You are on page 1of 7

Physics Education

Related content
- The Force Concept Inventory
Students' concept of force: the importance of Antti Savinainen and Philip Scott

understanding Newton's third law - Swirling tea leaves: problem and solution
Guy S M Moore

- College students' conceptions about


To cite this article: David E Brown 1989 Phys. Educ. 24 353 vector kinematics
Jose M Aguirre and Graham Rankin

View the article online for updates and enhancements. Recent citations
- Misconception of sound and conceptual
change: A cross sectional study on
students’ materialistic thinking of sound
Haim Eshach et al

- Pasi Nieminen et al

- The Effect of Using a Visual


Representation Tool in a Teaching-
Learning Sequence for Teaching Newton’s
Third Law
Antti Savinainen et al

This content was downloaded from IP address 164.73.224.2 on 12/05/2018 at 00:33


Phys. Educ. 24 11989). Printed In the UK

Students’ concept of force: the


importance of understanding
Newton’s third law
David E Brown

Data indicate that students have a naive view of materials, and (iii) a multiple choice diagnostic test.
force as a property ofobjects.Thissuggests The oral tutoring study involved a small sample of
that Newton‘s third law, which makes explicit five high-school students who had not yet taken
the relational quality of forces, may need to be physics.
stressed in teaching physics. In the tutoring study (described in more detail in
Brown and Clement (1987b) and Brown (1987)), 21
A number of studies conducted in recent years have pre-physics high-school students were given written
demonstrated a wide range of beliefs about physical explanations of why a table exerts a force upward on
phenomena which students have apparently formed abook resting on the table, with pre-and post-
on their own without the benefit of formal instruc- questions to assess the effect of the explanations.
tion. Most of thestudenterrorsthat have been The multiple choice diagnostic test was adminis-
documented in Newtonian mechanics have been on tered to seven physics classes in two high schools
questions designed to test conceptual understanding (see Brown and Clement (1987a) for the complete
of Newton’s first or second laws. Only a few (e.g. test).The science curriculum in the schools was
Maloney 1984, Terry and Jones 1986) treat the third typical for the United States with students taking
law. Thisemphasis on the first two of Newton’s physics generally in their final year of secondary
three laws is in keeping with the emphasis placed on education (17-18 years of age) following chemistry.
the first two laws in many American textbooks. All of the questions concerned the concept of force
By contrast, many textbooks treat the third law in in various contexts,and the majority could be
passing, either simply mentioning it briefly as an answered using a basic knowledge of Newton’s third
unsupported statement of fact or as an addendum to law. The test was administered at the beginning of
the section covering conservation of momentum. the year and again after all instruction in mechanics
The results of this study indicate that this type of had been completed in order to assess gains from
treatment is insufficient to counter the misconcep- instruction (teachers were not aware of the contents
tions students hold about the third law. This might of the test). Scores are reported only for students
be a small concern if the third law is in fact only an who took both the pre-course and post-course tests
insignificant piece of the Newtonian picture, but in (a total of 78 students).
this paper it is argued that the third law should be
treated as a much more significant part of an intro-
ductory physics course since it is important for Results
developing the students’ qualitative
concept of This section examines thedata from the studies
force. which converge on a general naive view of force as a
property of single objects (objects ‘having’ more or
Method less force and thus being more or less ‘force-full’).
The data in this paper come from three sources: (i) This is opposed to the Newtonian view of pairs of
an interviewing study involving oral tutoring, (ii) an forces arising from interactions between objects.
interviewing study involving tutoring with written
Interviewingstudyinvolving oral tutoring. Inthe
oral tutoring study, every one of the five students
David Brown is a Postdoctoral Research Associate in
the Physics Department, University of answered withhigh confidence that a moving cue
Massachusetts. He obtained his doctorate in Science ball exerts a greaterforce than the stationary billiard
Education in 1987. ball when they collide. In every case, the justifica-
0031-9120/89/060353+06 $02.50 0 1989 IOP Publlshlng Ltd 353
tion given for this conclusion was that the moving S4: I think it [the 40 pound block B] exerts a force
ball ‘had’more force, andthusexerteda greater up, but I don’t think it exerts enough to stopA [the
force in the collision. An example is given below: 200 poundupper block] from pushing B into the
S1: I think that the moving object has more force
ground. See,it just makes the thing slower. So say B
. . . I think the cue ball has greater force when they only weighed one pound, then A would have 199
collide because the [billiard] ball is then moved. pounds more than B would, and so it would push it
into the ground faster.
Three of the five students discussed the moving ball
This next student also felt the weight of each block
not only as ‘having’ force, but also as able totransfer
determined the force it could exert.
that force during the collision. For example:
S3: The force from the moving ball would be truns-
S5: I would say that A and B exert a force on each
other, but A exerts a largerforce, more weight, and
ferred tothe stationaryball, so the force would
covers the entire face of this box, with 200pounds of
move from the moving ball to the ball that wasn’t
pressure which is 160 more pounds pushing down on
moving.
the box.
These answers were given after a full page explana-
Interviewingstudyinvolvingtutoringwithwritten tion of Newton’s third lawwhich gave numerous
materials. Such a conception of force, as a property examples from everyday experience, yet none of the
of single objects rather than as arising from an students made even the slightest reference to the
interaction, can be observed in problems involving third law in their explanations.
static situations as well, as illustrated by the follow-
ing transcript segments fromthe tutoring study using Multiplechoice
diagnostic
test. That problems
written materials. In the steel blocks problem, a 200 involving the third law are difficult for studentseven
pound (90 kg) steel block (block A) rests on topof a after instruction was borne out by the results of the
40 pound steel block (block B), and the student is diagnostic test. For the 78 students tested, the thir-
asked to compare the force A exerts on B with the teen third law questionswere answered correctly
force B exerts on A (see figure l(d)). The following only 44% of the time on the post-test, a 21% gain
student expressed a belief that since theheavier from the average pre-test score of 23%. The data in
block ‘had’ more force, not only would it exert a the multiple choice diagnostic study also support the
greater force, it would push the lighter block into view that many students adopt a concept of force as
the ground. an innate oracquired property of objects rather than

140 kg
A B
lo) Stockcars
A B
Q Q lb) Stationaryboxes

A B

( c ) O f f l c e chairs

n Id) Steelblocks

4 L - l
280 g

I f ) Pulllng block A Figure 1. Problems


l e ) Bowler
( f r o m Maloney 19041 drawing out a ’force-
fullness’ conception.
as arising from an interaction between objects. Six Table 2. Percentage of a / / answers (correct and
problems in particular on the diagnostic would tend incorrect) consistent with a concept of force as a
to draw out this conception of force (see figure 1).In property of objects (n= 78).
each of theseproblems, there is an object which Pre-test (“/o) Post-test (“/o)
is moreor less unambiguously stronger, faster
heavier, more acting as an agent of causation than Stock cars 46 58
the other object, orsome combination of the above Stationary boxes 54 46
53 Office chairs 77
(cf Maloney 1984). Students with a concept of force Steel blocks 78 40
as an innate or acquired property of objects would be Bowler 99 94
expected to answer thatthe heavier, faster,etc,46 Pulling block A 51
object (the object which ‘has’ moreforce) would
exert the greaterforce, while the otherobject would Total 67 56
exert either a lesser force or no force at all. Each of
these problems had six answer choices which were
answers for this problem. On both the pre-test and
slight variations on the following: (1) Aexertsa
the post-test, of the students answering incorrectly,
greater force, ( 2 ) B exerts a greater force, (3) the
in all cases the answers consistent with a ‘force-
forces are equal, (4) only A exerts a force, (5) only
fullness’ conception of forcerepresented at least
B exerts a force, and (6) neither exerts a force. The
50% of the incorrect answers and for most problems
one exception is Pulling block A, which had only
this percentage was much higher. If students were
choices 1-3.
drawn equally to each of the distractors, one would
Asan example consider the bowler problem
expect that the two answers consistent with the view
(figure l(e)) in which the student is asked to com-
of force as property would claim only about 40% of
pare the force a 16 pound bowling ball exerts on a 4
the incorrect answers. As can be seen from the totals
pound pin when the ball strikes the pin. In this case
in table 3, 79% of the incorrect answers on the pre-
the student might consider the bowling ball to be
test and 85% of the incorrect answers on the post-
clearly more ‘force-full’ because it is moving, itis
test were of this form. This provides strong evidence
heavier, and it is more able to cause damage than
thatthe answers consistent with a ‘force-fullness’
the pin. The two answers (of the five possible incor-
view of force were quite compelling for students.
rect answers) consistent with this view of force are
Particularly striking are the results of the bowler
that the bowling ball exerts a greater force than the
problem. In this problemstudents might tendto
pin or thatthe ball exerts a forcewhile the pin exerts
think the bowling ball ‘has’ more force than the pin
no force at all.
since it is both heavier and it is moving. This would
Table 1 shows the percentage of students answer-
translate into answering that the bowling ball exerts
ing correctly for each of these six problems. Table 2
a greater force than the pin, which exerts a lesser
shows the overallpercentage of students giving
force or noforce at all. All incorrect answers (except
answers consistent with a ‘force-fullness’ view of
for a single response on the post-test) were of this
force for these six problems. Table 3 shows what
type for this problem on both the pre-test and the
percentage of incorrect answers the ‘force-fullness’
post-test. That a ‘force-fullness’ conception is so
answers represent. Thus, for example, table 2 shows
compelling to students onthis problem may explain
that onthepre-test, 46% of the students gave
its exceptional difficulty, as only 5% answered the
answersfor the stock cars problem which were
question correctly after a full year of traditional high
consistent with the view of force as an innate or
school physics instruction. Thus the diagnostic test
acquired property. Table 3 shows that these particu-
lar incorrect answers represent 56% of all incorrect data support the hypothesis that the great majority

Table 3. Percentage of incorrect answers consistent


Table 1. Percentage correct for the six ’force-fullness’ with a concept of force as a property of objects
problems ( n= 78). ( n = 78).

Pre-test (YO) Post-test (Yo) Pre-test (YO) Post-test (Yo)


Stock cars 18 27 Stock cars 56 79
Stationary boxes 10 32 Stationary boxes 60 68
Office
17 chairs 44 Office chairs 92 93
Steel blocks 12 51 Steel blocks 88 82
Bowler 1 5 Bowler 100 99
Pulling block A 29 47 85 Pulling block A 71
Total 14 33 Total 79 85
of students have a conception of force as a property epigrammatic version ‘for every action there is an
of objects, as the great majority gave answers con- equal and opposite reaction’.
sistent with this conception. Further, traditional
instruction seems to have had a disappointingly low Students’ view: force as an innate or acquired pro-
impact on this conception for these students. perty. The concept of force as embodied in the third
law and developed above does not seem to be the
Students’ concept of force: the importance of naive conception of force which most high-school
the third law students hold. Minstrell and Stimpson (1986) have
The above results indicate that thelow scores on the proposed that in addition to viewing forces as pushes
post-test of the multiple choice diagnostic may not or pulls, students treat force as aproperty of
imply simply a failure to remember a verbal state- objects. Such student reasoninghas also been
ment of the third law but rather may indicatea observed instudies at the University of Surrey
failure at adeeper conceptual level. This section will (Watts 1983) and at the University of Massachusetts
further explore the non-Newtonian ‘force-fullness’ (Brown and Clement 1987b, Brown 1987). Minstrell
view of force and examine the consequences of such and Stimpson (1986) list such factors as an object’s
a view. It is argued that if students can gain a deep weight, motion, activity or strength as important to
conceptual graspof Newton’s third law, they are in a students in determining an object’s force. Maloney
much better position to answer both qualitative and (1984) discusses various properties such as these
quantitative questions involving forces, which contribute to the students’ decision of which
object exerts the greater force.
The Newtonian view: forces arise from interactions. A student holding the view of force as a property
Beforeproceeding to a further discussion of stu- might considerheavy,
a fast-moving, strong
dents’ conceptions of force, it it helpful to consider Americanfootball player to have agreatdeal of
the Newtonian view. There are at least five ideas ‘force-fullness’. This football player would be able
important in a careful considerationof the third law to exertmore force onother people or objects
in classical mechanics which are elaborated below: ‘having’ less force than they would be able to exert
back. This is in directcontrast to the Newtonian
(i) A body cannot experience a force in isolation. concept in which a force does not exist except as
There cannot be a force on a body A without a arising from the interaction of two objects, the
second body B to exert the force. forces on eachobject being equivalent in magni-
(ii) Closely related to the above point is the fact tude.
that A cannot exert a force in isolation. A cannot
exert a force unless there is another body B to exert Consequencesofthe viewof force as a property.
aforce onA. We then say that AandB are There is some reason to believe that a naive view of
interacting. (Thus, for example, it is incorrect to say force as a property sabotagesbothconceptual
that an astronaut punching empty space with his fist understanding and quantitative problemsolving abi-
is exerting a force since there is nothing exerting a lity. In thearea of qualitativeconceptualunder-
force back against his fist.) The attractive or repul- standing,a well known failure of students to
sive force between two bodies arises as a result of consider forces as arising from interactions comes
the action of the two bodies on each other because from their attempts toanswer questions which draw
they are eitherincontact or experiencebetween out an ‘impetus’ conception-objects ‘having’ force
them a force acting at a distance. which keeps them in motion (Clement 1982,
(iii) At all moments of time the force A exerts on Fischbein et a1 1987).
B isof exactly the same magnitude as the force B Hellerand Reif (1984) presentevidence that if
exerts on A. students treated forces as arising only from interac-
(iv) An important implication of the above point tions, this would also aid quantitative problem solv-
is that neither force precedes the other force. Even ing. In this study, university physics students were
though one body might bemore ‘active’ than the given guidance in constructing accurate motion and
other body andthus might seem to initiate the force descriptions while solving problems.Those
interaction (e.g. a bowling ball striking a pin), the given guidance arrived at the correct final answer to
force body A exerts on body B isalways simulta- the quantitative problems 92% of the time, versus
neous with the force B exerts on A. only 21% of the time for the control group given no
(v) In the interaction of A with B, the force A guidance. Since much of the guidance in construct-
exerts on B is in a direction exactly opposite to the ing forcedescriptionscentredaround considering
direction of the force which B exerts on A. forces as interactions, this indicates that such a
Thus, the third law can be seen to be much more conception of force would aid problem solving if
involved than one might assume from the simple internalised by students.
356
The importance of the third law. Since the concept between two objects rather than force as an innate
of forces arising from interactions between objectsis or acquired property of single objects.
at the heart of the third law, it seems from these
considerations that acareful andextendedtreat- Conclusion
ment of Newton’s third law may be quite important The results of the pre-course diagnostic indicate that
in an introductory physics course. In teaching high-school students enter physics classes with pre-
Newton’s laws, nowhere else does such a conception conceptions in thearea of Newton’s third law.
of force play an integral role. The first law is con- Evidence from the post-course diagnostic indicates
cerned with the motion of single objects in the that these preconceptions are persistent and difficult
absence of net forces. The second law is concerned to overcome with traditionalinstructionaltech-
with the motion of single objects under the influence niques. The data from all three studies support the
of forces of often unknown origin. hypothesis thatthe persistence of preconceptions
It is in studying the third law that students must concerning the third law may result from students’
come to grips with the conception of forces arising general naive view of force as a property of single
solely from interactions. If students acquire a deep objects rather than as a relation between objects.
understanding of the third law, they might be less It has been arguedthat theconsequences of such a
apt to have difficulty with both quantitative prob- view of force extendbeyondproblems explicitly
lems (as demonstratedby the Heller and Reif study) treating the third law to all problems, both quantita-
and qualitative problems such as those drawing out tive and qualitative, which deal with forces.This
the ‘impetus’ misconception in which force is viewed suggests that ideas concerning the third law, which
as a properfy of a moving object causing it to keep makes explicit the relational quality of forces, may
moving. play a more importantrole than is ordinarily granted
in teaching. Helping studentsdevelopa mature
A simple solution? A possible solution is to simply conception of force will undoubtedly involve an
re-label students’ naive concept of force by calling it, extended and multi-pronged approach, but innova-
for example, ‘momentum’ or ‘kinetic energy’. Both tive strategiesfor teaching the third law should
of these can be considered properties of an object, comprise a significant part of the unit on forces and
both do depend on the object’s mass and speed, and Newton’s laws ratherthan receiving the cursory
both can be transferred, in whole or in part, in a treatment which the third law has usually been
collision. But this is not a satisfactory solution for at afforded.
least three reasons: (i) neither momentum nor kine-
tic energy cause motion (as many students view
force causing motion), they are simply properties of References
a moving object arising as a result of the motion of Brown D 1987 Using analogies and examples to help
that object. (ii) Momentum and kinetic energy vary students overcome misconceptions in physics: a
comparison of two teaching strategies EdD thesis
with the frame of reference. If a student were to University of Massachusetts
simply re-label her conception of force to be, for Brown D and Clement J 1987a Misconceptions
example, momentum, she might well ask how an concerning Newton’s law of action and reaction: the
object could have a lot of force (or strength or underestimated importance of the third law Proc. 2nd
forcefulness) in one frame of reference and none Int. Seminar, Misconceptions and Educational
Strategies in Science and Mathematics vol. I11 ed.
from anotherperspective. (iii) If a student is encour- J D Novak pp 39-53
aged to equate momentum with her naive concep- Brown D and Clement J 1987b Overcoming
tion of force, she is likely to add momenta to forces misconceptions in mechanics: a comparison of two
in solving problems or be confused about why it is example-based teaching strategies Conf. American
improper to do so. Educational Research Association (Washington, DC)
Clement J 1982 Students’ preconceptions in introductory
mechanics A m . J . Phys. 50 66-71
Conceptualchange
necessary. For the above Fischbein E, Stavy R and Ma-Naim H 1987 The
reasons, re-labelling the student’s naive concept of psychological structure of naive impetus conceptions
force is not a satisfactory solution to the problem of Proc. 2nd Int. Seminar, Misconceptions and
the naive view of force as aproperty and may lead to Educational Strategies in Science and Mathematics vol.
111 ed. J D Novak pp 143-51
even greater confusion (how many times have stu- Heller J I and Reif F 1984 Prescribing effective human
dents used the words ‘the force of momentum’ in a problem-solving processes: problem description in
physics class?). What is necessary is a modification physics Cognition and Instruction 1 177-216
of the concept itself. This modified conception of Maloney D P 1984 Rule-governed approaches to
physics-Newton’s third law Phys. Educ. 19 37-42
force should involve a deep understanding (rather Minstrel1 J and Stimpson G 1986 Students’ beliefs in
than a mere memorisation) of the third law, that is, mechanics: cognitive process frameworks Proc. 5th
a concept of forces arising only from interactions Conf. on Reasoning and Higher Education (Boise, ID)

357
Terry C and Jones G 1986 Alternative frameworks: Watts D M 1983 A study of schoolchildren’s alternative
Newton’s third law and conceptual change Eur. J . Sci. frameworks of the concept of force Eur. J . Sci. Educ.
Educ. 8 291-8 5 217-30
~ ~~

Solution: swirling tea leaves


(Problem on page 338)
This phenomenon arises from the viscosity of the is then directed upwards into the core. This
water. Next to the wall of the vessel, including the secondary flow is similar to that induced in
bottom, exists a ‘boundary layer’ in which the rotating blinding holes recently illustrated by the
velocity of the water increases from zero at the axial flow of gas induced in a tubular aircraft
wall to a value corresponding to the outer surface propeller shaft; warm gas travelling along the shaft
of a ‘core’ of water which rotates approximately can transmit enough heat to prevent the nose cone
freely within. After some time, the core rotates from icing (Owen 1987).
like a solid body with a constant angular velocity. If both the vessel and water rotate at the same
The free water surface of the core assumes the angular velocity the tea leaves are thrown towards
shape of a paraboloid of revolution, deepest at the the wall but they return to the centre if the vessel
centre and rising with increasing radius. This is held stationary. This may easily be verified
provides the radial pressure gradient necessary to experimentally.
maintain the circular motion of all annuli of water Other methods of study include the use of
beneath, analysed mathematically in the figure. aluminium powder, with a little detergent to aid
(The centre of the surface is the origin for y . ) wetting, illuminated perpendicularly to the viewing
Within the boundary layer near the bottom of the direction. Interest in this kind of problem arises
vessel, the angular velocity is less than within the from such considerations as the effect of the lunar
core above. But the pressure, due to the weight of gravitational torque acting on the liquid core of
water above, is greater towards the vessel walls the Earth, the formation of the red spot on Jupiter
and is least in the centre like a cyclone. The radial or the deposition of silt in river bends. Baker
pressure gradient is thus larger than that required (1966, 1968) provides a number of excellent
to maintain the reduced circular motion and causes laboratory methods for further study and cites the
the water to spiral inwards, transporting the heavy effect as being responsible for propelling a
tea leaves to the centre of the bottom. The poached egg to the centreof the bottom of a
velocity vectors at various depths are illustrated swirling pan of boiling water. The theory of
hypothetically in the sections. vorticity is perhaps best approached by a standard
When the leaves have been deposited the water textbook such as that of Massey (1983)”under
some conditions it is possible to observe an
increase in angular velocity with decreasing radius
like a tornado. The latter conditions can be
obtained by stirring a bucket of hot water (of
reduced viscosity) with a spoon at its perimeter
when floating objects dropped into the centre
rotate with a greater angular velocity. This tea-leaf
problem provides just a fleeting glimpse of a vast
subject, described in more detail by Drazin (1987).
Discussions with Dr John Hougtonand D r Keith
Scrimshaw are gratefully acknowledged.
Guy S M Moore
References
Baker D J Jr 1966 Demonstrations of fluid flow in a
rotating system, part 1 Am. J . Phys. 34, 647-52
-1968 Demonstrations of fluid flow in a rotating
system, part 2 Am. J . Phys. 36 980-6
Drazin P 1987 Fluid Mechanics Phys. Educ. 22 350-4
Massey B S 1983 Mechanics of FIuidr 5th edn (New
York: Van Nostrand) (see chapters ‘Circulation and
Vorticity’ and ‘Free Vortex’)
Owen M 1987 Preventing frozen noses Phys. Bull. 38
445-6

358

You might also like