 2005). The main
.laroche@u-psud.fr). point is that long-term memories are not, as was originally

492 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Brain Plasticity and Memory


Volume 13, Number 5, 2007
Copyright © 2007 Sage Publications
ISSN 1073-8584 Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013
thought, stable and essentially “hardwired,” that the mech- of LTP and the major cellular and molecular events that
anisms of plasticity in neural circuits that encode and store mediate LTP. The physiological and pharmacological
long-term memories are dynamic and ongoing throughout characteristics of LTP that make it a viable mechanism
the life of a memory. The function of this form of ongoing for learning and memory are that it can be long-lasting,
plasticity has not been clarified yet but may well serve to from weeks to several months, and that it is input-spe-
update or modify existing memories. It is well known that cific and associative in nature, endowing it with the abil-
a single memory over as short a time span of several days ity to process converging input in a manner reminiscent
or months evolves as it is incorporated into a vast database of associative learning. Shortly after the discovery of
of information. LTP and of some of its underlying molecular mecha-
What are the plasticity mechanisms that serve to store nisms, many groups exploited this knowledge to charac-
and update long-term memories? To date, the most widely terize the function of this particular property of synaptic
held view is that of synaptic strengthening, a process that plasticity in the behaving animal. The results of the past
occurs during learning whereby memories are stabilized three decades of research have provided strong support
and stored as modifications of synaptic strength within the for the idea that the type of synaptic change brought
existing neuronal circuits, as originally suggested by Hebb about by LTP plays a crucial role in memory function
(1949). The fact that certain neurons possess this property (see Martin and others 2000 for a review). In general,
to undergo activity-dependent synaptic strengthening was two main strategies have been used: one is to examine
demonstrated by Bliss and Lømo in 1973 when they first the “similarity” hypothesis in terms of both the occur-
reported their discovery of long-term potentiation (LTP) in rence of synaptic changes in certain brain regions during
the hippocampus, a brain structure involved in the forma- learning and the activation of biochemical/molecular
tion of many types of memory. To date, there is an enor- mechanisms known to underlie LTP. The second is to
mous amount of data suggesting that, indeed, the type of use means to block or enhance LTP and examine the
synaptic change that is brought about by LTP plays a cru- consequences on learning. Although it has proven a dif-
cial role in memory formation. There are, however, other ficult task, recent studies have provided evidence that
forms of brain plasticity, notably one more recently recog- increases in synaptic strength occur in brain during the
nized that is the converse of LTP, long-term depression formation of memories, for example, in hippocampal
(LTD), a form of activity-dependent long-lasting weaken- circuits during forms of associative learning that require
ing of synaptic strength. Although physiological evidence an intact hippocampus (Gruart and others 2006;
suggests it can be experimentally induced in the same Whitlock and others 2006) or in the motor cortex during
synapses that can also support LTP, only recently, with the procedural learning (Rioult-Pedotti and others 1998). In
understanding of the functional effect within the cell that addition, learning-induced synaptic strengthening was
LTD can induce, has it been envisaged as a potential mech- shown to occlude LTP, suggesting they share common
anism serving memory processes. Two other forms of mechanisms.
plasticity, synaptogenesis, the growth of new functional The primary mechanism for the induction of synaptic
synapses, and neurogenesis, the birth and growth of new potentiation in most brain pathways is the activation of a
neurons, originally considered to be mechanisms of plas- membrane protein assembly, the NMDA receptor, a slow-
ticity restricted to early development, have more recently acting, voltage-gated receptor selectively involved in the
been shown to be involved in memory processes in the induction of LTP (Fig. 2). The NMDA receptor acts as a
adult. Here, we review the advances made in our under- coincidence detector requiring two simultaneous events:
standing of these four forms of plasticity and their contri- the binding of glutamate released from presynaptic bou-
bution to the formation of long-term memories, focusing tons and a sufficient level of postsynaptic depolarization
on the hippocampus, and consider whether these different mediated via AMPA receptor activation. When these con-
forms of plasticity act as independent mechanisms or ditions are met, calcium, the triggering event in LTP,
whether they act together in a concerted manner. enters the postsynaptic neuron. Calcium rise is amplified
by the activation of metabotropic glutamate receptors
Synaptic Strengthening: Long-Term coupled to G-proteins, which, via the IP3 system, mobi-
Potentiation lize calcium from intracellular stores locally at the
activated spines (Bliss and Collingridge 1993). The
Long-term potentiation was first demonstrated in the importance of this mechanism for cognitive function has
rabbit dentate gyrus in 1973 by Bliss and Lømo. They been demonstrated in many studies showing that blocking
showed that brief high-frequency stimulation to the per- the NMDA receptor, either pharmacologically or by
forant path, the major input to the hippocampus convey- genetic inactivation in mutant mice, prevents the induc-
ing pretreated information from neocortical areas, leads tion of LTP and impairs many forms of learning (e.g.,
to an enduring increase in synaptic strength at dentate Davis and others 1992; Tsien and others 1996).
gyrus granule cell synapses (Fig. 1). Since then, some The NMDA receptor–induced rise in intracellular cal-
thousands of reports have followed, demonstrating LTP cium leads to the activation of numerous synaptic proteins
is a property shared by neurons in many different corti- via posttranslational modification and protein-protein
cal and subcortical regions of the brain across different interaction, constituting the biochemical machinery that
species, ranging from lizards to humans, and character- progressively leads to enduring modifications of the
izing the physiological and pharmacological properties synapse. This is mediated by a complex interplay between

Volume 13, Number 5, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 493

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Fig. 1. Examples of long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD) in different hippocampal subfields in the
awake rat. A, Schematic diagram of the tri-synaptic circuit of the hippocampus. The hippocampus includes fields CA1-3
and the dentate gyrus (DG). The perforant path (pp) originating from the entorhinal cortex makes “en passant” synapses
with dendrites of the granule cells of the DG. Axons from dentate granule cells, the mossy fibers (mf), contact pyramidal
cells of CA3, and their axons, the Schaffer collaterals (Sch), in turn contact the pyramidal cells of CA1. B, Stable LTP of
perforant path-granule cell synapses of the dentate gyrus induced by brief high-frequency stimulation of the perforant path
(arrow). C, Homosynaptic LTD in area CA1 induced by prolonged, low-frequency pairs of stimuli delivered to Schaffer col-
laterals (arrow). D, The induction of LTP in the dentate gyrus by high-frequency stimulation of the lateral perforant path
(filled circles) is associated with heterosynaptic LTD of the medial perforant path synapses on dentate granule cells (open
circles). EPSP = excitatory postsynaptic potential.

proteins of several signal-transduction cascades (Fig. 2). and colleagues (2005) reported evidence for AMPA recep-
The most well characterized of the kinases implicated in tor trafficking and insertion in synapses of a large fraction
LTP are PKC, PKA, TyrK, CaMKII, PI3K, and the of amygdala neurons following fear conditioning, and
MAPK/ERK (reviewed in Soderling and Derkach 2000). showed this was required for efficient fear memory. Kinase
Kinases are critical mediators of the early events responsi- recruitment of genomic programs in neurons leading to de
ble for the maintenance or stabilization of synaptic novo synthesis of proteins is a critical mechanism for long-
strengthening, and to date many of these kinases have been term stabilization of synaptic changes. Known for quite
implicated in the consolidation of long-term memories, some time was the fact that inhibitors of transcription or
reinforcing the idea that LTP-type mechanisms are acti- antibiotics blocking protein synthesis prevent the stabiliza-
vated during learning (e.g., Selcher and others 2002). For tion and therefore maintenance of the longer-lasting
example, numerous experiments have shown that these phases of LTP (e.g., Frey and Morris 1997) and that LTP
kinases are phosphorylated during, or immediately after, itself induces the transcription of genes in potentiated neu-
learning and that disrupting the normal activity of rons. Arguing in favor of the hypothesis that LTP mecha-
CaMKII, PKA, PKC, PI3K, or MAPK/ERK pharmaco- nisms are engaged during learning was the demonstration
logically or genetically generally causes learning and that the same inhibitors applied during or immediately
memory deficits in a variety of tasks. Activation of signal after learning prevent long-term, but not short-term, mem-
transduction pathways has two major consequences, the ory in a variety of tasks and the more recent evidence from
first being modification of the properties of synaptic recep- numerous experiments showing that learning is also asso-
tors; the second being activation of specific genomic pro- ciated with rapid gene regulation in different cell types and
grams. Modification of receptors occurs by phosphorylation brain regions as a function of the type of task.
of their subunits, change in the composition of heteromeric How is this genomic response initiated? A critical step is
receptors, recruitment of extra-synaptic AMPA receptors the expression of a class of immediate early genes (IEGs)
to synaptic sites, and mobilization of the trafficking/recy- encoding inducible transcription factors, which activate a
cling machinery to increase the number of AMPA recep- host of downstream effector genes to regulate their expres-
tors by insertion in the synaptic membrane. The exact sion (Fig. 3). This genomic response generally occurs
contribution of each of these mechanisms in memory for- within minutes after neuronal activation and is mediated
mation is not fully clarified. Recently, however, Rumpel via kinase-dependent activation of constitutively expressed

494 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Brain Plasticity and Memory

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Fig. 2. Schematic representation of the key steps in the molecular mechanisms mediating long-term potentiation in neu-
rons. AA = arachidonic acid; NO = nitric oxide; NOS = nitric oxide synthase; AC = adenylate cyclase; RE = endoplasmic retic-
ulum; mGluR = metabotropic glutamate receptor; PKA = protein kinase; MAPK = mitogen-activated protein kinase; PKC =
protein kinase C; TyrK = tyrosine-specific protein kinase; PI3K = phosphoinositide-3 kinase.

Volume 13, Number 5, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 495

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Fig. 3. Scheme of the main molecular steps involved in synaptic strengthening. Receptor and kinase cascade activa-
tion results in synaptic receptor modification and nuclear transcription of immediate early genes. Some encode synap-
tic proteins, whereas others encode inducible transcription factors such as egr1, which in turn activates transcription of
effector genes, leading to synthesis of the corresponding proteins required for persistent cell modification. Images show
an example of egr1 mRNA overexpression in the dentate gyrus 30 minutes after the induction of LTP, compared to a
control condition (Ct).

transcriptional regulators such as CREB. These activate and trafficked to the dendrites, allowing for rapid and localized
induce the expression of IEGs, the early genomic response changes at specific synapses. Many genes and proteins
required for triggering the mechanisms underlying persist- have been shown to be up- and down-regulated in a finely
ent cell modification. At present, the best-characterized tuned and coordinated manner in correlation with specific
molecular cascade involved in synaptic strengthening and aspects of LTP and memory processes. Both the full
memory consolidation is the MAPK-dependent transcrip- genomic response of neurons and the synaptic protein sig-
tional regulation of the IEG Egr1 (also known as Zif268 or nature resulting from translation of preexisting and newly
Krox24). Many studies have shown that components of this transcribed mRNAs that are associated with these
cascade such as MAPK/ERK, CREB, and Egr1 itself are processes are still, however, largely unknown and currently
activated in specific brain circuits when animals form dif- the subject of intense investigation using newly available
ferent types of memories such as fear memories, olfactory large-scale screening methods.
memories, and memories for specific environments or for A recent study adds support to the idea that synaptic
objects (reviews in Sweatt 2001; Davis and Laroche 2006). strengthening plays a central role in memory by providing
Moreover, inhibition of MAPK/ERK, CREB, or inactiva- evidence that the suppression of LTP after learning can
tion of Egr1 prevents both the maintenance of synaptic erase a previously established memory (Pastalkova and
plasticity and the formation of several types of memories others 2006). The authors showed that inhibiting PKMζ,
(e.g., Atkins and others 1998; Jones and others 2001; a kinase involved in the maintenance of LTP, can reverse
Bozon and others 2003). Other IEGs such as BDNF and or depotentiate LTP, and when injected in the hippocam-
Arg3.1 encoding proteins that are not acting as transcrip- pus even several days after spatial learning, causes retro-
tion factors but are targeted to the synapse also contribute grade amnesia, abolishing the stored memory. This
to synaptic plasticity and memory formation (Lee and oth- finding provides strong support to the idea that the main-
ers 2004; Plath and others 2006). In addition to transcrip- tenance of LTP over days is a necessary condition for
tional regulation, synaptic modification also involves the maintenance of memory (see Bliss and others 2006).
protein synthesis from preexisting mRNAs, and mRNAs To date, compelling evidence suggests that strengthening

496 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Brain Plasticity and Memory

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


of existing synapses in neural networks, via mechanisms of stimulation, electroconvulsive shock, stress, and novel
that endow neurons with the capacity to undergo rapid environmental conditions.
and enduring activity-dependent increases in synaptic Mechanisms of LTD in hippocampal areas are diverse.
weights as exemplified by LTP, is a type of brain plastic- NMDA-dependent LTD requires a rise in calcium as in
ity crucially involved in the formation and consolidation LTP, but the “decision” as to whether LTP or LTD is
of long-term memories. Despite this evidence, there are induced seems to depend on the amplitude, kinetics, and
still issues concerning the specific role for LTP mecha- localization of calcium influx. With a marked and rapid
nisms in processes of encoding and storage of informa- rise, LTP is induced, but with a modest and more lasting
tion, in different structures and for different types of increase favoring the activation of phosphatases rather
memories, that are the subject of intense debate. than kinases, LTD ensues. This dissociation between
the signaling events mediating NMDA-dependent LTP
Synaptic Elimination/Weakening: and LTD seems to reflect the activation of receptors con-
Long-Term Depression taining different NR2 subunits and synaptic location
(Collingridge and others 2004). Some forms of LTD,
In theory, synaptic elimination, the retraction and disap- however, that depend on group I mGluRs, have a strong
pearance of synaptic connections within complex net- presynaptic component and involve tyrosine phosphatases
works, is a means by which synaptic connections can be and the MAPK and PI3K-Akt-mTOR cascades. Although
lost. Based on the “use it or lose it” rule, the undoing or the signaling mechanisms of these different forms of LTD
loss of connections may reflect a mechanism for forget- require clarification, recent studies suggest that key mech-
ting or retrieval failure; alternatively, it may serve a means anisms underlying the expression of LTD include dephos-
to weaken unused connections, thereby promoting the phorylation of specific residues on GluR1-containing
emergence of patterns of reinforced connections in the AMPA receptors, lateral diffusion to extrasynaptic sites,
network, a selection process that could potentially be an and internalization of AMPA receptors (Beattie and others
advantage for information storage. Synaptic elimination 2000; Collingridge and others 2004). Protein synthesis is
or pruning is a property of the brain known to play a cru- also involved in certain forms of LTD.
cial role in specifying wiring of neural circuits during At present, it is unclear whether LTD per se has a role
development. Recent evidence suggests synapse elimina- in learning and memory as a cellular storage mechanism
tion can play a role in learning. One example is the selec- or whether this form of plasticity merely serves to adjust
tive, NMDA receptor–dependent elimination of spine or counterbalance synaptic strengthening. There are
synapses after auditory filial imprinting in the juvenile some indirect arguments in favor of the first hypothesis.
chicken (Bock and Braun 1999). Nevertheless, there is as For example, in the cerebellum, a brain structure in
yet little if any evidence that true synapse elimination which both LTD and LTP phenomena have been
could play a role in cognitive functions in the adult. observed but LTD seems to be the dominant form of
Functionally, however, the discovery that activity-dependent plasticity, there is a great deal of evidence to suggest that
LTD of synaptic efficacy can occur at hippocampal LTD mechanisms are implicated in forms of motor and
synapses under certain conditions of stimulation (e.g., associative learning for which this structure is essential
Dudek and Bear 1993) indicates that neurons have the (reviewed in De Zeeuw and Yeo 2005). In the hip-
mechanisms to trim down synaptic strength in a form that pocampus, a recent study has shown that LTD can be
can be functionally equivalent to synaptic elimination or induced by low-frequency stimulation when rats explore
selection. Reduction of synaptic weights can take several novel environments with objects, but not when this envi-
forms: de novo LTD, an activity-dependent depression at ronment has become familiar, suggesting a role for LTD
“naive” synapses; depotentiation, corresponding to rever- in complex spatial mapping (Kemp and Manahan-
sal or erasure of LTP; and heterosynaptic LTD, the weak- Vaughan 2007). Furthermore, impairment of hippocam-
ening of synaptic strength at nonactive synapses in pal LTD, with no apparent alteration in LTP, has been
association with potentiation of other neighboring observed in mutant mice with forebrain-specific deletion
synapses (Fig. 1). Research into the mechanisms and of serum response factor, an enhancer site for the expres-
functional role of LTD has long been hampered by the dif- sion of many IEGs. These mice display selective deficits in
ficulty to find reliable stimulus protocols for its induction memory for novel contexts in association with decreased
in the adult brain in vivo. However, it can readily be expression of serum response element–containing genes
induced in the juvenile, which is more likely to have little including Egr1- and LTD-related genes (Etkin and oth-
history of environmental experience or synaptic modifica- ers 2006). The authors suggest that LTD-like mecha-
tion, suggesting LTD may play a more important role nisms occurring during memorization of a novel context
during development for the refinement of synaptic con- may endow synapses with a greater potential to undergo
nectivity. LTD can, however, be observed in the adult in LTP-like strengthening during their subsequent recruit-
vivo under certain conditions in CA1 (Thiels and others ment when the animal learns an explicit memory related
1994; Doyère and others 1996) and in the dentate gyrus to that context.
(Kemp and Manahan-Vaughan 2007). In contrast, depo- As most modifiable synapses in the brain can support
tentiation and heterosynaptic LTD can be observed more both LTP- and LTD-like mechanisms, a possible key
reliably in the adult in vivo in many different brain struc- role of LTD in brain function could be to maintain the
tures and conditions, including long low-frequency trains overall synaptic drive in a network constant, a process

Volume 13, Number 5, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 497

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


referred to as synaptic scaling. Most neural network different cellular processes in a highly orchestrated hier-
models incorporate homeostatic processes to prevent archical sequence of events that mediate the competence,
saturation of the system and a potential block of its stor- placement, composition, size, and stability of functional
age capacity. Experimental evidence suggests that satu- synapses (Waites and others 2005). Many molecules,
rating LTP in a restricted cell population results in the including adhesion molecules such as the CAMs of the
inability to form new associative memories (Moser and cadherin and protocadherin superfamilies, SynCAM and
others 1998). Bidirectional plasticity demonstrates a neuroligin, serve as recognition sites or act to confirm cor-
mechanism whereby synaptic strengthening and weak- rect positioning and initiate contact, whereas others such
ening can occur instantaneously and simultaneously at as β-neurexin, NARP, and EphrinB are involved in
different synapses on a neuron or in different neurons in recruitment of glutamate receptors and postsynaptic scaf-
a given circuit, preventing chronic excitation of popula- folding proteins. Neuronal activity is implicated in regu-
tions of cells that could cause dysfunction. To date, it is lating the stability of the nascent synapse, with activity of
reasonably well established that one key mechanism of intracellular signaling pathways and trafficking of pro-
bidirectional plasticity is activity-dependent regulation teins exerting tight control over morphological and phys-
of cell surface expression of AMPA receptors, with iological maturation of the new synaptic contacts. In
LTP associated with insertion and LTD with internal- the adult, reactive synaptogenesis is often observed in
ization of AMPA receptors (Collingridge and others response to denervation, suggesting sprouting of axon
2004). There is physiological evidence for homeostatic collaterals and growth of new synapses to occupy vacated
responses to neural activity, for example, in the develop- sites.
ing cortex, when visual activation is increased, stimulat- In the adult, the search for learning-induced increases
ing frequencies that normally induce LTP but either no in synapse number has proven a difficult task and has led
longer do so or induce LTD. Conversely, visual depriva- to conflicting results. Some indirect arguments come from
tion promotes LTP over LTD (Perez-Otano and Ehlers studies showing modulation of synapse number by hor-
2005), and prolonged inhibition of cells can lead to an mones or stress in a manner that correlates with learning
increase in AMPA-mediated currents (Turrigiano and abilities. Living in enriched environments has also been
others 1998). Prolonged or high activation can reduce reliably found to result in dendritic expansion and
trafficking of NR1 subunits, whereas blocking neuronal increased spine and synapse numbers, and histological
activity increases trafficking (Perez-Otano and Ehlers analyses have shown changes in synapse number after
2005). In the adult mouse visual cortex, environmental certain learning paradigms (reviewed in Markham and
conditions can induce long-term homeostatic regulation Greenough 2004). A clear example is the NMDA-dependent
to switch plasticity from synaptic weakening to strength- increase in spine density observed in CA1 of the hip-
ening (Yashiro and others 2005). Finally, heterosynaptic pocampus 24 hours after training rats in a hippocampal-
depression may be essential for constraining synaptic dependent form of associative learning (Leuner and oth-
plasticity and maintaining cell function within a physio- ers 2003), or the finding of increased spine density in the
logical range while allowing locally induced synaptic amygdala after fear conditioning (Radley and others
strengthening. In the dentate gyrus, for example, induc- 2006). Several other studies, however, failed to find an
tion of LTP at one subset of synapses is always associ- overall increase in synapse number after learning,
ated with LTD at other synapses on the same neurons although many aspects of synapse restructuring were
(Doyère and others 1997). Evidence also suggests that observed in a consistent manner (see Geinisman 2000).
the maintenance of LTP at a given input can be unstable Difficulties associated with structural analyses ex vivo
and subject to dynamic modification by ongoing activity include the impracticality to analyze time-courses of
of neighboring synapses (Fonseca and others 2004). As changes in a dynamic manner and to ascertain which neu-
described below, other means of preventing saturation of rons and synapses have been active or strengthened dur-
synaptic plasticity associated with memory could ing learning. In addition, finding no change in the overall
include remodeling of connectivity patterns via mecha- synapse density in a given structure does not exclude the
nisms such as synaptogenesis and neurogenesis. possibility for increased density of certain types of spines
at the expense of others, or homeostatic processes with
Synaptogenesis and Synapse Remodeling increases and decreases in different portions of dendrites
or in different neurons (e.g., Rusakov and others 1997;
Although it has long been speculated that structural Harris and others 2003). A similar picture emerges with
rearrangements and remodeling of neural networks could LTP, where there is little evidence to suggest that the over-
be a prime candidate mechanism underlying the persist- all number of synapses per neuron change with LTP,
ence of memories, a long-standing issue is whether the although localized outgrowth of filopodia-like protrusions
cellular mechanisms of memory lead to true synaptogen- from dendrites in the immediate vicinity of potentiated
esis, the growth of new synapses, or merely to morpho- synapses occurs (Marrone and Petit 2002).
logical remodeling of existing synapses. In development, Several types of morphological changes at existing
synaptogenesis occurs over a protracted period starting synapses have, however, been observed after LTP and after
during embryogenesis and extending in the postnatal learning (reviews in Geinisman 2000; Harris and others
period to about day 90 in the rodent’s hippocampus. The 2003). They include changes in synapse curvature, in size
process is mediated by cell-cell interactions that control of the active zone and spine volume, increases in synaptic

498 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Brain Plasticity and Memory

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


length, and the appearance of perforations of the synapses that among the most reliable changes observed are a large
and multiple-synapse boutons where one presynaptic ter- increase in the density of multisynaptic boutons, particu-
minal makes contact with distinct postsynaptic spines. In larly in CA1; increased spine density; and increased size
contrast, LTD has been reported to induce spine shrinkage and density of perforated synapses, mostly in CA3 and the
(Zhou and others 2004). Modification of the morphologi- dentate gyrus. The analysis also revealed a greater propen-
cal characteristics of the synapse appears, in general, to sity for cortical areas to display learning-related increases
follow a sequential order of events that lead to an increase in spine density than hippocampal areas, whereby the hip-
in synaptic efficacy. In hippocampal LTP, for example, pocampus may be more subject to homeostatic changes.
there appears first to be a change in the curvature of the This suggests that morphological changes induced by
synapse showing an increase in the proportion of concave learning may differ in a regional-specific manner.
over convex synapses that occurs between 2 minutes and
24 hours following induction of LTP. Although it is unclear
Neurogenesis
what determines the shape of the synapse, it has been sug-
gested that concave synapses may be induced by an Contrary to the traditional dogma that neurogenesis, the
increase in the size of the presynapse caused by increased birth and growth of new functional neurons, is a strictly
transmitter release that forces the postsynapse to curve developmental phenomenon, Altman (1962) was the
around it, and by the breakdown of actin filaments in the first to suggest that new neurons continue to be added to
postsynaptic density (PSD) causing the change postsynap- the adult brain throughout life. This is now clearly estab-
tically. The outcome is to draw the receptors closer to the lished in different species (reviewed in Gross 2000),
synaptic cleft, thereby increasing the potential for the post- including in humans (Eriksson and others 1998). In the
synapse to be activated. This change in curvature may last mammalian brain, adult neurogenesis is to date thought
no longer than five days following the induction of LTP, to be confined to the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus
with a tendency for synapses to return to a convex shape, and olfactory bulb, although it may occur in other brain
suggesting that repeated activation may be required to regions in conditions of brain damage. Adult neurogen-
maintain lasting changes. A second form of reorganization esis is a slow process that involves proliferation of pro-
is the appearance of perforated synapses with a discontin- genitor cells, commitment to a neuronal phenotype,
uous PSD resulting from the disassembly of adhesion mol- morphological and physiological maturation with the
ecules that span the synaptic cleft. It has been suggested development of functional neuronal characteristics, and
that perforations and progressive partition could be an synaptic integration into existing neural networks. In the
intermediary event prior to synapse splitting, giving rise to dentate gyrus (Fig. 4), neural progenitor cells located in
two morphologically separated synapses (Luscher and oth- the subgranular zone give rise to transient amplifying
ers 2000). Synapse splitting, however, has not been reli- progenitors that in turn differentiate into new immature
ably observed, at least shortly after LTP (Harris and others neurons and migrate locally into the granular cell layer
2003). In the hippocampus, electrophysiological evidence where they mature into dentate granule cells. They
suggests there are “silent” synapses, containing NMDA receive synaptic inputs from entorhinal cortex neurons
but not AMPA receptors; however, LTP has been shown to via the perforant path and extend their axons to estab-
convert silent synapses to AMPA-containing, active lish synapses onto CA3 pyramidal cells, thus becoming
synapses (Isaac and others 1995; Liao and others 1995). functionally integrated into cortico-hippocampal cir-
The appearance of perforated synapses is associated with cuitry (reviews in Ming and Song 2005; Bruel-Jungerman
an increased expression of AMPA receptors and might be and others 2007). Although thousands of new neurons
a morphological correlate of this conversion, leading to are born each day, a large proportion, will not complete
several independent transmission units (Geinisman 2000). maturation and die by apoptosis within the first few
The appearance of perforated synapses maintaining com- weeks (Kempermann and others 2003). Several factors
partmentalized units may also enable more efficient can regulate neurogenesis and influence the destiny of
cycling of receptors in and out of the membrane and the newborn cells, promoting either their survival or death.
shuttling of receptors between the extra-synaptic and These include nearly all neurotransmitters, hormones,
active zones of the synapse (Marrone and Petit 2002). and growth factors, as well as epigenetic factors and
Thus, in all, both the induction of synaptic plasticity and extrinsic/environmental factors such as stress, social iso-
learning can induce remodeling of neural circuits either lation, drug abuse, alcohol consumption, and aging as
through de novo synaptogenesis or a range of morpholog- negative regulators, and physical exercise, environmen-
ical changes that can lead to increased synaptic strength tal enrichment, and learning as positive regulators
and the appearance of new functional units, including (reviews in Abrous and others 2005; Ming and Song
branching spines and multisynaptic boutons, together with 2005).
the functional conversion of silent to active synapses. It is What the contribution of neurogenesis to cognitive
clear, however, that full characterization of these changes functions is and whether it provides another mechanism
after learning in different structures and in relation to dif- of plasticity subserving learning and memory are still a
ferent forms of memory requires further investigation. matter of debate. In a series of studies, Gould and col-
Marrone (2007), conducting a meta-analysis of structural leagues (1999) demonstrated that types of learning that
changes after hippocampal-dependent learning, concluded depend on the integrity of the hippocampus, including

Volume 13, Number 5, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 499

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Fig. 4. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus. Top, an example of BrdU-labeled newly generated cells in the granule cell layer
(gcl) of the dentate gyrus. The birthdating marker BrdU labels mitotically active cells (dark) on sections counterstained with
nuclear fast red to label dentate granule cells (pink). Bottom, Confocal laser microscopic images of immunostained sec-
tions showing co-localization of BrdU (red) with the neuron-specific marker NeuN (green). Bottom images reprinted with
permission from Bruel-Jungerman et al. 2006. Longterm potentiation enhances neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus.
J Neurosci 26:5888–93. Copyright 2006 by the Society for Neuroscience.

spatial learning in the water-maze and trace eye-blink conditions, learning can rescue young neurons from
conditioning, enhance the survival of 1-week-old new- death, perhaps by increasing activity-dependent integra-
born neurons in adult rats, whereas no effect was found tion of immature neurons. Moreover, a recent study ana-
after learning tasks that do not require an intact hip- lyzing learning-induced gene expression in young
pocampus. These findings suggest that learning can res- newborn neurons provided evidence that these neurons
cue young neurons from death. Several studies confirmed are recruited into the circuits involved in spatial memory
the pro-survival effect of hippocampal-dependent learn- (Kee and others 2007).
ing (e.g., Lemaire and others 2000; Leuner and others If neurogenesis plays a role in learning and memory,
2004), but others reported no change or sometimes a then disruption of neurogenesis is likely to result in
decrease in neurogenesis (van Praag and others 2000; impaired cognitive performance. Again here, an influential
Olariu and others 2005; Snyder and others 2005). Reasons study showed that experimental reduction of neurogenesis
for these contrasting results are not fully clarified, in the adult impairs trace eye-blink conditioning but not
although several variables may account for the discrepan- hippocampal-independent delay conditioning (Shors and
cies, including differences in learning paradigms and the others 2001). The deleterious effect of neurogenesis reduc-
degree of physical activity and stress induced, and the age tion or blockade has been observed in several other tasks,
and maturation of the cells at the time of learning (reviewed including contextual fear conditioning, some forms of spa-
in Bruel-Jungerman and others 2007). Overall, however, tial learning, and performance in working memory tasks
it seems reasonable for the time being to conclude that (e.g., Madsen and others 2003; Saxe and others 2006;
certain types of hippocampal-dependent learning can pro- Winocur and others 2006), with the exception of spatial
mote hippocampal neurogenesis and that this is sensitive learning in the water-maze (Shors and others 2002), where
to task difficulty and hippocampal demand. Under these neurogenesis seems to be involved in long-term storage

500 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Brain Plasticity and Memory

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


rather than acquisition of spatial information (Snyder and 2001). Overall, these findings strongly suggest that the
others 2005). Thus, at present it seems that new young addition of new neurons translates into changes in net-
neurons in their first few weeks are an advantage for the work properties, increasing the computational capacity
acquisition and/or the consolidation of long-term memory of the circuits within which they are embedded.
of certain types of learning, in particular, when learning Moreover, newly generated naive neurons appear to be
imposes a high hippocampal demand on a relational mem- particularly suited to respond to, and integrate informa-
ory system for associating events in space and/or time. tion coming from, cortical afferents to the hippocampus,
Several other studies also provide a strong correlative providing a cell population particularly well adapted for
link between hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive recruitment during the formation of new memories.
abilities. For example, housing adult animals in com- Direct evidence also points to another, unsuspected
plex, enriched environments where they have opportuni- link between LTP and neurogenesis: that the occurrence
ties for learning and for interacting socially is known to of LTP enhances the rate of hippocampal neurogenesis
improve learning and memory abilities. This is associ- (Bruel-Jungerman and others 2006). This study showed
ated with increased hippocampal neurogenesis (Fig. 5), that the induction of LTP in the dentate gyrus increases
and the new neurons produced during enrichment were the proliferation of new neurons and results weeks later
shown to be crucial for the improvement of the capacity in a larger population of surviving new neurons.
to store new information in long-term memory (Bruel- Moreover, when LTP impinges on young, recently born
Jungerman and others 2005). Neurogenesis during neurons aged one to two weeks, they have a much higher
enrichment does not seem, however, to be required for probability of survival (Fig. 6). Thus, LTP in the dentate
enrichment-induced facilitation of spatial learning gyrus can both increase the production of new neurons
(Meshi and others 2006), suggesting either a task speci- and promote survival of young neurons. The underlying
ficity or a primary involvement of young neurons in con- mechanisms may involve NMDA receptor activation, as
solidation rather than acquisition of new information. it was shown that (1) excitatory stimuli can act directly
Enrichment also promotes synaptogenesis in various on progenitor cells to favor neuronal production in an
hippocampal areas, including in dentate gyrus and CA3 NMDA receptor–dependent manner (Deisseroth and
target cells (reviewed in Markham and Greenough others 2004) and (2) that, during a critical period soon
2004), an effect that might well be a consequence of the after birth, survival of new neurons is competitively reg-
increased number of surviving, functionally integrated ulated by their own NMDA receptors (Tashiro and oth-
newborn neurons after enrichment. Thus, neurogenesis ers 2006). It is also possible that LTP at surrounding
appears to be another mechanism of plasticity involved mature cells provides a cellular/molecular microenviron-
in memory functions. New neurons may in fact serve ment that favors cell proliferation and the survival of
several functions depending on their birth date and age: recently born neurons, perhaps via the release of neuro-
Those that are coming to the brink of their maturation transmitters and/or growth factors. In any case, the
stage at the time of learning might be directly involved demonstration that LTP promotes neurogenesis presents
in the learning and memory process at hand, whereas the intriguing hypothesis that the occurrence of activity-
more immature ones and those that will mature after dependent synaptic plasticity in the dentate gyrus during
learning may serve as a pool of “naive” neurons that learning may provide signals involved in learning-
could be both beneficial for later learning experiences induced neurogenesis.
and detrimental for remembering past experiences
(Bruel-Jungerman and others 2007).
Conclusion
How can newborn neurons participate to memory
processes? The functional relevance of immature adult- In this brief review, we have outlined four candidate
generated neurons seems to rely on the particular physi- mechanisms of plasticity in the brain that appear to be
ological properties they have, making them particularly essential for forming and retaining memories. Research
inclined to undergo activity-dependent plasticity. over the past decades has clearly established that neural
Several recent reports have highlighted their high circuits in the brain undergo continuous, dynamic mod-
excitability and sensitivity to synaptic plasticity. They ification in relation to learning and memory formation,
show that young dentate gyrus neurons have a lower in the form of synaptic strengthening and weakening,
threshold for LTP induction and produce stable LTP synapse formation and remodeling, and neurogenesis.
more readily than more mature neurons (Snyder and oth- Our current understanding of the role of these mecha-
ers 2001; Saxe and others 2006), presumably due to the nisms of plasticity in the laying down of memories
specific membrane properties observed in one- to three- departs from the early conceptual thinking as alternative
week-old cells, greater NMDA receptor sensitivity and solutions to the problem of the cellular mechanisms of
calcium entry upon synaptic activation (Schmidt-Hieber memory, toward the emerging idea that they act as asso-
and others 2004). As a functional readout of this, neuro- ciate partners, even if the exact functions of each of
genic factors such as environmental enrichment and these mechanisms are not yet fully clarified. Empirical
physical exercise, which promote neurogenesis and data so far have considerably strengthened the case for
improve memory abilities, also facilitate LTP in the den- synaptic strengthening via mechanisms exemplified by
tate gyrus (van Praag and others 2000; Duffy and others LTP as a critical feature of memory consolidation, pro-

Volume 13, Number 5, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 501

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Fig. 5. The contribution of neurogenesis to memory function. A-D, Environmental enrichment increases neurogenesis
in the dentate gyrus, and the antimitotic drug MAM reduces neurogenesis. Images show a larger number of BrdU-
immunostained labeled newly born cells in the dentate gyrus of rats exposed to enriched environments for 3 hours a
day for 14 days (a, c), compared to rats receiving MAM during enrichment (b, d). Bottom, Environmental enrichment
increases the ability of rats to form a long-term memory of objects that they have briefly been exposed to before the
test (red triangles), compared to control rats (open circles), which can form short-term but not long-term recognition
memory. Rats injected with an antimitotic drug to stop neurogenesis during enrichment do not benefit from enrichment
(green triangles). Thus, the birth of new neurons is a crucial plasticity mechanism subserving the improved capacity to
memorize objects. gcl = granule cell layer; sgz = subgranular zone. Adapted from Bruel-Jungerman E, Laroche S,
Rampon C. 2005. New neurons in the dentate gyrus are involved in the expression of enhanced long-term memory fol-
lowing environmental enrichment. Eur J Neurosci 21:513–21.

viding evidence that the type of synaptic change that is foresee in the context of learning and memory in the
brought about by LTP is an essential mechanism to store adult; however, synaptic weakening, a form of “functional
memory representations and leads to retrievable memo- elimination,” also appears critical, if not as a cellular
ries. True synaptic elimination as yet is more difficult to storage mechanisms per se then at least as an essential

502 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Brain Plasticity and Memory

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Fig. 6. Long-term potentiation promotes proliferation and survival of newly born dentate granule neurons. A, The induc-
tion of LTP at perforant path-dentate gyrus synapses increases the proliferation of newly generated cells in the dentate
gyrus. Representative sections immunostained for BrdU (dark) with nuclear fast red counterstaining (pink) were obtained
24 hours after BrdU was injected on day 4 after LTP induction. B, LTP also promotes the survival of young new neurons
born one or two weeks before LTP induction. In this case, BrdU was injected and LTP induced 2 weeks later.
Immunostained sections were obtained four weeks after BrdU injection. C, A significant fraction of these young neu-
rons express the IEG egr1 (zif268) in response to LTP. Confocal laser microscopic images of double-immunostaining for
BrdU (red) and Zif268 (green) 2 hours after LTP induction. Reprinted with permission from Bruel-Jungerman et al. 2006.
Long-term potentiation enhances neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. J Neurosci 26:5888–93. Copyright 2006 by
the Society for Neuroscience.

homeostatic mechanism for fine-tuning of neural con- The evidence as broadly outlined here suggests these
nectivity. Similarly, important advances have been made four mechanisms of plasticity are not completely inde-
in strengthening the idea that synaptogenesis, morpholog- pendent. Synaptic strengthening, demonstrated by LTP,
ical and functional synapse remodeling, and neurogenesis is often associated with LTD at neighboring synapses;
in certain brain regions are key mechanisms underlying LTD modifies the capacity for LTP, and LTP modifies
structural shaping of neural connectivity in the process of the capacity for LTD. The induction of LTP progres-
memory stabilization. sively induces functional and structural reorganization of

Volume 13, Number 5, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 503

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


synapses and synaptic growth, whereas LTD can induce Bruel-Jungerman E, Davis S, Rampon C, Laroche S. 2006. Long-term
shrinkage of synapses. Finally, the activity-dependent potentiation enhances neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. J
Neurosci 26:5888–93.
maturation and stabilization of new neurons during adult Bruel-Jungerman E, Laroche S, Rampon C. 2005. New neurons in the
neurogenesis, associated with synaptic outgrowth, results dentate gyrus are involved in the expression of enhanced long-term
in an increased capacity for LTP, and LTP itself promotes memory following environmental enrichment. Eur J Neurosci 21:
proliferation and survival of newborn neurons. Thus, 513–21.
Bruel-Jungerman E, Rampon C, Laroche S. 2007. Adult hippocampal
these four types of plasticity appear to act as partners in neurogenesis, synaptic plasticity and memory: facts and hypothe-
a multifaceted mechanism to create specific neural net- ses. Rev Neurosci. In press.
works during the laying down of memories, even if they Collingridge GL, Isaac JT, Wang YT. 2004. Receptor trafficking and
may retain specific, independent functional contributions synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:952–62.
to memory function, in particular, in relation to the tem- Davis S, Butcher SP, Morris RGM. 1992. The NMDA receptor antag-
onist, D-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoate (D-AP5) impairs spatial
poral dynamics of memory processes. Importantly, if the learning and LTP in vivo at intracerebral concentrations compara-
four types of plasticity are implicated in memory func- ble to those that block LTP in vitro. J Neurosci 12:21–34.
tion, a high degree of structure specificity in their Davis S, Laroche S. 2006. MAP kinase/ERK signalling and memory
involvement is apparent. The clearest example is with stabilisation: a review. Gene Brain Behav 5:61–72.
De Zeeuw CI, Yeo CH. 2005. Time and tide in cerebellar memory for-
adult neurogenesis, which under normal conditions is mation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:667–74.
restricted to only a few brain structures and therefore Deisseroth K, Singla S, Toda H, Monje M, Palmer TD, Malenka RC.
cannot be regarded as a general “learning device.” Other 2004. Excitation-neurogenesis coupling in adult neural stem/
types of structure specificities have been outlined, for progenitor cells. Neuron 42:535–52.
instance, the apparent higher propensity of cortical Doyère V, Errington ML, Laroche S, Bliss TVP. 1996. Low-frequency
trains of paired stimuli induce long-term depression in area CA1
regions to undergo learning-dependent synaptogenesis. but not in dentate gyrus of the intact rat. Hippocampus 6:52–7.
Key issues for the future are now to define precisely in Doyère V, Srebro B, Laroche S. 1997. Heterosynaptic LTD and depo-
which condition, phase, or process of learning and mem- tentiation in the medial perforant path of the dentate gyrus in the
ory these four types of brain plasticity are involved and freely moving rat. J Neurophysiol 77:571–8.
Dudek SM, Bear MF. 1993. Bidirectional long-term modification of
which structures and neural circuits express these mech- synaptic effectiveness in the adult and immature hippocampus. J
anisms in relation to different types of memories. As a Neurosci 13:2910–8.
final note, the contribution of these four types of plastic- Duffy SN, Craddock KJ, Abel T, Nguyen PV. 2001. Environmental
ity to memory function is further highlighted by the enrichment modifies the PKA-dependence of hippocampal LTP and
emerging evidence that dysfunctional mechanisms of improves hippocampus-dependent memory. Learn Mem 8:26–34.
Eriksson PS, Perfilieva E, Bjork-Eriksson T, Alborn AM, Nordborg C,
plasticity are, in large part, responsible for deficient Peterson DA, Gage FH. 1998. Neurogenesis in the adult human
memory that occurs with aging and for a wide range of hippocampus. Nat Med 4:1313–17.
pathological disorders including neurodegenerative, neu- Etkin A, Alarcon JM, Weisberg SP, Touzani K, Huang YY, Nordheim A,
rological, and psychiatric diseases. and others. 2006. A role in learning for SRF: deletion in the adult
forebrain disrupts LTD and the formation of an immediate memory
of a novel context. Neuron 50:127–43.
References Fonseca R, Nagerl UV, Morris RGM, Bonhoeffer T. 2004. Competing
for memory: hippocampal LTP under regimes of reduced protein
Abrous DN, Koehl M, Le Moal M. 2005. Adult neurogenesis: from synthesis. Neuron 44:1011–20.
precursors to network and physiology. Physiol Rev 85:523–69. Frey U, Morris RGM. 1997. Synaptic tagging and long-term potentia-
Alberini CM. 2005. Mechanisms of memory stabilization: are consol- tion. Nature 385:533–6.
idation and reconsolidation similar or distinct processes? Trends Geinisman Y. 2000. Structural synaptic modifications associated with hip-
Neurosci 28:51–6. pocampal LTP and behavioral learning. Cereb Cortex 10:952–62.
Altman J. 1962. Are new neurons formed in the brains of adult mam- Gould E, Beylin A, Tanapat P, Reeves A, Shors TJ. 1999. Learning
mals? Science 135:1127–8. enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nat
Atkins CM, Selcher JC, Petraitis JJ, Tzaskos JM, Sweatt JD. 1998. The Neurosci 2:260–5.
MAPK cascade is required for mammalian associative learning. Gross CG. 2000. Neurogenesis in the adult brain: death of a dogma.
Nature Neurosci 7:602–9. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:67–73.
Beattie EC, Carroll RC, Yu X, Morishita W, Yasuda H, von Zastrow M, Gruart A, Munoz MD, Delgado-Garcia JM. 2006. Involvement of the
and others. 2000. Regulation of AMPA receptor endocytosis by a CA3-CA1 synapse in the acquisition of associative learning in
signaling mechanism shared with LTD. Nat Neurosci 3: behaving mice. J Neurosci 26:1077–87.
1291–300. Harris KM, Fiala JC, Ostroff L. 2003. Structural changes at dendritic
Bliss TVP, Collingridge GL. 1993. A synaptic model of memory: long- spine synapses during long-term potentiation. Philos Trans R Soc
term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361:31–9. Lond B Biol Sci 358:745–8.
Bliss TVP, Collingridge GL, Laroche S. 2006. ZAP and ZIP, a story to Hebb DO. 1949. The organisation of behaviour. New York: John
forget. Science 313:1058–9. Wiley.
Bliss TVP, Lømo T. 1973. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic Isaac JTR, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC. 1995. Evidence for silent
transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit fol- synapses: implications for the expression of LTP. Neuron
lowing stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol (Lond) 232: 15:427–34.
331–56. Jones MW, Errington ML, French PJ, Fine A, Bliss TVP, Garel S, and
Bock J, Braun K. 1999. Blockade of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor others. 2001. A requirement for the immediate early gene Zif268
activation suppresses learning-induced synaptic elimination. Proc in the expression of late LTP and the consolidation of long-term
Natl Acad Sci U S A 96:2485–90. memories. Nat Neurosci 4:289–96.
Bozon B, Kelly A, Josselyn SA, Silva AJ, Davis S, Laroche S. 2003. Kee N, Teixeira CM, Wang AH, Frankland PW. 2007. Preferential
MAPK, CREB and zif268 are all required for the consolidation of incorporation of adult-generated granule cells into spatial memory
recognition memory. Phil Trans R Soc B 358:805–14. networks in the dentate gyrus. Nat Neurosci 10:355–62.

504 THE NEUROSCIENTIST Brain Plasticity and Memory

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013


Kemp A, Manahan-Vaughan D. 2007. Hippocampal long-term depres- Rioult-Pedotti MS, Friedman D, Hess G, Donoghue JP. 1998.
sion: master or minion in declarative memory processes? Trends Strengthening of horizontal cortical connections following skill
Neurosci 30:111–18. learning. Nat Neurosci 1:230–4.
Kempermann G, Gast D, Kronenberg G, Yamaguchi M, Gage FH. 2003. Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R. 2005. Postsynaptic recep-
Early determination and long-term persistence of adult-generated new tor trafficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science
neurons in the hippocampus of mice. Development 130:391–9. 308:83–8.
Lee JL, Everitt BJ, Thomas KL. 2004. Independent cellular processes Rusakov DA, Davies HA, Harrison E, Diana G, Richter-Levin G, Bliss
for hippocampal memory consolidation and reconsolidation. TVP, and others. 1997. Ultrastructure synaptic correlates of spatial
Science 304:839–43. learning in rat hippocampus. Neuroscience 80:69–77.
Lemaire V, Koehl M, Le Moal M, Abrous DN. 2000. Prenatal stress pro- Saxe MD, Battaglia F, Wang JW, Malleret G, David DJ, Monckton JE,
duces learning deficits associated with an inhibition of neurogenesis and others. 2006. Ablation of hippocampal neurogenesis impairs
in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:11032–7. contextual fear conditioning and synaptic plasticity in the dentate
Leuner B, Falduto J, Shors TJ. 2003. Associative memory formation gyrus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:17501–6.
increases the observation of dendritic spines in the hippocampus. J Schmidt-Hieber C, Jonas P, Bischofberger J. 2004. Enhanced synaptic
Neurosci 23:659–65. plasticity in newly generated granule cells of the adult hippocam-
Leuner B, Mendolia-Loffredo S, Kozorovitskiy Y, Samburg D, Gould pus. Nature 429:184–7.
E, Shors TJ. 2004. Learning enhances the survival of new neurons Selcher JC, Weeber EJ, Varga AW, Sweatt JD, Swank M. 2002. Protein
beyond the time when the hippocampus is required for memory. J kinase signal transduction cascades in mammalian associative con-
Neurosci 24:7477–81. ditioning. Neuroscientist 8:122–31.
Lewis DJ. 1979. Psychobiology of active and inactive memory. Shors TJ, Miesegaes G, Beylin A, Zhao M, Rydel T, Gould E. 2001.
Psychol Bull 86:1054–83. Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace
Liao D, Hessler NA, Malinow R. 1995. Activation of postsynaptically memories. Nature 410:372–6.
silent synapses during pairing-induced LTP in CA1 region of Shors TJ, Townsend DA, Zhao M, Kozorovitskiy Y, Gould E. 2002.
hippocampal slice. Nature 375:400–4. Neurogenesis may relate to some but not all types of hippocampal-
Luscher C, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC, Muller D. 2000. Synaptic plas- dependent learning. Hippocampus 12:578–84.
ticity and dynamic modulation of the postsynaptic membrane. Nat Snyder JS, Hong NS, McDonald RJ, Wojtowicz JM. 2005. A role for
Neurosci 3:545–50. adult neurogenesis in spatial long-term memory. Neuroscience
Madsen TM, Kristjansen PE, Bolwig TG, Wortwein G. 2003. Arrested 130:843–52.
neuronal proliferation and impaired hippocampal function follow- Snyder JS, Kee N, Wojtowicz JM. 2001. Effects of adult neurogenesis
ing fractionated brain irradiation in the adult rat. Neuroscience on synaptic plasticity in the rat dentate gyrus. J Neurophysiol
119:635–42. 85:2423–31.
Markham J, Greenough WT. 2004. Experience-driven brain plasticity: Soderling TR, Derkach VA. 2000. Postsynaptic protein phosphoryla-
beyond the synapse. Neuron Glia Biol 1:351–63. tion and LTP. Trends Neurosci 23:75–80.
Marrone DF. 2007. Ultrastructural plasticity associated with hippocampal- Sweatt JD. 2001. The neuronal MAP kinase cascade: a biochemical
dependent learning: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol Learn Mem 87: signal integration system subserving synaptic plasticity and mem-
361–71. ory. J Neurochem 76:1–10.
Marrone DF, Petit TL. 2002. The role of synaptic morphology in neu- Tashiro A, Sandler VM, Toni N, Zhao C, Gage FH. 2006. NMDA-
ral plasticity: structural interactions underlying synaptic power. receptor-mediated, cell-specific integration of new neurons in adult
Brain Res Rev 38:291–308. dentate gyrus. Nature 442:929–33.
Martin SJ, Grimwood PD, Morris RGM. 2000. Synaptic plasticity and Thiels E, Barrionuevo G, Berger TW. 1994. Excitatory stimulation
memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci during postsynaptic inhibition induces long-term depression in
23:649–711. hippocampus in vivo. J Neurophysiol 72:3009–16.
Meshi D, Drew MR, Saxe M, Ansorge MS, David D, Santarelli L, and Tsien JZ, Huerta PT, Tonegawa S. 1996. The essential role of hip-
others. 2006. Hippocampal neurogenesis is not required for behav- pocampal CA1 NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity in
ioral effects of environmental enrichment. Nat Neurosci 9:729–31. spatial memory. Cell 87:1327–38.
Ming GL, Song H. 2005. Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian cen- Turrigiano GG, Leslie KR, Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Nelson SB.
tral nervous system. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:223–50. 1998. Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocorti-
Moser EI, Krobert KA, Moser MB, Morris RGM. 1998. Impaired spa- cal neurons. Nature 391:892–6.
tial learning after saturation of long-term potentiation. Science van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. 2000. Neural consequences
281:2038–42. of environmental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:191–8.
Olariu A, Cleaver KM, Shore LE, Brewer MD, Cameron HA. 2005. A Waites CL, Craig AM, Garner CC. 2005. Mechanisms of vertebrate
natural form of learning can increase and decrease the survival of synaptogenesis. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:251–74.
new neurons in the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 15:750–62. Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, Bear MF. 2006. Learning
Pastalkova E, Serrano P, Pinkhasova D, Wallace E, Fenton AA, Sacktor induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science
TC. 2006. Storage of spatial information by the maintenance 313:1093–7.
mechanism of LTP. Science 313:1141–4. Winocur G, Wojtowicz JM, Sekeres M, Snyder JS, Wang S. 2006.
Perez-Otano I, Ehlers MD. 2005. Homeostatic plasticity and NMDA Inhibition of neurogenesis interferes with hippocampus-dependent
receptor trafficking. Trends Neurosci 28:229–38. memory function. Hippocampus 16:296–304.
Plath N, Ohana O, Dammermann B, Errington ML, Schmitz D, Gross Yashiro K, Corlew R, Philpot BD. 2005. Visual deprivation modifies both
C, and others. 2006. Arc/Arg3.1 is essential for the consolidation presynaptic glutamate release and the composition of perisynaptic/
of synaptic plasticity and memories. Neuron 52:437–44. extrasynaptic NMDA receptors in adult visual cortex. J Neurosci
Radley JJ, Johnson LR, Janssen WG, Martino J, Lamprecht R, Hof PR, 25:11684–92.
and others. 2006. Associative Pavlovian conditioning leads to an Zhou Q, Homma KJ, Poo MM. 2004. Shrinkage of dendritic spines
increase in spinophilin-immunoreactive dendritic spines in the lat- associated with long-term depression of hippocampal synapses.
eral amygdala. Eur J Neurosci 24:876–84. Neuron 44:749–57.

Volume 13, Number 5, 2007 THE NEUROSCIENTIST 505

Downloaded from nro.sagepub.com at Universitats-Landesbibliothek on December 22, 2013

You might also like