You are on page 1of 6

ll

Article

A B

C D

E F

G H

Figure 5. During Downward FRH, Firing Rates Depress as a Function of Time Spent Asleep
(A) Schematic of extended sleep analysis for one neuron. Individual epochs of a state (NREM in this example) are found within an extended sleep episode, and the
neuron’s mean firing rate is calculated in each one; these values are then plotted against the start time of that epoch, aligned to the start of the whole episode (t0).
(B) As in (A), but showing an example extended wake episode.
(C) Correlation between firing rate in NREM and REM and time from start of extended sleep in the reopened hemisphere. All data points are shown in the left
panels; the right panels show data in 10 groups of equal size for ease of visualization (dots show mean ± SEM). Pearson r and associated p values were computed
on the ungrouped data (NREM, n = 4,036 data points; REM, n = 3,161 data points). Firing rates were Z scored to the extended sleep episode.

(legend continued on next page)

Neuron 109, 530–544, February 3, 2021 537


ll
Article

Both NREM and REM Sleep Contribute to Homeostatic decrease in firing rate was similar across both behavioral states
Recovery (Figure 8E), and it was not correlated with the amount of time
An important question that our data and analyses so far do not spent asleep (Figure 8F). Thus, we conclude that sleep specif-
address is the impact of different sleep states on FRH. Both ically enables a reduction in firing rate driven by homeostatic
REM and NREM sleep have been proposed to play key roles in plasticity.
driving plasticity (Diekelmann and Born, 2010; Abel et al.,
2013; Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). To determine whether either DISCUSSION
state plays a principal role in driving FRH, we analyzed all
NREM-REM-NREM episodes (sleep triplets) (Figure 7A) that It has been theorized that bidirectional stabilization of firing rates
occurred during the two days of homeostatic recovery and around a set point is a critical feature that allows developmental
then estimated the effect of the intervening REM episode by or experience-dependent synaptic changes to refine network
comparing for each triplet the mean firing rate at the end of the architecture without fatally destabilizing activity (Abbott and
first NREM episode to the mean firing rate at the beginning of Nelson, 2000; Marder and Prinz, 2002; Tetzlaff et al., 2011; Tur-
the second NREM episode. We averaged firing rates over rigiano, 2012; Litwin-Kumar and Doiron, 2014). Although upward
5 min and took care to avoid transition effects by excluding FRH is known to occur in vivo, whether this process is indeed
1 min of data at every transition. This analysis confirmed that bidirectional and whether upward and downward homeostasis
REM sleep has no basal effect on firing rate in control neurons, share mechanistic features have been open questions. Here
whereas a significant decrease in firing rate was evident for neu- we used MD followed by ER to potentiate firing in V1 and then
rons in the reopened hemisphere (Figures 7B and 7C). To quan- analyzed the behavior of individual neurons over time in freely
tify the effect of NREM, we next performed the same analysis but behaving animals. MD-ER produced a 2-fold potentiation of
for REM-NREM-REM triplets; because of the short duration of firing that was NMDAR dependent and accompanied by non-
REM episodes, we computed mean firing rates in 90-s bins at uniform changes in synaptic strengths. This was followed by
the end and start of the first and second REM episodes, downward FRH, which slowly returned individual firing rates
excluding 30 s at the transition (Figure 7D). Again, we saw no ef- close to their initial values independently of NMDAR signaling,
fect in control neurons and a decrease in firing rate in reopened and was accompanied by a scaling down of synaptic strengths.
neurons (Figures 7E and 7F). These findings show that FRH oc- This is consistent with homeostatic synaptic scaling, although
curs during both NREM and REM sleep states, suggesting that we cannot rule out the involvement of additional non-NMDAR-
the feature or features of brain states that enable downward dependent mechanisms. We found that downward FRH is pro-
FRH are likely common to both REM and NREM sleep. moted by sleep, the opposite of upward FRH (Hengen et al.,
2016). This does not reflect a general role for sleep in all forms
Non-homeostatic Firing Rate Changes Happen of synaptic depression, because the early phase of MD (driven
Independently of Animals’ Behavioral State by LTD-like mechanisms) unfolded independently of sleep and
MD initially depresses firing rates over the first 2 days of MD, via wake states. Our data show that the role of sleep/wake states
LTD at thalamocortical synapses and additional changes at in- in promoting circuit plasticity is nuanced and that the induction
tracortical synapses (Heynen et al., 2003; Maffei et al., 2006; of upward and downward FRH is segregated by behavioral state.
Miska et al., 2018). This allowed us to test whether sleep and Although sensory deprivation is a venerable paradigm for
wake states also gate this non-homeostatic form of plasticity. inducing neocortical plasticity (Espinosa and Stryker, 2012;
During early MD, neuronal firing rates are stable for a variable Gainey and Feldman, 2017), there has been a dearth of ap-
period before starting to decline. Using an automated algorithm proaches for increasing, rather than decreasing, neocortical
to detect the start of the drop for each neuron, we found that it firing to study downward homeostasis. Prolonged dark expo-
happened quickly (over 6–12 h) in individual neurons (Figures sure has been proposed to induce homeostatic plasticity within
8A and 8B) but that the timing was variable: all our recorded reg- V1 (Goel and Lee, 2007), but more recent work has shown that
ular spiking neurons could be classified as early-drop neurons, in changes in mEPSC amplitude during dark exposure unfold via
which most of the drop occurred during the first 12-h light period metaplastic rules that are distinct from those that induce syn-
after MD (Figures 8A and 8C), or late-drop neurons, in which aptic scaling (Bridi et al., 2018; Chokshi et al., 2019). Dark
most of the drop occurred during the second 12-h light period af- exposure does not affect firing rates in V1 over the first day
ter MD (Figures 8B and 8D). When we analyzed the relationship or so (Torrado Pacheco et al., 2019) and thus would not be ex-
between sleep/wake behavior and the drop in neuronal firing pected to induce synaptic scaling, and light reexposure after
rates during the first or second 12-h light periods (for early- 60 h in the dark only elevates firing for 20 min, not long
drop and late-drop neurons, respectively) we found that neither enough to trigger slow forms of homeostatic plasticity (Torrado
group had a bias toward wake or sleep. The magnitude of the Pacheco et al., 2019). In contrast, MD followed by ER induces a

(D) Difference in firing rate between the last and the first NREM (left) or REM (right) epoch within an extended sleep episode, averaged across all episodes. Bars
show mean ± SEM. One-sample t test compared with mean = 0; n = 74 episodes; ## p < 106, ### p < 1017.
(E and F) As in (C) and (D), but for NREM and REM in the control hemisphere (NREM, n = 2,933 data points; REM, n = 2,249 data points). In (F), one-sample t test
compared with mean = 0; n = 47 episodes.
(G and H) As in (C) and (D), but for active wake (AW, n = 4,156 data points) and quiet wake (QW, 5,131 data points) in the reopened hemisphere. In (H), one-sample
t test compared with mean = 0; n = 70 episodes.

538 Neuron 109, 530–544, February 3, 2021


ll
Article

B C

D E

Figure 6. Sleep Deprivation (SD) during Downward FRH Slows the Restoration of Firing Rates
(A) Diagram of the SD paradigm and analysis. Yellow and gray rectangles represent 12 h of light and dark, respectively. Animals were sleep deprived during two
12-h periods during the light phase of ER2 and ER3. The 12-h bins corresponding to the dark phases were used to calculate average firing rates for individual
neurons. The change in firing rate was then calculated across each SD period and for the last light period (no SD).
(B) Change in firing rate for each recorded neuron in the SD condition (n = 22 neurons from 4 animals) and control condition (n = 36 neurons from 5 animals). Left,
effect of SD on ER2 (change in firing rate between time points 1 and 2). Middle, effect of SD on ER3 (change in firing rate between time points 2 and 3). Right, effect
of last light period (change in firing rate between time points 3 and 4, when no SD occurred). Yellow dashed lines indicate no change. **p = 0.0079, two-sample
t test; # p = 0.0449, ## p = 0.0084, #### p < 105; one-sample t test versus mean of 0.
(C) Average change in firing rate for each neuron across periods of sleep and wake for the control group, and periods of prolonged wake and recovery sleep for the
SD group. Only data for the two 12-h SD periods (and corresponding times in the control dataset) were used. Data for both periods were pooled. The yellow
dashed line indicates no change. *p = 0.0374, ***p = 0.0001, ****p < 104; one-way ANOVA (p < 104) with Tukey-Kramer post hoc; # p = 0.0247, #### p < 106;
linear model comparing all group means to a mean of 0.

(legend continued on next page)

Neuron 109, 530–544, February 3, 2021 539


ll
Article

A Figure 7. Downward FRH Occurs during Both


NREM and REM Sleep
(A) Schematic for the analysis of NREM1-REM-NREM2
triplets.
(B) Firing rate values are Z scored within each triplet
and then averaged across all triplets for a given
B C neuron. Each dot represents a neuron, and mean firing
rate values for the same neuron are linked by hori-
zontal lines. ****p < 0.0001; Wilcoxon sign-rank test.
(C) Difference in mean firing rate values between the
first 5 min of NREM2 and the last 5 min of NREM1.
Black lines indicate mean ± SEM. The yellow dashed
line indicates no change. ****p < 0.0001; one-sample t
test versus mean of 0.
(D) As in (A), but for REM1-NREM-REM2 triplets.
(E) As in (B), but for REM1-NREM-REM2 triplets. *p =
0.05; Wilcoxon sign-rank test.
(F) As in (C), but for REM1-NREM-REM2 triplets. *p =
0.0383; one-sample t test versus mean of 0.

The process of downward FRH revealed


here bears several similarities to upward
FRH (Hengen et al., 2016). They both unfold
slowly over 2 days, are accompanied by
D synaptic scaling of excitatory synapses in
the correct direction to contribute to restora-
tion of firing, and bring individual neuronal
firing rates back to an individual set point. It
was thus surprising to find that they have a
E F diametrically opposite dependence upon
behavioral state: upward FRH occurs during
active waking, whereas downward FRH
occurs during sleep. These findings show
that upward and downward homeostatic
compensation do not operate simultaneously
within neuronal circuits and add complexity
to the idea that the function of sleep is to pro-
vide a window of opportunity for homeostatic
plasticity (Wang et al., 2011).
SHY proposes that wakefulness drives
learning-related increases in synaptic
strengths and firing rates, whereas sleep
renormalizes activity by downregulating
synaptic strengths, a process dependent
on activity patterns during NREM sleep
(Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). A combination of
robust and long-lasting increase in firing in V1 that develops structural, electrophysiological, and molecular evidence has
over several hours and is blocked by NMDAR antagonists, sug- been put forward in support of SHY (de Vivo et al., 2017; Vyazov-
gesting (consistent with previous work by Toyoizumi et al., skiy et al., 2008, 2009; Liu et al., 2010; Diering et al., 2017), but
2014) that it is a consequence of Hebbian synaptic plasticity contradictory data have also been reported (Yang et al., 2014;
induced by the restoration of correlated visual drive through Chauvette et al., 2012; Aton et al., 2014; Durkin and Aton,
the previously closed eye. 2016), and in some cases the interpretation of these findings

(D) Average time spent in sleep (left, blue) or wake (right, gray) during 4 12-h light periods: baseline, SD1, SD2, and no SD (corresponding to periods indicated in A).
SD decreased time spent in sleep and increased time spent in wake during these light periods. **p = 0.0012, ***p = 0.0007, ****p < 104; one-way ANOVA (p < 105)
with Tukey-Kramer post hoc.
(E) As in (D), but for the 12-h dark periods immediately following the 12-h light period for the corresponding label. One-way ANOVA (p = 0.31) with Tukey-Kramer
post hoc (all p > 0.28).

540 Neuron 109, 530–544, February 3, 2021


ll
Article

A B Figure 8. Non-homeostatic Firing Rate


Changes Happen Independently of Animals’
Behavioral State
(A) Two example neurons whose activity begins
decreasing in the first light period after MD (early
drop). Dashed lines indicate the baseline firing rate
for each neuron. White/gray bars in the background
indicate 12 h of light/dark.
(B) As in (A), but for two neurons whose drop-in firing
rate begins during the second light period after MD
(late drop).
(C) Average baseline-normalized firing rate of all
early-drop neurons. The dashed line indicates the
C D
baseline firing rate.
(D) As in (C), but for late-drop neurons.
(E) Change in firing rate across sleep- or wake-dense
epochs for all neurons in their respective 12-h drop
period. Control S, n = 38 epochs; control W, n = 12
epochs; deprived S, n = 38 epochs; deprived W, n =
26 epochs.
(F) Correlation between change in firing rate during
drop and percentage of time spent asleep in the
same period. Each data point represents the average
change in firing rate across early-drop (circles) or
late-drop (diamonds) neurons, and each color rep-
E F resents a different animal (n = 6 animals). Percentage
of time asleep is calculated per animal in the early-
drop or late-drop 12-h period. The black dashed line
represents no change. The black solid line shows the
linear fit to the data.

decreases in synaptic strength (Tononi and


Cirelli, 2014), whereas other studies high-
light a role for NREM sleep in synaptic
has been questioned (Frank, 2012; Frank and Cantera, 2014; potentiation following learning (Yang et al., 2014; Aton et al.,
Timofeev and Chauvette, 2017; Puentes-Mestril and Aton, 2014; Durkin and Aton, 2016). In this context, the similar effects
2017). In particular, it has been unclear whether sleep and of REM and NREM on downward FRH could be explained in two
wake cause global oscillations in firing rates under baseline con- ways. First, the plasticity mechanism or mechanisms driving
ditions, when animals have not experienced a dramatic plasticity downward FRH in V1 may be equally enabled during both brain
or learning event. Such oscillations have been observed in the states, which would put interesting constraints on the mecha-
hippocampus and frontal cortex but were not uniform across nisms of the gating process. The second possibility is that
the neuronal population (Miyawaki and Diba, 2016; Watson distinct plasticity mechanisms expressed in REM and NREM
et al., 2016; Miyawaki et al., 2019). In the absence of plasticity in- cooperate to restore activity to baseline levels. This might be ad-
duction, we observe stable firing during relatively long, consoli- vantageous if these mechanisms act on different parameters of
dated sleep/wake states in V1, both here and previously (Hengen neuronal plasticity, such as structural and functional synaptic
et al., 2016), indicating that in V1, sleep and wake do not by changes. More precise manipulations of behavioral state, such
themselves drive global changes in excitability. Furthermore, as deprivation restricted to REM sleep, would help shed light
we find that downward firing rate changes driven by Hebbian on this question.
plasticity during early MD unfold independently of sleep and Our results paint a nuanced and complex picture of the impact
wake states. Thus, it is not the case that all decreases in synaptic of sleep and wake on neuronal plasticity. The effect of sleep on
strength and firing preferentially occur during sleep. firing rate changes is not uniform but instead depends on the
NREM and REM sleep have been proposed to serve distinct particular forms of synaptic plasticity activated, which may
roles in the regulation of neuronal firing and plasticity, so it is help explain the diversity of outcomes in experiments designed
interesting that downward FRH occurs during both sleep states to test the role of sleep in the induction of plasticity (Raven
(Figure 7). REM sleep has been implicated in firing rate de- et al., 2018; Frank and Seibt, 2018). Although our data argue
creases in hippocampus and cortex (Grosmark et al., 2012; against a constitutive role of sleep in reducing circuit excitability
Niethard et al., 2016), in pruning and maintenance of dendritic (Figure 5) (see Hengen et al., 2016), we find that when it is
spines (Li et al., 2017), and more broadly in memory consolida- induced through manipulations of experience, downward FRH
tion (Boyce et al., 2017). SHY instead proposes that slow wave (and presumably the underlying downscaling of synapses) oc-
activity during NREM sleep is the specific driver of homeostatic curs preferentially during sleep. This finding is consistent with

Neuron 109, 530–544, February 3, 2021 541


ll
Article

data showing that SD interferes with molecular signaling cas- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS


cades that are important for scaling down (Diering et al., 2017).
Funding sources were R01-EY025623 and R35-NS111562 (to G.G.T.).
The most parsimonious explanation for our data is that rather
Computational resources were provided by NSF XSEDE computing resources
than constitutively inducing downscaling, sleep is permissive (MCB090163) and the Brandeis high-performance computing cluster (HPCC),
for the expression of downward homeostatic plasticity when it which is partially supported by DMR-1420382.
is induced by perturbations to the circuit.
A surprising finding of this study is that upward and downward AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
FRH are gated by distinct sleep/wake states. What is the mech-
Conceptualization, A.T.P. and G.G.T.; Methodology, A.T.P., J.B., and G.G.T.;
anism by which this is achieved, and what is the purpose of this
Software, A.T.P., J.B., and Y.G.; Formal Analysis, A.T.P., J.B., and Y.G.; Inves-
segregation? One promising lead may be the starkly contrasting tigation, A.T.P.; Writing, A.T.P. and G.G.T.; Visualization, A.T.P. and J.B.; Data
neuromodulatory environments that neocortical neurons are Curation, A.T.P.; Supervision, G.G.T.; Funding Acquisition, G.G.T.
exposed to during sleep and wake (Lee and Dan, 2012). Neuro-
modulators may regulate plasticity mechanisms by modulating DECLARATION OF INTERESTS
neuronal activity (Goard and Dan, 2009; Herrero et al., 2008) or
G.G.T. is a member of the Neuron advisory board. The authors declare no
by directly acting on signaling pathways involved in plasticity
competing interests.
induction, such as the Homer1-mGluR complex (Diering et al.,
2017). The molecular pathways that regulate synaptic scaling Received: November 27, 2019
up and down are mostly divergent but show some overlap (Turri- Revised: September 15, 2020
giano, 2017; Styr et al., 2019). One possible reason for the tempo- Accepted: October 30, 2020
ral segregation of upward and downward homeostasis could be Published: November 23, 2020
to reduce interference or saturation of the molecular pathways
REFERENCES
that mediate homeostatic plasticity. A complementary possibility
is that behavioral state gating ensures strong unidirectional ho- Abbott, L.F., and Nelson, S.B. (2000). Synaptic plasticity: taming the beast.
meostatic compensation for defined periods; this may have ben- Nat. Neurosci. 3 (Suppl ), 1178–1183.
efits in terms of computation and learning by providing strong Abel, T., Havekes, R., Saletin, J.M., and Walker, M.P. (2013). Sleep, plasticity
compensation when it is needed most and perhaps by allowing and memory from molecules to whole-brain networks. Curr. Biol. 23,
unopposed Hebbian changes during limited windows of time. R774–R788.
Aton, S.J., Suresh, A., Broussard, C., and Frank, M.G. (2014). Sleep promotes
cortical response potentiation following visual experience. Sleep (Basel) 37,
STAR+METHODS 1163–1170.
Barnes, S.J., Sammons, R.P., Jacobsen, R.I., Mackie, J., Keller, G.B., and
Detailed methods are provided in the online version of this paper Keck, T. (2015). Subnetwork-Specific Homeostatic Plasticity in Mouse Visual
and include the following: Cortex In Vivo. Neuron 86, 1290–1303.
Blackman, M.P., Djukic, B., Nelson, S.B., and Turrigiano, G.G. (2012). A critical
d KEY RESOURCES TABLE
and cell-autonomous role for MeCP2 in synaptic scaling up. J. Neurosci. 32,
d RESOURCE AVAILABILITY 13529–13536.
B Lead Contact
Boyce, R., Williams, S., and Adamantidis, A. (2017). REM sleep and memory.
B Materials Availability Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 44, 167–177.
B Data and Code Availability Bridi, M.C.D., de Pasquale, R., Lantz, C.L., Gu, Y., Borrell, A., Choi, S.-Y., He,
d EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS K., Tran, T., Hong, S.Z., Dykman, A., et al. (2018). Two distinct mechanisms for
d METHOD DETAILS experience-dependent homeostasis. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 843–850.
B Surgical procedures Campbell, I.G., Gustafson, L.M., and Feinberg, I. (2002). The competitive
B Continuous single-unit recordings in freely behaving NMDA receptor antagonist CPPene stimulates NREM sleep and eating in
animals rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 348–357.
B Automated spike extraction, clustering and sorting Caporale, N., and Dan, Y. (2008). Spike timing-dependent plasticity: a Hebbian
B Transcardial perfusions learning rule. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 25–46.
B Histology Chauvette, S., Seigneur, J., and Timofeev, I. (2012). Sleep oscillations in the
B CPP injections thalamocortical system induce long-term neuronal plasticity. Neuron 75,
1105–1113.
B Sleep deprivation
Chokshi, V., Gao, M., Grier, B.D., Owens, A., Wang, H., Worley, P.F., and Lee,
B Slice electrophysiology
H.-K. (2019). Input-Specific Metaplasticity in the Visual Cortex Requires
B Semi-automated behavioral state scoring
Homer1a-Mediated mGluR5 Signaling. Neuron 104, 736–748.e6.
B Firing rate analyses
de Vivo, L., Bellesi, M., Marshall, W., Bushong, E.A., Ellisman, M.H., Tononi,
B Sleep analyses G., and Cirelli, C. (2017). Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across
d QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS the wake/sleep cycle. Science 355, 507–510.
Diekelmann, S., and Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev.
SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION Neurosci. 11, 114–126.
Diering, G.H., Nirujogi, R.S., Roth, R.H., Worley, P.F., Pandey, A., and Huganir,
Supplemental Information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j. R.L. (2017). Homer1a drives homeostatic scaling-down of excitatory synapses
neuron.2020.11.001. during sleep. Science 355, 511–515.

542 Neuron 109, 530–544, February 3, 2021

You might also like