You are on page 1of 4

5&95

Music Therapy
Today
WFMT online journal
Volume 14, No. 1

Music Therapy Today publishes ar%cles that are related to music therapy
educa%on, prac%ce, and research. Categories may include, but are not li-
mited to Editorials, Presiden%al Notes, Posi%on Statements, Curriculum
Reports, Clinical Case Studies, Research Reports, Service Projects, World
Congresses Proceedings, Interviews, Book Reviews, and Online Resources.

2018 WFMT. All rights reserved. ISSN: 1610-191X

47
5&95

Implementing a Song as a Reward for


MUSICTHERAPYTODAY, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

Transition from FreeAPlay Time to a


Group Activity
Kumi Sato
Shigeki Sonoyama

Abstract Resumen

Even though there are substantial numbers A pesar de que hay un número considerable
of studies investigating transition strategies, de estudios que investigan estrategias de tran-
there is still an interest in and the need for sición, todavía existe un interés y la necesidad
research on transition due to its frequent oc- de investigar sobre la transición debido a su
currence in everyday life. Research suggests frecuente ocurrencia en la vida cotidiana. Los
implementation of music as a prompt effec- estudios existentes sobre este tema sugieren
tive to promote smooth transitions; how- la implementación de la música como un
ever, the effect of music implemented as a medio eficaz para promover transiciones sin
reward has not been discussed yet. Since re- problemas; sin embargo, el efecto de la mú-
wards are used as commonly as prompts in sica implementada como recompensa no se
educational settings, the current study exam- ha discutido aún. Debido a que las recompen-
ined the effect of a song as a reward for sas se utilizan con tanta frecuencia como pau-
younger children with developmental disabi- tas en entornos educativos, el estudio actual
lities during transitions, in comparison with comparó el efecto de una canción como re-
the effect of a song as a prompt. The parti- compensa para niños con trastornos del desa-
cipants were three children at the age of 3 or rrollo durante las transiciones, con el efecto de
4, who required support to initiate and com- una canción como aviso. Los participantes
plete transitions, especially from free-play eran tres niños con edades de 3 ó 4 años, que
time to a group activity. The result indicates requerían apoyo para iniciar y completar las
two of the participants initiated the transi- transiciones, especialmente del tiempo de
tion faster when the song was implemented juego libre a una actividad grupal. El resultado
as a prompt (Intervention A), whereas the indica que dos de los participantes iniciaron la
time they took after initiation of the transi- transición más rápidamente cuando la can-
tion was reduced when the song was imple- ción se implementó como aviso (Intervención
mented as a reward (Intervention B). For the A), mientras que el tiempo que tomaron des-
other participant, implementing the song as pués del inicio de la transición se redujo
a reward was effective to decrease the time cuando la canción se implementó como re-
needed for initiating and completing the compensa (Intervención B). Para el otro par-

48
5&95

transition. The effect of these interventions ticipante, la implementación de la canción


MUSICTHERAPYTODAY, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

on their independence during the transition como recompensa fue efectiva para disminuir
will be discussed also. el tiempo necesario para iniciar y completar la
transición. También se discutirá el efecto de
Keywords: song, reward, transitions, children estas intervenciones en su nivel de indepen-
with developmental disabilities, single-sub- dencia durante la transición.
ject research design
Palabras clave: canción, recompensa, transi-
ciones, niños con discapacidades del desarrollo.

Implementing a song as a reward for transi- ving a larger selection of alternative rewards
tion from free-play time to a group activity as would be beneficial for practitioners because
interest in and needs of therapeutic use of they can attempt to find which type of re-
music is growing in the area of education, re- wards is suitable for the child and provide the
searchers have investigated its effect to teach best support. Contrary to importance and
children, especially individuals with disabili- popularity of rewards in educational settings
ties who need special support. Since learning (Hoffmann, Huff, Patterson, & Nietfeld, 2009),
in a similar environment, which children with- research on use of music as a reward is limited
out disabilities have, is one of the general in music therapy literature. Lim (2010) stated
goals for children with disabilities, research in that music stimuli worked as both a prompt
music therapy has shown how music can as- and an automatic reward, but the function
sist them acquiring necessary or expected of music as a reward was not directly exa-
skills (Katagiri, 2009; De Mers, Tincani, Van mined in this study. Although research has
Norman, & Higgins, 2009; Register, Darrow, explored effective use of music as a prompt,
Standley, & Swedberg, 2007). Some studies potential effect of music as a reward has not
were conducted in a school or home setting investigated yet.
so that the participants can maintain the skills
after termination of the music therapy inter- Research on effective strategies incorpora-
vention without additional training (Kern, ting rewards is needed in other area of dis-
Wakeford, & Aldridge, 2007; Kern, Wolery, & ciplines also. Sterling-Turner and Jordan
Aldridge, 2007; Pasiali, 2004; Register & (2007) conducted a literature review of re-
Humpal, 2007). Music was used to deliver a search on interventions to support transi-
cue or create a structure in these studies; in tions in individuals with autism, and they
other word, music was provided to promote pointed out most of the available studies exa-
specific behaviors before the target behaviors mined interventions using some kinds of
occur. prompts including verbal prompts/audio
cues, visual support, and video priming.
In practical educational settings, however, re- They argued the need for further studies in-
wards are selected and offered as much as vestigating consequent components in tran-
prompts, depending on social context and sitions. Even though there is a substantial
environmental conditions, to enhance appro- number of empirical studies about promo-
priate behaviors of children. Therefore, ha- ting smooth transitions, interest in research

49
5&95

associated with transitions is ongoing due to The effect of music in promoting smooth
MUSICTHERAPYTODAY, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

its frequent occu-rrence in daily life and the transitions has been examined also (Gad-
likelihood that children with disabilities find berry, 2011; Register & Humpal, 2007). The
it challenging. results of these studies demonstrated musi-
cal interventions decreased transition times
A few studies about the effect of rewards in as well as increased independence in the par-
transition times include the research con- ticipants during transitions. In the guideline
ducted by Waters, Lerman, and Hovanetz for identifying appropriate transition support,
(2009). The participants were two 6-year music and singing are also listed as an exam-
old boys diagnosed with autism, and they ple of auditory prompts besides verbal war-
had difficulty terminating a preferred activ- nings and timers (Hume, Sreckovic, Snyder, &
ity and initiating a non-preferred activity. Carnahan, 2014). However, music was incor-
The results indicate that a visual schedule, porated as a prompt in these models. There-
which is a commonly used prompt, would fore, the current study will examine the effect
not be effective by itself, and it should be of music provided as a reward in transition
combined with appropriate rewards and times, compared to the effect of music pro-
limited access to preferred activities, that vided as a prompt.
is extinction. Cote, Thompson, and McKer-
char (2005) provided three typically deve- Furthermore, since transition requires a se-
loping toddlers with interventions for tran- quence of tasks including terminating an
sitioning from the play area to the toileting engaged activity, physically moving or shif-
area. They found that the participants’ ting attention, and preparing for the next
compliance increased if access to preferred activity, what part of transition a child find
activities was not allowed after the initial it challenging should be different. Some
instruction; moreover, the effect was even children might need support to start physi-
more significant when a reward (e.g. a toy cally moving even though they can finish
to carry with) was delivered with the ex- the previous activity without any prompts
tinction procedure, compared to when a (Sterling-Turner & Jordan, 2007). Others
verbal warning was given 2 minutes prior might need prompts to terminate an en-
to a transition. Hanley, Tiger, and Ingvarsson gaged activity though they can quickly clean
(2009) investigated strategies to increase up and move to a different area once they
preschoolers’ selection of non-preferred finish the previous activity. Hume, Srec-
but academically important activities during kovic, Snyder, and Carnahan (2014) argued
free-play time. Although their research was how important it is to individualize transi-
not conducted in scheduled transitions, tion support because the best intervention
they encouraged the participants to transi- and when it should be implemented varies
tion from a preferred activity to a non-pre- depending on the child’s chronological/de-
ferred activity in the free-play period. The velopmental age or abilities. In addition to
results revealed embedded reinforcement, comparing the effect of music as a reward
such as decorating the activity area with and its effect as a prompt during transitions,
popular children’s cartoon characters, in- the authors will deeply discuss how diffe-
creased and maintained the participants’ rently each participant in this study, who
engagement in originally non-preferred ac- had different learning needs, responded to
tivities. the interventions.

50

You might also like