You are on page 1of 14

Accelerat ing t he world's research.

jurnal american phychology


asosiation tentang motivasi
st. khusnul chotimah

Related papers Download a PDF Pack of t he best relat ed papers 

Relat ionships among Fourt h Graders' Reading Anxiet y, Reading Fluency, Reading Mot ivat ion, a…
Zuhal Çelikt ürk Sezgin

Makale Hakkında
Zuhal Çelikt ürk Sezgin

Ort aokul Öğrencilerinin Okumaya İlişkin Kaygı ve Tut umlarının Okuma Alışkanlığı Üzerindeki Et kisi: Bir Y…
Yasemin Baki
Journal of Educational Psychology © 2010 American Psychological Association
2010, Vol. 102, No. 4, 773–785 0022-0663/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0020084

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Reading Motivation as Predictors of Reading Literacy:


A Longitudinal Study

Michael Becker Nele McElvany


Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, and
Technical University, Dortmund

Marthe Kortenbruck
Free University, Berlin

The purpose in this study was to examine the longitudinal relationships of intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation with reading literacy development. In particular, the authors (a) investigated reading amount
as mediator between motivation and reading literacy and (b) probed for bidirectional relationships
between reading motivation and reading literacy, controlling for previous reading literacy. A total of 740
students participated in a longitudinal assessment starting in Grade 3, with further points of measurement
in Grades 4 and 6. Structural equation models with latent variables showed that the relationship between
intrinsic reading motivation and later reading literacy was mediated by reading amount but not when
previous reading literacy was included in the model. A bidirectional relationship was found between
extrinsic reading motivation and reading literacy: Grade 3 reading literacy negatively predicted extrinsic
reading motivation in Grade 4, which in turn negatively predicted reading literacy in Grade 6. Implica-
tions for research and practice are discussed.

Keywords: reading literacy, intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, reading frequency, elementary
school

In 2000, the International Reading Association published a participating students stated that they read only once or twice a
position statement that listed “the development and maintenance of month or less (Mullis, Martin, Kennedy, & Foy, 2007).
a motivation to read” as one of the key prerequisites for deriving These results are particularly alarming in light of the recent
meaning from print (International Reading Association, 2000). research identifying reading motivation and reading amount as
This statement illustrates the growing acknowledgment of the important predictors of reading literacy (Baker & Wigfield, 1999;
importance of reading motivation in research and practice in the Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; Taboada, Tonks, Wigfield, & Guthrie,
last two decades. However, more than half of the Grade 4 students 2009). One of the fundamental distinctions in motivational re-
assessed in a recent U.S. national survey stated that reading was search is between extrinsic and intrinsic motivation (Deci & Ryan,
not their favorite activity and that they did not read frequently for 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, &
enjoyment (Donahue, Daane, & Yin, 2005). The 2006 Progress in Davis-Kean, 2006). To date, however, reading research has fo-
International Reading Literacy Study reported generally positive cused primarily on the role of intrinsic reading motivation. Em-
attitudes toward reading among Grade 4 students, but 37% of pirical findings show that mediating variables, such as reading
amount, help to shape the influence of intrinsic motivation, but it
remains unclear whether the same holds for extrinsic motivation
(e.g., Guthrie, Wigfield, Metsala, & Cox, 1999). There is thus a
This article was published Online First October 11, 2010. need for longitudinal investigations covering both intrinsic and
Michael Becker, Center for Educational Research, Max Planck Institute extrinsic reading motivation to examine mediator variables that
for Human Development, Berlin, Germany; Nele McElvany, Center for might help to explain the relations observed between motivation
Educational Research, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, and achievement. This study is meant to advance scientific under-
Berlin, Germany, and Institute for School Development Research, Depart-
standing of these issues by examining the complex relationships
ment of Education and Sociology, Technical University, Dortmund, Ger-
many; Marthe Kortenbruck, Department of Education and Psychology,
among intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation, reading amount,
Free University, Berlin, Germany. and reading literacy from a longitudinal perspective.
We are grateful to Jürgen Baumert for providing the opportunity to
realize the Berlin Longitudinal Reading Study and Cordula Artelt for Reading Literacy
advice for implementing this project. We also thank Susannah Goss for
editorial assistance. It is widely acknowledged that the success of a modern society
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael is dependent on the level of literacy of its population. In its
Becker, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Center for Educa- Programme for International Student Assessment, the Organisation
tional Research, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany. E-mail: for Economic Co-operation and Development defined reading
mibecker@mpib-berlin.mpg.de literacy as the ability “to understand, use and reflect on written
773
774 BECKER, MCELVANY, AND KORTENBRUCK

texts in order to achieve one’s goals, to develop one’s knowledge interest, and excitement of reading. In this sense, reading is per-
and potential, and to participate effectively in society” (Organisa- formed for no sake but its own reward, and the activity is accom-
tion for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2001, p. 21). panied by positive emotions and perceived as highly satisfying
However, learning to read is a lengthy and complex process (Pintrich & Schunk, 2002; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Taboada et al.,
requiring psycholinguistic, perceptual, cognitive, and social skills 2009). Extrinsically motivated reading, in contrast, is directed
(Gee, 2001; Snow, 2002). Beyond the basic acquisition of the toward obtaining external recognition, rewards, or incentives (e.g.,
alphabetic system (i.e., letter–sound correspondences and spelling attention from parents or teachers, good grades; Deci & Ryan,
patterns), reading expertise implies decoding, vocabulary knowl- 1985; Wang & Guthrie, 2004), toward living up to external ex-
edge, and text comprehension. pectations, or toward avoiding punishment (Hidi, 2000). External
Decoding skills are an essential part of reading fluency, defined sources of influence on extrinsic reading motivation may vary
as the ability to read a text accurately, quickly, and with proper depending on the age group—younger children are influenced
expression (e.g., Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp, & Jenkins, 2001) and are primarily by their parents, whereas older children are also influ-
crucial for proficient reading (National Institute of Child Health enced by school and peers—and the role of instrumental motives
and Human Development [NICHD], 2000; Verhoeven & Van may differ also.
Leeuwe, 2008). Vocabulary has also been shown to be a critical
predictor of reading comprehension; indeed, vocabulary acquisi- Reading Literacy and Reading Motivation
tion and development of reading literacy are interlinked (Aarn-
outse & van Leeuwe, 1998; Baker, Simmons, & Kameenui, 1995; Theoretical models and empirical research underline the impor-
Nagy, 1988; NICHD, 2000). Text comprehension is often con- tance of motivational variables for reading literacy. The good-
ceived of as the “essence of reading” (Durkin, 1993). According to information-processing model of Pressley, Borkowski, and
Kintsch (1998), text comprehension can be seen as a combination Schneider (1989) integrates cognitive capacity, general strategies,
of text-based processes that integrate previous knowledge to a metacognition, previous knowledge, and motivation. Deci and
mental representation of the text. It is thus a form of cognitive Ryan (1985) postulated a stronger need for competence and self-
construction in which the individual takes an active role. Text determination in intrinsically motivated activities, leading to
comprehension entails deep-level problem-solving processes that higher performance in those activities (in this case, reading). At the
enable readers to construct meaning from text and derives from the same time, it has been argued that higher reading skills affect
(intentional) interaction between reader and text (Duke & Pearson, motivational beliefs (Morgan & Fuchs, 2007).
2002; Durkin, 1993). Several empirical studies have examined the relationship be-
When children begin learning to read systematically in school, tween intrinsic reading motivation and reading literacy and usually
the emphasis is initially on the acquisition of word-recognition have found a moderate positive association (Baker & Wigfield,
skills. Relative to languages with deep orthographies, German has 1999; Guthrie et al., 1999; Taboada et al., 2009; Unrau & Schlack-
a regular orthography, and most readers in the German-speaking man, 2006; for Germany, see McElvany et al., 2008; Schaffner &
countries progress from the alphabetic to the orthographic stage of Schiefele, 2007). Guthrie et al. (1999) found that reading motiva-
reading in their third year of schooling (Klicpera & Gasteiger- tion still explained a significant amount of the variance in text
Klicpera, 1993; Seymour, Aro, & Erskine, 2003). In the subse- comprehension among Grade 10 students when controlling for
quent years, the focus shifts to more complex aspects of the covariates such as past achievement, amount of reading, reading
reading and comprehension processes— especially fluency, vocab- efficacy, and socioeconomic status. In a study with elementary
ulary knowledge, and text comprehension (Snow, Scarborough, & school children, Taboada et al. (2009) found that internal motiva-
Burns, 1999). tion and cognitive processes (e.g., background knowledge, self-
Besides social and institutional variables, strong influences on these generated questions) made significant independent contributions to
processes include individual cognitive, motivational, and volitional variance in reading comprehension and in fact explained the equiv-
factors. As outlined above, reading motivation has been identified as alent of 3 months’ growth in reading comprehension. In evalua-
a key predictor of reading literacy in theoretical models and empirical tions of their intervention program CORI (Concept Oriented Read-
research (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; McElvany, Kortenbruck, & ing Instruction; Guthrie et al., 2004), which was designed to
Becker, 2008; NICHD, 2000). enhance reading comprehension, intrinsic reading motivation, and
strategic knowledge, the researchers led by Guthrie and Wigfield
Reading Motivation found the promotion of intrinsic reading motivation to be associ-
ated with an increase in reading comprehension.
Reading motivation can be defined as “the individual’s personal Fewer longitudinal studies have referred specifically to intrinsic
goals, values, and beliefs with regard to the topics, processes, and reading motivation. However, Gottfried, Fleming, and Gottfried
outcomes of reading” (Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000, p. 405). The (2001) reported a significant positive relationship between intrin-
distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation is fundamen- sic reading motivation at a young age (7 years) and later reading
tal in motivation theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000; achievement (8 and 9 years). (For a review of studies investigating
Wigfield et al., 2006). Sources of intrinsic reading motivation the relationship between children’s reading and children’s compe-
include positive experience of the activity of reading itself, books tency beliefs and goal orientations, see Morgan & Fuchs, 2007.)
valued as a source of enjoyment, the personal importance of There has been scarce empirical coverage of the relationship
reading, and interest in the topic covered by the reading material. between extrinsic reading motivation and reading processes. Ad-
Therefore, Guthrie and Wigfield (2000) defined intrinsic reading ditionally, as mentioned above, the sources of extrinsic motivation
motivation as the disposition to read purely for the enjoyment, differ with age. Parents are typically still the most important social
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC READING MOTIVATION 775

influence for elementary school students, whereas peers have (1999) found that both intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motiva-
increasing influence at secondary level. The available results tend tion correlated significantly with total reading amount, explaining
to indicate a negative relationship, with high extrinsic motivation 10.7% and 12.4% of the variance, respectively.
being related to poorer reading skills (Schaffner & Schiefele, 2007; These results led Wang and Guthrie (2004) to formulate a
Unrau & Schlackman, 2006; Wang & Guthrie, 2004). In their theoretical model that proposes a functional chain, in which read-
review of seven reading programs, Gear, Wizniak, and Cameron ing motivation increases reading amount, which in turn increases
(2004) examined whether incentive systems for students facilitate reading literacy. As expected, their empirical findings showed a
or hinder learning and motivation. Their findings suggest that negative association between extrinsic reading motivation and
rewards can have positive effects on intrinsic motivation and reading achievement and a positive association between intrinsic
performance under certain conditions (e.g., when the reward in- reading motivation and reading achievement. However, inconsis-
volves spontaneous and sincere positive feedback and when stu- tent with previous findings, reading amount did not mediate the
dents are rewarded frequently and immediately after successful relationship of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation with reading
performances). The precise nature of the relationship between achievement. This pattern of results may be attributable to the
extrinsic motivation and reading literacy thus remains unclear; cross-sectional design of the study. Previous studies have also
further research in this domain is clearly warranted. postulated reciprocal relationships among reading motivation,
Research on mediating variables that may influence the rela- reading behavior, and reading literacy (Cunningham & Stanovich,
tionship between motivational and cognitive processes in the read- 1997; McElvany et al., 2008; Morgan & Fuchs, 2007). However,
ing context has taken two main approaches. First, the effects of cross-sectional designs cannot disentangle the reciprocal patterns
reading motivation on strategy use and deeper level comprehen- of influence between different constructs. There is a clear need for
sion processes have been examined; the results of this research longitudinal research to further elucidate the relationships between
approach are inconsistent (e.g., Naceur & Schiefele, 2005). A the reading-related constructs from a developmental perspective,
second, possibly more promising approach has investigated read- their potential mediators, and the role of prior achievement.
ing behavior, in terms of reading amount, as a mediator of the
correlation between reading motivation and text comprehension.
Purpose in the Present Study
Reading Amount This study makes three key contributions to the emerging liter-
Guthrie et al. (1999) discussed several mechanisms theoretically ature on reading motivation. It examines, from a longitudinal
proposed to underlie the potential mediating role of reading perspective, how reading amount and reading literacy are associ-
amount. First, more frequent reading might enhance reading effi- ated, first, with intrinsic reading motivation and, second, with
ciency, as reading processes become better automatized. Increased extrinsic reading motivation. Third, it probes for bidirectional
decoding and strategy use might free up more cognitive resources relationships between reading motivation and reading literacy,
for higher order information processing, leading to better compre- taking the effects of previous reading achievement into account.
hension. A second plausible explanation is that frequent reading Our first research aim concerns the relationship between intrin-
leads to an increase in students’ prior knowledge. This facilitates sic reading motivation and reading literacy. As outlined above,
comprehension on the text-based level and hence the construction previous research indicates a positive relationship between intrin-
of the situational model (Kintsch, 1998). A third explanation is that sic reading motivation and reading literacy. Going one step further,
frequent reading supports reading self-concept and self-efficacy we hypothesized that Grade 4 intrinsic motivation would posi-
beliefs, leading to the selection of more difficult texts. Fourth, it is tively predict Grade 6 reading literacy (Research Aim 1.1) and that
conceivable that the attunement of cognitive and motivational this association would be mediated by Grade 4 reading amount
goals leads to automatized and habitualized processes. (Research Aim 1.2).
Several studies have reported links between reading behavior By analogy, the second research aim concerns the relationship
and reading achievement. Cipielewski and Stanovich (1992) between extrinsic motivation and reading literacy. Previous find-
showed that, even when controlling for cognitive abilities, children ings on this relationship are inconsistent (see above). We therefore
who read in their leisure time outperformed their peers on a explored the nature of the relationship between Grade 4 extrinsic
reading achievement test. Anderson, Wilson, and Fielding (1988) reading motivation and Grade 6 reading literacy (Research Aim
found the time children spend reading outside school to be the best 2.1). Furthermore, we analyzed whether a mediating effect of
predictor of growth in school reading achievement between Grades reading amount can indeed be detected for extrinsic motivation. It
2 and 5 (see also Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Taylor, Frye, & is conceivable that extrinsically motivated children also read more
Maruyama, 1990, for an experimental study design). The more frequently, with corresponding effects on their reading literacy
recent 2003 National Assessment of Educational Progress report (Research Aim 2.2).
found that children who read more understand texts better than Our third research aim was to integrate previous reading literacy
those who read less (Donahue et al., 2005). within an extended theoretical model and to probe for bidirectional
In studies investigating the relationship between reading moti- relationships of reading literacy with intrinsic/extrinsic motivation
vation and reading amount, highly motivated children have been (see Morgan & Fuchs, 2007). We hypothesized that Grade 4
found to read more frequently than less motivated children. For reading motivation would be influenced by Grade 3 reading liter-
example, Wigfield and Guthrie (1997) found reading amount to be acy (Research Aim 3.1). Finally, we investigated whether the
predicted by reading motivation in Grade 4 and 5 students. In a effects of intrinsic/extrinsic reading motivation and Grade 4 read-
second study with elementary school children, Guthrie et al. ing amount on Grade 6 reading literacy would persist when Grade
776 BECKER, MCELVANY, AND KORTENBRUCK

3 reading literacy was included as predictor of all constructs in the higkeitstest (Heller & Perleth, 2000), which is analogous in struc-
model (Research Aim 3.2). ture to the CFT.
Rasch analysis was used to create two ability scales, one for text
Method comprehension and one for vocabulary. The items were linked
through external calibration samples and fitted a one-dimensional
Participants and Design Rasch model. Item parameters were estimated based on these
samples. Given the good fit of the Rasch model, measurement
A total of 740 students from 54 classes in 22 Berlin elementary equivalence can be assumed (Kolen & Brennan, 2004). The reli-
schools participated in the longitudinal study. The average age at abilities of the tests across the two waves of assessment range from
the end of Grade 3 was just over 9 years (M ⫽ 9.1 years, SD ⫽ good to satisfactory (text comprehension, Cronbach’s ␣t1 ⫽ .78,
0.5). Boys were slightly in the majority (53%). Data on students’ ␣t2 ⫽ .69; vocabulary, Cronbach’s ␣t1 ⫽ .69, ␣t2 ⫽ .78).
family background showed that 71% lived in two-parent homes; Decoding was evaluated with a speeded 70-item multiple-choice
63% spoke only German at home, whereas 37% either used Ger- test (Würzburger Leise Leseprobe [Würzburg Silent Reading
man and another language or spoke only another language within Test]; Küspert & Schneider, 1998) that required one of four
the family. One of the criteria for the selection of the participating pictures to be matched to a given word (e.g., foot or thermometer).
schools was their location in various districts of Berlin, which Correct answers were coded as 1; incorrect answers or unchecked
ensured a mix of social backgrounds.
answers were coded as 0. A sum score with a maximum of 70
Data were collected within the framework of the Berlin Longi-
points was then computed. Different item sets were administered at
tudinal Reading Study LESEN 3– 6, which was conducted by the
pre- and posttest. For test security reasons, two versions of each
Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany.
test, differing only in the order of the items, were used at each
The main purpose in the study was to track children’s reading
point of measurement.
development from Grade 3 to Grade 6 and to identify individual,
social, and institutional factors influencing this development. A Intrinsic reading motivation. The measures of intrinsic and
subsample of 104 students participated in a family-based reading extrinsic motivation administered were based on our previous work
intervention program between Grades 3 and 4 (McElvany, 2008; but were expanded for this study (McElvany, 2008; McElvany,
McElvany & Artelt, 2009).1 Becker, & Lüdtke, 2009; McElvany et al., 2008). Intrinsic reading
The data analyzed in the following were collected in three motivation at Grade 4 was measured with three factors, each with one
waves: at the end of Grade 3 in June 2003 (T1), in the middle of to four indicators. Four items, three of which were positively phrased
Grade 4 in January 2004 (T2), and at the end of Grade 6 in May (“I like reading,” “Reading is fun,” “I read because I like reading
2006 (T3). In all three waves, students were assessed in classrooms stories”) and one of which was negatively phrased (“I think reading is
during regular school hours by trained experimenters. Most stu- boring”), assessed the intrinsic value attached to the activity of read-
dents in Berlin transfer to secondary schooling after 6 years of ing. Students rated their agreement with the items on a 4-point
elementary education. However, some students of the students in Likert scale (1 ⫽ disagree completely to 4 ⫽ agree completely).
our sample (N ⫽ 55) transferred to secondary school after Grade The scale had a high reliability (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .89). Two
4. These students, and students who were absent for the in-school additional items tapped the intrinsic value attached to books (“I am
assessment, were tested at the Max Planck Institute. Another pleased when I get a new book to read,” “When my parents give
subset of the sample (N ⫽ 45) was assessed at the institute under me a book as present, I am interested in it”). Cronbach’s ␣ was
the same conditions on all measurement occasions. acceptable at .63. Last, importance of reading was measured with
a single-item indicator (“I prefer watching television to reading”).
Measures Extrinsic reading motivation. Extrinsic motivation to read
was also measured with three factors: Extrinsic motivation pro-
Reading literacy. Reading literacy, as all other variables used
vided by parents was assessed with three unipolar items (“I read
in the following analyses, was operationalized as a latent variable
because my parents find it important that I read a lot,” “I read
(see below for technical procedures). Three indicators of processes
because my parents want me to,” “I read because I want my
of different complexity were used to specify the factor of reading
parents to be proud of me”). Responses were given on a 4-point
literacy, namely, text comprehension, vocabulary, and decoding.
Likert scale (1 ⫽ disagree completely to 4 ⫽ agree completely).
Grade 3 text comprehension was assessed with a sample of texts
The reliability of the scale was good (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .78).
with multiple-choice questions from the Hamburger Lesetest
(HAMLET 3– 4; Lehmann, Peek, & Poerschke, 1997). In Grade 6, Extrinsic motivation provided by school was measured with a
texts from the Diagnostischer Test Deutsch (Nauck & Otte, 1980) single indicator: “I read because I have to read for school.” Finally,
were applied. Texts and tasks with different item difficulties were extrinsic motivation resulting from instrumental goals was mea-
selected to cover a broad spectrum of ability. Tasks ranged in sured with three items (“I read because it is important in life to be
complexity from simple comprehension questions to more com-
plex questions requiring inferential comprehension. 1
Participation in the reading program (RP) and early transfer to second-
A set of 15 items from the CFT vocabulary test (German ary school (Gy) were also controlled for in statistical analyses: The vari-
version; Weiss, 1987) assessed students’ Grade 3 vocabulary. This ables were included in the models as predictors for the constructs in Grades
test covers basic and colloquial vocabulary from key areas of life 4 (RP) and 6 (RP, Gy). The pattern of results was consistent with that
and does not require any special knowledge. Grade 6 vocabulary presented here. For reasons of clarity, the models are therefore presented
was assessed with the vocabulary subtest of the Kognitiver Fä- without the control variables in the present article.
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC READING MOTIVATION 777

a good reader,” “I read because I can learn a lot through reading,” points of measurement or to drop out of a study completely. In
“I read because it is important to me to know a lot”). The scale order to avoid reduction of sample size or biased results, and to
displayed good reliability (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .77). capitalize on all of the information available, we chose the full
Reading amount. Reading amount was assessed through stu- information maximum likelihood estimation option in Mplus for
dent self-reports and parent questionnaires and measured with the analyses. This option allowed us to include participants with
three factors, each with one or two indicators of reading length/ partially missing values.
reading frequency: Reading length (student assessment) was mea- All main variables (text comprehension, vocabulary, decoding,
sured with the question “How long do you usually read each day?” intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, reading amount) were
Reading length (parent assessment) was assessed with two ques- examined for systematic dropout. In general, the marginal differ-
tions “On average, how many hours does your child read outside ences were not statistically significant. Hence, nonsystematic
school on a weekday?”/“. . . on a weekend day?” The response dropout can be assumed for these variables (Kortenbruck, 2007).
categories for these items were as follows: 1 ⫽ not at all, 2 ⫽ less
than half an hour, 3 ⫽ 30 to 60 minutes, 4 ⫽ 1 to 2 hours, 5 ⫽ Results
more than 2 hours. Reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽
.69). Reading frequency was tapped with two items in the student
Descriptive Statistics
questionnaire (“How often do you read for pleasure?” and “Do you
read during the school holidays?”). Students rated the frequency of Table 1 presents intercorrelations between the latent variables.
their reading on a 5-point Likert scale (response options 1 ⫽ never, As expected, there were high intercorrelations among achievement
2 ⫽ rarely, 3 ⫽ sometimes, 4 ⫽ often, and 5 ⫽ always). Reliability variables as well as among motivational/behavioral variables and
was adequate (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .82). lower intercorrelations across the construct areas. For example,
Grade 3 reading literacy correlated more closely with Grade 6
Statistical Analyses reading literacy than it did with the three latent indicators of
intrinsic reading motivation at Grade 4 (see also Table 2).
All statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS 15 and Mplus
Confirmatory factor analyses revealed that reading literacy in
Version 5.1. We assumed that a single dimension (reading literacy)
Grade 3 and 6 can be represented by two factors, one for each
underlies the three facets of text comprehension, decoding, and
measurement point. Model fit was still satisfactory when the factor
vocabulary and thus expected these facets to load highly on one
loadings were set to be invariant, ␹2(8) ⫽ 41.84, p ⬍ .05, CFI ⫽
factor. Exploratory factor analysis was used to test the unidimen-
.97, RMSEA ⫽ .07, with residuals correlated for vocabulary and
sionality of the latent variable reading literacy. The factor loadings
decoding.
of reading literacy in Grades 3 and 6 were specified to be equal in
In terms of construct validity, as expected, separate confirma-
the models to ensure measurement invariance across time.
tory factor analyses for intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and
Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis was used to ex-
reading amount confirmed that the expected factor structure—with
amine the underlying factor structure of the items designed to
three first-order factors as indicators of each construct—fit the data
measure intrinsic reading motivation, extrinsic reading motivation,
well: intrinsic motivation, ␹2(12) ⫽ 50.74, p ⬍ .05, CFI ⫽ .98,
and reading amount. The parameters of the factor loadings were
RMSEA ⫽ .08; extrinsic motivation, ␹2(12) ⫽ 79.45, p ⬍ .05,
specified to be free.
CFI ⫽ .95, RMSEA ⫽ .09; reading amount, ␹2(3) ⫽ 10.06, p ⬍
All of the following models were computed with Mplus 5.1
.05, CFI ⫽ .99, RMSEA ⫽ .06. Additionally, an overall confir-
(Muthén & Muthén, 1998 –2008) using the type ⫽ general option.
matory factor analysis for the second-order factor structure con-
We considered using the type ⫽ complex option, but problems
firmed that a three-factor model, ␹2(143) ⫽ 510.13, p ⬍ .05,
occurred in the computational process.2 As the main findings were
CFI ⫽ .92, RMSEA ⫽ .07, fit the data better than a two-factor
comparable in the two types of model specification, we relied on
model combining intrinsic and extrinsic motivation to form a
the conventional error estimation.
single factor, ␹2(145) ⫽ 707.145, p ⬍ .05, CFI ⫽ .88, RMSEA ⫽
We tested the hypotheses using structural equation modeling.
.08. Likewise, other two-factor solutions and the one-factor solu-
The fit of the models tested was evaluated on the basis of two
tion had a statistically significantly poorer fit: reading amount
goodness-of-fit indices: the comparative fit index (CFI) and the
combined with intrinsic motivation, ␹2(146) ⫽ 542.71, p ⬍ .05,
root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). Acceptable
CFI ⫽ .91, RMSEA ⫽ .07; reading amount combined with ex-
model fit is indicated by CFI values greater than .95 and RMSEA
trinsic motivation, ␹2(145) ⫽ 720.49, p ⬍ .05, CFI ⫽ .87,
values of .05 or less. The chi-square difference test was used to
RMSEA ⫽ .08; one-factor model, ␹2(146) ⫽ 742.23, p ⬍ .05,
compare the fit of the nested models (Hu & Bentler, 1999).
Bootstrapping was used to test the indirect, mediating effect of
reading amount. Bootstrapping has become the most widespread 2
The type ⫽ complex option would in general be the more desirable
method for testing mediation, as it does not require the normality estimation method because it corrects for the standard errors resulting
assumption. If the bootstrap confidence interval does not include from the hierarchical data structure: Students are nested within classes
zero, there is a 95% probability that the indirect effect is significant and are therefore not randomly chosen. As there are more degrees of
(Shrout & Bolger, 2002). freedom (157) than clusters (N ⫽ 50) in the models, there is no
guarantee that the estimation of the model parameters is trustwo-
Missing Data rthy (http://www.statmodel.com/discussion/messages/12/164.html?
1191440281). However, the pattern of results emerging when the
Incomplete data often present a challenge in longitudinal re- type ⫽ complex option was applied was consistent with that emerging
search. It is not unusual for participants to be absent at one or more when the type ⫽ general option was applied.
778 BECKER, MCELVANY, AND KORTENBRUCK

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics: Correlations Between the First Order Latent Variables

Correlations

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Reading literacy
1. Reading literacy (Grade 3) —
2. Reading literacy (Grade 6) .90ⴱⴱⴱ —
Intrinsic motivation
3. Value of reading 1, activity
(Grade 4) .36ⴱⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱⴱ —
4. Value of reading 2, books
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
(Grade 4) .24 .23 .78ⴱⴱⴱ —
5. Importance of reading
(Grade 4) ⫺.17ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.24ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.50ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.42ⴱⴱⴱ —
Extrinsic motivation
6. Extrinsic motivation
through parents (Grade 4) ⫺.47ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.57ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.15ⴱⴱ ⫺.01 .13ⴱⴱ —
7. Extrinsic motivation
through school (Grade 4) ⫺.45ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.51ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.05 .17ⴱⴱⴱ .56ⴱⴱⴱ —
8. Instrumental motivation
(Grade 4) ⫺.12ⴱ ⫺.26ⴱⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.18ⴱⴱⴱ .57ⴱⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱⴱ —
Reading amount
9. Reading frequency (student
report; Grade 4) .37ⴱⴱⴱ .47ⴱⴱⴱ .72ⴱⴱⴱ .66ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.49ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.18ⴱⴱ ⫺.25ⴱⴱⴱ .26ⴱⴱⴱ —
10. Reading length (student
report; Grade 4) .18ⴱⴱⴱ .13ⴱ .48ⴱⴱⴱ .46ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.28ⴱⴱⴱ .02 ⫺.05 .27ⴱⴱⴱ .56ⴱⴱⴱ —
11. Reading length (parent
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
report; Grade 4) .45 .47 .44 .38 ⫺.29 ⫺.18 ⫺.14 .10 .63 .45ⴱⴱⴱ —
ⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .05. p ⬍ .01. p ⬍ .001.

CFI ⫽ .87, RMSEA ⫽ .08. Hence, the following analyses are CFI ⫽ .98, RMSEA ⫽ .05. Consistent with the hypothesis formu-
based on a three-factor model comprising intrinsic and extrinsic lated with respect to Research Aim 1.1, Grade 6 reading literacy
motivation as well as reading amount. was positively predicted by Grade 4 intrinsic motivation (␤ ⫽ .32,
p ⬍ .001).
Intrinsic Reading Motivation and Comprehension The second model additionally took into account Grade 4 read-
We used structural equation modeling to test the extent to which ing amount (see Figure 1). Model fit can be regarded as good,
Grade 6 reading literacy can be predicted by Grade 4 intrinsic ␹2(83) ⫽ 188.11, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽ .97, RMSEA ⫽ .04. When
motivation and whether this relationship is mediated by Grade 4 Grade 4 reading amount was controlled for, the association be-
reading amount. We expected, based on our hypotheses, that tween Grade 4 intrinsic reading motivation and Grade 6 reading
motivation would predict reading amount and that increased read- literacy was no longer statistically significant (␤ ⫽ ⫺.07, p ⫽ .73).
ing amount would predict higher reading literacy. Two models However, Grade 4 intrinsic motivation and reading amount were
were estimated. The first model specified the association between highly correlated (␤ ⫽ .85, p ⬍ .001), and Grade 6 reading literacy
Grade 4 intrinsic reading motivation and Grade 6 reading literacy. was statistically significantly predicted by Grade 4 reading amount
The model fit can be considered as good, ␹2(32) ⫽ 81.67, p ⬍ .05, (␤ ⫽ .46, p ⬍ .05). The mediating effect of reading amount on the

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics: Correlations Between the Latent First and Second Order Variables (First
Order Factors for Reading Literacy Grade 3 and 6; Second Order Factors for Intrinsic and
Extrinsic Motivation and Reading Amount)

Correlations

Variable 1 2 3 4 5

1. Reading literacy (Grade 3) —


2. Reading literacy (Grade 6) .90ⴱⴱⴱ —
3. Intrinsic reading motivation (Grade 4) .37ⴱⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱⴱ —
4. Extrinsic reading motivation (Grade 4) ⫺.52ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.64ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.09† —
5. Reading amount (Grade 4) .41ⴱⴱⴱ .43ⴱⴱⴱ .85ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.14ⴱ —
† ⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .10. p ⬍ .05. p ⬍ .001.
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC READING MOTIVATION 779

ε ε ε ε ε ε

dt78 dt79 dt76 dt73 dt29 dt81 dt80


.92*** -.82*** .66*** .55*** .84***
.91*** 1
im4val1 im4val2 im4imp

.83***
.91*** -.55***
Intrinsic
Motivation 4

-.07
Comp 6 ε
.85*** .68***

.46* .56***
Amount 4 Literacy 6 Decod 6 ε
.67***
.93***
.62***
.85*** Vocab ε
lv4freq lv4dust lv4dup
.82*** .85*** 1 .65*** .82***
dt42 dt43 dt95 deo1 deo2

ε ε ε ε

Figure 1. Associations among intrinsic reading motivation (im), reading amount, and reading literacy (stan-
dardized path coefficients ␤). dt ⫽ items from student questionnaire; deo ⫽ items from parents’ questionnaire;
lv ⫽ reading behavior; comp ⫽ comprehension; decod ⫽ decoding; vocab ⫽ vocabulary. ␹2(83) ⫽ 118.11, p ⬍
2 2
.001, CFI ⫽ .97, RMSEA ⫽ .04, RLiteracy6 ⫽ .16, RAmount4 ⫽ .73. ⴱ p ⱕ .05. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⱕ .001.

association between reading literacy and intrinsic motivation was ing effect of reading amount on the association between reading
small but statistically significant (␤ ⫽ .39, BC bootstrap 95% CFI literacy and extrinsic motivation was statistically significant (␤ ⫽
[.05, .73]). Hence, our findings supported the hypothesized medi- ⫺.04, BC bootstrap 95% CFI [⫺.08, ⫺.00]) but negligible in size.
ating effect of Grade 4 reading amount (Research Aim 1.2). The negative direct effect of extrinsic reading motivation on
reading literacy thus remained significant when we controlled for
Extrinsic Reading Motivation and Comprehension reading amount. The indirect effect of extrinsic reading motivation
on reading literacy via reading amount was marginally statistically
We next estimated two analogous models to investigate how significant. This finding makes sense in light of the negative
extrinsic reading motivation and reading literacy are associated association of extrinsic motivation with both reading amount and
and whether their relationship is also mediated by reading amount. reading literacy, as compared with the positive association be-
Again, we expected, based on our hypotheses, that motivation tween reading amount and reading literacy.
would predict reading amount and that increased reading amount In conclusion, extrinsic motivation was negatively associated
would predict higher reading literacy. The first model specified the with reading amount as well as with reading literacy, and reading
relationship between Grade 4 extrinsic reading motivation and amount did only weakly mediate the relationship between extrinsic
Grade 6 reading literacy. Model fit can be regarded as good, reading motivation and reading literacy (Research Aim 2.2).
␹2(32) ⫽ 135.00, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽ .94, RMSEA ⫽ .07. Grade 6
reading literacy was found to be negatively predicted by Grade 4
extrinsic reading motivation (␤ ⫽ ⫺.59, p ⬍ .001; Research Is There a Bidirectional Relationship of Motivation
Aim 2.1). and Reading Literacy?
In the second model, Grade 4 reading amount was included in
the analysis (see Figure 2; Research Aim 2.2). Model fit was good In the final step, previous reading literacy was included in the
(see Figure 2 for details). The negative association between Grade models. The goals in these analyses were twofold. First, we ex-
4 extrinsic reading motivation and Grade 6 reading literacy was pected not only that reading literacy would be influenced by
still strong and statistically significant (␤ ⫽ ⫺.56, p ⬍ .001). motivation but also that motivation would be influenced by read-
However, Grade 6 reading literacy was also positively predicted by ing literacy. Second, we expected that the mediator effect of
Grade 4 reading amount (␤ ⫽ .35, p ⬍ .001), and there was a small reading amount would persist even when we controlled for previ-
negative association between Grade 4 reading amount and Grade 4 ous reading literacy. As a mediator effect was found for intrinsic
extrinsic motivation (␤ ⫽ ⫺.12, p ⬍ .05). In particular, students but not for extrinsic motivation, meaning that the potential effects
with high extrinsic motivation reported lower amounts of reading of reading amount are attributable solely to intrinsic motivation, all
in terms of both reading length and reading frequency. These variables were included in a single model (see Figure 3 and the
students also showed lower reading literacy later on. The mediat- Appendix).
780 BECKER, MCELVANY, AND KORTENBRUCK

ε ε ε ε
deo1 deo2 dt95 dt42 dt43

.64*** .85*** 1 .84*** .83***

lv4dup lv4dust lv4freq

.74*** .63*** .89*** .68***


Comp 6 ε

Amount 4
.34*** .57***
Literacy 6 Decod 6 ε
.83***
Vocab 6 ε
-.12* -.56***

Extrinsic
Motivation 4

.92***
.62*** .59***

em4p em4sc em4ins


.72*** .69***
.69*** .79*** 1 .71***
.79***
dt65 dt67 dt70 dt75 dt72 dt74 dt63

ε ε ε ε ε ε

Figure 2. Associations among extrinsic reading motivation (em), reading amount, and reading literacy
(standardized path coefficients ␤). dt ⫽ items from student questionnaire; deo ⫽ items from parents’ question-
naire; lv ⫽ reading behavior; comp ⫽ comprehension; decod ⫽ decoding; vocab ⫽ vocabulary. ␹2(83) ⫽
2 2
298.80, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽ .92, RMSEA ⫽ .06, RLiteracy6 ⫽ .49, RAmount4 ⫽ .02. ⴱ p ⱕ .05. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⱕ .001.

The model fit can be considered good, ␹2(261) ⫽ 762.79, p ⬍ strongly predicted reading amount (␤ ⫽ .81, p ⬍ .001), but a
.001, CFI ⫽ .92, RMSEA ⫽ .05. The model explained a total of statistically significant association between reading amount and
86% of the variance in Grade 6 reading literacy. Consistent with later reading literacy was no longer found (␤ ⫽ .09, p ⫽ .50)
the hypothesis formulated with respect to Research Aim 3.1, when Grade reading 3 literacy was included in the model
Grade 3 reading literacy positively predicted intrinsic motiva- (Research Aim 3.2). When Grade 3 reading literacy was ac-
tion (␤ ⫽ .37, p ⬍ .001) and negatively predicted extrinsic counted for, the predictive association between Grade 4 extrin-
motivation (␤ ⫽ ⫺.52, p ⬍ .001). Grade 3 reading literacy was sic motivation and Grade 6 reading literacy persisted but was
also marginally statistically significantly associated with read- markedly weaker (␤ ⫽ ⫺.24, p ⬍ .001). Nevertheless, the overall
ing amount (␤ ⫽ .11, p ⫽ .08) and strongly predicted Grade 6 pattern of results shows that students with lower reading literacy
reading literacy (␤ ⫽ .74, p ⬍ .001). Intrinsic motivation still displayed higher levels of extrinsic motivation (e.g., reading to please
their parents) in Grade 4 and that higher extrinsic motivation predicted
lower reading literacy in Grade 6.
To summarize, when we accounted for prior achievement, the
correlations between intrinsic motivation, reading amount, and
Intrinsic Grade 6 reading literacy were weaker and no longer significant. In
Motivation 4
.01 .74*** contrast, Grade 3 achievement negatively predicted Grade 4 ex-
.37***
trinsic motivation and strongly positively predicted Grade 6 read-
.81*** ing literacy. The negative predictive effect of extrinsic motivation
.14* .11† .09
on later reading literacy persisted when prior achievement was
Literacy 3 Amount 4 Literacy 6 controlled.

-.52*** -.01 -.24*** Discussion


Extrinsic
Motivation 4 Summary and Interpretation

Figure 3. Associations among Grade 3 and Grade 6 reading literacy, The research aims guiding this study were twofold. First, we
Grade 4 intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation, and Grade 4 reading examined the relationship between intrinsic/extrinsic reading mo-
amount (standardized path coefficients ␤). ␹2(261) ⫽ 762.79, p ⬍ .05, tivation and reading literacy, as well as the potential mediating
CFI ⫽ .92, RMSEA ⫽ .05, RLiteracy6 2 2
⫽ .86, RIntrinsic Motivation4 ⫽ .14,
effect of reading amount, from a longitudinal perspective. Second,
2 2 †
RExtrinsic Motivation4 ⫽ .27, RAmount4 ⫽ .73. p ⬍ .10. ⴱ p ⱕ .05. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⱕ .001. we probed for bidirectional relationships of reading motivation and
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC READING MOTIVATION 781

reading achievement, investigating whether intrinsic/extrinsic ened, but remained statistically significant, when we controlled for
reading motivation and reading amount not only influence reading Grade 3 reading literacy. Our findings confirmed the expected
literacy but are themselves predicted by previous reading achieve- bidirectional relationship between extrinsic motivation and reading
ment. Additionally, we examined the pattern of relations emerging literacy: Grade 3 reading negatively predicted Grade 4 extrinsic
among motivation, reading amount, and later achievement when motivation, which was negatively related to Grade 6 reading
controlling for prior achievement. literacy, even when we controlled for Grade 3 reading literacy.
Consistent with our hypotheses, the data confirmed that Grade 4 Extrinsically motivated children read because they, for example,
intrinsic reading motivation was positively related to Grade 6 want to please their parents. The bidirectional relationship might
reading literacy. This relationship was mediated by reading imply that early reading failure leads to higher extrinsic motiva-
amount. In other words, children who see reading as a desirable tion, with children reading only when they have to, which in turn
activity tend to read more frequently and thus develop better leads to poorer reading skills. Morgan and Fuchs (2007) argued
reading skills (see also Guthrie et al., 1999). The initial relation- that these children avoid reading. However, our complex model
ship between Grade 4 intrinsic motivation and Grade 6 reading supports this conclusion only to a certain degree: Initially, we
achievement was as high as that reported by Unrau and Schlack- indeed found a negative relationship between extrinsic reading
man (2006) but smaller than that reported by Wang and Guthrie motivation and reading amount, but this relationship did not re-
(2004). main statistically significant when we controlled for previous
However, a different picture emerged for extrinsic reading mo- reading literacy in the more complex model. Therefore, it can be
tivation, which was negatively correlated with reading literacy and concluded that reading amount is strongly determined by the level
was not found to be substantially mediated by reading amount. In of the individual’s intrinsic motivation (and, to a certain degree, by
other words, children who read for extrinsic reasons (e.g., parental prior reading literacy) but that there is no significant additional
pressure) have poorer reading skills than do children with lower effect of extrinsic reading motivation. The negative relationship
extrinsic motivation. between extrinsic reading motivation and reading literacy may also
When previous reading literacy was taken into account, the be explained by an inadequate focus on the text, resulting from
pattern of relationships changed in some respects. Our findings ineffective strategies and inaccurate inferences (Wang & Guthrie,
indicate high stability of reading achievement from Grade 3 to 2004). From this perspective, it is possible that extrinsically mo-
Grade 6: Good readers in Grade 3 tend to still be good readers in tivated readers use surface-level strategies, such as guessing and
Grade 6; poor readers in Grade 3 tend to still be comparatively memorization, and fail to screen out nonsensical ideas (Elliott &
poor readers in Grade 6. When past achievement was taken into Dweck, 1988; Pintrich & Schrauben, 1992).
account, the effect of intrinsic motivation mediated by reading A complex picture emerges from our data: The paths from
amount was overshadowed by the direct effect of Grade 3 reading reading amount to later reading literacy just failed to reach statis-
literacy on Grade 6 reading literacy (cf. the findings reported by tical significance when prior reading literacy was included as a
Wang & Guthrie, 2004, who still found a significant relationship predictor in the model, underlining the importance of early com-
between motivation and achievement but using grades rather than petence for later development. Reading amount was strongly pre-
test scores as indicators of prior achievement). One possible ex- dicted by intrinsic reading motivation, which in turn was deter-
planation for prior achievement attenuating the effect of motiva- mined partly by early reading literacy. Additionally, the direct
tion relates to the high stability of reading achievement, which has predictive value of early reading literacy for later reading amount
been confirmed by several longitudinal studies examining the corresponds with the long-term findings of Cunningham and
development of reading achievement (e.g., Aarnoutse, van Stanovich on reading development from Grade 1 to Grade 11.
Leeuwe, Voeten, & Oud, 2001; Morgan, Farkas, & Hibel, 2008). They concluded that rapid acquisition of reading ability might help
Given this stability, little variance in achievement can be indepen- children to develop a lifetime habit of reading (Cunningham &
dently explained. Another explanation is that the earlier achieve- Stanovich, 1997, p. 934).
ment measure also includes the variance associated with motiva- Overall, the present empirical findings are well embedded in
tional aspects. Therefore, it cannot be strictly concluded from the the framework guiding this research. The distinction between
present results that intrinsic motivation has no influence on reading intrinsic and extrinsic motivation is well established in motiva-
literacy; the results rather speak against additional effects of in- tional research (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2000;
trinsic motivation when past achievement, confounded with intrin- Wigfield et al., 2006). It has been suggested that intrinsically
sic motivation, is taken into account. However, Grade 4 intrinsic motivated children invest more time and effort to fully under-
reading motivation was strongly predicted by Grade 3 reading stand texts. As a result, they tend to achieve deeper levels of
literacy. This association between past achievement and later mo- text comprehension (Baker, Dreher, & Guthrie, 2000; Schiefele,
tivation seems to indicate that individuals enjoy activities they are 1999). Our analyses further substantiate the idea that reading
good at and are thus motivated to engage in them in the future. amount (in terms of frequency and length of reading) is related
These findings are not entirely incompatible with the hypothesis of to reading motivation and mediates the relationship between
a bidirectional relationship (cf. Morgan and Fuchs, 2007, who intrinsic motivation and achievement when motivation and pre-
concluded from their review study that there is a bidirectional vious reading literacy are controlled. Still, there appear to be
relationship between reading motivation and reading literacy). differences in how the relationship between motivation and
Against the background of this discussion, the findings for achievement is established and mediated: Distinct relationships
extrinsic motivation are all the more remarkable: In contrast to and processes seem to underlie intrinsic and extrinsic motiva-
intrinsic motivation, the negative relationship between Grade 4 tion in the context of reading. The finding that the two types of
extrinsic reading motivation and Grade 6 reading literacy weak- motivation have differential implications for behavior is in line
782 BECKER, MCELVANY, AND KORTENBRUCK

with results reported by Wang and Guthrie (2004) on the basis motivation does not explain future achievement above the level
of a cross-sectional sample. Our finding of a statistically sig- attained in Grade 3. From a theoretical point of view, the problem
nificant negative effect of extrinsic motivation on later achieve- is thus not always that students fail to learn because they lack
ment with no concurrent positive effect of intrinsic motivation motivation; rather, students lack motivation because they do not
further informs the discussion about different forms of motiva- experience progress and competence. The present findings indicate
tion and the potential corruption of intrinsic by extrinsic moti- that this holds even for the very young students analyzed here. In
vation (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Higgins, Lee, Kwon, & order to motivate students, teachers must therefore offer them the
Trope, 1995). At the same time, the results have important experience of progress and competence. Additionally, the negative
implications for educators and parents, in terms of the impor- effects of extrinsic motivation on later reading literacy have clear
tance of avoiding measures that support students’ extrinsic implications for teachers and parents. Student reading motivated
rather than intrinsic reading motivation (Gottfried et al., 2001). by the wish to please parents or teachers does not promote achieve-
Our findings thus confirm that the distinction between intrinsic ment gains over time. In sum, these findings are of high relevance
and extrinsic motivation is both valid for practice and useful for for practice and research, especially in calling attention to how
reading research. educators and parents articulate reading-related expectations and
to the detrimental impact of extrinsic motivation on the develop-
Strengths, Limitations, and Outlook ment of reading literacy.

The present study has important theoretical, methodological, References


and statistical strengths. Both intrinsic and extrinsic reading mo-
tivation were examined. Reading literacy was not specified solely Aarnoutse, C., & van Leeuwe, J. (1998). Relation between reading com-
in terms of text comprehension but as a multifaceted construct with prehension, vocabulary, reading pleasure, and reading frequency. Edu-
vocabulary and decoding as additional indicators of reading cational Research and Evaluation, 4, 143–166. doi:10.1076/
achievement. The latent longitudinal design was a clear statistical edre.4.2.143.6960
Aarnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., Voeten, M., & Oud, H. (2001). Develop-
strength. Not only were students examined at three points of
ment of decoding, reading comprehension, vocabulary and spelling
measurement but all variables were estimated on a latent level and during the elementary school years. Reading and Writing, 14, 61– 89.
were hence free from measurement error. doi:10.1023/A:1008128417862
Nevertheless, certain limitations should be considered. First, it Anderson, R. C., Wilson, P. T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988). Growth in
was possible to quantify intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading
reading amount only on the basis of aggregated questionnaire data. Research Quarterly, 23, 285–303. doi:10.1598/RRQ.23.3.2
Future research should implement more differentiated measure- Baker, L., Dreher, M. J., & Guthrie, J. T. (Eds.). (2000). Engaging young
ments of each construct, potentially including other measurement readers: Promoting achievement and motivation. New York, NY: Guil-
instruments, such as diaries or checklists. Second, we examined a ford Press.
sample of elementary school children from Grades 3 to 6. Future Baker, L., & Wigfield, A. (1999). Dimensions of children’s motivation for
reading and their relations to reading activity and reading achievement.
studies should determine whether our findings can be generalized
Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 452– 477. doi:10.1598/RRQ.34.4.4
to other age groups. Third, we cannot exclude the possibility that Baker, S., Simmons, D., & Kameenui, E. J. (1995). Vocabulary acquisi-
variance in Grade 3 reading achievement is confounded with tion: Curricular and instructional implications for diverse learners
motivation. (Technical Report No. 14). Eugene, OR: National Center to Improve the
Further questions remain unresolved and warrant attention in Tools of Educators.
future studies. For example, how exactly is the relationship be- Cipielewski, J., & Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Predicting growth in reading
tween extrinsic motivation and reading literacy mediated? What ability from children’s exposure to print. Journal of Experimental Child
are the processes underlying this relationship? At this stage of Psychology, 54, 74 – 89. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(92)90018-2
research, the mechanisms remain unclear, and further investigation Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition
is necessary: whether the importance of extrinsic motivation is due and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 33, 934 –945. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.934
to the corruption of intrinsic by extrinsic motivation, whether the
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review
negative valence of the activity itself is caused by extrinsic moti- examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psy-
vation associated with some form of pressure, or how else this chological Bulletin, 125, 627– 668. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627
aspect of motivation might be relevant for performance develop- Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
ment. Future research needs to investigate how parents and edu- determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Plenum.
cators can mitigate the negative effects of extrinsic motivation on Donahue, P. L., Daane, M. C., & Yin, Y. (2005). The nation’s report card:
students’ reading development. Reading 2003 (Publication No. NCES 2004 – 453). Washington, DC:
In conclusion, this study advances research knowledge by dis- U.S. Government Printing Office.
entangling the roles of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation for read- Duke, N. K., & Pearson, P. D. (2002). Effective practices for developing
ing literacy and reading amount and highlighting the substantial reading comprehension. In A. E. Farstrup & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), What
research has to say about reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 205–242).
effect of prior achievement on later motivation. This effect has
Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
important implications for practice. Many teachers and parents go Durkin, D. (1993). Teaching them to read (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn &
to great lengths to increase or maintain students’ intrinsic reading Bacon.
motivation. However, our longitudinal findings underline the im- Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation
portance of enabling early experiences of reading competence: and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
Reading literacy is highly stable over time, and earlier intrinsic 5–12. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.5
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC READING MOTIVATION 783

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading Familie [Promotion of reading competence in the family context]. Mün-
fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, ster, Germany: Waxmann.
and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 239 –256. doi: McElvany, N., & Artelt, C. (2009). Systematic reading training in the
10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_3 family: Development, implementation, and evaluation of the Berlin
Gear, A., Wizniak, R., & Cameron, J. (2004). Rewards for reading: A Parent–Child Reading Program. Learning and Instruction, 19, 79 –95.
review of seven programs. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 50, doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.02.002
200 –203. McElvany, N., Becker, M., & Lüdtke, O. (2009). Die Bedeutung familiärer
Gee, J. (2001). Reading as situated language: A sociocognitive perspective. Merkmale für Lesekompetenz, Wortschatz, Lesemotivation und
Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 44, 714 –725. doi:10.1598/ Leseverhalten [The role of family variables in reading literacy, vocab-
JAAL.44.8.3 ulary, reading motivation, and reading behavior]. Zeitschrift für Entwick-
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Continuity of lungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 41, 121–131. doi:
academic intrinsic motivation from childhood through late adolescence: 10.1026/0049-8637.41.3.121
A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 3–13. McElvany, N., Kortenbruck, M., & Becker, M. (2008). Lesekompetenz und
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.3 Lesemotivation: Entwicklung und Mediation des Zusammenhangs durch
Guthrie, J. T., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and motivation in Leseverhalten [Reading literacy and reading motivation: Their develop-
reading. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr ment and the mediation of the relationship by reading behavior].
(Eds.). Reading research handbook (Vol. 3, pp. 403– 422). Mahwah, NJ: Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 3/4, 207–219. doi:10.1024/
Erlbaum. 1010-0652.22.34.207
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Barbosa, P., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A., Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., & Hibel, J. (2008). Matthew effects for whom?
Davis, M. H., . . . Tons, S. (2004). Increasing reading comprehension Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 31, 187–198.
and engagement through concept-oriented reading instruction. Journal Morgan, P. L., & Fuchs, D. (2007). Is there a bidirectional relationship
of Educational Psychology, 96, 403– 423. doi:10.1037/0022- between children’s reading skills and reading motivation? Exceptional
0663.96.3.403 Children, 73, 165–183.
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Metsala, J. L., & Cox, K. E. (1999). Motiva- Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Kennedy, A. M., & Foy, P. (2007). IEA’s
Progress in International Reading Literacy Study in Primary School in
tional and cognitive predictors of text comprehension and reading
40 countries. Chestnut Hill, MA: TIMSS & PIRLS International Study
amount. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 231–256. doi:10.1207/
Center, Boston College.
s1532799xssr0303_3
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998 –2008). Mplus user’s guide. Los
Heller, K. A., & Perleth, C. (2000). KFT 4–12⫹R. Kognitiver Fähigkeit-
Angeles, CA: Author.
stest für 4 bis 12 Klassen, Revision [Cognitive Abilities Test for Grades
Naceur, A., & Schiefele, U. (2005). Motivation and learning: The role of
4 to 12, revision]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
interest in construction of representation of text and long-term retention:
Hidi, S. (2000). An interest researcher’s perspective: The effects of extrin-
Inter-and intra-individual analyses. European Journal of Psychology of
sic and intrinsic factors on motivation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harack-
Education, 20, 155–170. doi:10.1007/BF03173505
iewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal
Nagy, W. (1988). Teaching vocabulary to improve reading comprehen-
motivation and performance (pp. 309 –339). San Diego, CA: Academic
sion. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Press. doi:10.1016/B978-012619070-0/50033-7
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000).
Higgins, E. T., Lee, J., Kwon, J., & Trope, Y. (1995). When combining
Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scien-
intrinsic motivation undermines interest: A test of activity engagement
tific research literature on reading and its implications for reading
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 749 –767.
instruction: Reports of the subgroups (Report of the National Reading
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.749 Panel, NIH Publication No. 00 – 4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Govern-
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in ment Printing Office.
covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alterna- Nauck, J., & Otte, R. (1980). Diagnostischer Test Deutsch [Diagnostic test
tives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/ German]. Braunschweig, Germany: Westermann.
10705519909540118 Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2001).
International Reading Association. (2000). Excellent reading teachers: A Knowledge and skills for life: First results from PISA 2000. Paris,
position statement of the International Reading Association. Journal of France: Author.
Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 44, 193–200. Pintrich, P. R., & Schrauben, B. (1992). Students’ motivational beliefs and
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cam- their cognitive engagement in classroom academic tasks. In D. H.
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Schunk & J. L. Meece (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom (pp.
Klicpera, C., & Gasteiger-Klicpera, B. (1993). Lesen und Schreiben: 149 –183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Entwicklung und Schwierigkeiten [Reading and writing: Development Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Motivation in education: Theory,
and difficulties]. Bern, Switzerland: Huber. research, and application (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Merrill
Kolen, M. J., & Brennan, R. L. (2004). Test equating, scaling, and linking: Prentice Hall.
Methods and practices (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Springer. Pressley, M., Borkowski, J. G., & Schneider, W. (1989). Good information
Kortenbruck, M. (2007). Entwicklung der Lesekompetenz: Einfluss von processing: What it is and how education can promote it. International
Motivation und Geschlecht [Development of reading literacy: The in- Journal of Educational Research, 13, 857– 867. doi:10.1016/0883-
fluence of motivation and gender]. (Unpublished Diplom thesis). Free 0355(89)90069-4
University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations:
Küspert, P., & Schneider, W. (1998). Würzburger Leise Leseprobe (WLLP) Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psy-
[Würzburg Silent Reading Test]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. chology, 25, 54 – 67. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020
Lehmann, R. H., Peek, R., & Poerschke, J. (1997). HAMLET 3– 4: Ham- Schaffner, E., & Schiefele, U. (2007). Auswirkungen habitueller Lesemo-
burger Lesetest für 3 bis 4 Klassen [HAMLET 3– 4: Hamburg Reading tivation auf die situative Textrepräsentation [Effects of habitual reading
Test for Grades 3 to 4]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz. motivation on the situative representation of text]. Psychologie in Erz-
McElvany, N. (2008). Förderung von Lesekompetenz im Kontext der iehung und Unterricht, 54, 268 –286.
784 BECKER, MCELVANY, AND KORTENBRUCK

Schiefele, U. (1999). Interest and learning from text. Scientific Studies of Unrau, N., & Schlackman, J. (2006). Motivation and its relation to reading
Reading, 3, 257–279. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0303_4 achievement in an urban middle school. Journal of Educational Re-
Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy search, 100, 81–101. doi:10.3200/JOER.100.2.81-101
acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, Verhoeven, L., & Van Leeuwe, J. (2008). Prediction of the development of
94, 143–174. doi:10.1348/000712603321661859 reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psy-
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non- chology, 22, 407– 423. doi:10.1002/acp.1414
experimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psycho- Wang, J. H., & Guthrie, J. T. (2004). Modeling the effects of intrinsic
logical Methods, 7, 422– 445. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422 motivation, extrinsic motivation, amount of reading, and past reading
Snow, C. E. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward a research and achievement on text comprehension between U.S. and Chinese students.
development program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 162–186. doi:10.1598/RRQ.39.2.2
RAND. Weiss, R. (1987). Wortschatz (WS) und Zahlenfolgen (ZF): Ergänzungst-
Snow, C. E., Scarborough, H. S., & Burns, M. S. (1999). What speech- ests zum Grundintelligenztest CFT-20 [Basic Intelligence Test, Scale 2,
language pathologists need to know about early reading. Topics in CFT 20]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.
Language Disorders, 20, 48 –58. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., & Davis-Kean, P.
Taboada, A., Tonks, S., Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. (2009). Effects of (2006). Development of achievement motivation. In W. Damon & N.
motivational and cognitive variables on reading comprehension. Read- Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., pp. 933–1002).
ing and Writing, 22, 85–106. doi:10.1007/s11145-008-9133-y New York, NY: Wiley.
Taylor, B. M., Frye, B., & Maruyama, J. (1990). Time spent reading and Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Motivation for reading: An over-
reading growth. American Educational Research Journal, 27, 351–362. view. Educational Psychologist, 32, 57–58.

(Appendix follows)
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC READING MOTIVATION 785

Appendix

Full Model of Figure 3

ε ε ε ε ε ε

dt78 dt79 dt76 dt73 dt29 dt81 dt80

.91*** .92*** -.82*** .66*** .55*** .84*** 1

im4val1 im4val2 im4imp


.74***
.83***
.91*** -.55***
Intrinsic
Motivation 4
ε Comp. 3 .01 Comp 6 ε
.75*** .37*** .74***
.81***
.63*** .62***
ε Decod 3 .11+ .09
Literacy 3 Amount 4 Literacy 6 Decod 6 ε
.67*** .65***
.61*** lv4dup
.79*** .93***
deo1 ε .78***
.14* .82***
ε Vocab 3 Vocab 6 ε
-.52*** -.01 lv4freq lv4dust deo2
.85*** 1 ε
.82*** -.24***
dt42 dt43 dt95

Extrinsic ε ε
Motivation 4

.90***
.64*** .59***

em4p em4sc em4ins


.71*** .69***
.69*** .79*** .71***
.80*** 1
dt65 dt67 dt70 dt75 dt72 dt74 dt63
ε ε ε ε ε ε
Note. Residual variances of the indicators of literacy in Grades 3 and 6 were constrained to be equal over
time; indicators of literacy in Grades 3 and 6 were allowed to correlate specifically for each domain (e.g.,
decoding 3 with decoding 6). For decoding (r ⫽ .51, SE ⫽ .05) as well as for vocabulary (r ⫽ .17, SE ⫽ .08),
the correlation was statistically significant (not depicted here). im ⫽ intrinsic motivation; em ⫽ extrinsic
motivation; dt ⫽ items from student questionnaire; deo ⫽ items from parents’ questionnaire; lv ⫽ reading
behavior; comp ⫽ comprehension; decod ⫽ decoding; vocab ⫽ vocabulary. ⴱ p ⱕ .05. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⱕ .001.

Received June 15, 2009


Revision received April 12, 2010
Accepted May 6, 2010 䡲

You might also like