You are on page 1of 3

Missing comma No methods to collect

Missing comma between PUBERTAL TIMING AND RACIAL IDENTITY


between elements 1
data explicitly
elements (see: p88) (see: p88)
Incorrect slash (see: p95) (Interview, Observation
Correction: ,and Measures Correction: ,and
Method Correction: parent or etc.)
guardian Demographic information. All adolescents provided infor-
Participants mation regarding their gender, age, race/ethnicity, grade level,
Missing
comma The participants were 217 adolescent females ranging in age household composition, parental marital status, and parental edu-
between
elements from 14 to 18 with an average age of 15.7 (SD = 1.2) years. The cation level.
(see: adolescents reported their racial/ethnic background as African Pubertal timing. Pubertal timing was assessed using the Pu-
p88) American (67%), Afro-Latino (18%), Biracial/multiracial (6%), bertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen, Crockett, Richards, &
Correcti Caribbean Black (7%) and African (2%).1 The participants re- Boxer, 1988). The girls indicated the extent to which they had
on: ,or ported that their adult guardian (usually the mother) was either experienced pubertal growth during the past year with responses
married/cohabiting (46%), separated/divorced/widowed (30%), or ranging from 1 (has not yet started) to 4 (development completed).
single/never married (24%). The participants also reported the Girls reported on their growth spurt, body hair, skin changes, and
following educational levels for this parent/guardian: less than a breast development. The total PDS scores for the four items were Methods to co
high school diploma (8%), high school diploma (23%), 1 year of averaged to maintain the original metric with acceptable reliability (Interview, Ob
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

college or an associate’s degree (30%), a bachelor’s degree (29%), etc.). See p31
( a= .60). The average PDS scores for this sample of Black girls
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

a graduate degree (8%), or missing (2%). Household composition was 3.40 (SD = .49). The PDS scores were standardized within
was reported as married (37%), female headed (41%), male each age (14, 15, 16, 17, and 18 years), which generated a
headed (2%), parent/extended family (5%), grandparent/extended
variable, pubertal timing, with a mean of zero and a standard
family
deviation of one. Higher scores indicated earlier maturation
(6%), other (1%) or missing (8%).
relative to same- age peers. Participants also indicated the age at
which they began menarche.
Procedure The Multidimensional Inventory of Black Identity (MIBI).
The current study was part of a larger mixed-methods study of A shortened version of the MIBI was utilized to assess racial
No explicit qua
adolescent experiences and mental health. The primary criteria centrality and racial regard (Martin, Wout, Nguyen, Sellers, & measurement
were being of African descent and enrolled in high school, and Gonzalez, 2017). The scale consists of items with responses rang- (Validity, reliab
two separate methodologies were used. Initially, adolescents were ing from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The racial See p31
re- cruited from high schools in a southeastern city (N = 87; centrality subscale (a = .75) assesses the degree to which being
40%). Approval was obtained from two school districts, and eight Black is central to respondents’ self-definition. A sample item is,
schools were selected on the basis of the principals’ willingness to “I have a strong attachment to other Black people.” The private
partic- ipate in the research study. Participation was granted if regard subscale (a = .68) measures the extent to which respon-
parental consent forms were returned, and the response rate dents feel positive about being Black. A sample item is, “I feel
ranged from 10% to 30% per classroom with an average response good about Black people.” The public regard subscale ( a = .81)
rate of 20%. The participants signed assent forms, were reminded measures respondents’ belief of how others view Blacks. A
that their participation was voluntary and were explicitly told that sample item is, “In general, others respect Black people.”
the results from their paper-and-pencil questionnaires would Neighborhood racial composition. Each respondent’s resi-
remain confi- dential. The questionnaires were administered by dential address was used to obtain the percentage of African
undergraduate, African American female research assistants. Americans in their respective block group from the 2010 U.S.
Completion time ranged from 30 to 60 min, and participants were Census. The block group was used because it is the smallest and
compensated $20 and debriefed upon completion. most precise unit of analysis (see Collins & Williams, 1999). A
The second procedure used Facebook to increase the study single variable was used from the census data: percentage of Methods to col
sample (N = 130; 60%). A paid advertisement was placed on African American residents per block group. (Interview, Ob
Facebook that targeted youth between the ages of 13 and 18 in the School racial composition. The ethnic/racial composition of etc.). See p31
continental United States. The advertisement ran during peak each high school was assessed using archival data from the Na-
hours of adolescent consumption, typically Wednesday through tional Center for Education Statistics. The percentage of students
Sunday between 3 PM and 11 PM. An interested adolescent would classified as African American, White, Hispanic, and Asian/Pa-
click on the advertisement and was directed to the initial page in cific Islander was obtained for each participant’s respective high
SurveyMonkey to assess the adolescent’s race/ethnicity. If the school during the year the study was conducted. A single variable
adolescent was not of African descent, they were informed that was used from the archival data: percentage of African American
they were ineligible. If the adolescent was of African descent, they students per school.
were informed of their eligibility and asked to provide their resi- Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression (CES-D) No explicit qua
dential address. Once this information was provided, all interested Scale. The shortened 12-item version of the CES-D assessed the measurement
adolescents were mailed a packet containing parental consent frequency of depressive symptoms experienced within the last (Validity, reliab
forms, adolescent assent forms, information sheets, and self- week (Roberts, Lewinshon, & Seeley, 1999). The subscale in- See p31
addressed return envelopes. Once adolescents returned the packet, cluded 12 items (a= .82) with responses ranging from 0 (rarely)
they were e-mailed a link to complete the questionnaire in Sur-
veyMonkey. The return rate was approximately 30% for the Fa- 1
cebook recruitment procedure. Upon completion of the survey, Given the diversity of terms adolescents chose for racial self-
designation, the term Black will be used for the sample throughout the rest
youth were compensated electronically with gift cards or mailed of the article.
cash.
2 SEATON AND CARTER

to 3 (most or all of the time). A sample item is, “I felt that


each regression path was constrained to be equal across groups. The
everything I did was an effort.”
difference in fit between the unconstrained and constrained models
illustrated whether specific paths were moderated by
Data Analytic Strategy school/neighborhood racial composition. Separate multigroup path
models for school racial composition and neighborhood racial
Path analyses were used to examine the hypothesized relations composition were conducted.
between pubertal timing, depressive symptoms, racial identity,
and school and neighborhood racial composition. All path models
included adolescent age and recruitment method as covariates. Results
Correlated error between adolescent age and pubertal timing were The results indicate variation along the pubertal development
permitted. The fit for all path models was evaluated using multiple dimensions (see Table 1). The average age of menarche was 12.1
indicators of model fit: chi-square, the CFI, and the root mean (SD = 1.7), ranging from age 8 to 16. The means, standard
squared error of approximation (RMSEA). Hu and Bentler (1999) deviations, and zero-order correlations for the study variables
suggest CFI values greater than or equal to .95 and RMSEA were presented in Table 2.
values less than or below .08 represent acceptable model fit. More Already include parentheses.
See p95 Correction: 0.04 –
fo- cused tests were used to examine model fit, namely
Testing Direct Relations 0.14 without breckets
modification indices and standardized residuals testing the
difference between predicted and observed covariances. The The model fit was adequate, y2(6) = 2.73, p = .01, (CFI =
overall chi-square, a statistical test of the lack of fit based on over 0.94; RMSEA = 0.09; 90% CI [0.04 –0.14]), explaining 15% of
identifying restric- tions on the target model, was also reported. the variance in depressive symptoms. The results suggested that
Nonsignificant chi- squares suggest good model fit; however, this late developing adolescent girls were more likely to report depres-
estimation is a function of sample size and is usually significant sive symptoms than average and early developing girls
with large sam- ples. (B = —2.88, p < .01). Adolescent girls with high levels of racial
We conducted the analyses in three stages. The first stage centrality (B = 1.08, p < .05) and low levels of private regard
examined whether pubertal timing and racial identity dimensions reported high levels of depressive symptoms (B = —2.04, p <
(i.e., racial centrality, public regard, and private regard) were .01). Already include parentheses.
related to depressive symptoms. The second stage examined
See p95 Correction: 0.04 –
whether racial identity dimensions moderated the relation between 0.14 without breckets
pubertal timing and depressive symptoms. The analysis for this Testing for Moderated Relations
Percentagestage involved
in words followedmean centering pubertal timing and racial identity
by number Racial centrality. The model fit was good, y2(6) = .414, p =
Correction:dimensions
percentage (without number).
to avoid problems with multicollinearity, to make
See p118
.87, (CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.01; 90% CI [0.01–0.05]). Exam-
coefficients interpretable (Jaccard, Wan, & Turrisi, 1990). Three ination of the Pubertal Timing × Racial Centrality interaction
product terms were created: Pubertal Timing × Racial Centrality, indicated a significant, negative association between pubertal
Pubertal Timing × Public Regard, and Pubertal Timing × Private timing and depressive symptoms, (B = —2.17, p < .012). This
Regard. The third stage examined whether the relations between interaction was probed at the mean, one standard deviation above
pubertal timing, racial identity, and depression varied as a the mean, and one standard deviation below the mean (Aiken &
function of school and neighborhood racial composition. The West, 1991). The results indicated that high racial centrality levels
school and neighborhood variables used a 60% cutoff such that were related to depressive symptoms among adolescent girls with
neighbor- hoods and schools with percentages 60% or greater late pubertal timing relative to girls with early pubertal timing (see
were consid- ered “majority Black” and neighborhoods, and Figure 1).
schools less than 60% were considered “not majority Black” (see Public regard. The model fit was good, y2(6) = .416, p =
Bower, Thorpe, Rohde, & Gaskin, 2014). Thirty-two percent (n = .87, (CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.01; 90% CI [0.01–0.05]). Exam-
69) of adoles- cents lived in residential enclaves with Black ination of the Pubertal Timing × Public Regard interaction indi-
densities 60% or greater, and 64% (n = 139) lived in cated a significant, negative association between pubertal timing
neighborhoods with Black densities less than 60%. Fifty-two and depressive symptoms (B = —2.17, p < .012). The results
percent (n = 114) of adolescents attended schools that were indicated that low public regard levels were related to depressive
predominantly Black and 48% (n = symptoms among adolescent girls with early pubertal timing rel-
103) attended schools that were not predominantly Black. The ative to girls with late pubertal timing (see Figure 2).
analysis involved a multigroup strategy, with Majority Black or Not
Majority Black representing two groups. The model was
simultaneously fit to establish a common model form in the groups
with no equality constraints across groups (unconstrained model). Table 1
Two models were run in which the regression weights were Variation in Pubertal Dimensions
constrained (constrained model) to be equal across the groups, and
a model in which the regression weights and covariance’s were Pubertal Not yet Barely Definitely
indicators started started started Completed Missing
constrained to be equal across the groups (fully constrained
model). Comparisons of the nested models were performed such Growth spurt 5% 9% 20% 59% 7%
that a significant chi-square difference would indicate that the Body hair 1% 4% 27% 61% 8%
models varied as a function of the racial composition. To deter- Skin 4% 12% 42% 34% 9%
Breast
mine which parameters are significantly different between groups, 0% 8% 40% 44% 8%
PUBERTAL TIMING AND RACIAL IDENTITY 3
Table 2
Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations for the Study Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Age
2. Recruita .37××
3. Pubertal timing .00 .02
4. Racial centrality .05 .05 —.01
5. Private regard —.01 —.06 —.11 .55××
6. Public regard —.02 —.18×× —.27×× .21×× .34××
7. Neighborhood composition —.06 —.14 —.06 .04 .09 .05
8. School composition .08 .20×× —.02 —.13 —.07 —.01 .22××
9. Depressive symptoms .06 .13 —.16× —.03 —.26 ××
—.03 .00 —.07
M 15.7 .00 4.7 5.8 3.6 41.7 58.2 13.3
SD 1.2 .99 1.9 1.3 1.3 28.2 34.1 7.1
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

a
0 = in-schools, 1 = online.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

×
p < .05. ×× p < .01.

Private regard. The model examining private regard indi-


regard and pubertal timing interaction on depressive symptoms,
cated no moderation between pubertal timing and depressive
Ay2(A1) = 4.81, p < .03, differed significantly such that the
symptoms. No multigroup analyses were conducted for school and
interaction was stronger for girls attending not majority Black
neighborhood racial composition.
schools (B = —1.65, p < .001) than for girls attending majority
Black schools (B = .24, p < .64).
Multi-Group Analyses Neighborhood racial composition. The change chi-square
School racial composition. The first path model evaluated tests showed that the nested models evaluating racial centrality
racial centrality as a moderator of the direct and indirect paths and public regard as moderators of the direct and indirect paths be-
between pubertal timing and depressive symptoms. The change tween pubertal timing and depressive symptoms were not signif-
chi-square test showed that the constrained and fully constrained icantly different from each other (see Tables 3 and 4). The asso-
models were significantly different from each other, Ay2(A15) = ciations between pubertal timing, racial centrality, public regard
37.09, p < .05, indicating that school racial composition moder- and depressive symptoms were not moderated by neighborhood
ated one or more parameters (see Table 3). The racial centrality racial composition.
and pubertal timing interaction on depressive symptoms,
Ay2(A1) = 5.51, p < .01, differed significantly such that the Discussion
interaction was stronger for girls attending not majority Black
The results bridge empirical research on pubertal timing with
schools (B = —2.26, p < .001) than for girls attending majority
racial identity to understand the puberty-depression link. We ex-
Black schools (B = —.39, p < .38). amined whether racial centrality, public regard and private regard
The second multigroup analyses evaluated public regard and the moderated the relation between pubertal timing and depressive
change chi-square test showed that the constrained and fully con- symptoms among Black adolescent girls. These variables have not
strained models were significantly different from each other, been examined together in previous research. The results indicated
Ay2(A15) = 37.77, p < .05, indicating that school racial compo- that high racial centrality levels were linked to depressive symp-
sition moderated one or more parameters (see Table 4). The public toms for adolescent girls with late pubertal timing relative to their

Figure 1. Interaction between centrality and pubertal timing. Figure 2. Interaction between public regarding and pubertal timing.
77–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12175
Rivas-Drake, D., Syed, M., Umaña-Taylor, A., Markstrom, C., French, S., Roberts, R. E., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seeley, J. R. (1991). Screening for adolescent
Schwartz, S. J., . . . the Ethnic and Racial Identity in the 21st Century depression: a comparison of depression scales. Journal of the American
Study Group. (2014). Feeling good, happy, and proud: A meta-analysis Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 58 – 66.
of positive ethnic-racial affect and adjustment. Child Development, 85, Rogol, A. D., Clark, P. A., & Roemmich, J. N. (2000). Growth and pubertal

You might also like