You are on page 1of 10

Health Communication

ISSN: 1041-0236 (Print) 1532-7027 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hhth20

Framing as a Concept for Health Communication:


A Systematic Review

Lars Guenther, Maria Gaertner & Jessica Zeitz

To cite this article: Lars Guenther, Maria Gaertner & Jessica Zeitz (2020): Framing as a
Concept for Health Communication: A Systematic Review, Health Communication, DOI:
10.1080/10410236.2020.1723048

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2020.1723048

Published online: 29 Jan 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 21

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hhth20
HEALTH COMMUNICATION
https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2020.1723048

Framing as a Concept for Health Communication: A Systematic Review


a,b
Lars Guenther , Maria Gaertnerc, and Jessica Zeitzc
a
Institute for Journalism and Communication Studies, University of Hamburg; bCentre for Research on Evaluation, Science and Technology (CREST),
Stellenbosch University; cInstitute of Communication Research, Friedrich Schiller University Jena

ABSTRACT
Over the past decades, research in the fields of both framing and health communication has grown
exponentially. However, to date, no systematic review has been conducted about how framing – as
a concept – has been used in health communication. The present study provides a systematic review of
the current research, applying a quantitative content analysis to the published literature on framing in health
communication. Articles published in peer-reviewed scientific journals (N = 316) were analyzed in order to
describe common characteristics and topics, as well as the degree to which the sociological and psycholo-
gical tradition of framing, different types of frames, and visual framing have been used. Furthermore, framing
effect studies were examined to see whether they contained covariates, competitive frames, and/or gain and
loss framing. The results reveal that the most common topics in the literature on framing in health
communication were related to cancer, nutrition, and vaccination. Most articles use quantitative research
designs, most commonly surveys with experimental designs; the majority of articles are based on the
psychological tradition of framing. Thematic and generic frames occur almost equally, while the lack of
research on visual frames is clearly noticeable. More than half of the effect studies test the effects of gain and
loss framing, whereas competitive frames are under-studied. The results of this systematic review help to
identify both current trends and research gaps on framing in health communication, and how it differs from
framing in communication science overall – valuable information that can inform future research.

Introduction different from its application in communication science overall


A vast body of literature has developed on framing, both for (e.g., Von Sikorski & Matthes, 2019), but no systematic review
communication science in general (e.g., D’Angelo et al., 2019; has ever examined how the concept has been used in health
Entman, 1993; Reese, 2010; Scheufele & Tewksbury, 2007) and communication. The goal of the present study is to fill this
for health communication in particular (e.g., Guenther, conceptual gap. Framing is an important concept in health
Froehlich, Milde, Ruhrmann, & Heidecke, 2015; Rothman & communication, for instance, dealing with representations of
Salovey, 1997). Framing is one of the most popular theoretical health information in the media and with effects of different
approaches used in communication science (e.g., Cacciatore, presentations on audiences (e.g., Guenther et al., 2015;
Scheufele, & Iyengar, 2016; D’Angelo & Kuypers, 2010), but Guenther, Milde, Ruhrmann, & Heidecke, 2013).
despite its popularity, it has received little attention on an over- The present study applies a systematic review of peer-
arching, analytical level, and even less in its application in health reviewed published literature on framing in health communica-
communication (see also Von Sikorski & Matthes, 2019). tion, using content analysis. This approach is significant, since
Despite the often-stated theoretical and conceptual weakness conceptual analyses can indicate where current research stands
of the concept (e.g., Borah, 2011; Scheufele & Iyengar, 2011; and where its gaps are located, which might guide future
Scheufele & Tewksbury, 2007) and the different methodological research directions (Guenther & Joubert, 2017). One of the
approaches used (e.g., Kohring & Matthes, 2002; Matthes & main aims of the present study is to compare its findings to the
Kohring, 2008; Scheufele & Scheufele, 2010), research on fram- findings from systematic reviews of framing in general commu-
ing has grown steadily over the years (e.g., D’Angelo et al., 2019). nication science (Borah, 2011; Matthes, 2009), which might in
However, the number of systematic reviews of research on turn help identify the extent to which researchers in health
framing is limited in communication science (e.g., Borah, 2011; communication have used the concept differently from research-
Matthes, 2009). This is also true for the specific field of health ers in other fields of communication science. Some researchers
communication, which is surprising, as in this research field, see framing as a “fractured paradigm” (Entman, 1993, p. 51);
there are even meta-analyses available that deal with specific thus, this systematic review is especially valuable for identifying
aspects of framing – such as the effects of gain and loss framing the common characteristics and topics of research in this area
(e.g., O’Keefe & Jensen, 2006, 2007; O’Keefe & Nan, 2012). The (see also Kim, Park, Yoo, & Shen, 2010), as well as the degree to
application of framing in health communication could be which the sociological and psychological tradition of framing,

CONTACT Lars Guenther lars.guenther@uni-hamburg.de Institute for Journalism and Communication Studies, University of Hamburg, Grindelberg 7,
Hamburg 20144, Germany.
© 2020 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
2 L. GUENTHER ET AL.

and the different types of frames (e.g., generic and thematic 1989; Goffman, 1974) and a psychological (e.g., Tversky &
frames) have been used in health communication. Kahneman, 1981; see also Scheufele & Iyengar, 2011) conceptual
tradition of framing. The dual nature can be summarized as
frames in external texts, images, or news (sociological) and
Frames, framing, and framing in health frames in individuals’ minds (psychological) (Borah, 2011).
communication The psychological tradition can further be divided into equiva-
lence framing (based on prospect theory and dealing with dif-
Framing in communication science
ferent linguistic presentations of the same information; Tversky
For communication science in general, framing researchers & Kahneman, 1981) and emphasis framing (presenting a topic
hail from a broad variety of scientific fields and disciplines, differently through selection and salience of relevant facts, such
including sociology, economics, political science, psychology, as episodic vs. thematic framing; Scheufele & Iyengar, 2011).1
cognitive linguistics, and communication science (e.g., Borah, Recently, some researchers have defined the application of
2011; D’Angelo, 2002; Reese, 2010; Scheufele & Tewksbury, framing in communication studies as the communication science
2007). This interdisciplinary diversity of fields might be one tradition of framing (e.g., Matthes, 2014). If communicators are
reason for why frames are not clearly defined, conceptualized, successful in the sense that journalists or members of the public
and operationalized (Scheufele, 2004; Scheufele & Scheufele, adopt their frames, communication science researchers refer to
2010). Frames “have at least four locations in the communica- frame building and frame setting (Matthes, 2014; Tewksbury &
tion process: the communicator, the text, the receiver and the Riles, 2018). The process in which communicators or journalists
culture” (Entman, 1993, p. 52). develop frames is referred to as frame production.
While many different definitions abound as to what a frame is Current research in communication science indicates that
(e.g., Gamson & Modigliani, 1989; Reese, 2010), Entman (1993) how communicators (such as journalists) produce frames
provided the one that is most widely accepted: framing stresses seems to be less important to researchers (see also D’Angelo
certain aspects of reality and pushes others into the background et al., 2019). For instance, Borah (2011) found that only 2% of
through the processes of selection and salience (see also Entman, research articles deal with frame production. Furthermore,
Matthes, & Pellicano, 2009). “To frame is to select some aspects of researchers seem to focus on textual frames but not visual
perceived reality and make them more salient in a communicating ones (D’Angelo et al., 2019; Matthes, 2014). Borah (2011) also
text, in such a way as to promote a particular problem definition, found that communication scientists more often focus on the
causal interpretation, moral evaluation, and/or treatment recom- sociological tradition of framing and less often on the psy-
mendation for the item described” (Entman, 1993, p. 52). In this chological tradition. This is in line with the methods being
definition, ‘problem definition’ refers to issues and actors, ‘causal used by researchers: most scholars who apply framing use
interpretation’ defines who (persons) or what (situations) is content analyses, distantly followed by experimental surveys
responsible for a problem, ‘moral evaluation’ indicates ways to (Borah, 2011). Identifying whether these findings are also true
evaluate an issue, and ‘treatment recommendation’ proposes how for research in health communication is one of the aims of the
best to solve a problem (Matthes, 2014). Thus, problem definition, present study.
causal interpretation, moral evaluation, and treatment recom- Framing researchers also differentiate between two types of
mendation have been described as the frame elements (Kohring frames. (1) Generic frames are found across themes (Iyengar,
& Matthes, 2002; Matthes & Kohring, 2008). They systematically 1991), such as the differentiation between episodic and the-
organize and structure information, creating packages of inter- matic framing (see also Scheufele & Iyengar, 2011) or frames
pretation (see also Gamson & Modigliani, 1989); hence, frames such as ‘attribution of responsibility’, ‘conflict’, ‘human inter-
are more than just topics (Reese, 2010). est’, ‘economic consequences’, and ‘morality’ (e.g., Semetko &
Although some researchers have called for a paradigm to unify Valkenburg, 2000). This type is also referred to as consistent
research on framing, this would not be desirable for D’Angelo frames (Borah, 2011). (2) Thematic frames can be identified as
(2002, p. 871), who sees one major advantage of the knowledge applicable to a specific topic (e.g., Entman, 1993; Kohring &
accumulation of different perspectives: a so-called “multiparadig- Matthes, 2002). Borah (2011) refers to these as unique frames.
matic research program.” For him, the “‘fractured’ nature of the Trends in the current research in communication science
concept is its strength” (D’Angelo et al., 2019, p. 14). A diversity of indicate that researchers are more frequently concerned with
approaches provides an interdisciplinary quality to research on thematic than generic frames. Matthes (2009) found that of
framing (Reese, 2010). Still, a lack of clear conceptualization and the studies he analyzed, only 22% analyzed generic frames
operationalization of framing has led to a high number of framing (the conflict frame was the most-used generic frame).
studies that are distinct from each other, with contradicting con- Similarly, Borah (2011) found that only a third of all articles
ceptual definitions of frames and framing (Scheufele & Scheufele, she sampled used generic frames. These findings might be
2010). To this day, researchers actively discuss the definition of different for the field of health communication research, due
what a frame actually is, and how framing is similar or different to the dominance of gain and loss framing. Furthermore, in
compared to related approaches, such as agenda setting and communication science overall, framing effects studies have
priming (e.g., Borah, 2011; Scheufele & Iyengar, 2011; Scheufele tested single frames in experimental conditions more often
& Tewksbury, 2007). than multiple or competing frames (Borah, 2011), i.e., settings
Despite all the debates around conceptualizing and operatio- in which participants receive a set of mixed frames.
nalizing frames, researchers can still differentiate between Identifying whether this is also true for the field of health
a sociological (e.g., Entman, 1993; Gamson & Modigliani, communication is one of the aims of the present study.
HEALTH COMMUNICATION 3

Framing in health communication designed as a systematic review, exploring the current state of
the research literature and its gaps.
Some of the tendencies described for framing as a theoretical
concept in communication science might also be true for the
specific field of health communication. Framing can be a useful
The present study
concept for health communication – describing, for instance,
how health information is presented in the media and how Since there are two broad foundations of framing – socio-
different presentations of health impact audiences (Guenther logical and psychological (e.g., Borah, 2011; Matthes,
et al., 2015, 2013). Consequently, framing can be used to design 2014) – and since scholars in health communication have
persuasive messages to influence an audience to perform desired produced findings relating to both traditions, the first
health-related behaviors (such as those involved in preventing research question (RQ) is interested in the extent to
and detecting diseases) (Von Sikorski & Matthes, 2019). which these traditions have been applied in health
Under the sociological tradition of framing in health com- communication.
munication, a variety of health issues have been studied.
These often deal with questions such as whether individual RQ1: How much of the published literature on framing in
or societal attributions of responsibility are represented (e.g., health communication focuses on the sociological or the psy-
Kim & Willis, 2007, for obesity); to what degree prevention, chological tradition of framing?
detection, and treatment of diseases are represented (e.g.,
Atkin, Smith, McFeters, & Ferguson, 2008, for breast cancer); Since there are two types of frames – thematic and generic
and if representations reflect incidence or mortality rates (see frames (e.g., Borah, 2011) – the second RQ is interested in the
also Jensen, Moriarty, Hurley, & Stryker, 2010). It seems that extent to which these types of framing have been used in
much research is devoted to specific health topics, such as health communication.
how cancer is represented in the media (e.g., Jensen et al.,
2010), but the content perspective of the literature on framing RQ2: How much of the published literature on framing in
has not yet been systematically analyzed. health communication focuses on thematic or generic frames?
In terms of the psychological tradition of framing, much
research in health communication is devoted to the effects of Framing effect studies in health communication seem to be
gain and loss frames (e.g., O’Keefe & Jensen, 2007, 2008; O’Keefe particularly interested in the effects of gain and loss frames;
& Wu, 2012), often based on the assumptions of prospect theory a remarkable number of meta-analyses are available (O’Keefe &
(see Rothman & Salovey, 1997). Gain frames “emphasize desir- Jensen, 2006, 2007; O’Keefe & Nan, 2012). Therefore, the third RQ
able consequences associated with compliance with the advo- asks:
cated viewpoint, [whereas loss frames highlight] undesirable
consequences associated with noncompliance (O’Keefe & RQ3: What are the common characteristics of the published
Jensen, 2008, p. 52). A re-occurring question is whether gain- literature on framing effects in health communication, and how
framed or loss-framed messages are more effective for preven- many of these studies focus on gain and loss framing?
tion behaviors compared to detection behaviors (see also
Gallagher & Updegraff, 2012). Thus, much research has been
devoted to the question of whether one type of framing is more Method
effective in promoting healthy behavior than another; however,
Sample
studies have produced mixed results.
While gain and loss frames can be considered generic frames, While many publications deal with framing in health com-
research in health communication has also focused on the effects munication, the present study was focused on published lit-
of thematic frames (Guenther et al., 2015, 2013), but it is assumed erature of high quality (see also Borah, 2011). We equated
that this has only happened to a limited degree (Von Sikorski & quality with peer review and thus decided that our unit of
Matthes, 2019). Consequently, one might assume that health analysis would be published articles in peer-reviewed scienti-
communication is dominated by framing effects studies that fic journals. We were also only interested in literature using
deal with gain and loss framing and thus with generic frames. the concept of framing in health communication contexts.
Nevertheless, examples of effects of thematic frames include Following the PRISMA model (Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, &
research that focuses on the question of whether these frames Altman, 2009), we used the search term “fram* ‘health com-
trigger specific health-related perceptions, knowledge, or beha- munication’” in Web of Science (N = 461) and on Google
vioral intentions (e.g., Riles, Sangalang, Hurley, & Tewksbury, Scholar (N = 1300) in August and September 2018. All 1,761
2015). Some of the dependent variables used in these studies refer articles that were found were screened for their use of the
to the frame elements outlined before (Entman, 1993). concept of framing in a health communication context, indi-
While framing seems to be used as a theoretical concept by cated in their titles, abstracts, or keywords. In addition to our
scholars in health communication, this kind of research has definition of published peer-reviewed literature, we also
not been systematically synthesized. To date, no systematic excluded non-English articles and ‘gray’ literature that was
reviews have been performed; hence, no overview has been listed among the findings (books, book chapters, dissertations,
given of how the concept is applied in this specific field of theses, and conference proceedings). This resulted in a sample
research. Based on these observations, the present study is of N = 307 articles.
4 L. GUENTHER ET AL.

We noted that six meta-analyses were among these articles; Intercoder reliability was assessed using a random sample
consequently, we checked the reference lists of five of the available of 15% (n = 42) of the total articles after several coder training
meta-analyses2 to identify other relevant articles (N = 119) that sessions. Using Cohen’s Kappa, the coders reached satisfac-
were not captured by the search term and the databases used. This tory results, with all scores higher than κ = .82; thus, the two
extended the sample size to N = 426 articles. Finally, we down- coders independently coded the sample articles.
loaded the available full texts of the articles and examined their
relevance in detail again.3 This resulted in a sample size of N = 316
Results
articles that represent the base for our investigation. An overview
of all articles included in the sample can be obtained from email- In terms of sample characteristics, the year in which the most
ing the corresponding author. articles from the study sample were published was 2016
(n = 43; 14%). Figure 1 indicates an increase in publication
output, with volatilities. Many of the articles were published
Quantitative content analysis in Health Communication (n = 61; 19%), the Journal of Health
Communication (n = 24; 8%), or Health Psychology (n = 13;
A systematic, quantitative content analysis helped to answer
4%). The average number of authors was three (M = 3.05;
the RQs. For the content analysis, we differentiated between
SD = 2.014), with most articles having two (n = 108; 34%),
formal and content-related categories. The codebook used in
three (n = 62; 20%), one (n = 52; 17%), or four author(s)
this study can be obtained from the corresponding author.
(n = 42; 13%).
There were 965 unique authors listed. Among them, one
Formal categories author was counted 19 times – the highest number of appear-
Formal categories were directly extracted from the title pages of ances. However, this was an anomaly; 856 authors (89%) were
the articles. We coded the publication year, the journal the article counted only once. In terms of their affiliations, most authors’
was published in (openly coded), and the number of authors institutions were located in the US (n = 637; 66%). The UK
(also openly coded). For each author we coded their name, ranked second (n = 57; 6%), followed by the Netherlands
affiliation, and gender. For gender identification, the authors’ (n = 51; 5%), Australia (n = 39; 4%), Canada (n = 32; 3%),
names and website presence guided the coding. The affiliations and Germany (n = 24; 3%). Figure 2 displays a world map of
also determined whether or not authors were cooperating inter- authors’ institutions, highlighting that only 30 unique coun-
nationally. We also coded the country in which the study was tries were counted. International cooperation was only
conducted, which was not always in line with the authors’ observed for 28 articles (9%), which increased only slightly
affiliations. The formal categories in the present study were over the years sampled.
drawn from research studies with similar goals (Borah, 2011; In line with these findings, a majority of the studies were
Guenther & Joubert, 2017; see also Kim et al., 2010). set up in the US (n = 167; 53%), followed by the UK (n = 17;
5%), the Netherlands (n = 12; 4%), and Australia (n = 11; 4%).
Content-related categories Of the studies sampled, 20 (6%) did not clearly indicate in
The content-related categories were determined by coders which country their study was conducted, and 22 were set up
reading the abstract and keywords and browsing through in more than one country (7%). Finally, in terms of the
the article. First, we openly coded the health topics under author’s gender, a majority of the authors were identified as
investigation; at a later stage, they were categorized similar female (n = 573; 59%). While a dominance of female authors
to the classification provided by Kim et al. (2010). Often, titles was evident throughout, the number of female authorships
and keywords indicated the topics under study. also slightly increased over the years sampled.
In line with RQ1, we first coded the methods used in the The openly coded topics were classified into subcategories
study (e.g., content analysis, survey), whether the study (see also Kim et al., 2010). The most common topics studied
employed a qualitative or quantitative method, and the tradi- in the sampled articles were cancer (n = 68; 22%); obesity,
tion (sociological or psychological) of framing (Borah, 2011). foods, diets, and nutrition (n = 44; 14%); vaccination (n = 36;
We extended this to include a third category called ‘frame 11%); tobacco, cigarettes, and smoking (n = 27; 9%); sexually
production’, added for studies that dealt with processes transmitted infections and diseases (n = 23; 7%); sport topics
through which frames are created. (steroids, physical activity, exercise, and fitness) (n = 17; 5%);
In line with RQ2, we coded the types of frames, with and HIV/AIDS (n = 17; 5%). For the three most dominant
thematic frames defined as unique frames that are topic- topics, Figure 1 demonstrates that cancer was important from
specific and with generic frames defined as consistent frames the beginning, while vaccination only became important later.
used across studies, such as gain and loss frames (Borah, 2011; Topics that were not often investigated include mental health
Matthes, 2009). Furthermore, we also coded whether the (n = 14; 4%), alcohol and drinking (n = 11; 4%), drugs (n = 7;
studies used visual frames or not. For all studies that were 2%), health policy (n = 6; 2%), diabetes (n = 2; .6%), and
identified as framing effects studies (see also Borah, 2011), we allergies (n = 1; .3%)
coded whether participants received only one frame or a mix In terms of RQ1, most articles sampled had used experi-
of competitive frames within the experimental conditions of mental survey designs (n = 206; 65%), followed by content
the study (Borah, 2011). Moreover, we also coded whether analyses at a distant second (n = 60; 19%). Theoretical papers
covariates were present. Lastly, in line with RQ3, we coded (n = 15; 5%) and interviews (n = 9; 3%) had even lower
whether framing effects studies used gain or loss frames. frequencies. Case studies (n = 3; 1%) and focus group
HEALTH COMMUNICATION 5

50

45

40

35

30

25

20

15

10

0
1987 1990 1993 1995 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014 2015 2016 2017

obesity cancer vaccinations total

Figure 1. Comparison of total publication output and publication output concerning the three most dominant topics under study.

Figure 2. A world map of authors’ institutions and their publication frequencies.

discussions (n = 2; 1%) were a small minority. In line with mixed-methods (n = 9; 3%) were the minority. Table 1 shows
these findings, most articles were based on the psychological that the methodologies used in these articles varied systemati-
tradition of framing (n = 218; 69%). Almost a quarter was cally by framing tradition and by methodological design (i.e.,
based on the sociological tradition (n = 76; 24%), while three qualitative or quantitative): Studies following the psychological
articles (.9%) used both traditions of framing. Only four tradition predominantly used experimental surveys and (to
articles (1%) dealt with processes of frame production. a lesser degree) meta-analyses, which were more often than not
Furthermore, most articles used quantitative methods quantitative. Studies following the sociological tradition of
(n = 266; 84%); qualitative methods (n = 26; 8%) and framing predominantly used content analysis, also often
6 L. GUENTHER ET AL.

Table 1. Methodologies used, in comparison to framing traditions, methodological designs, and types of frames.
Methodologiesa
Meta-analysis Content analysis Interview Focus group discussion Case study Experimental survey design
Tradition of framing
Sociological tradition / 60 (21.1%) 6 (2.1%) 2 (.7%) 3 (1.1%) /
Psychological tradition 4 (1.4%) / / / / 206 (72.5%)
Processes of frame production / / 3 (1.1%) / / /
Quantitative vs. qualitative designs
Quantitative 3 (1.1%) 51 (18%) 1 (.4%) / / 205 (72.2%)
Qualitative / 6 (2.1%) 8 (2.8%) 2 (.7%) 3 (1.1%) 1 (.4%)
Mixed-methods 1 (.4%) 3 (1.1%) / / / /
Type of frame
Generic 3 (1.1%) 3 (1.1%) 1 (.4%) / / 143 (50.4%)
Thematic / 55 (19.4%) 8 (2.8%) 2 (.7%) 3 (1.1%) 63 (22.2%)
a
We excluded theoretical articles and those whose methodology included more than one methodology or did not fall under one of the categories provided here
(n = 284).

quantitatively; in some rare cases, they used interviews, focus doubled since 2010, compared to 2000. This fact is an indicator
group discussions, or case studies – that is, methodologies for of the increasing popularity of framing as a concept in health
which a qualitative design is more likely. communication.
In terms of RQ2, the articles sampled in this study dealt It might be considered a positive development that the
almost equally with generic (n = 151; 50%) and thematic reviewed articles were published in 147 different journals by
(n = 146; 48%) frames.4 Table 1 also shows that the use of 965 different authors. This might indicate of the breadth of
generic versus thematic frames varied with respect to the applying framing in health communication. The fact that only
methodologies used in the sampled studies: generic frames 12 researchers have published five or more articles over the
dominate in experimental surveys and meta-analyses, while entire period is a possible indication that relatively few
thematic frames are more commonly used in content analyses, researchers are leading framing research on health commu-
interviews, focus group discussions, and case studies. nication. Likewise, the concept of framing has been the sub-
Furthermore, the analysis revealed that visual frames were ject of many different journals, even though almost one-third
included in a minority of studies (n = 27; 9%): four studies of the articles reviewed (27%) were published in journals that
(1%) were solely about visual frames, and 23 studies used specifically specialize in health communication, such as
a mix of textual and visual frames (7%). Health Communication or Journal of Health Communication.
In terms of RQ3, for framing effects studies (n = 213), Many articles in this sample (54%) were written by two or
competitive frames were tested in only nine articles (4%). three authors; fewer were written by a single author. This
Hence, most studies tested single frames in experimental might be interpreted to indicate that when it comes to
conditions. A sizable majority of studies tested influences of research on framing in health communication, researchers
covariates (n = 208; 98%). Furthermore, a majority 64% seem to collaborate, although not as much on an international
(n = 137) of all framing effects studies tested effects of gain level. Considering that the sample contained only English-
and loss framing. language articles, there seems to be a dominance of Western
countries. In this context, the trend toward international
cooperation has developed in recent years, albeit with only
Discussion
a slightly significant increase. The positive shift toward insti-
The aim of this study was to analyze how framing is applied as tutional cooperation and internationalization are probably
a concept of health communication, providing a systematic good signs for the future of this discipline (see also Kim
review of the peer-reviewed published literature in this field. et al., 2010). Still, the world map in Figure 2 shows that for
The overarching research question dealt with how framing is English-language articles on framing in health communica-
used in scientific publications and which current trends and tion – as sampled here – there are less research contributions
research gaps exist in the field of health communication from the southern hemisphere, in particular from Africa,
research. Asia, and the Middle East. This does not automatically mean
The 316 articles in the sample were published between 1987 that this research does not exist, but it shows that readers of
and 2018. The highest number of articles in a single year was English-language journals do not encounter many research
published in 2016. The results indicate that research on framing items from these regions. In addition, more than half of the
in health communication has been steadily and significantly authors in this sample (59%) were female. This has increased
increasing in terms of the number of peer-reviewed research slightly over time, showing a positive trend of gender diversity
outputs. Kim et al. (2010) also observed a general tendency of that is highly uncommon in many scientific fields.
increased research output in their analysis of studies published in The most frequently cited topics in the study’s sample were
the journal Health Communication. While only a few studies on cancer, obesity and nutrition, and vaccines. This is barely in line
framing were published before 2006, since 2007 the number of with Kim et al.’s (2010) study on the published literature in
published articles has increased. According to the results of the Health Communication, and it might indicate that researchers
current study, the number of published articles has more than who are interested in framing choose specific health-related
HEALTH COMMUNICATION 7

topics for their studies. Only occasionally did studies in the thematic frames, researchers with a psychological foundation
present sample examine other health issues, such as health policy seem to test generic frames.
or dental health. As a result, while a great variety of thematic The findings emphasized so far, and their discussion and
references was identified, most of the studies focused on only implications, need to be taken into account against the backdrop
three topics. What is in line with Kim et al.’s (2010) results is that of this study’s limitations. While sampling and data collection were
studies related to cancer seem to be a major contributor to done systematically, in terms of sample, we only used one search
research in health communication, and to research on framing string in a small number of databases. In addition, we were only
in health communication. Therefore, future research should able to include the studies to which we had access; hence, we
expand to a broader variety of health topics. cannot guarantee the representativeness of our sample. We also
In terms of the methods used in the studies sampled, more only included peer-reviewed research articles and thus missed the
than half of the articles used a survey with experimental broader gray literature. We also only considered English-language
design, while only 19% of the studies carried out a content articles, although framing in health communication is a research
analysis. Furthermore, most research was based on the psy- topic that is addressed in many different countries with articles
chological tradition of framing and employed quantitative published in many different languages. If more languages were
methodologies. This contradicts the findings of Borah included, maybe the observed dominance of the US would be
(2011), where most framing studies followed the sociological slightly different. Despite the study’s goal to provide a general
tradition. Hence, this finding of the present study might overview on the studied topic, not all of the conducted research
indicate a unique fingerprint of the application of framing work could be taken into account, such as research items that are
to health communication that deserves further elaboration. not published. Hence, like many systematic reviews, the present
The finding mentioned above might be caused by the high study contains bias in that sense that it only included published
proportion of framing effect studies in the present sample – studies articles in English-language journals. Future research could
that predominantly deal with gain and loss frames. This finding broaden the scope of the present study by including more litera-
probably also explains why the articles sampled almost equally deal ture in different languages. Finally, a qualitative analysis of the
with generic and thematic frames. This finding also contradicts research articles sampled here could identify issues of framing in
those of Matthes (2009) and Borah (2011), again indicating health communication in even greater detail (see also Borah,
a unique characteristic of the application of framing to health 2011), but this was beyond the scope of the present investigation.
communication that future research should explore further. For instance, future research could take into account the different
What matches previous work on the overall application of definitions, conceptualizations, and operationalization of frames
framing in communication science (e.g., Borah, 2011) is the fact across studies, which would help working toward a clearer defini-
that very few studies in research on framing in health commu- tion of frames and delineating conceptual confusion regarding
nication seem to focus on frame production. However, investi- framing, agenda setting, and priming (e.g., Scheufele & Iyengar,
gating how frames are developed is needed for a more complete 2011; Scheufele & Tewksbury, 2007). Furthermore, the present
understanding of the framing concept. Therefore, future inves- study used categories developed in other systematic reviews, such
tigations could focus more on this topic, for instance, analyzing as traditions and types of framing (e.g., Borah, 2011; Matthes,
how frames are developed in different communicator contexts, 2009); however, in health communication, additional categories
such as how health journalists, general journalists, and campaign might be worth exploring. Hence, analyzing further categories
strategists develop frames and what goals they try to achieve. beyond the ones of the present study remains a task for future
Furthermore, the findings of the present study indicate a strong research.
focus on text-based frames, while only a minority of studies In sum, based on the outcomes of the present study and
concentrate on visual framing. D’Angelo et al. (2019) stressed keeping its limitations in mind, framing in health communication
that the analysis of visual framing remains a challenge for com- seems to show similarities with how framing is applied in com-
munication science. They also confirmed that studies on visual munication science overall, especially in terms of the lack of
framing tend to be rare and that it is more common to observe research focusing on frame production, visual frames, and com-
a mix of text-based and visual frames. Indeed, 92% of all studies petitive frames in effect studies (Borah, 2011). However, there also
in the present sample focused solely on text-based framing. As seem to be some unique features, such as the dominance of
a result, further research might be required in visual framing. psychological studies and the equal attention given to generic
In terms of the framing effect studies, only a small minority of and thematic frames. Consequently, further research could focus
the present sample tested competitive frame conditions. This on existing research gaps, in order to develop a broader under-
result confirmed Borah’s (2011) finding. So far, it seems that little standing of the framing process and framing effects in the domain
research has attended to mixed frames in health communication of health communication. Until this is accomplished also in health
effect studies. Future research could therefore focus more on communication, “framing research remains robust and troubled”
understanding the effects of mixed frames in experimental set- (D’Angelo et al., 2019, p. 24).
tings. Although research on competitive frames has increased over
the last decade, many open questions remain. The fact that most
framing effect studies in the present sample deal with gain and loss Notes
frames might indicate a gap in the research dealing with the effects
1. It should be highlighted that some researchers rather prefer
of thematic frames. The findings of this study indicate a slight a narrow definition of framing and framing effects, in which
correlation between the two foundations of framing: while they only refer to equivalency framing as the definition of frames
researchers with a sociological foundation seem to identify derived from psychology (Cacciatore et al., 2016; Scheufele &
8 L. GUENTHER ET AL.

Iyengar, 2011). The present study uses a broader definition that is T. Hanitzsch (Eds.), Handbook of Journalism studies (pp. 175–190).
more in line with Borah (2011) and Matthes (2014), who rather Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
relate the psychological tradition of framing to the fact that Gallagher, K. M., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Health message framing effects
individuals’ minds are being analyzed. Both equivalency and on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: A meta-analytic review. Annals of
emphasis framing can be used to analyze individuals’ minds, as Behavioral Medicine, 43, 101–116. doi:10.1007/s12160-011-9308-7
well as potential effects of framing. Gamson, W. A., & Modigliani, A. (1989). Media discourse and public
2. We only had access to five of the six identified meta-analyses. opinion on nuclear power: A constructionist approach. American
3. Availability relates to the fact that in many cases, the authors of Journal of Sociology, 95(1), 1–37. doi:10.1086/229213
the current study did not have access to some of the articles. In Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of
these cases, we used ResearchGate and emailed the authors, but in experience. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
more cases than not this process was unsuccessful. Articles to Guenther, L., Froehlich, K., Milde, J., Ruhrmann, G., & Heidecke, G.
which we had no access had to be excluded from the sample. (2015). Effects of valenced media frames of cancer diagnoses and
4. In this case, we excluded theoretical papers, leading to a sample therapies: Quantifying the transformation and establishing of eva-
size of 301. Additionally, for four articles (1.3%), it could not be luative schemas. Health Communication, 30(11), 1055–1064.
determined whether they used thematic or generic frames. doi:10.1080/10410236.2014.917839
Guenther, L., & Joubert, M. (2017). Science communication as a field of
research: Identifying trends, challenges and gaps by analysing research
Acknowledgments papers. Journal of Science Communication, 16(1), 1–19. doi:10.22323/
2.16020202
The authors would like to thank both editors and anonymous reviewers Guenther, L., Milde, J., Ruhrmann, G., & Heidecke, G. (2013). Das
for providing helpful feedback to their work. The authors would also like Thema Krebs und die Medien: Über die Wirkung von
to thank Daniela Mahl for commenting on an earlier version of the Medienframes auf Rezipienten [Cancer and the media: Effects of
manuscript and all authors who provided access to their articles in media frames on recipients]. In C. Rossmann & M. Hastall (Eds.),
cases in which we had no institutional access. Medien und Gesundheitskommunikation: Befunde, Entwicklungen,
Herausforderungen [Media and health communication: Findings,
developments, challenges] (pp. 49–64). Baden-Baden, Germany:
Nomos.
Disclosure of potential conflict of interest
Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible?: How television frames political
There is no conflict of interest. issues. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Jensen, J. D., Moriarty, C. M., Hurley, R. J., & Stryker, J. E. (2010).
Making sense of cancer news coverage trends: A comparison of
Data availability statement three comprehensive content analyses. Journal of Health
Communication, 15(2), 136–151. doi:10.1080/10810730903528025
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the Kim, H. J., Park, S., Yoo, S., & Shen, H. (2010). Mapping health com-
corresponding author [LG], upon reasonable request. munication scholarship: Breadth, depth, and agenda of published
research in health communication. Health Communication, 25(6–7),
487–503. doi:10.1080/10410236.2010.507160
ORCID Kim, S. H., & Willis, L. A. (2007). Talking about obesity: News framing
of who is responsible for causing and fixing the problem. Journal of
Lars Guenther http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7760-0416 Health Communication, 12(4), 359–376. doi:10.1080/
10810730701326051
Kohring, M., & Matthes, J. (2002). The face(t)s of biotech in the nineties:
References How the German press framed modern biotechnology. Public
Understanding of Science, 11, 143–154. doi:10.1088/0963-6625/11/2/304
Atkin, C. K., Smith, S., McFeters, C., & Ferguson, V. (2008). Matthes, J. (2009). What’s in a frame? A content analysis of media
A comprehensive analysis of breast cancer news coverage in leading framing studies in the world’s leading communication journals,
media outlets focusing on environmental risks and prevention. Journal 1990–2005. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 86(2),
of Health Communication, 13(1), 3–19. doi:10.1080/10810730701806912 349–367. doi:10.1177/107769900908600206
Borah, P. (2011). Conceptual issues in framing theory: A systematic Matthes, J. (2014). Framing. Baden-Baden, Germany: Nomos.
examination of a decade’s literature. Journal of Communication, 61 Matthes, J., & Kohring, M. (2008). The content analysis of media frames:
(2), 246–263. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2011.01539.x Toward improving reliability and validity. Journal of Communication,
Cacciatore, M. A., Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2016). The end of framing 58(2), 258–279. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00384.x
as we know it … and the future of media effects. Mass Communication Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred
and Society, 19(1), 7–23. doi:10.1080/15205436.2015.1068811 reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The
D’Angelo, P. (2002). News framing as a multiparadigmatic research PRISMA statement. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 62(10),
program: A response to Entman. Journal of Communication, 52(4), 1006–1012. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.005
870–888. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02578.x O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2006). The advantages of compliance or
D’Angelo, P., & Kuypers, J. A. (2010). Introduction. In P. D’Angelo & the disadvantages of non-compliance? A meta-analytic review of the
J. Kuypers (Eds.), Doing news framing analysis (pp. 1–13). New York, relative persuasive effectiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed mes-
NY: Routledge. sages. In C. S. Beck (Ed.), Communication yearbook 30 (pp. 1–43).
D’Angelo, P., Lule, J., Neuman, W. R., Rodriguez, L., Dimitrova, D. V., & Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Carragee, K. M. (2019). Beyond framing. A forum for framing O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2007). The relative persuasiveness of
researchers. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 96(1), gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encountering disease pre-
12–30. doi:10.1177/1077699018825004 vention behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Health
Entman, R. M. (1993). Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured Communication, 12(7), 623–644. doi:10.1080/10810730701615198
paradigm. Journal of Communication, 43(4), 51–58. doi:10.1111/ O’Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2008). Do loss-framed persuasive
j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x messages engender greater message processing than do
Entman, R. M., Matthes, J., & Pellicano, L. (2009). Framing politics in the gain-framed messages? A meta-analytic review. Communication
news: Nature, sources and effects. In K. Wahl-Jorgensen & Studies, 59(1), 51–67. doi:10.1080/10510970701849388
HEALTH COMMUNICATION 9

O’Keefe, D. J., & Nan, X. (2012). The relative persuasiveness of gain- and operational implications for measuring frames and framing effects.
loss-framed messages for promoting vaccination: A meta-analytic In P. D’Angelo & J. Kuypers (Eds.), Doing news framing analysis (pp.
review. Health Communication, 27(8), 776–783. doi:10.1080/ 110–134). New York, NY: Routledge.
10410236.2011.640974 Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2011). The state of framing research:
O’Keefe, D. J., & Wu, D. (2012). Gain-framed messages do not motivate A call for new directions. Retrieved from http://pcl.stanford.edu/
sun protection: A meta-analytic review of randomized trials compar- research/2011/scheufele-framing.pdf
ing gain-framed and loss-framed appeals for promoting skin cancer Scheufele, D. A., & Tewksbury, D. (2007). Framing, agenda setting, and
prevention. International Journal of Environmental Research and priming: The evolution of three media effects models. Journal of
Public Health, 9(6), 2121–2133. doi:10.3390/ijerph9062121 Communication, 57(1), 9–20. doi:10.1111/j.0021-9916.2007.00326.x
Reese, S. D. (2010). Finding frames in a web of culture: The case of the Semetko, H. A., & Valkenburg, P. M. V. (2000). Framing European
war on terror. In P. D’Angelo & J. Kuypers (Eds.), Doing news framing politics: A content analysis of press and television news. Journal of
analysis (pp. 17–42). New York, NY: Routledge. Communication, 50(2), 93–109. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2000.
Riles, J. M., Sangalang, A., Hurley, R. J., & Tewksbury, D. (2015). tb02843.x
Framing cancer for online news: Implications for popular perceptions Tewksbury, D., & Riles, J. M. (2018). Framing in an interactive news
of cancer. Journal of Communication, 65(6), 1018–1040. doi:10.1111/ environment. In P. D’Angelo (Ed.), Doing news framing analysis II.
jcom.12183 Empirical and theoretical perspectives (pp. 137–162). New York,
Rothman, A. J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate London: Routledge.
healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin, Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the
121(1), 3–19. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.3 psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453–458. doi:10.1126/
Scheufele, B. T. (2004). Framing-effects approach: A theoretical and science.7455683
methodological critique. Communications – the European Journal Von Sikorski, C., & Matthes, J. (2019). Framing-Effekte im
of Communication Research, 29(4), 401–428. doi:10.1515/ Gesundheitsbereich [Framing effects in health communication]. In
comm.2004.29.4.401 C. Rossmann & M. Hastall (Eds.), Handbuch der
Scheufele, B. T., & Scheufele, D. A. (2010). Of spreading activation, Gesundheitskommunikation [Handbook of Health Communication]
applicability, and schemas: Conceptual distinctions and their (pp. 1–13). Wiesbaden, Germany: Springer VS.

You might also like